Abstract
Objective
Optimism and pessimism are associated with cardiovascular disease mortality and progression, however the biological mechanism remains unclear. This study investigates the association between optimism/pessimism and concentrations of seven inflammation and hemostasis markers.
Methods
This cross-sectional study used data from the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA), a study of 6814 persons aged 45–84 with no history of clinical cardiovascular disease. The Life-Orientation Test—Revised (LOT-R) was used to measure dispositional optimism and pessimism. Regression analyses were used to estimate associations of optimism and pessimism with interleukin-6 (IL-6), C-reactive protein (CRP), fibrinogen, homocysteine, factor VIII, D-dimer, and plasmin-antiplasmin, before and after adjustment for sociodemographics, depression, cynicism, health behaviors, BMI, hypertension, and diabetes.
Results
Higher scores on the LOT-R (positive disposition) were related to lower concentrations of IL-6 (p=0.001), fibrinogen (p<0.001) and homocysteine (p=0.031). Associations were stronger for the pessimism subscale. After adjustment for demographics, the percentage differences in inflammatory markers corresponding to a 2-standard deviation increase in pessimism were 6.01% (p=0.001) for IL-6; 10.31% (p=0.001) for CRP; 2.47% (p<0.0001) for fibrinogen, and 1.36% (p=0.07) for homocysteine. Associations were attenuated but significant after adjustment for sociodemographics, depression, cynical distrust, and behaviors. Further adjustment for hypertension, BMI and diabetes reduced associations for CRP and IL-6. Pessimism remained associated with a 1.36% (p=0.02) increase in fibrinogen in the fully adjusted model. Factor VIII, D-dimer and plasmin-antiplasmin were not associated with the LOT-R or subscales.
Conclusions
Pessimism is related to higher levels of inflammation. Health behaviors, BMI, hypertension and diabetes appear to play a mediating role.
Keywords: Psychosocial factors, inflammation, coagulation, epidemiology, risk factors
Several studies have linked life-orientation, as reflected by levels of optimism and pessimism, to cardiovascular disease risk (1–8). Optimism is generally defined as the tendency to believe that future expectations and goals will be met, and is different than simply the absence of depression. Although prior studies have conceptualized optimism and pessimism as opposing poles of a continuum, recent studies show they may be better characterized as distinct, negatively related factors (9–12). Optimism has been linked to lower cardiovascular disease (CVD) and coronary heart disease (CHD) mortality (5), lower rates of nonfatal myocardial infarction (MI), slower progression of carotid atherosclerosis (4, 7, 8), better outcomes after coronary artery bypass graft procedures and fewer re-stenoses after percutaneous transluminal coronary angioplasty (13). The related construct of pessimism, defined as the general tendency to expect negative outcomes, has been shown to be associated with increased adverse cardiac events, including MI and death from CHD (6), and interacts with socioeconomic status in increasing rates of hypertension (14).
There are several mechanisms through which optimism and pessimism may affect cardiovascular disease and its progression, but these mechanisms have not been well established in empirical studies. Elucidating these mechanisms is important for strengthening causal inferences regarding the health consequences of life orientation and will also enhance our understanding of the etiology of cardiovascular disease generally. One possible mechanistic pathway involves health behaviors. Life orientation may affect the adoption and maintenance of behaviors such as diet and physical activity, which are known risk factors for cardiovascular disease. Dispositional optimism has been reported to be associated with nonsmoking, increased physical activity, moderate alcohol consumption, and more fruits, vegetables and whole grains in the diet (15–17). This finding may result from optimistic persons being more likely to anticipate benefits of behavioral change and hence may be more likely to adopt and maintain these changes over time.
It is also plausible that optimism and pessimism affect health through biological mechanisms independent of health behaviors that have been linked to chronic inflammation and CVD. For example, dispositional optimism may downregulate the autonomic nervous system (ANS) and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis (18–20). In contrast, a pessimistic disposition may stimulate chronically increased sympathetic tone and stimulation of the HPA axis (20–21), which in turn may induce chronic inflammation and hemostasis. Constructs related to optimism and pessimism, such as positive affect and cynical distrust, have been shown to be associated with IL-6 and CRP (22–23). However, to our knowledge, the relationship between optimism and pessimism and markers of inflammation and hemostasis has not yet been investigated.
In this study, we explore the relationship between life-orientation and seven circulating inflammation and hemostasis markers that have been previously associated with increased CVD risk: IL-6, CRP, homocysteine, fibrinogen, D-dimer, factor VIII, and plasmin antiplasmin (24). We use data from the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA), a large and diverse cohort of individuals from 45 to 84 years of age. We hypothesized that increased optimism and decreased pessimism would be inversely related to levels of inflammation and hemostatic markers, and these associations would be attenuated but remain significant after adjustment for traditional CVD risk factors, behaviors, depression, and cynical distrust. We investigated life orientation as a single bipolar scale, as well as differentiated into two distinct but negatively related constructs (optimism and pessimism). We hypothesized that stronger associations would be observed with the two-factor approach.
Methods
MESA is a longitudinal study, supported by the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (NHLBI), with the goal of identifying risk factors for subclinical atherosclerosis and its progression. The study cohort consists of 6814 men and women between the ages of 45 to 84 years of age, free of clinical CVD at the baseline exam. Individuals were recruited from 6 field centers around the United States using a variety of population-based approaches including lists of area residents, HCFA lists of area residents (for participants ≥65 of age), area residents enrolled in a union health plan, and random digit dialing. Only persons free of clinical cardiovascular disease at baseline were eligible. Among those screened and deemed eligible, the participation rate was 59.8%. The cohort was 38% white, 28% black, 22% Hispanic and 12% Chinese at baseline. Repeat examinations have occurred every 1.5–2 years with a total of four examinations to date. Details of the study design have been published elsewhere (25). The baseline visit took place between July 2000 and August 2002. The first follow-up visit (during which optimism and pessimism were assessed) occurred between September 2002 and February 2004.
Dispositional optimism, defined as the tendency to expect good outcomes in the future, was measured using the Life Orientation Test—Revised (LOT-R) at the first follow-up visit. This scale is a 6-item revision of the original LOT (9, 26). The LOT-R has been shown to have adequate internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.76), as well as 4-week (0.79) and 13-week (0.79) test re-test reliability (26–27). It includes 3 positively worded and 3 negatively worded items. The items were assessed on a 4-point Likert scale, and negatively worded items are reverse-coded and added to the positively worded items, creating a bipolar scale, with higher scores indicating an optimistic disposition. Factor analysis has shown that positively and negatively worded items may be summed separately, and loaded onto two separate subscales, rather than opposing poles (9, 10–12). We investigated both approaches in order to make appropriate comparisons to prior studies and fully explore the effects of optimism and pessimism on hemostatic and inflammatory factors. We created three partly overlapping measures: 6-item full-scale LOT (LOT-R), 3 positively worded items (optimism subscale), and 3 negatively worded items (pessimism subscale). Higher values on the LOT-R and optimism subscale measures correspond to a more positive orientation, whereas higher levels on the pessimism subscale indicate a negative orientation. Each measure was categorized into quartiles for descriptive analyses, with higher quartiles corresponding to higher scores within the designated scale or subscale.
Inflammatory markers examined include IL-6, CRP, fibrinogen, and homocysteine. Hemostatic markers investigated include factor VIIIc, D-dimer, and plasmin-antiplasmin. These markers were selected because they are commonly used inflammatory and hemostatis markers that have been previously linked to the occurrence of CVD events or to the development of atherosclerosis (28–35). The inflammatory markers, IL-6, CRP, and fibrinogen, have been reported to be independent risk factors for CVD mortality by contributing to endothelial dysfunction and plaque formation (28–29, 31). Homocysteine is thought to induce oxidative stress and inflammation, promoting the formation of foam cells and atherosclerotic plaques, and is therefore considered a marker or precursor for inflammation in this study (36). Markers of hemostasis chosen for this study, D-dimer, factor VIII, and plasmin-antiplasmin, are part of the coagulation cascade and fibrin cross-linking system (31–35). Upregulation of the coagulation pathway and dysfunction of the fibrinolytic system is hypothesized to contribute to the incidence of cerebrovascular accidents and myocardial infarction by increasing susceptibility for ischemic episodes (37). Psychosocial factors like optimism and pessimism may affect components of the inflammation or hemostasis pathway, thereby affecting risk for incident CVD events.
Blood was drawn from all participants in the fasting state (or after a non-caffeine, non-fat snack) at the baseline exam, and standardized methods were used for processing and shipping to a central laboratory (Laboratory for Clinical Biochemistry Research, University of Vermont, Burlington, VT). IL-6 was measured by an ultrasensitive enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) from R&D systems (Minneapolis, MN), and the laboratory analytical CV for this assay was 6.3%. High-sensitivity CRP and fibrinogen were measured by nephelometry using the BNII nephelometer (N High Sensitivity to CRP and N antiserum to human fibrinogen, Dade Behring Inc, Deerfield, IL). The intra-assay and inter-assay analytical CVs for CRP ranged from 2.3 to 4.4% and from 2.1 to 5.7%, respectively. Corresponding CVs for fibrinogen were 2.7% and 2.6%, respectively. Plasma homocysteine was measured by fluorescence polarization immunoassay with an IMx Analyzer (IMx Homocysteine Assay; Axis Biochemicals ASA, Oslo, Norway). The laboratory analytical CV range was 3.8 to 5.1%. Factor VIII coagulant activity was measured using the Sta-R analyzer (STA-Deficient VIII; Diagnostica Stago, Parsippany, NJ, USA) with an analytical CV of 10%, D-dimer by immunoturbidometric methods on the Sta-R analyzer (Liatest D-DI; Liatest VWF, Diagnostica Stago, Parsippany, NJ, USA) with an analytical CV of 8%, and plasmin-antiplasmin by a two-site ELISA that utilizes two monoclonal antibodies, with an analytical CV of 1.7%.
Sociodemographic information, medical history, health behavior and cardiovascular risk factor characteristics were obtained by questionnaires during the baseline exam. Education was classified into three groups: less than high school, complete high school or equivalent, and complete college or higher education. Household income over the past 12 months was divided into 5 categories: <$12,000, $12–25,000, $25–40,000, $40–75,000, $75,000+. Race and ethnicity were reported based on the participants’ responses to questions modeled after the 2000 US Census measure.
Behavioral risk factors included smoking, diet, and physical activity. Cigarette smoking was quantified by pack-years of use. Physical activity was assessed using a questionnaire adapted from the Cross-Cultural Activity Participation (38), and a composite measure of activity time and level of intensity were used to calculate total intentional exercise activity in MET-min/week. Dietary patterns were gauged using a 120-item, modified-Block style, food frequency questionnaire, and were summarized using four factors derived using factor analysis: “fats and processed meats” (Western), “vegetables and fish” (Chinese), “beans, tomatoes, and refined grains” (Hispanic), and “whole grains and fruits” (prudent) (39).
Other covariates included diabetes, hypertension, body mass index, recent infection, current use of medications known to alter inflammatory response, fasting state, cynical distrust and depression. Diabetes was defined by the 2003 American Diabetes Association’s criteria of fasting plasma glucose >126 mg/dl (40), or use of insulin or other diabetes medications. Hypertension was classified by the JNC IV 1997 criteria, of diastolic blood pressure >=90 or systolic blood pressure >=140 or self-reported history of hypertension or current use of any hypertensive medications. Body mass index was calculated as weight over height (in meters) squared. Participants were categorized as having a recent infection if they reported having a cold/flu, sinus, urinary or tooth infection, bronchitis or pneumonia in the past 2 weeks. Medication use was a binary variable created for individuals that were currently using lipid-lowering medications, nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs, or aspirin. Participants were considered to be fasting if the last thing the person ate or drank was greater than 8 hours prior to the blood draw. Depression was measured using the 20-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies-Depression questionnaire (CES-D) (41). Cynical distrust was measured by an 8-item subset of the Cook-Medley hostility scale, which has previously been shown to relate to CVD (42–44).
Biomarkers of inflammation and hemostasis were log-transformed for analyses, and back-transformed to the original metric as geometric means for presentation. In descriptive analyses, we examined gradients of inflammatory and hemostatic markers as well as key covariates across quartiles of optimism, after adjusting for age, sex and race/ethnicity using Chi-squared tests. Linear regression was used to estimate the association of each log-transformed inflammatory and hemostatic marker with the optimism measures after adjustment for potential confounders or mediators. Given that no clear threshold effects were observed in descriptive analyses, optimism and pessimism were investigated as continuous predictors. In order to facilitate interpretation, exponentiated regression coefficients from regression models with logged continuous outcomes are reported as percent differences in the circulating level of the biomarkers associated with a difference in the optimism or pessimism levels equivalent to 2 standard deviations in the sample.
The associations of optimism/pessimism with inflammation and hemostasis markers were initially investigated in a base model (model 1), which was adjusted for demographic characteristics (age and sex), fasting state, recent infections, and medication use. In models for subscales we also adjusted for the opposing LOT-R subscale (ie. if optimism was the predictor, then the analysis was adjusted for pessimism). Next, the confounding effect of socioeconomic position and negative psychosocial factors was assessed in model 2 by adding measures of race/ethnicity, household income and education, depression and cynical distrust to the base model. Model 3 additionally adjusted model 2 for health behaviors, including pack-years of cigarette use, diet, and physical activity. Model 4 further adjusted model 3 for biomedical risk factors including hypertension, BMI and DM. Heterogeneity in associations of optimism with the outcomes by race/ethnicity, sex, and depression were examined by including appropriate interaction terms in model 1. Of 63 tests, only 6 were statistically significant. Inspection of statistically significant interactions using plots revealed no consistent or substantial effect modification so analyses were subsequently adjusted for these factors.
All reported probability values correspond to 2-tailed tests, which were considered significant at the 0.05 level. The study was approved by Institutional Review Boards at all participating institutions. All subjects gave written informed consent.
Results
Means for selected characteristics of the full sample (6195), and for those in each quartile of the LOT-R and its subscales are shown in Table 1. The mean age was 62.1 years; the sample was 52% female and 38.5% self-identified as white, 11.8% as Chinese, 27.8% as African American, and 22.0% as Hispanic. The geometric means (standard deviation) of the biomarkers for the full sample were 1.24 pg/mL (1.22 pg/mL) for IL-6, 1.92 mg/dL (3.2 mg/dL) for CRP, 339.31 mg/dL (73.9 mg/dL) for fibrinogen, 8.87 (3.75) for homocysteine, 0.22 umol/L (0.87 umol/L) for D-dimer, 150.83% (67.0 %) for Factor VIII, and 4.43 nM (2.19 nM) for plasmin-antiplasmin.
Table 1.
Optimism Measure |
Mean Score |
Mean Age, y |
% Female |
% White |
Mean Income, $ |
Mean Education Y |
Physical Activity, met-min/wk |
% Current Smokers |
Prudent Diet Quartile |
Mean BMI |
% DM |
% HTN |
% Recent Infection |
% Using Medication |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Full sample (n=6195) | 62.2 | 53 | 38 | 48851 | 13.04 | 1553 | 13 | 2.03 | 28.34 | 13 | 45 | 22 | 53 | |
LOT-R | 19.9 | |||||||||||||
Quartile 1 | 15.7 | 62 | 53 | 40 | 44842 | 12.7 | 1515 | 15 | 1.93 | 28.44 | 14 | 45 | 22 | 52 |
Quartile 2 | 20.1 | 61.5 | 51 | 41 | 50900 | 13.42 | 1609 | 13 | 2.01 | 28.32 | 12 | 42 | 23 | 53 |
Quartile 3 | 22.5 | 61.9 | 53 | 44 | 55785 | 13.84 | 1600 | 11 | 2.13 | 28.37 | 10 | 45 | 22 | 55 |
Quartile 4 | 24 | 62.7 | 53 | 31 | 50811 | 12.99 | 1573 | 10 | 2.15 | 27.85 | 10 | 44 | 24 | 53 |
P for trend | 0.044 | 0.78 | 0.002 | 0.002 | 0.002 | 0.61 | 0.001 | 0.002 | 0.02 | 0.001 | 0.58 | 0.348 | 0.329 | |
Optimism Subscale | 10 | |||||||||||||
Quartile 1 | 6.8 | 61.8 | 54 | 48 | 47161 | 12.94 | 1400 | 15 | 1.95 | 28.37 | 13 | 43 | 22 | 53 |
Quartile 2 | 9.5 | 61.5 | 51 | 46 | 53895 | 13.66 | 1635 | 13 | 2.07 | 28.24 | 11 | 42 | 23 | 55 |
Quartile 3 | 11 | 61.7 | 53 | 38 | 53013 | 13.54 | 1648 | 12 | 2.03 | 28.61 | 13 | 45 | 22 | 54 |
Quartile 4 | 12 | 62.7 | 51 | 29 | 46618 | 12.78 | 1590 | 11 | 2.06 | 28.11 | 11 | 46 | 23 | 52 |
P for trend | 0.001 | 0.21 | 0.001 | 0.008 | 0.33 | 0.02 | 0.004 | 0.10 | 0.09 | 0.29 | 0.03 | 0.77 | 0.222 | |
Pessimism Subscale | 5.1 | |||||||||||||
Quartile 1 | 3 | 62.6 | 54 | 42 | 55365 | 13.49 | 1619 | 11 | 2.14 | 27.99 | 10 | 42 | 23 | 55 |
Quartile 2 | 4 | 61.2 | 52 | 43 | 54227 | 13.91 | 1540 | 11 | 2.1 | 28.49 | 10 | 45 | 23 | 56 |
Quartile 3 | 5.5 | 61.7 | 51 | 40 | 46701 | 12.97 | 1613 | 14 | 1.98 | 28.34 | 13 | 44 | 22 | 52 |
Quartile 4 | 8.3 | 61.9 | 53 | 34 | 42901 | 12.56 | 1478 | 15 | 1.89 | 28.54 | 15 | 47 | 23 | 51 |
P for trend | 0.022 | 0.35 | 0.001 | 0.002 | 0.002 | 0.26 | 0.001 | 0.002 | 0.008 | 0.002 | 0.01 | 0.882 | 0.012 |
Of the 6195 participants at the first MESA follow-up exam on whom optimism was measured, 5220–5358 (depending on the outcome measure) had complete information on all relevant covariates and were included in subsequent analyses. The internal consistency of the LOT-R was 0.73, and was fairly similar (0.66 – 0.78) across both sexes and all four race/ethnic groups. Factor analysis of the 6-item LOT-R confirmed loading of items onto two separate factors, termed “optimism subscale” and “pessimism subscale”. Similar reliabilities were observed for the optimism and pessimism subscales (Cronbach’s alpha 0.72 and 0.71, respectively). The correlation between the optimism and pessimism subscales was −0.33 (p<0.0001). The correlations between the LOT-R and the CES-D and the pessimism subscale and CES-D were −0.35 and 0.30 respectively. The correlations between the LOT-R and cynical distrust and between pessimism and cynical distrust were −0.19 and 0.27, respectively.
Generally, older individuals tended to have higher levels of optimism and lower levels of pessimism although differences in age across quartiles were not graded (Table 1). There was no association between optimism or pessimism and gender. The percent of minority participants increased with increasing levels of both the optimism and the pessimism subscales. The most consistent associations between life orientation measures and socioeconomic indicators were observed for the pessimism subscale: higher pessimism levels were associated with lower income and education in a graded fashion. In general, higher levels of optimism and lower levels of pessimism were also associated with greater physical activity (statistically significant for optimism subscale only), non-smoking, diets richer in whole grains and vegetables, lower BMI and less diabetes mellitus. In all cases associations were most consistent for the pessimism subscale.
In bivariate analyses (Table 2), higher scores on the LOT-R were generally associated with lower levels of IL-6, CRP, fibrinogen and homocysteine (although associations with CRP were not statistically significant). No clear patterns were observed for D-dimer, factor VIII and plasmin-antiplasmin. In general, the association between the full scale LOT-R and IL-6, CRP, fibrinogen, and homocysteine were driven by the pessimism factor: all four markers were positively associated with higher scores on the pessimism subscale but were not consistently associated with the optimism subscale. D-dimer, factor VIII and plasmin-antiplasmin were not consistently associated with the optimism or pessimism subscales and therefore are not reported in subsequent analyses.
Table 2.
Optimism Measure | IL-6 (pg/mL) | CRP (mg/dL) | Fibrinogen (mg/dL) | Homocysteine (umol/L) | D-dimer (ug/mL) | Factor VIII (%) | Plasmin-antiplasmin (nM) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Full sample (n=6195) | 1.24 | 1.92 | 339.31 | 8.87 | 0.22 | 150.83 | 4.43 |
LOT-R | |||||||
Quartile 1 | 1.21 | 1.75 | 342.1 | 8.91 | 0.21 | 149.75 | 4.36 |
Quartile 2 | 1.22 | 1.77 | 336.57 | 8.83 | 0.21 | 150.14 | 4.29 |
Quartile 3 | 1.14 | 1.67 | 334.22 | 8.71 | 0.21 | 148.34 | 4.28 |
Quartile 4 | 1.13 | 1.68 | 333.45 | 8.67 | 0.21 | 148.56 | 4.26 |
P value for trend | 0.001 | 0.39 | <0.001 | 0.03 | 0.89 | 0.81 | 0.32 |
Optimism Subscale | |||||||
Quartile 1 | 1.19 | 1.68 | 339.61 | 8.94 | 0.21 | 148.03 | 4.32 |
Quartile 2 | 1.2 | 1.74 | 337.48 | 8.77 | 0.21 | 150.07 | 4.35 |
Quartile 3 | 1.17 | 1.76 | 336.6 | 8.71 | 0.22 | 149.46 | 4.23 |
Quartile 4 | 1.16 | 1.74 | 336 | 8.79 | 0.21 | 149.49 | 4.29 |
P value for trend | 0.36 | 0.72 | 0.5 | 0.09 | 0.86 | 0.83 | 0.26 |
Pessimism Subscale | |||||||
Quartile 1 | 1.13 | 1.64 | 332.99 | 8.7 | 0.21 | 148.59 | 4.27 |
Quartile 2 | 1.19 | 1.73 | 336.8 | 8.68 | 0.2 | 150.88 | 4.33 |
Quartile 3 | 1.2 | 1.75 | 338.05 | 8.94 | 0.22 | 149.26 | 4.3 |
Quartile 4 | 1.22 | 1.81 | 342.41 | 8.87 | 0.21 | 149.86 | 4.33 |
P value for trend | 0.004 | 0.05 | <0.001 | 0.005 | 0.09 | 0.79 | 0.64 |
Table 3 shows the multivariate adjusted percent differences in inflammatory factors associated with a subscale difference equivalent to 2 standard deviations (2SD) in the optimism and pessimism subscales. These were estimated using a model with optimism and pessimism subscales entered as continuous variables. After adjustment for age, sex, current infection, current medication use, fasting state, and opposing subscale, a 2SD increase in the optimism subscale was associated with a 7.67% increase in CRP (p=0.02). The optimism subscale was not significantly associated with IL-6, fibrinogen or homocysteine in any of the four models. Further adjusting for race/ethnicity, income, education, depression and cynical distrust (model 2) resulted in attenuation of the association with CRP (4.93% increase, p=0.12). In the final model, after accounting for effects of health behaviors, hypertension, diabetes mellitus and BMI, a 2SD increase in the optimism subscale was associated with a 5.47% increase in CRP, but this difference was not quite statistically significant (p=0.09).
Table 3.
LOT-R Subscale | IL-6 | CRP | Fibrinogen | Homocysteine | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Optimism | %diff | 95% CI | p | %diff | 95% CI | p | %diff | 95% CI | p | %diff | 95% CI | p |
Model 1 | 1.95 | (−1.52, 5.54) | 0.27 | 7.67 | (1.43, 14.3) | 0.02 | 0.44 | (−0.61, 1.51) | 0.41 | −0.71 | (−2.14, 0.74) | 0.33 |
Model 2 | 0.66 | (−2.9, 4.31) | 0.72 | 4.93 | (−1.29, 11.5) | 0.12 | −0.46 | (−1.55, 0.64) | 0.41 | −1.00 | (−2.5, 0.52) | 0.20 |
Model 3 | 2.42 | (−1.4, 6.3) | 0.21 | 7.80 | (1.11, 14.9) | 0.02 | −0.45 | (−1.6, 071) | 0.45 | −1.07 | (−2.64, 0.51) | 0.18 |
Model 4 | 1.23 | (−2.3, 4.9) | 0.50 | 5.47 | (−0.74, 12.1) | 0.09 | −0.69 | (−1.8, 0.44) | 0.23 | −1.22 | (−2.78, 0.36) | 0.13 |
Pessimism | ||||||||||||
Model 1 | 6.01 | (2.4, 9.75) | 0.001 | 10.31 | (3.91, 17.09) | 0.001 | 2.47 | (1.4, 3.56) | <0.001 | 1.36 | (−0.1, 2.85) | 0.07 |
Model 2 | 3.29 | (−0.42, 7.13) | 0.08 | 8.16 | (1.58, 15.15) | 0.01 | 1.77 | (0.62, 2.92) | 0.002 | 0.69 | (−0.88, 2.28) | 0.39 |
Model 3 | 3.71 | (−0.2, 7.74) | 0.06 | 8.25 | (1.42, 15.53) | 0.02 | 1.86 | (0.66, 3.07) | 0.002 | 0.89 | (−0.74, 2.54) | 0.28 |
Model 4 | 1.26 | (−2.33, 4.98) | 0.50 | 4.26 | (−1.99, 10.9) | 0.19 | 1.36 | (0.2, 2.53) | 0.02 | 0.62 | (−1.0, 2.27) | 0.45 |
Model 1: Adjusted for age, sex, current infection, current medication use, fasting state, and opposing subscale
Model 2: Additionally adjusts model 1 for race, education, income, depression, and cynical distrust
Model 3: Additionally adjusts model 2 for smoking, exercise, and diet
Model 4: Additionally adjusts model 3 for BMI, diabetes, and hypertension
Associations of inflammatory markers with the pessimism subscale were stronger and more consistent than those observed for the optimism subscale. In the base model, a 2SD increase in the pessimism subscale was significantly related to a 6.01% increase in IL-6 (p=0.001), 10.31% increase in CRP (p=0.001), and a 2.47% increase in fibrinogen (p<0.0001). The 1.36% increase associated with homocysteine was not quite statistically significant (p=0.07). After sociodemographic and other psychosocial factors were added into the model (model 2), a 2SD increase in pessimism was still associated with a 3.29% increase in IL-6 (p=0.08), an 8.16% increase in CRP (p=0.01) and a 1.77% increase in fibrinogen (p=0.002). Further adjustment for health behaviors in model 3 did not have much additional effect. A 2SD increase in pessimism remained associated with higher levels of inflammatory markers: 3.71% for IL-6 (p=0.06), 8.25% for CRP (p=0.02), and 1.86% for fibrinogen (p=0.002). Adjustment for hypertension, BMI, and diabetes in model 4 diminished associations with IL-6 and CRP to 1.26% (p=0.5) and 4.26% (p=0.19), respectively. Associations of pessimism with fibrinogen remained statistically significant in the fully adjusted model (a 2SD increase in pessimism was associated with a 1.36% increase in fibrinogen (p=0.02)).
Discussion
Overall, this study showed that higher scores on the Life Orientation Test-Revised scale (indicative of higher levels of optimism generally) were associated with lower levels of IL-6, CRP, fibrinogen and homocysteine, but not D-dimer, factor VIII, or plasmin-antiplasmin. When the LOT-R was divided into optimism and pessimism subscales, the pessimism subscale was more strongly associated with inflammatory markers than the optimism subscale, with higher levels of pessimism being associated with higher levels of IL-6, CRP, and fibrinogen in a dose-response fashion. The relationship held after the effects of sociodemographics, negative psychosocial factors, and health behaviors were accounted for. Results are compatible with a mediating role of BMI, HTN and DM, as adjustment for these factors greatly diminished the association between pessimism and IL-6, CRP, and fibrinogen. Only fibrinogen remained associated with pessimism after full adjustment, which suggests that other unstudied mechanisms may also be involved. In our model, the strength of the associations of pessimism with inflammatory markers are similar to those observed for increase of 10 years of age, but smaller than those observed for diabetes. The magnitudes of the effect sizes are also similar to those reported for cynical distrust in the same sample (22).
Our results add to the growing body of literature on the effect psychosocial factors on CVD development and progression. This study is the first to investigate the associations of optimism and pessimisms with inflammation and coagulation. We found that the pessimism subscale had a stronger association with the outcome variables than the full LOT-R or the optimism subscale, and the associations were independent of the effects of depression and cynical distrust. Other studies that have analyzed optimism and pessimism factors separately have found similar results. Raikkonen et al. reported that the pessimism subscale had a stronger association with ambulatory blood pressure than either the full scale LOT-R or the optimism subscale (12). Shen (2004) also found that pessimism was a stronger predictor than optimism of worse physical functioning after 6-weeks of cardiac rehabilitation (45). Taken together, these results suggest that pessimism has more pronounced physiologic effects than optimism. However it is also plausible that the so-called pessimism items (ie. negatively worded items) are simply better measures of dispositional optimism than the positively worded items, and that dispositional optimism in the relevant psychosocial construct.
In a model adjusted for demographics and pessimism, optimism was associated with higher rather than lower CRP. However this finding was not consistent across models or different inflammatory markers and may be spurious. Although optimism specifically has not previously been linked to chronic inflammation, trait positive affect, a similar attribute, was associated with lower IL-6 and CRP among women aged 50 to 74 years in the Whitehall Study (23). Similarly, Ryff et al. (2004) reported an inverse association between higher levels of “purpose in life”, a factor in the eudaimonic well-being scale, and IL-6 in older women (ages 65 and older) (46). Our findings are congruent with Pressman & Cohen’s (2005) “main effect model” which postulates that behavioral and biological pathways mediate the link between psychologic traits and the immune and cardiovascular systems (19). In our study, once behavioral risk factors were controlled for in Model 3, the associations with pessimism diminished, as would be expected for a mediating role in this model. Giltay et al. (2007) also reported that optimism was associated with health behaviors such as non-smoking, increased physical activity, and better diet, adding further support to the model (15). This may occur because people with higher levels of optimism believe they have the ability to change the course of their health, and thereby tend to adopt better health behaviors. These individuals may also have better self-care behaviors, obtaining more information about their disease and adhering to treatment regimens (19).
Dispositional optimism and pessimism may also directly influence the autonomic nervous system (ANS) and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis (18–20). Lower levels of blood pressure and cortisol have been reported among those with greater optimism (12, 45, 47). Similarly, Fredrickson et al. (1998) demonstrated faster cardiovascular recovery from acute increases in blood pressure and heart rate in response to negative emotional stimuli among those with higher levels of optimism (48). In contrast, a pessimistic disposition may provoke feelings of self-blame, fatalism, and catastrophic thinking, which leads to chronically increased sympathetic tone and cortisol levels (20–21). We did not directly investigate these mechanisms but the persistence of associations with fibrinogen in the fully adjusted models suggests that other mechanisms, perhaps involving physiologic stress processes, could play a role.
The effects of dispositional optimism or pessimism may have long-term, systemic effects throughout the life course. Two prospective cohort studies reported that more optimistic individuals have lower rates of MI and mortality from CHD death (5–6) than pessimistic individuals over 10–15 years. Matthews et al. (2004), report that higher pessimism scores resulted in greater increases over time in mean carotid intima-media thickness (IMT) over the course of 10.4 years (49). The present study adds a plausible mechanism for these findings: if pessimistic orientation is developed in young adulthood, the chronically increased levels of inflammatory factors may contribute to the progression and clinical manifestation of CVD.
We did not find any association between the LOT-R and three measured hemostatic markers (D-dimer, factor VIII and plasmin-antiplasmin). We are not aware of other studies that have investigated the association of life-orientation with hemostasis. While factor VIII and D-dimer have been shown to increase with acute stress and sympathetic nervous system activation (49–53), it may be that they do not remain chronically elevated in relation to trait dispositional pessimism or low optimism.
This study was conducted in a large, diverse, well-characterized sample free of cardiovascular disease. While excluding individuals with clinical CVD may have resulted in underestimates of associations, it precluded confounding by disease status. We used the Life Orientation Test—Revised, a widely used, validated measure of dispositional optimism, to measure optimism and pessimism (10–11, 26, 54). We measured life-orientation at the first follow-up visit, which was two years after the initial visit, when inflammatory markers were measured. However, life-orientation is considered a stable trait, and remains relatively unchanged over the course of an individual’s lifetime (5). Therefore, it is unlikely that individual scores on the LOT-R changed significantly over the course of the 2 years between visits. Analyses were cross-sectional, limiting causal inferences. However, given that dispositional optimism is a stable trait, usually developed in young adulthood (21, 55), it is likely an antecedent factor to mediating health behaviors and biologic pathways that affect CVD progression and outcomes.
In summary, we found that pessimism was associated with higher levels of markers for systemic inflammation in a large, healthy, diverse population. These associations were independent of depression and cynical distrust. Our results are consistent with a mediating role of health behaviors, BMI, hypertension, and diabetes mellitus. This may have implications for programs aimed towards primary or secondary prevention of cardiovascular disease, as the cultivation of optimism may be an important feature of program design.
Acknowledgments
Funding and Contract Support: MESA was supported by contracts N01-HC-95159 through N01-HC- 95165 and N01-HC-95169 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. This work was supported by R01 HL07683.
We thank the other investigators, the staff, and the participants of the MESA study for their valuable contributions. A full list of participating MESA investigators and institutions can be found at www.mesa-nhlbi.org. The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.
Abbreviations
- CVD
cardiovascular disease
- CHD
coronary heart disease
- MESA
Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis
- HTN
hypertension
- BMI
body mass index
- DM
diabetes mellitus
- LOT-R
Life-Orientation Test—Revised
- NHLBI
National Heart, Lung and Blood Institute
- IL-6
interleukin-6
- CRP
C-reactive protein
- CES-D
Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scale
- ELISA
enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay
- ANS
autonomic nervous system
- HPA
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal
- IMT
intima-media thickness
- MI
myocardial infarction
- 2SD
two standard deviations
Footnotes
Disclosures: No conflicts of interest to disclose.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Hemingway H, Marmot M. Evidence based cardiology: Psychosocial factors in the aetiology and prognosis of coronary heart disease. Systematic review of prospective cohort studies. BMJ. 1999;318(7196):1460. doi: 10.1136/bmj.318.7196.1460. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Everson-Rose SA, Lewis TT. Psychosocial factors and cardiovascular diseases. Annu Rev Public Health. 2005;26:469. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Rosengren A, Hawken S, Ounpuu S, Sliwa K, Zubaid M, Almahmeed WA, Blackett KN, Sitthi-amorn C, Sato H, Yusuf S INTERHEART investigators. Association of psychosocial risk factors with risk of acute myocardial infarction in 11119 cases and 13648 controls from 52 countries (the INTERHEART study): Case-control study. Lancet. 2004;364(9438):953. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(04)17019-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Scheier MF, Matthews KA, Owens JF, Magovern GJ, Sr, Lefebvre RC, Abbott RA, Carver CS. Dispositional optimism and recovery from coronary artery bypass surgery: The beneficial effects on physical and psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989;12;57(6):1024–40. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.57.6.1024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Giltay EJ, Kamphuis MH, Kalmijn S, Zitman FG, Kromhout D. Dispositional optimism and the risk of cardiovascular death: the Zutphen elderly study. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166(4):431. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.4.431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kubzansky LD, Sparrow D, Vokonas P, Kawachi I. Is the glass half empty or half full? A prospective study of optimism and coronary heart disease in the normative aging study. Psychosom Med. 2001;63(6):910. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200111000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Matthews KA, Raikkonen K, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Kuller LH. Optimistic attitudes protect against progression of carotid atherosclerosis in healthy middle-aged women. Psychosom Med. 2004;66(5):640. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000139999.99756.a5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Scheier MF, Matthews KA, Owens JF, Schulz R, Bridges MW, Magovern GJ, Carver CS. Optimism and rehospitalization after coronary artery bypass graft surgery. Arch Intern Med. 1999;159(8):829. doi: 10.1001/archinte.159.8.829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Scheier MF, Carver CS, Bridges MW. Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): A reevaluation of the life orientation test. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;67(6):1063. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.67.6.1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kubzansky LD, Kubzansky PE, Maselko J. Optimism and pessimism in the context of health: Bipolar opposites or separate constructs. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2004;30(8):943. doi: 10.1177/0146167203262086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Herzberg PY, Glaesmer H, Hoyer J. Separating optimism and pessimism: A robust psychometric analysis of the revised life orientation test (LOT-R) Psychol Assess. 2006;18(4):433. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.18.4.433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Raikkonen K, Matthews KA. Do dispositional pessimism and optimism predict ambulatory blood pressure during school days and nights in adolescents? J Pers. 2008;76(3):605. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00498.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Helgeson VS, Fritz HL. Cognitive adaptation as a predictor of new coronary events after percutaneous transluminal coronary angioplasty. Psychosom Med. 1999;61(4):488. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199907000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Grewen K, Girdler SS, West SG, Bragdon E, Costello N, Light KC. Stable pessimistic attributions interact with socioeconomic status to influence blood pressure and vulnerability to hypertension. J Womens Health Gend Based Med. 2000;9(8):905. doi: 10.1089/152460900750020946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Giltay EJ, Geleijnse JM, Zitman FG, Buijsse B, Kromhout D. Lifestyle and dietary correlates of dispositional optimism in men: The Zutphen elderly study. J Psychosom Res. 2007;63(5):483. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2007.07.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kohut M, Cooper MM, Nickolaus MS, Russell DR, Cunnick JE. Exercise and psychosocial factors modulate immunity to influenza vaccine in elderly individuals. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences. 2002;57(9):M557. doi: 10.1093/gerona/57.9.m557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Tinker LF, Rosal MC, Young AF, Perri MG, Patterson RE, Van Horn L, Assaf AR, Bowen DJ, Ockene J, Hays J, Wu L. Predictors of dietary change and maintenance in the Women’s Health Initiative Dietary Modification Trial. J Am Diet Assoc. 2007;107(7):1155. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2007.04.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Rozanski A, Kubzansky LD. Psychologic functioning and physical health: A paradigm of flexibility. Psychosom Med. 2005;67:S47. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000164253.69550.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Pressman SD, Cohen S. Does positive affect influence health? Psychol Bull. 2005;131(6):925–71. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Das S, O’Keefe JH. Behavioral cardiology: Recognizing and addressing the profound impact of psychosocial stress on cardiovascular health. Curr Atheroscler Rep. 2006;8(2):111–8. doi: 10.1007/s11883-006-0048-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Freres DR, Gillham JE. The promotion of optimism and health. In: Vollrath ME, editor. Handbook of personality and health. New York, NY, US: John Wiley & Sons Ltd; 2006. pp. 315–335. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Ranjit N, Diez-Roux AV, Shea S, Cushman M, Seeman T, Jackson SA, Ni H. Psychosocial factors and inflammation in the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. Arch Intern Med. 2007 Jan 22;167(2):174–81. doi: 10.1001/archinte.167.2.174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Steptoe A, O’Donnell K, Badrick E, Kumari M, Marmot M. Neuroendocrine and inflammatory factors associated with positive affect in healthy men and women: The Whitehall II study. Am J Epidemiol. 2008;167(1):96–102. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Zakai NA, Katz R, Jenny NS, Psaty BM, Reiner AP, Schwartz SM, Cushman M. Inflammation and hemostasis biomarkers and cardiovascular risk in the elderly: the Cardiovascular Health Study. J Thromb Haemost. 2007 Jun;5(6):1128–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1538-7836.2007.02528.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bild DE, Bluemke DA, Burke GL, Detrano R, Diez Roux AV, Folsom AR, Greenland P, Jacob DR, Jr, Kronmal R, Liu K, Nelson JC, O’Leary D, Saad MF, Shea S, Szklo M, Tracy RP. Multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis: Objectives and design. Am J Epidemiol. 2002;156(9):871. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Scheier MF, Carver CS. Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychol. 1985;4(3):219. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.4.3.219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Carver CS, Scheier MF, Weintraub JK. Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989;56(2):267. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.56.2.267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Ikonomidis I, Stamatelopoulos K, Lekakis J, Vamvakou GD, Kremastinos DT. Inflammatory and non-invasive vascular markers: the multimarker approach for risk stratification in coronary artery disease. Atherosclerosis. 2008 Jul;199(1):3–11. doi: 10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2008.02.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Ross R. Atherosclerosis is an inflammatory disease. Am Heart J. 1999 Nov;138(5 Pt 2):S419–20. doi: 10.1016/s0002-8703(99)70266-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Homocysteine Studies Collaboration. Homocysteine and risk of ischemic heart disease and stroke: a meta-analysis. JAMA. 2002 Oct 23–30;288(16):2015–22. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.16.2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Morange PE, Bickel C, Nicaud V, Schnabel R, Rupprecht HJ, Peetz D, Lackner KJ, Cambien F, Blankenberg S, Tiret L. AtheroGene Investigators. Haemostatic factors and the risk of cardiovascular death in patients with coronary artery disease: the AtheroGene study. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol. 2006 Dec;26(12):2793–9. doi: 10.1161/01.ATV.0000249406.92992.0d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Folsom AR, Aleksic N, Park E, Salomaa V, Juneja H, Wu KK. Prospective study of fibrinolytic factors and incident coronary heart disease: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol. 2001 Apr;21(4):611–7. doi: 10.1161/01.atv.21.4.611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Lowe GD, Rumley A. Use of fibrinogen and fibrin D-dimer in prediction of arterial thrombotic events. Thromb Haemost. Aug;199982(2):667–72. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Sturgeon JD, Folsom AR, Longstreth WT, Jr, Shahar E, Rosamond WD, Cushman M. Hemostatic and inflammatory risk factors for intracerebral hemorrhage in a pooled cohort. Stroke. 2008 Aug;39(8):2268–73. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.107.505800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Smith A, Patterson C, Yarnell J, Rumley A, Ben-Shlomo Y, Lowe G. Which hemostatic markers add to the predictive value of conventional risk factors for coronary heart disease and ischemic stroke? The Caerphilly Study Circulation. 2005 Nov 15;112(20):3080–7. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.105.557132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Zhou J, Austin RC. Contributions of hyperhomocysteinemia to atherosclerosis: Causal relationship and potential mechanisms. Biofactors. 2009 Mar-Apr;35(2):120–9. doi: 10.1002/biof.17. Review. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Chan MY, Andreotti F, Becker RC. Hypercoagulable states in cardiovascular disease. Circulation. 2008 Nov 25;118(22):2286–97. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.108.778837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.LaMonte MJ, Durstine JL, Addy CL, Irwin ML, Ainsworth BE. Physical activity, physical fitness, and Framingham 10-year risk score: The cross-cultural activity participation study. J Cardiopulm Rehabil. 2001;21(2):63. doi: 10.1097/00008483-200103000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Nettleton JA, Steffen LM, Mayer-Davis EJ, Jenny NS, Jiang R, Herrington DM, Jacobs DR., Jr Dietary patterns are associated with biochemical markers of inflammation and endothelial activation in the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis (MESA) Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;83(6):1369. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/83.6.1369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Diagnosis and classification of diabetes mellitus. Diabetes Care. 2008;31(Suppl 1) doi: 10.2337/dc08-S055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Radloff L. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Journal of Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1(3):385–401. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Julkunen J, Salonen R, Kaplan GA, Chesney MA, Salonen JT. Hostility and the progression of carotid atherosclerosis. Psychosom Med. 1994 Nov-Dec;56(6):519–25. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199411000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Matthews KA, Owens JF, Kuller LH, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Jansen-McWilliams L. Are hostility and anxiety associated with carotid atherosclerosis in healthy postmenopausal women? Psychosom Med. 1998 Sep-Oct;60(5):633–8. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199809000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Everson SA, Kauhanen J, Kaplan GA, Goldberg DE, Julkunen J, Tuomilehto J, Salonen JT. Hostility and increased risk of mortality and acute myocardial infarction: the mediating role of behavioral risk factors. Am J Epidemiol. 1997 Jul 15;146(2):142–52. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Shen B, McCreary CP, Myers HF. Independent and mediated contributions of personality, coping, social support, and depressive symptoms to physical functioning outcome among patients in cardiac rehabilitation. J Behav Med. 2004;27(1):39. doi: 10.1023/b:jobm.0000013643.36767.22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ryff CD, Singer BH, Dienberg Love G. Positive health: Connecting well-being with biology. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2004;359(1449):1383. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2004.1521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Polk D, Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Skoner DP, Kirschbaum C. State and trait affect as predictors of salivary cortisol in healthy adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005;30(3):261–72. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Fredrickson BL, Levenson RW. Positive emotions speed recovery from the cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. Cognition & Emotion. 1998;12(2):191–220. doi: 10.1080/026999398379718. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Steptoe A, Marmot M. Psychosocial, hemostatic, and inflammatory correlates of delayed poststress blood pressure recovery. Psychosom Med. 2006;68(4):531. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000227751.82103.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Steptoe A, Kunz-Ebrecht S, Owen N, Feldman PJ, Rumley A, Lowe GD, Marmot M. Influence of socioeconomic status and job control on plasma fibrinogen responses to acute mental stress. Psychosom Med. 2003;65(1):137–44. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000039755.23250.a7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Wirtz PH, Ehlert U, Emini L, Rudisuli K, Groessbauer S, von Kanel R. Procoagulant stress reactivity and recovery in apparently healthy men with systolic and diastolic hypertension. J Psychosom Res. 2007;63(1):51–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2007.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.von Kanel R, Mills PJ, Ziegler MG, Dimsdale JE. Effect of beta2-adrenergic receptor functioning and increased norepinephrine on the hypercoagulable state with mental stress. Am Heart J. 2002;144(1):68–72. doi: 10.1067/mhj.2002.123146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.von Kanel R, Kudielka BM, Haeberli A, Stutz M, Fischer JE, Patterson SM. Prothrombotic changes with acute psychological stress: Combined effect of hemoconcentration and genuine coagulation activation. Thromb Res. 2008 Jul; doi: 10.1016/j.thromres.2008.05.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Sumi K. Optimism, social support, stress, and physical and psychological well-being in Japanese women. Psychol Rep. 1997;81(1):299–306. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1997.81.1.299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Roberts BW, Walton KE, Viechtbauwer W. Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the live course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol Bull. 2006;01;132(1):1–25. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]