Skip to main content
Elsevier Sponsored Documents logoLink to Elsevier Sponsored Documents
. 2010 Apr 1;67(7):624–631. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.029

Effects of Motivation and Medication on Electrophysiological Markers of Response Inhibition in Children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

Madeleine J Groom a,, Gaia Scerif b, Peter F Liddle c, Martin J Batty a, Elizabeth B Liddle a, Katherine L Roberts a, John D Cahill a, Mario Liotti d, Chris Hollis a
PMCID: PMC2845810  PMID: 19914599

Abstract

Background

Theories of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) posit either executive deficits and/or alterations in motivational style and reward processing as core to the disorder. Effects of motivational incentives on electrophysiological correlates of inhibitory control and relationships between motivation and stimulant medication have not been explicitly tested.

Methods

Children (9–15 years) with combined-type ADHD (n = 28) and matched typically developing children (CTRL) (n = 28) performed a go/no-go task. Electroencephalogram data were recorded. Amplitude of two event-related potentials, the N2 and P3 (markers of response conflict and attention), were measured. The ADHD children were all stimulant responders tested on and off their usual dose of methylphenidate; CTRLs were never medicated. All children performed the task under three motivational conditions: reward; response cost; and baseline, in which points awarded/deducted for inhibitory performance varied.

Results

There were effects of diagnosis (CTRL > ADHD unmedicated), medication (on > off), and motivation (reward and/or response cost > baseline) on N2 and P3 amplitude, although the N2 diagnosis effect did not reach statistical significance (p = .1). Interactions between motivation and diagnosis/medication were nonsignificant (p > .1).

Conclusions

Motivational incentives increased amplitudes of electrophysiological correlates of response conflict and attention in children with ADHD, towards the baseline (low motivation) amplitudes of control subjects. These results suggest that, on these measures, motivational incentives have similar effects in children with ADHD as typically developing CTRLs and have additive effects with stimulant medication, enhancing stimulus salience and allocation of attentional resources during response inhibition.

Key Words: ADHD, electrophysiology, motivation, response inhibition, stimulant medication


Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by age-inappropriate levels of hyperactivity, impulsivity, and inattention. Children with ADHD exhibit a range of cognitive difficulties, including impaired inhibition of the prepotent motor response in go/no-go and stop signal tasks (1,2), leading some to suggest a central role for response inhibition deficits (3). Impaired response inhibition in ADHD is associated with underactivation of a fronto-striatal-thalamic circuit involved in cognitive control (4) and reduced amplitude of two event-related potentials (ERPs), the N2 and P3 (5–8), which in healthy individuals increase on trials requiring motor inhibition or conflict resolution (9,10).

Children with ADHD also show atypical regulation of motivational state: they respond to rewards when they are delivered immediately and with regularity but are less sensitive to rewards that are delayed or intermittent (11,12). It has been suggested that deficits in higher cognitive functions in ADHD, including response inhibition, conflict processing, and attention, are a consequence of impairments in the self-regulation of arousal and motivational state (13,14). This raises the interesting question of whether incentives given to enhance motivation will improve inhibitory performance in children with ADHD. With the exception of two studies (15,16), most behavioral studies examining this hypothesis have argued in favor of independence of inhibitory and motivational deficits (17–24). However, methodological weaknesses limit their interpretation, including the absence of a baseline (no or low motivation) (17,20); the presentation of baseline and/or motivated conditions in a fixed order such that, even after counterbalancing, order effects were uncontrolled and not always equivalent between groups (19,21,22); and baseline and motivational conditions delivered in separate sessions or hours apart, potentially weakening the context provided by the baseline (23). Another important question, which to our knowledge has not yet been explored, is whether there are effects of motivation on covert measures of neural function.

We investigated effects of motivation on neural correlates of response conflict and attention in ADHD using a go/no-go paradigm, in which two motivational conditions and a baseline (low motivation) condition were presented repeatedly, randomized within session. Electroencephalography (EEG) data were collected while children performed the task; amplitudes of N2 and P3 were measured. We predicted significantly smaller N2 and P3 amplitudes in the ADHD group than the control group. Incentives were delivered continuously and immediately. We therefore predicted that if inhibitory deficits are related to impaired motivation in ADHD, incentives would significantly enhance amplitudes of the ADHD group to at least the same level as the baseline (low motivation) amplitudes of the control group.

In addition, we investigated relationships between stimulant medication (methylphenidate [MPH]) and motivation in the ADHD group using a within-subjects design. Methylphenidate is believed to increase availability of dopamine in the striatum, enhancing salience of task-relevant stimuli (25,26), improving inhibitory performance (27–29), and normalizing latency (30) and amplitude (31) of N2 and P3. It is currently unknown whether other means of altering arousal, such as providing motivational incentives, have similar effects on neurocognitive function as those produced by stimulant medications, and there is relatively little research investigating the effects of medication on such correlates. If motivational incentives and stimulant medication produce complementary effects on neural correlates of response inhibition, this would provide some support for the combined use of medication and behavioral reinforcement in the treatment of ADHD. Although this approach was supported by initial findings of the Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD (MTA) (32) with lower medication doses required for symptom control in children receiving combined medication and behavioral management, there have, as yet, been no explicit, published comparisons of the effects of motivational incentives and methylphenidate on neural correlates of inhibitory control. If medication and motivation act synergistically, N2 and P3 amplitudes will be differentially larger in children with ADHD in the high-motivation conditions when medicated than when unmedicated. Alternatively, each intervention may result in independent improvements in neural functioning.

Methods and Materials

Participants were 28 children with ADHD (27 male participants), aged 9 to 15 years, and 28 typically developing control subjects matched on age, gender, and socioeconomic status (SES). Ethical approval was granted by the Multicentre Research Ethics Committee (MREC). Informed written parental consent and verbal participant assent were obtained from all participants. Table 1 summarizes demographic and clinical information.

Table 1.

Clinical and Demographic Characteristics of CTRL and ADHD Groups

CTRL (n = 28)
ADHD (n = 28)
Comparison
Mean SD Mean SD t (df = 54) p
Age 12.54 1.81 12.51 1.75 .07a .946
IQ 104.93 14.31 90.86 11.71 4.027 <.001
SDQ
 Hyperactive 1.68 1.70 9.00 1.66 16.32a <.001
Conners
 DSM-Hyperactive 43.64 3.27 84.96 7.23 25.37a <.001
 DSM-Inattentive 43.65 3.22 73.50 7.96 18.36a <.001
 DSM-Total 44.25 3.32 81.61 7.69 23.62a <.001
ADHD-RSc
 Off medications 70.08 4.24 2.92b .004
 On medications 64.31 10.51
Order of Testingd
 Off medications first 13 13
 On medications first 15 15

ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; ADHD-RS, ADHD-Rating Scale-IV; Conners, Conners Rating Scale-Revised (Long Form) ADHD index; CTRL, control group; DSM, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Health Disorders; IQ, intelligence quotient; SDQ, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire.

a

Independent-samples t test comparing control group (CTRL) and ADHD group.

b

Paired-samples t test comparing off and on medication scores in the ADHD group; this analysis was conducted on 26 participants who completed both the off and on medication sessions.

c

Scores on the DuPaul ADHD Rating Scale were converted to t scores using published population percentiles (38).

d

Order of testing was counterbalanced and randomized within the ADHD group. Participants in the control group were never medicated but were assigned to the same order of testing as the ADHD participant with whom they were pairwise-matched.

Recruitment of ADHD Participants

Figure 1 presents recruitment information. Children with a diagnosis of ADHD were referred to the study by child psychiatrists and pediatricians. An initial telephone interview screened for broad inclusion and exclusion criteria, before an in-depth assessment in person using the following measures: Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) (33); Conners Rating Scale-Revised (Long Form) (34); Development and Well Being Assessment (DAWBA), a semistructured clinical interview (35); and Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) (36). With the exception of the SCQ, which was administered to parents only, all were completed by parents and teachers. This, along with the child's medical notes, formed the basis for a consensus diagnostic conference involving C.H. and another experienced child and adolescent psychiatrist. Only cases with a confirmed diagnosis of ADHD-combined subtype and an established clinical response to MPH were included, unless they met one of the following exclusion criteria: comorbid diagnosis (or symptoms above a specified threshold) of tic disorder, pervasive developmental disorder, learning disability (IQ <70) assessed using the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (37), and neurological disorder. Children with comorbid anxiety disorder, oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) (n = 13, 46%), and conduct disorder (n = 8, 29%) were not excluded.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Flow chart showing recruitment and assessment of participants in control (CTRL) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) groups. ASD, Autism Spectrum Disorder; ERP, event-related potential; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging.

Recruitment of Control Participants

Letters detailing the study were sent via local schools to parents. Right-handed children with no psychiatric diagnosis were selected for further assessment if they matched demographically a member of the ADHD group. Exclusion criteria were those listed above for the ADHD group in addition to score >5 on the hyperactivity subscale of the SDQ and/or score >1 SD above normative means on the Conners Rating Scale-Revised (Long Form) ADHD index. Anxiety, depression, and conduct problems were not excluded, although no control subjects met criteria for these disorders.

Paradigm

The paradigm was a modified version of the visual go/no-go task, programmed using E-Prime (version 1.1, Psychology Software Tools). Stimuli were presented centrally on a color monitor positioned approximately 57 cm in front of participants. Interstimulus interval (ISI) was randomly jittered between 2.8 and 3.8 sec; stimulus duration was 100 msec. Participants were instructed to fixate on a central point and press a response button (Cedrus Superlab button box, Cedrus Corporation, San Pedro, California) each time a frequent “go” stimulus appeared and to refrain from responding to an infrequent “no-go” stimulus. There were 600 trials in total with 150 (25%) no-go trials, presented in three different motivational conditions (200 trials per condition). The paradigm was presented in the context of a “space” theme; go stimuli were green aliens measuring 43-mm height by 40-mm width; no-go stimuli were black aliens of equal size. Participants were encouraged to “catch” as many green aliens as possible while avoiding catching the black aliens. A time limit (reaction time [RT] cap) was imposed on go trials: participants lost 1 point for each slow or missed response (visual feedback given 1000 msec poststimulus) and gained 1 point for each timely response.

To minimize between- and within-subject variance in inhibition rate, the RT cap was dynamically altered throughout the experiment by a tracking algorithm. Prior to the start of the experiment, participants performed a practice session comprising 20 go trials, which used a staircase procedure to identify the shortest time within which they could respond. This value became the lower bound of the tracking algorithm during the main experiment (floor RT). The initial value of the tracking algorithm for each motivational condition was set at this value plus 200 msec. This RT cap was altered dynamically, based on performance on no-go trials: following failed inhibits (FI), the cap increased by 25 msec to improve the chance of success on the next no-go trial; each successful inhibit (SI) resulted in a 25 msec decrease. There was an upper bound of 900 msec for all participants.

Motivational Conditions

Participants performed the go/no-go task in three different motivational conditions: baseline (low motivation); reward; and response cost. In the baseline condition, participants gained or lost 1 point following each SI or FI, respectively. In the reward condition, 5 points were given for each SI; in the response cost condition, 5 points were deducted for each FI. Thus, in the baseline condition, the value attached to inhibiting on a no-go trial was equal to that of responding quickly on a go trial. In the motivated conditions, the value attached to successfully inhibiting on a no-go trial was greater than the value attached to responding quickly on a go trial, resulting in relatively greater incentive to inhibit.

Stimuli were presented in five blocks, with each motivational condition randomly presented once within each block. At the end of each condition, a visual display detailed how many points had been lost and gained for go and no-go trials. At the end of each block, instructions emphasized either the need to withhold responses to the no-go or make timely responses to the go stimulus, depending upon whether participants achieved 50% successful inhibitions during the block.

Inhibition rates are presented in Table 2 and demonstrate the success of the algorithm in minimizing between-subject and between-session variance in inhibition rate. Because of the tracking algorithm, this index was blunted as a behavioral measure of inhibitory control. The median value of the RT cap for each condition was used as an alternative index: the lower the median cap, the harder the participant was trying to inhibit. To correct for between-subject and between-session differences in overall response speed, the floor RT was subtracted from the median cap for each condition, yielding the RT cap − floor differential. To assess the effect of motivational incentives on participant behavior, a rating scale was administered at the end of each testing session, asking the children to report which condition they “liked the most” and which condition made them “most careful.” The frequency with which each condition was chosen in each group and session was scored.

Table 2.

Descriptive Data for Behavioral Measures and ERP Amplitudes in CTRL and ADHD Groups

Diagnosis
Medication (n = 22)
CTRL (n = 28)
ADHD (n = 23)
ADHD Off Medication
ADHD On Medication
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
RT Cap − Floor Differential (msec) Baseline 328.92 182.05 337.94 171.91 339.34 166.28 280.70 213.96
Reward 264.18 172.68 237.46 165.45 240.48 159.96 208.54 184.56
Response Cost 235.17 175.18 244.67 165.72 254.11 169.32 247.75 208.71
Inhibition Rate Baseline 39.57 15.53 39.08 15.23 39.08 15.23 44.54 18.34
Reward 46.50 13.14 47.23 15.60 47.23 15.60 51.31 17.05
Response Cost 44.86 13.08 45.23 13.57 45.23 13.57 47.08 15.83
N2 Go Trials (μV) Baseline −9.37 4.15 −7.92 3.83 −7.74 3.74 −9.65 3.56
Reward −9.30 4.22 −8.82 3.71 −8.83 3.84 −9.79 3.69
Response Cost −9.02 3.96 −8.17 3.49 −7.87 3.36 −9.48 3.88
N2 No-Go Trials (μV) Baseline −12.16 4.64 −9.69 4.56 −8.83 3.36 −9.47 3.81
Reward −12.47 5.03 −10.25 3.91 −9.57 3.32 −10.47 3.59
Response Cost −12.13 4.75 −9.36 4.14 −8.87 3.88 −10.51 3.89
P3 Go Trials (μV) Baseline 3.56 2.44 2.10 2.73 2.15 2.78 4.15 2.86
Reward 4.03 2.56 2.24 2.57 2.24 2.63 4.73 2.98
Response Cost 4.17 2.60 2.53 2.85 2.52 2.92 4.48 2.67
P3 No-Go Trials (μV) Baseline 6.10 3.49 3.86 3.50 3.89 3.58 6.04 3.66
Reward 6.37 3.58 4.46 3.95 4.46 4.04 6.74 3.41
Response Cost 6.14 3.72 4.54 3.70 4.42 3.74 6.09 3.67

Values for inhibition rate must be interpreted in the context of the tracking algorithm employed within the paradigm that minimized between-subject and between-session differences in the ratio of failed to successfully inhibited no-go trials.

ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; CTRL, control group; ERP, event-related potential; RT, reaction time.

Medication Effects

Participants in the ADHD group performed the go/no-go paradigm once on and once off their usual methylphenidate dose (order counterbalanced within the group). For the off medication session, they were required to stop taking their medication 36 hours beforehand. Group mean methylphenidate dose was 1.11 (SD = .42) mg/kg. Symptoms were assessed each time using the DuPaul et al. (38) ADHD-Rating Scale-IV; Table 1 shows a significant decrease in symptoms on medication compared with off medication.

Procedure

Participants attended on two separate days, each consisting of an EEG session and a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) session (not reported here), with the order of testing counterbalanced and held constant across the 2 days. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder participants were tested off and on medication; control subjects were never medicated but were tested twice to control for practice effects. Control (CTRL) and ADHD groups were pairwise matched on sociodemographic variables and on the order in which each pair completed EEG and fMRI testing. In between-group statistical analyses, the off-medication session of each ADHD participant was compared with the equivalent session of their matched control subject. Data from two participants in the ADHD group who did not complete the off-medication ERP session were excluded from analysis; the control participants with whom they were matched remained in the analysis to increase power.

Electrophysiological Data Recording

Data were collected using a Biosemi Active II System (Biosemi, Amsterdam, Netherlands) with 128-channel montage of silver/silver chloride (Ag/AgCl) electrodes, sampled at 256 Hz. Additional electrodes were placed at the inner orbital ridge and the outer canthus of each eye to record eye movements and on each mastoid to record other artefacts. During data collection, voltage signals were referenced to an electrode placed to the left of electrode Cz.

Electrophysiological Data Processing

Analysis was performed using Brain Vision Analyzer (BVA) 1.05 (Brain Products, Munich, Germany). After removal of noisy/flat channels, data were re-referenced to the average reference and filtered using .5 Hz high-pass and 30 Hz low-pass zero-phase shift Butterworth filters with slope of 24 dB per octave. Data were segmented into epochs, 2800 msec in length, with a prestimulus period of 650 msec. Large epochs were created at this stage to improve reliability of ocular correction, conducted using a regression method (39). Epochs with activity exceeding ±100 μV or less than 2.5 μV for more than 500 msec within an epoch were excluded. Baseline correction was performed using a 200 msec prestimulus reference period. Stimulus-locked epochs (−200 to 1000 msec peristimulus window) were averaged for the following trial types within each motivational condition: go (go trials with response within 100 to 900 msec of stimulus onset); no-go (all no-go trials). A number of participants in both groups (CTRL = 16; ADHD off medication = 20; ADHD on medication = 15) had too few SI trials (<20) for reliable ERP averaging. To avoid losing participants with insufficient trials, ERPs were measured in the no-go waveform, collapsed across SI and FI trials. A number of statistical procedures (Supplement 1) were performed to ensure this does not confound investigation of the experimental hypotheses.

Peak detection was performed using a semiautomated procedure in which peaks were detected automatically by the BVA software and checked by an expert blind to group status, according to the following criterion: N2 (maximal negative peak in a 220–400 msec time window) at Fz and Cz; P3 (mean amplitude in a 400–700 msec time window) at Cz and Pz. The early portion of the waveform often contained a negative shift; to provide a more reliable measure of N2 amplitude, the peak was calculated as the difference in amplitude between the N2 and the preceding positive peak. The P3 was measured as mean amplitude because in the majority of datasets, the activity in this time window occurred over a long time period and it was frequently difficult to identify one specific peak.

Statistical Analysis

All analyses were conducted in two parts: first, effects of diagnosis were investigated by comparing the CTRL group with the ADHD group off medication. Second, effects of medication in the ADHD group were investigated by comparing the off and on medication sessions within subjects. In both analyses, motivation was a within-subjects factor with three levels (baseline, reward, and response cost). Analysis of ERP amplitudes included a within-subjects factor trial with two levels (go, no-go). Effect sizes (partial eta squared; ηp2) are reported. Main effects and interactions significant at p < .05 (two-tailed) and trends (p < .1) were followed up with further analysis. Effects of motivation were investigated with planned Helmert contrasts comparing motivated conditions (reward and response cost) with baseline and reward with response cost.

Five participants (two ADHD off medication; two ADHD on medication; one ADHD both sessions) had fewer than 20 artifact-free no-go trials for ERP averaging and were excluded from analysis. Final sample sizes were 28 CTRL and 23 ADHD off medication for diagnostic analyses and 22 ADHD participants for medication analyses.

The data were checked for homogeneity of variance and normal distribution. Outliers (standard deviation greater than 2.5 from group mean) were excluded. The data were checked to ensure two female subjects in the sample (one ADHD, one CTRL) were not outliers; results remained robust to exclusion of these participants. All analyses described above were rerun with IQ and ODD diagnosis included as covariates. Neither were significant predictors in any analysis; the results are reported without the covariates included.

Results

Effects of Motivation on Visual Analogue Scales

Participants responded to the motivational manipulations within the paradigm, choosing the reward condition (CTRL: 91.1%; ADHD: 82%) above response cost (CTRL: 6.7%; ADHD: 7.7%) or baseline (CTRL: 2.2%; ADHD: 10.3%) as the condition they “liked the most.” Neither group (χ2 = 2.494, p > .1) nor session (χ2 = 3.209, p > .1) affected these choices. Both groups more frequently chose the response cost condition (CTRL: 81.8%; ADHD: 72.5%) than either reward (CTRL: 18.2%; ADHD: 17.5%) or baseline (CTRL: 0% ADHD: 10%) as the one that made them “most careful” with no significant group (χ2 = 4.641, p = .098) or session (χ2 = .082, p > .1) effects on these choices.

Effects of Motivation on Performance

Data for the RT cap − floor differential are shown in Table 2. Statistical analysis conducted to investigate the effects of diagnosis and motivation on the RT differential revealed no effect of diagnosis [F(1,52) < 1, ηp2 < .001] but a significant effect of motivation [F(2,104) = 10.972, p < .001, ηp2 = .17] with smaller mean differential in the motivated conditions (reward and response cost) than baseline (p < .001). The diagnosis × motivation interaction was nonsignificant [F(2,104) < 1, ηp2 = .009]. Analysis investigating the effects of medication and motivation on the differential revealed no effect of medication [F(1,25) < 1, ηp2 = .03] but a significant effect of motivation [F(2,50) = 7.569, p = .001, ηp2 = .23], with smaller differential in the motivated conditions than baseline (p = .006). The medication × motivation interaction was nonsignificant [F(2,50) = 2.920, p > .1, ηp2 = .07].

Effects of Diagnosis and Motivation on ERP Measures

Event-related potential data relevant to all statistical analyses are shown in Table 2. Event-related potential waveforms are shown in Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Stimulus-locked waveforms of CTRL group and ADHD group, off and on medication. Panels show waveforms for baseline condition (left), reward condition (center), and response cost condition (right). Within each panel, frontal electrode (Fz) is shown on the left and central electrode (Cz) is shown on the right. The top row shows go trials, and the bottom row shows no-go trials. Solid line represents the CTRL group; dotted line represents the ADHD group off medication; dashed line represents the ADHD group on medication. The waveform for the ADHD group off medication includes one participant excluded from the within-subjects medication comparison due to low trial numbers. ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; CTRL, control.

N2

Analysis revealed significantly greater amplitude on no-go than go trials [F(1,49) = 22.065, p < .001, ηp2 = .31] and a main effect of motivation [F(2,98) = 3.568, p = .032, ηp2 = .06] with greater amplitude for reward than response cost (p = .005). Group differences were in the predicted direction (CTRL > ADHD) but did not reach significance [F(1,49) = 2.723, p = .1, ηp2 = .05]. The diagnosis × motivation interaction [F(2,98) = 1.111, p > .1, ηp2 = .022] and the diagnosis × motivation × trial interaction [F(2,98) < 1, ηp2 = .011] were nonsignificant.

P3

There were significant main effects of diagnosis [F(1,49) = 5.164, p = .027, ηp2 = .1], trial [F(1,49) = 33.364, p < .001, ηp2 = .41], and motivation [F(2,98) = 5.118, p = .008, ηp2 = .1] on P3 amplitude, with greater amplitude for CTRL than ADHD, for no-go than go trials, and for reward and response cost compared with baseline (p = .003). The diagnosis × motivation interaction [F(2,98) < 1, ηp2 = .008] and the diagnosis × motivation × trial interaction [F(2,98) = 1.254, p > .1, ηp2 = .025] were nonsignificant.

Effects of Medication and Motivation on ERP Measures

N2

One extreme outlier was excluded. Amplitude was significantly greater on than off medication [F(1,20) = 5.844, p = .025, ηp2 = .23] and for no-go than go trials [F(1,24) = 5.298, p = .03, ηp2 = .18]. There was a main effect of motivation [F(2,40) = 4.752, p = .014, ηp2 = .19] with greater amplitude in motivated conditions than baseline (p = .045) and in the reward condition compared with response cost (p = .036). The medication × motivation [F(2,48) = .621, p > .1, ηp2 = .03] and medication × motivation × trial [F(2,48) = 2.314, p > .1, ηp2 = .09] interactions were nonsignificant.

P3

Amplitude was significantly greater on than off medication [F(1,21) = 24.490, p < .001, ηp2 = .54] and for no-go than go trials [F(1,21) = 17.691, p < .001, ηp2 = .46]. There was a main effect of motivation [F(2,42) = 4.00, p = .02, ηp2 = .16] with greater amplitude in both motivated conditions compared with baseline (p = .007) and no significant difference between reward and response cost (p > .1). The medication × motivation [F(2,50) = .52, p > .1, ηp2 = .02] and medication × motivation × trial [F(2,50) = 1.374, p > .1, ηp2 = .05] interactions were nonsignificant.

Discussion

This study investigated the effects of motivational incentives and medication on electrophysiological indexes of response conflict and attention in children with ADHD. To ensure neural correlates were invoked to a similar degree in all children, a tracking algorithm minimized between- and within-subject variance in the ratio of failed to successfully inhibited no-go trials. Analysis of the self-report visual analogue scales confirmed that participants responded to the motivational incentives: participants in both groups and sessions chose the reward condition as the one they liked the most and the response cost condition as the one that made them most careful. The success of the tracking algorithm and the motivational incentives was critical to assessing effects of diagnosis, medication, and motivation on the ERPs.

Effects of Diagnosis and Motivation

In support of previous research, the P3, an index of attention to a task-relevant stimulus (40) was significantly reduced in the ADHD group. N2 amplitude, an index of response inhibition (9) or conflict detection/resolution (10), was reduced, although this fell just short of statistical significance. Analysis revealed significant increases in N2 and P3 amplitudes in both groups in the motivated conditions compared with the baseline (low motivation) condition, indicating greater activation of processes underlying response conflict and attention to a task-relevant (salient) stimulus when the incentive to inhibit increased. Motivational incentives enhanced amplitudes of the ADHD group, bringing them closer to the baseline amplitudes of the control group. There was no interaction between diagnosis and motivation. In previous behavioral studies (17–23), the absence of interaction has been interpreted as evidence of independence of inhibitory and motivational deficits in ADHD, supporting the dual pathway model (24). The present findings clearly show, however, that motivational incentives were effective in enhancing electrophysiological markers of inhibitory control in ADHD, although the effects were not differentially greater than in control subjects. This suggests first that, in support of previous studies, the ADHD children were neither hypersensitive nor hyposensitive to these immediate, consistent incentives (12) and second that, to achieve the same level as control children in both the baseline and motivated conditions, children with ADHD may require incentives that differ quantitatively or qualitatively from those that produce effects in typically developing children. It will therefore be important in future research to investigate other factors that influence the effectiveness of incentives on inhibitory control in ADHD, such as reward magnitude, reinforcement history, and reinforcement scheduling (11,12). This may help clarify the nature of relationships between inhibitory and motivational deficits in this population. Of note, the diagnosis effect for the N2 did not quite reach statistical significance, possibly providing insufficient scope for measuring relationships between this index and motivation.

Effects of Medication and Motivation

Within-subjects analysis revealed significant effects of medication on N2 and P3 amplitudes, supporting the relatively limited research in this area (27–31) and identifying a possible locus for the effects of methylphenidate on symptom severity. Interactions were statistically nonsignificant, suggesting that medication and motivation have additive, rather than synergistic, effects on these neural correlates. With regard to the MTA study, which reported that stimulant medication was effective at lower doses when combined with behavioral therapy (32), our findings suggest these interventions may act independently. Follow-up in the MTA revealed that behavioral treatment alone was less effective than medication alone, a finding that is consistent with the larger effect sizes for medication than motivational incentives in the present study. Of course, the motivational incentives employed here were not designed to replicate behavioral modification programs used in clinical practice, but the results suggest that both operant conditioning (reward and response cost) and stimulant medication impact these particular electrophysiological indexes of response conflict and allocation of attentional resources. It will also be important to determine whether the effects identified in the present study are generalizable to other response inhibition paradigms and to children with less stringently defined ADHD, including those with the inattentive subtype.

Limitations

Although the sample sizes were large compared with many other ERP studies in this field (e.g., [5,7,17]), statistical power may have been insufficient to detect critical interactions. However, several methodological steps were taken to maximize power: careful matching of samples on age, gender, and SES; conducting analysis on the equivalent session for each matched CTRL-ADHD pair; tracking algorithm to reduce between-subjects variance; and careful preprocessing of EEG data. Power calculations revealed 79% power to detect an effect size of .4 and 84% power to detect an effect of .3 for the diagnosis by motivation and medication by motivation interactions, respectively. Nonsignificant interactions are therefore likely to be small. Low numbers of successfully inhibited no-go trials meant it was not possible to investigate neural correlates specific to successful inhibition. Previous studies have identified differences in the morphology of the waveform on FI and SI trials, particularly with regard to the amplitude and latency of the N2 and P3 (9,10). A number of additional analyses (Supplement 1) were conducted to ensure that collapsing across FI and SI trials does not confound interpretation of the effects of diagnosis, medication, or motivation presented here.

Conclusions

This is the first study to report effects of motivation on ERP correlates of response conflict and attention in children with ADHD. Stimulant medication also increased N2 and P3 amplitude. Methylphenidate treatment and motivational incentives had independent rather than synergistic effects on these ERP markers, with relatively greater effects of medication than motivation. Future research should investigate under controlled conditions whether the additive effects reported here for motivational incentives and stimulant medication in ADHD is replicated for behavioral and symptomatic outcomes.

Acknowledgments

Funding for this study was provided by the Wellcome Trust, Grant Ref: 076448/Z/05/Z. Patients were recruited with the approval and assistance of Nottinghamshire Healthcare National Health Service (NHS) Trust, United Kingdom.

We gratefully acknowledge the assistance of Dr. Carolyn Nahman and Dr. Rachel Duffy in recruiting and assessing participants in the attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder group and Nottinghamshire Healthcare NHS Trust. We also thank the participants and families who took part in the study.

The authors reported no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

Footnotes

Supplementary material cited in this article is available online.

Supplementary data

Supplement 1
mmc1.pdf (72KB, pdf)

References

  • 1.Nigg J.T. Is ADHD a disinhibitory disorder? Psychol Bull. 2001;127:571–598. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.5.571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Willcutt E.G., Doyle A.E., Nigg J.T., Faraone S.V., Pennington B.F. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:1336–1346. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Barkley R.A. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, self-regulation, and time: Toward a more comprehensive theory. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1997;18:271–279. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Aron A.R., Poldrack R.A. The cognitive neuroscience of response inhibition: Relevance for genetic research in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:1285–1292. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Brandeis D., van Leeuwen T.H., Rubia K., Vitacco D., Steger J., Pascual-Marqui R.D. Neuroelectric mapping reveals precursor of stop failures in children with attention deficits. Behav Brain Res. 1998;94:111–125. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(97)00174-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Dimoska A., Johnstone S.J., Barry R.J., Clarke A.R. Inhibitory motor control in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Event-related potentials in the stop-signal paradigm. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54:1345–1354. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00703-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Fallgatter A.J., Ehlis A.C., Seifert J., Strik W.K., Scheuerpflug P., Zillessen K.E. Altered response control and anterior cingulate function in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder boys. Clin Neurophysiol. 2004;115:973–981. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.11.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Pliszka S.R., Liotti M., Woldorff M.G. Inhibitory control in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Event-related potentials identify the processing component and timing of an impaired right-frontal response-inhibition mechanism. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;48:238–246. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00890-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Falkenstein M., Hoormann J., Hohnsbein J. ERP components in go/nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition. Acta Psychol (Amst) 1999;101:267–291. doi: 10.1016/s0001-6918(99)00008-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Nieuwenhuis S., Yeung N., Van den Wildenberg W., Ridderinkhof K.R. Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a go/no-go task: Effects of response conflict and trial type frequency. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2003;3:17–26. doi: 10.3758/cabn.3.1.17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Sagvolden T., Aase H., Zeiner P., Berger D. Altered reinforcement mechanisms in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behav Brain Res. 1998;94:61–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Wickens J.R., Tripp E.G. Altered sensitivity to reward in children with ADHD: Dopamine timing is off. Behav Brain Sci. 2005;28:445–446. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Borger N., van der Meere J. Motor control and state regulation in children with ADHD: A cardiac response study. Biol Psychol. 2000;51:247–267. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(99)00040-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Sergeant J.A. The cognitive-energetic model: An empirical approach to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2000;24:7–12. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(99)00060-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Konrad K., Gauggel S., Manz A., Scholl M. Lack of inhibition: A motivational deficit in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and children with traumatic brain injury. Child Neuropsychol. 2000;6:286–296. doi: 10.1076/chin.6.4.286.3145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Slusarek M., Velling S., Bunk D., Eggers C. Motivational effects on inhibitory control in children with ADHD. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40:355–363. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200103000-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Crone E.A., Jennings J.R., van der Molen M.W. Sensitivity to interference and response contingencies in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44:214–226. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Desman C., Petermann F., Hampel P. Deficit in response inhibition in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Impact of motivation? Child Neuropsychol. 2008;14:483–503. doi: 10.1080/09297040701625831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Huang-Pollock C.L., Mikami A.Y., Pfiffner L., McBurnett K. ADHD subtype differences in motivational responsivity but not inhibitory control: Evidence from a reward-based variation of the stop signal paradigm. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2007;36:127–136. doi: 10.1080/15374410701274124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Iaboni F., Douglas V.I., Baker A.G. Effects of reward and response costs on inhibition in ADHD children. J Abnorm Psychol. 1995;104:232–240. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.104.1.232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Oosterlaan J., Sergeant J.A. Effects of reward and response cost on response inhibition in AD/HD, disruptive, anxious, and normal children. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1998;26:161–174. doi: 10.1023/a:1022650216978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Shanahan M.A., Pennington B.F., Willcutt E.W. Do motivational incentives reduce the inhibition deficit in ADHD? Dev Neuropsychol. 2008;33:137–159. doi: 10.1080/87565640701884238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Wodka E.L., Mahone E.M., Blankner J.G., Larson J.C., Fotedar S., Denckla M.B. Evidence that response inhibition is a primary deficit in ADHD. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2007;29:345–356. doi: 10.1080/13803390600678046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Sonuga-Barke E.J. Psychological heterogeneity in AD/HD—a dual pathway model of behaviour and cognition. Behav Brain Res. 2002;130:29–36. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00432-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Volkow N.D., Wang G.J., Fowler J.S., Telang F., Maynard L., Logan J. Evidence that methylphenidate enhances the saliency of a mathematical task by increasing dopamine in the human brain. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:1173–1180. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.7.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Volkow N.D., Wang G.J., Fowler J.S., Ding Y.S. Imaging the effects of methylphenidate on brain dopamine: New model on its therapeutic actions for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:1410–1415. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.DeVito E.E., Blackwell A.D., Clark L., Kent L., Dezsery A.M., Turner D.C. Methylphenidate improves response inhibition but not reflection-impulsivity in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2009;202:531–539. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1337-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Scheres A., Oosterlaan J., Swanson J., Morein-Zamir S., Meiran N., Schut H. The effect of methylphenidate on three forms of response inhibition in boys with AD/HD. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2003;31:105–120. doi: 10.1023/a:1021729501230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Wilson H.K., Cox D.J., Merkel R.L., Moore M., Coghill D. Effect of extended release stimulant-based medications on neuropsychological functioning among adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2006;21:797–807. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2006.06.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Sunohara G.A., Malone M.A., Rovet J., Humphries T., Roberts W., Taylor M.J. Effect of methylphenidate on attention in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): ERP evidence. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;21:218–228. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00023-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Pliszka S.R., Liotti M., Bailey B.Y., Perez R., Glahn D., Semrud-Clikeman M. Electrophysiological effects of stimulant treatment on inhibitory control in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2007;17:356–366. doi: 10.1089/cap.2006.0081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Jensen P.S., Arnold L.E., Richters J.E., Severe J.B., Vereen D., Vitiello B. A 14-month randomized clinical trial of treatment strategies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56:1073–1086. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.12.1073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38:581–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Conners C.K. Multi-Health Systems; North Tonawanda, NY: 1997. Conners' Ratings Scales: Revised Technical Manual. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Goodman R., Ford T., Richards H., Gatward R., Meltzer H. The Development and Well-Being Assessment: Description and initial validation of an integrated assessment of child and adolescent psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2000;41:645–655. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Rutter M., Bailey A., Lord C. Western Psychological Services; Los Angeles: 2003. SCQ: Social Communication Questionnaire. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Wechsler D. Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1999. The Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.DuPaul G.J., Power T.J., Anastopoulos A.D., Reid R. The. Guilford Publications; New York: 1998. ADHD Rating Scale-IV: Checklists, Norms and Clinical Interpretations. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Gratton G., Coles M.G., Donchin E. A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1983;55:468–484. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(83)90135-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Donchin E. Presidential address, 1980: Surprise! … Surprise? Psychophysiology. 1981;18:493–513. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb01815.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplement 1
mmc1.pdf (72KB, pdf)

RESOURCES