Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Psychiatr Res. 2009 Oct 12;44(6):393–404. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.09.005

Proactive, Reactive, and Romantic Relational Aggression in Adulthood: Measurement, Predictive Validity, Gender Differences, and Association with Intermittent Explosive Disorder

Dianna Murray-Close, Jamie M Ostrov, David A Nelson, Nicki R Crick, Emil F Coccaro
PMCID: PMC2849926  NIHMSID: NIHMS146067  PMID: 19822329

Abstract

The psychometric properties of a recently introduced adult self-report of relational aggression are presented. Specifically, the predictive utility of proactive and reactive peer-directed relational aggression, as well as romantic relational aggression, are explored in a large (N = 1387) study of adults. The measure had adequate reliability and validity and the subscales demonstrated unique predictive abilities for a number of dependent variables. In particular, reactive but not proactive relational aggression was uniquely associated with history of abuse, hostile attribution biases, and feelings of distress regarding relational provocation situations. Reactive relational aggression was also more strongly related to anger and hostility than proactive aggression. In addition, relational aggression in the context of romantic relationships was uniquely related to anger, hostility, impulsivity, history of abuse, hostile attribution biases, and emotional sensitivity to relational provocations, even when controlling for peer-directed relational aggression. Gender differences in overall levels of relational aggression were not observed; however, males were most likely to engage in peer-directed proactive and reactive relational aggression whereas females were most likely to engage in romantic relational aggression. In a second study (N = 150), relational aggression was higher in a sample of adults with Intermittent Explosive Disorder than in a sample of healthy controls or psychiatric controls. The findings highlight the importance of assessing subtypes of relational aggression in adult samples. Ways in which this measure may extend research in psychology and psychiatry are discussed.


An important contributor to maladaptive functioning in both childhood and adulthood is involvement in aggressive conduct (Crick, Werner, et al., 1999). Recently, investigators have emphasized the importance of examining the correlates of relational forms of aggression (e.g., social exclusion) in addition to the physical forms of aggressive conduct (e.g., hitting, assault) that have traditionally captured the majority of empirical attention (Crick & Grotpeter, 1995). The purpose of the present study was to validate a measure of relational aggression in adulthood and examine the factors associated with each subtype (i.e., proactive, reactive, and romantic) of relational aggression. Moreover, given the lack of research assessing gender differences in relational aggression in adulthood, we explored whether men and women differed in their use of each subtype of relational aggression. Finally, we examined whether relational aggression was elevated in a group of adults with a history of impulsive aggression (i.e., Intermittent Explosive Disorder).

Aggression, defined as behaviors intended to hurt, harm, or injure another person (see Dodge, Coie, & Lyman, 2006) can vary in form. Physical aggression, which harms others via physical force or the threat of physical force, consists of such behaviors as hitting, pushing, kicking and punching (Dodge et al., 2006; Crick & Grotpeter, 1995). Relational aggression, in which the relationship serves as the vehicle of harm, includes spreading malicious rumors, lies, gossip or secrets, as well as intentionally ignoring (i.e., silent treatment) or excluding a person from an activity or group interaction (Crick & Grotpeter, 1995). Relational aggression may be manifested in both direct and covert ways (Crick, Werner, et al., 1999), which distinguishes it from similar aggression constructs such as indirect aggression (see Björkqvist, 1994).

Although a substantial body of research has provided evidence that involvement in relationally aggressive behavior in childhood and adolescence is associated with maladaptive outcomes (see Crick, Werner, et al., 1999), little work has examined the correlates of relational aggression in adulthood. The limited studies indicate that relationally aggressive behavior among emerging adults (18 to 25-year-olds) is associated with serious adjustment difficulties (Burton et al., 2007; Goldstein, Chesir-Teran, & McFaul, 2008; Lento-Zwolinski, 2007; Miller & Lynam, 2003; Storch, Bagner, Geffken, & Baumeister, 2004; Werner & Crick, 1999). Additional research with older samples is clearly warranted.

However, one limitation of studies assessing relational aggression in adulthood is a lack of reliable and valid measures. Indeed, the majority of published relational aggression measures have been developed for use during early and middle childhood and most rely upon peer nominations, observations, and teacher-report methods (for a review, see Crick, Ostrov, & Kawabata, 2007). This is a significant limitation if a researcher or clinician wants to assess relational aggression among individuals in non-group contexts (e.g., clinical practice) or when other informants are not available or may not be knowledgeable about the participants’ behavior (see Little, Jones, Henrich, & Hawley, 2003).

An important consideration in the measurement of adult relational aggression is the function of such aggressive behavior. Psychologists and psychiatrists have distinguished aggressive behaviors that are planned and displayed to serve a goal-directed end (i.e., proactive aggression or premeditated aggression) from those that are more impulsive and displayed in response to a perceived threat and out of hostility or anger (i.e., reactive aggression or impulsive aggression; Dodge, 1991; Stanford, Houston, Mathias, Villemarette-Pittman, Helfritz, & Conklin, 2003; Vitaro, Gendreau, Tremblay, & Oligny, 1998). Moreover, a number of studies have documented the divergent nature of proactive and reactive aggression (Dodge & Coie, 1987; Fite et al., 2006; Poulin and Boivin, 2000; Pulkkinen, 1996; Raine et al., 2006), including evidence supporting the discriminant validity of these functions of aggression (Card & Little, 2006; Crick & Dodge, 1996; Day, Bream & Pal, 1992; Hubbard, Dodge, Cillessen, Coie & Schwartz, 2001; Dodge, Lochman, Harnish, Bates, & Pettit, 1997; Hubbard et al., 2002, 2004; Price & Dodge, 1989; Salmivalli & Nieminen, 2002; Sanford et al., 2003; Vitaro et al., 1998; Waschbusch, Willoughby, & Pelham, 1998; Waschbusch, Pelham, Jennings, Greiner, Tarter, & Moss, 2002). Thus, in the present study, the measure of relational aggression in adulthood included subscales assessing both proactive and reactive functions of relational aggression.

Despite these recent advances, most research examining the unique correlates of proactive and reactive functions of aggression have focused on physical forms of aggression in child or adolescent samples. In these studies, consistent with its emotional nature, reactive physical aggression is associated with impulsivity, hostility, anger, and skin conductance reactivity during a provoking task (Hubbard et al., 2002; Raine et al., 2006). Reactive aggression is also associated with hostile attribution bias (HAB), or the tendency to assume that others’ intentions are malicious during ambiguous conflict situations (Crick & Dodge, 1996; Dodge & Coie, 1987; Schwartz et al., 1998; for a meta-analysis, see Orobio de Castro, Merk, Koops, Veerman, & Bosch, 2005; for a review, see Crick & Dodge, 1994). In regard to relational aggression, researchers have similarly found it to be associated with HAB and feelings of distress following ambiguous provocation. However, whereas physically aggressive individuals exhibit HAB regarding instrumental provocations (e.g., damage to property or physical harm), relationally aggressive children display HAB regarding relational provocations (e.g., relational slights or social exclusion; Crick, 1995; Crick et al., 2002; cf., Nelson, Mitchell, & Yang, 2008; see Bailey & Ostrov, 2008 for a study with young adults). Finally, researchers have found that history of childhood abuse is associated with reactive, but not proactive, aggression (Dodge et al., 1997).

Beyond functions of aggression, another important consideration in the measurement of adult relational aggression is the context in which the aggression occurs. Most research with children and adolescents focuses on relational aggression enacted against peers or friends (e.g., Crick & Grotpeter, 1995; Grotpeter & Crick, 1996). However, as romantic relationships emerge in adolescence and become increasingly salient into adulthood (Furman & Buhrmester, 1985), it is important to explore the use of relational aggression in the context of such relationships. Indeed, initial work indicates that relational aggression enacted against romantic partners is associated with romantic relationships of relatively low quality (Linder et al., 2002) and with indices of maladjustment (e.g., depression, drug and alcohol use; Bagner et al., 2007). As such, a valid measure of adult relational aggression should attend to these behaviors occurring between romantic partners.

This study also examines whether adult relational aggression is tied to gender. In childhood, gender differences in relational aggression are often but not always documented, with girls exhibiting greater levels of relational aggression than boys (see Crick et al., 2007 for review). However, assessments of gender differences in relational aggression during adulthood are more limited. As males and females enter adolescence and adulthood, cross-sex interactions become increasingly common (Maccoby, 1990). As a result, the relationally aggressive behaviors more characteristic of elementary school girls may become more common among males as mixed-sex interactions increase. In fact, preliminary research with young adults suggests that there are no gender differences in relational aggression (e.g., Bailey & Ostrov, 2008; Basow et al., 2007; Burton et al., 2007; Loudin, Loukis, & Robinson, 2003) or that males are more relationally aggressive than females (Storch et al., 2004). In addition, studies have found no differences in the use of relational aggression against a romantic partner (Linder et al., 2002).

Finally, limited research is available addressing the association between relational aggression and clinical diagnoses in adulthood. However, preliminary research with children suggests that heightened levels of relational aggression are associated with clinical problems such as ADHD (e.g., Zalecki & Hinshaw, 2004), ODD (Keenan, Coyne, & Lahey, 2008; Ohan & Johnston, 2005) and Borderline features (Crick, Murray-Close, & Woods, 2005). In a similar vein, relational aggression may be associated with clinically-relevant disorders among adults. For example, adults with intermittent explosive disorder (IED), a disorder characterized by impulsive aggressive outburst, may also exhibit elevated levels of relational aggression. In fact, a recent study demonstrated that adults with IED were more likely than their peers to exhibit a hostile attribution bias regarding ambiguous provocations (Coccaro, Noblett, & McCloskey, 2009), which is a risk factor for involvement in aggression (Crick & Dodge, 1994). Thus, our final goal was to examine whether adults with IED engaged in heightened levels of relational aggression. In addition, given the overlap between physical and relational aggression in previous studies, we examined whether the association between IED and relational aggression remained when controlling for participants’ physically aggressive conduct. This analysis allowed us to investigate the utility of including measures of relational aggression, in addition to traditional measures of physical aggression, in clinical settings.

Goals and Hypotheses

Few psychometrically sound methods are currently available for investigating relational aggression in adulthood. The first goal of the present study was to evaluate the psychometric properties of a self-report measure of relationally aggressive behavior (developed by Morales and Crick, 1998) in a large (N = 1387) community sample of adults (Study 1). To date, this instrument has been used only with college samples (e.g., Bailey & Ostrov, 2008; Goldstein et al., 2008; Lento-Zwolinski, 2007; Miller & Lynam, 2003; Ostrov & Houston, 2008). Several hypotheses were tested in relation to this goal. First, romantic relational aggression, peer-directed proactive relational aggression, and peer-directed reactive relational aggression were expected to exhibit good internal consistency and stability over time. Second, it was expected that a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) would confirm that a three-factor model would yield the best fit for the data. In addition, the inter-correlations between these subscales were predicted to be moderate in nature, indicating the shared but yet unique components of each construct. Third, in keeping with past findings (e.g., Dodge et al., 1997; Hubbard et al., 2002) we predicted that reactive relational aggression would be uniquely correlated with indices of anger, impulsivity, hostility, abuse, HAB, and feelings of distress following relational provocation. Fourth, we expected romantic relational aggression to be uniquely associated with all of these indices, even when controlling for peer-directed proactive and reactive aggression. The second goal was to examine gender differences in relational aggression in adulthood; given previous research findings (e.g., Bailey & Ostrov, 2008), no gender differences in relational aggression were expected. The third goal was to examine whether relational aggression was elevated in adults with a history of impulsive aggression (i.e., IED) when compared to healthy controls and psychiatric controls (N = 150; Study 2). We expected that adults with IED would exhibit heightened levels of relational aggression when compared to their peers, and that this effect would persist even when controlling for physically aggressive conduct.

Study 1: Psychometric Properties of Self Reports of Relational Aggression

Method

Participants

The participants in this study include unrelated male and females from a population based twin sample from the PENN Twins Cohort (Coccaro & Jacobson, 2006) who completed a variety of questionnaires in a mailed survey. Data from only one member of any twin pair (i.e., where both members responded to the survey), randomly selected, were used in this study. Participants in the study (800 females and 587 males) ranged in age from 25 to 45 per inclusion criteria of the twin cohort (M age = 33.80, SD = 5.89). Mean income was between $50,000 and $75,000. The racial profile of the participant sample showed an overrepresentation of Caucasian participants as compared with African-American and other races, with Caucasian participants representing approximately 93% of the total sample.

Procedure

The mailed survey was conducted according to the method of Dillman (2000) and had a response rate of approximately 71%. Participants were given $10 with the survey form which they kept whether or not they completed the survey. The surveys were returned to a third-party research organization (Research Data, Inc., Richmond, VA) who scanned the survey results and checked all data prior to sending it on to the investigators.

Measures

Self-Report of Aggression and Social Behavior Measure (SRASBM)

The SRASBM is an instrument developed by Morales and Crick (1998). Two subscales concerning romantic relational aggression and victimization were recently used in Linder, Crick, and Collins (2002) and had acceptable Cronbach’s alphas. The full measure consists of 16 relational aggression items. Specifically, 5 assess peer-directed proactive relational aggression (e.g., “I have threatened to share private information about my friends with other people in order to get them to comply with my wishes”), 6 assess peer-directed reactive relational aggression (e.g., “When I am not invited to do something with a group of people, I will exclude those people from future activities”), and 5 assess romantic relational aggression (e.g., “If my romantic partner makes me mad, I will flirt with another person in front of him/her”). The romantic relational aggression subscale includes items that assess both proactive and reactive functions. However, there were not enough items to consider these functions separately (i.e., only 1 proactive item). Subscale scores were calculated by computing the mean of all items in the subscale, with higher scores indicating higher levels of relational aggression. In the present study, the response scale for the SRASBM was adapted from its original 7-point scale to range from 0 (“never”) to 4 (“very often”). The SRASBM has been used in a few recent studies and acceptable internal consistency has been demonstrated for a total relational aggression scale (Goldstein et al., 2008; Miller & Lynam, 2003; Schad, Szwedo, Antonishak, Hare, & Allen, 2008) as well as for proactive and reactive relational aggression subscales (Bailey & Ostrov, 2008; Lento-Zwolinski, 2007). In addition, one-month test-retest reliability has been excellent (r > .75) for proactive and reactive functions of relational peer aggression (Ostrov & Houston, 2008).

Buss Perry Aggression Questionnaire (BPAQ; Buss & Perry, 1992)

The BPAQ is a 29-item self-report questionnaire including subscales assessing anger (7 items, e.g., “I have trouble controlling my temper”) and hostility (8 items, e.g., “I wonder why sometimes I feel so bitter about things”). This measure also includes subscales of physical aggression and verbal aggression that were not used in the present study. The items are scored on a five-point scale ranging from 1 (“extremely uncharacteristic of me”) to 5 (“extremely characteristic of me).” Items for each subscale were summed to yield anger and hostility subscales, respectively.

Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11; Patton, Stanford, & Barratt, 1995)

The BIS-11 is a 34 item self-report questionnaire developed to assess impulsive behavior, taking into account the multi-factorial nature of the construct (e.g., “I don’t pay attention,” “I say things without thinking,” “I act on impulse”). Each item is scored on a four-point scale: 1 (“rarely/never”), 2 (“occasionally”), 3 (“often”), and 4 (“almost always/always”). Items are then summed to yield a total impulsivity score. Past internal consistency reliability coefficients for the BIS-11 ranged from .79 to .83 (Patton et al., 1995).

Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ; Bernstein et al., 1994)

The CTQ is a self-administered measure that assesses five types of childhood maltreatment: emotional abuse (e.g., “Called names by family”), physical abuse (e.g., “Hit hard enough to leave bruises”), sexual abuse (e.g., “Was touched sexually”), emotional neglect (e.g., “Did not feel loved”) and physical neglect (“Wore dirty clothes”). Responses are coded on a five-point scale that ranges from 1 (“never true”) to 5 (“very often true”). Higher scores indicate more severe abuse or neglect. The CTQ has been validated in more than 2,000 participants from clinical and community samples. Reliability (Cronbach’s α’s) of the scales ranges from .66 to .92 (e.g., Bernstein & Fink, 1998; Bernstein et al., 1994). Scores are stable over time and show criterion validity as well as convergent and discriminant validity with other abuse instruments (Bernstein et al., 1994).

State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI; Spielberger, 1996)

The STAXI is a 44-item self-report instrument that assesses state and trait anger. The STAXI measures the intensity of feelings of anger (state anger), the disposition to experience anger (trait anger), behaviorally expressed anger (anger-out), suppressed anger (anger-in), and self-control of anger behavior (anger control). Trait anger is composed of two subscales, angry temperament, which measures a general propensity to experience and express anger without provocation, and angry reaction, which measures the disposition to express anger when criticized or treated unfairly by others. In addition, anger expression and its major components (i.e., anger out, anger in, anger control) are measured. The items are scored on a 4-point scale that ranges from 1 (“not at all”) to 4 (“very much so”). Available internal consistency reliability coefficients for the STAXI (Cronbach’s α’s) range from .64 to.92 (Spielberger, 1996).

Assessment of Attribution and Emotional Response to Socially Ambiguous Stimuli (Social Information Processing-Attribution and Emotion Response Questionnaire: SIP-AERQ)

The SIP-AERQ consisted of 8 vignettes written specifically for adults modeled after similar vignettes used by Crick et al. (2002; see Coccaro, Noblett, & McCloskey, 2009). Each vignette presented a socially ambiguous situation in which an adverse action is directed at the person who the participant is asked to identify with. Although the measure includes vignettes of instrumental (4 items; e.g., being hit by someone) and relational provocation (4 items; e.g., a friend shares one of your secrets), only the relational provocation items were included in the present study. Each vignette was then followed by four Likert-scaled questions that assessed direct hostile intent (e.g., “My friend wanted me to feel stupid for asking to keep my secret”), indirect hostile intent (e.g., “My friend wanted to expose my secret”), instrumental non-hostile intent (e.g., “My friend wanted to impress other people with their secret knowledge about me”), and neutral or benign intent (e.g., “My friend forgot that this was an important secret for me”). Scores for the hostile intent (direct or indirect) were summed across vignettes to yield an overall hostile intent attribution score (scores for instrumental and benign intent were not included in this study). The four attribution items were followed by two items designed to reflect negative emotional response (i.e., “How likely is it that you would be angry if this happened to you?” and “How likely is it that you would be upset with yourself if this happened to you?”). The emotional response items were followed by the same Likert ratings as the four attribution items, which ranged from 0 (“Not at all likely”) to 3 (“Very likely”). Scores for the two emotional response items were summed across vignettes to yield an overall emotional reactivity score. Previous research has documented the favorable psychometric properties of this measure, including factor structure, reliability, and convergent and discriminant validity (Coccaro, Noblett, & McCloskey, 2009). These response items were adapted from similar items currently being used in assessments in the laboratory of Kenneth Dodge, Ph.D.

Results

Subscale Reliability and Short-Term Stability

Results indicated adequate reliability for the aggression subscales. Alpha coefficients for the Total SRASBM score and for the three SRASBM subscale scores were .83 for Total Relational Aggression, .69 for Peer-directed Proactive Relational Aggression, .72 for Peer-directed Reactive Relational Aggression, and .66 for Romantic Relational Aggression. In addition, we examined the stability of the relational aggression scales in a sub-study of 153 of 200 twin subjects, selected at random, tested on two occasions, separated by about eight months (mean 8.0 ± 0.6 months, range = 6–9 months). The longitudinal correlations were: .66 (p < .001) for Total Relational Aggression, .59 (p < .001) for Peer-directed Proactive Relational Aggression, .65 (p < .001) for Peer-directed Reactive Relational Aggression, and .68 (p < .001) for Romantic Relational Aggression.

Factor Structure and Distinction of Constructs

Confirmatory factor analyses were carried out using the Mplus program. In line with empirically derived arguments which suggest that Likert-type data should be treated as ordinal scales rather than as interval variables in statistical analysis, particularly when there is skewness in the indicators (see Yang et al., 2003, for a review), self-reports of relational aggression were treated as categorical rather than continuous. A confirmatory factor analysis was run to test the measurement of the relational aggression constructs: (Peer-directed) Proactive Relational Aggression, (Peer-directed) Reactive Relational Aggression, and Romantic Relational Aggression. Preliminary results indicated that two items cross-loaded on two factors; thus, these items were dropped, yielding an overall measure of 14 items. Standardized factor loadings for each item are presented in Table 1. The three-factor measurement model of the aggression constructs (i.e., proactive, reactive, and romantic) fit the data of the sample satisfactorily, as shown by the goodness of fit indices at the base of Table 1. Intercorrelations among the three different latent constructs indicated that proactive and reactive peer-directed aggression were highly correlated (ϕ = .85, p < .0001). Romantic relational aggression was also correlated with proactive (ϕ = .75, p < .0001) and reactive (ϕ = .68, p < .0001) peer-directed relational aggression.

Table 1.

Standardized Factor Loadings of the Aggression Constructs and Goodness of Fit Indices.

Item # Content
Peer-directed Proactive Relational Aggression Items
B My friends know that I will think less of them if they do not do what I want them to do. .71
D When I want something from a friend of mine, I act “cold” or indifferent towards them until I get what I want. .80
J I have threatened to share private information about my friends with other people in order to get them to comply with my wishes. .77
P I have intentionally ignored a person until they gave me my way about something. .80
Peer-directed Reactive Relational Aggression Items
C When I am not invited to do something with a group of people, I will exclude those people from future activities. .68
F When I have been angry at, or jealous of someone, I have tried to damage that person’s reputation by gossiping about him or her or by passing on negative information about him/her to other people. .68
G When someone does something that makes me angry, I try to embarrass that person or make them look stupid in front of his/her friends .72
I When I am mad at a person, I try to make sure she/he is excluded from group activities (going to the movies or to a bar). .74
L I have spread rumors about a person just to be mean. .70
Relational Aggression in Romantic Relationships Items
A I have threatened to break up with a romantic partner in order to get him/her to do what I wanted. .69
E I have tried to make my romantic partner jealous when mad at him/her. .81
K I have cheated on my romantic partner because I was angry at him/her. .60
M I have given my romantic partner the silent treatment when my feelings were hurt in some way by him or her. .58
O If my romantic partner makes me mad, I will flirt with another person in front of him/her. .76

Note. Goodness of Fit: χ2 = 322.68, df = 53, p < .0001, TLI = .97, CFI = .95, RMSEA = .06

The relatively high correlations obtained between the constructs led us to test for the distinction of constructs. Thus, the second step in analysis was to test competing models regarding the relative independence of the three aggression constructs with weighted least-squares estimation. Our baseline model was a three-construct model, in which separate aggression constructs (items representing proactive relational, reactive relational, and romantic relational aggression) were hypothesized to provide the best fit for the data. Tests of competing models, in which one or more of the constructs were merged, were completed and the results compared with the baseline model.

Table 2 shows the results of analyses we conducted to compare the baseline model with competing one- and two-factor models. A one-factor model reflects the collapse of all aggression subtypes into one solitary aggression (Proactive + Reactive + Romantic) construct. In contrast, two-factor models included several iterations in which two of three aggression constructs were collapsed. For example, one two-factor model was based on the collapse of the Proactive Relational and Reactive Relational aggression factors, thereby yielding a model which included Romantic Relational aggression and Proactive/Reactive Relational aggression as constructs.

Table 2.

Comparison of the Baseline Model with Competing Two-Factor and One-Factor Alternatives.

P + R R + ROM P + ROM P + R + ROM
χ2diff 72.35 175.40 110.76 202.32
dfdiff 2 2 2 3

p < .0001 < .0001 < .0001 < .0001

Note. P + R = Proactive and Reactive Relational aggression treated as one construct in the model.

R + ROM = Reactive and Romantic Relational aggression treated as one construct in the model.

P + ROM = Proactive and Romantic Relational aggression treated as one construct in the model.

P + R + ROM = Proactive, Reactive, and Romantic Relational aggression treated as one construct in the model.

Each of the competing models were compared with the baseline model. As indicated in Table 2, the chi-square of these competing models increased significantly, which implies greater discrepancy between the models and the data. Accordingly, competing models did not fit the data as well as the baseline three-construct model. Table 2 shows the chi-square difference (χ2diff) and corresponding differences in the degrees of freedom (dfdiff) for each analysis. The chi-square difference indicates the increase in the chi-square relative to the baseline model. Thus, the three aggression constructs are well distinguished, even though they are highly correlated.

Unique Role of Proactive and Reactive Relational Aggression in Predicting Outcomes

In addition to assessing the factor structure of the SRASBM, we examined the predictive validity of the proactive and reactive relational aggression subscales. Because reactive aggression is enacted in response to provocation or frustration (Crick & Dodge, 1996), we expected that reactive relational aggression would be uniquely associated with anger, hostility, impulsivity, hostile attribution bias, and emotional distress. As noted earlier, we also expected it to be associated with a history of abuse. To assess the unique role of proactive and reactive relational aggression in predicting these concurrent outcomes, a series of hierarchical regressions were conducted. In particular, each set of analyses were performed twice, with the order of entry of the independent variables reversed in order to allow for the assessment of the unique contributions of peer-directed proactive and reactive aggression. The dependent variables consisted of the following scales: Buss-Perry anger scale, Buss-Perry hostility scale, BIS, CTQ, STAXI, hostile attribution bias in response to relational provocations, and emotional distress in response to relational provocations. In the first set of regressions, we examined the unique association between proactive relational aggression and these concurrent outcomes. In each regression, reactive relational aggression was entered at Step 1, and proactive relational aggression was entered at Step 2. The results, presented in Table 3, indicated that proactive relational aggression was uniquely associated with anger (Buss-Perry and STAXI), hostility, and impulsivity above and beyond reactive relational aggression.

Table 3.

Unique Associations between Proactive Relational Aggression and Outcome Variables

Model Dependent Variable Step Predictor B t-Value R2, ΔR2 F, ΔF
1 BPAQ Anger 1 Reactive Rel Agg .28*** 10.81 .078 116.86***
2 Proactive Rel Agg .11*** 3.54 .008 12.53***
2 BPAQ Hostility 1 Reactive Rel Agg .39*** 15.65 .151 244.81***
2 Proactive Rel Agg .10** 3.17 .006 10.05**
3 BIS 1 Reactive Rel Agg .17*** 6.27 .028 39.36***
2 Proactive Rel Agg .08* 2.15 .004 5.85*
4 CTQ 1 Reactive Rel Agg .16*** 5.77 .024 33.30***
2 Proactive Rel Agg .04 1.25 .001 1.56
5 STAXI 1 Reactive Rel Agg .37*** 14.58 .134 212.55***
2 Proactive Rel Agg .16*** 5.19 .017 26.91***
6 HAB 1 Reactive Rel Agg .29*** 11.39 .087 129.66***
2 Proactive Rel Agg .05 1.50 .002 2.25
7 Feelings of Distress 1 Reactive Rel Agg .26*** 9.82 .066 96.42***
2 Proactive Rel Agg .01 .36 .000 .13

Note. BPAQ = Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire; Agg = Aggression; Rel = Relational; BIS = Barratt Impulsivity Scale; CTQ = Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; STAXI = State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory; HAB = Hostile Attribution Biases.

*

p < .05;

**

p < .01;

***

p < .001

In the second set of analyses, proactive relational aggression was entered at Step 1, and reactive relational aggression was entered at Step 2. This allowed us to examine the unique association between reactive relational aggression and each outcome variable. The results, presented in Table 4, indicated that reactive relational aggression, above and beyond proactive relational aggression, was significantly associated with anger (Buss-Perry and STAXI), hostility, impulsivity, abuse, hostile intent attributions in response to relational provocations, and emotional sensitivity in response to relational provocations.

Table 4.

Unique Associations between Reactive Relational Aggression and Outcome Variables.

Model Dependent Variable Step Predictor B t-Value R2, ΔR2 F, ΔF
1 BPAQ Anger 1 Proactive Rel Agg .24*** 9.02 .056 81.35***
2 Reactive Rel Agg .22*** 6.81 .031 46.34***
2 BPAQ Hostility 1 Proactive Rel Agg .29*** 11.19 .083 125.16***
2 Reactive Rel Agg .33*** 10.97 .074 120.43***
3 BIS 1 Proactive Rel Agg .15*** 5.58 .022 31.13***
2 Reactive Rel Agg .12*** 3.73 .010 13.92***
4 CTQ 1 Proactive Rel Agg .12*** 4.34 .014 18.82***
2 Reactive Rel Agg .13*** 3.98 .011 15.84***
5 STAXI 1 Proactive Rel Agg .32*** 12.39 .100 153.53***
2 Reactive Rel Agg .27*** 9.00 .050 81.00***
6 HAB 1 Proactive Rel Agg .20*** 7.64 .041 58.29***
2 Reactive Rel Agg .27*** 8.41 .047 70.76***
7 Feelings of Distress 1 Proactive Rel Agg .16*** 5.86 .025 34.33***
2 Reactive Rel Agg .25*** 7.79 .042 60.66***

Note. BPAQ = Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire; Agg = Aggression; Rel = Relational; BIS = Barratt Impulsivity Scale; CTQ = Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; STAXI = State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory; HAB = Hostile Attribution Biases.

***

p < .001

Accordingly, only reactive relational aggression was associated with history of abuse, hostile attribution biases and emotional sensitivity. However, both proactive and reactive relational aggression were associated with anger, hostility and impulsivity. Follow-up analyses were run to examine the relative strength of the correlations between proactive and reactive relational these factors. Comparisons of correlations from the same sample (see Cohen & Cohen, 1983) indicated that reactive relational aggression was more strongly correlated with anger [Buss=Perry: t(1375) = −2.11, p < .05; STAXI: t(1376) = −2.19, p < .05] and hostility, t(1376) = −4.39, p < .001, than was proactive relational aggression. Thus, although both proactive and reactive relational aggression were associated with these outcomes, the association was stronger for reactive relational aggression.

Unique Role of Romantic Relational Aggression in Predicting Outcomes

In the next set of analyses, we examined the unique association between romantic relational aggression and the dependent variables. We expected that these factors would be uniquely associated with romantic relational aggression, even when controlling for proactive and reactive peer-directed relational aggression. To address this hypothesis, a series of hierarchical regression analyses were conducted. In each regression, proactive and reactive peer-directed relational aggression were entered at Step 1, and romantic relational aggression was then entered at Step 2. This allowed us to examine the unique relation between romantic relational aggression and each outcome, controlling for peer-directed aggression. The results, presented in Table 5, indicated that romantic relational aggression was related to anger (Buss-Perry and STAXI), hostility, impulsivity, abuse, hostile attribution biases, and feelings of distress regarding relational provocation.

Table 5.

Unique Associations between Romantic Relational Aggression and Outcome Variables.

Model Dependent Variable Step Predictor B t-Value R2, ΔR2 F, ΔF
1 BPAQ Anger 1 Proactive Rel Agg .12*** 3.63 .087 64.90***
Reactive Rel Agg .21*** 6.68
2 Romantic Rel Agg .13*** 4.11 .011 16.89***
2 BPAQ Hostility 1 Proactive Rel Agg .10** 3.16 .157 127.49***
Reactive Rel Agg .33*** 10.91
2 Romantic Rel Agg .14*** 4.72 .014 22.25**
3 BIS 1 Proactive Rel Agg .08* 2.40 .032 22.65***
Reactive Rel Agg .12*** 3.73
2 Romantic Rel Agg .17*** 5.49 .021 30.11***
4 CTQ 1 Proactive Rel Agg .04 1.25 .025 16.99***
Reactive Rel Agg .13*** 3.90
2 Romantic Rel Agg .10** 2.99 .006 8.91*
5 STAXI 1 Proactive Rel Agg .16*** 5.17 .15 120.48***
Reactive Rel Agg .27*** 8.91
2 Romantic Rel Agg .16*** 5.56 .019 30.96***
6 HAB 1 Proactive Rel Agg .04 1.39 .09 66.73***
Reactive Rel Agg .27*** 8.51
2 Romantic Rel Agg .07* 2.19 .003 4.81*
7 Feelings of Distress 1 Proactive Rel Agg .01 .33 .066 47.95***
Reactive Rel Agg .25*** 7.77
2 Romantic Rel Agg .15*** 4.93 .016 24.31***

Note. BPAQ = Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire; Agg = Aggression; Rel = Relational; BIS = Barratt Impulsivity Scale; CTQ = Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; STAXI = State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory; HAB = Hostile Attribution Biases.

*

p < .05;

**

p < .01;

***

p < .001

Gender Differences

To explore possible gender differences for the subtypes of relationally aggressive behavior, a 3 (Aggression Subtype: Proactive, Reactive and Romantic Relational Aggression) × 2 (gender) repeated measures analysis of variance (RM-ANOVA) was conducted with aggression scores serving as the repeated measure. A main effect for aggression subtype emerged, F(2, 1368) = 442.21, p < .001, η2 = .39. Follow-up contrasts indicated that romantic relational aggression (M = .59; SD = .48) was more prevalent than reactive relational aggression (M = .47; SD = .48), which was in turn more prevalent than proactive relational aggression (M = .26, SD = .38). As expected, there were no gender differences in overall levels of the relational aggression subtypes. However, a significant interaction between aggression subtype and gender emerged, F(2, 1368) = 48.41, p < .001, η2 = .07. Follow up analysis of variance (ANOVA) analyses examining gender differences in each subtype of aggression indicated that males were more proactively relationally aggressive (M = .31, SD = .42) than females (M = .22, SD = .35), F(1, 1383) = 19.03, p < .001, η2 = .014. In addition, males (M = .53, SD = .51) exhibited more reactive relational aggression than females (M = .43, SD = .45), F(1, 1380) = 14.91, p < .001, η2 = .011. However, females (M = .65, SD = .50) engaged in more romantic relational aggression than males (M = .52, SD = .44), F(1, 1373) = 21.90, p < .001, η2 = .016.

Study II: Patient-Control Study

Method

Participants

To assess the ability of the SRASBM to discriminate between participants with and without problems with aggression, 50 participants without any current or past history of Axis I or II Disorder, 50 participants with Axis I (n = 25) and/or Axis II (n = 25) disorders, and 50 participants meeting DSM-IV Criteria for Personality Disorder and for Research Criteria for Intermittent Explosive Disorder were studied. These participants were independent from, and unrelated to, those participants included in Study 1 described above. These participants were recruited from the community from Public Service Announcements highlighting studies of personality style including aggressiveness. Participants with a life history of Bipolar disorder, Schizophrenia (or other psychotic disorder), or mental retardation were excluded from this study. Demographic characteristics of the Study 2 sample are shown in Table 6.

Table 6.

Demographic, Functional, and Behavioral Data: IED/PD vs. Control Participants for Study 2

Variable IED/PD Participants (n = 50) Psych Control Participants (n = 50) Healthy Control Participants (n = 50) Statistic p
Demographic
 Age 38.2 ± 9.7 36.6 ± 12.7 36.9 ± 17.4 F(2,147) = 0.45 .636
 Gender (M/F) 26/24 22/28 29/21 χ2 = 1.98, df = 2 .373
 Race (White/Non-White) 30/20 33/17 34/16 χ2 = 0.76, df = 2 .684
 SES (I/II/III/IV/V) 4/27/7/6/6 5/26/10/3/6 6/34/4/2/4 χ2 = 7.15, df = 8 .521
Functional
 GAF 58.5 ± 7.3a 69.0 ± 10.8b 86.1 ± 4.7c F(2,147) = 151.4 <.001
Behavioral
 BPAQ Physical Aggression 26.8 ± 10.2a 18.5 ± 8.5b 16.8 ± 8.8b F(2,139) = 15.94 <.001

Note. Means in rows with different superscripts are significantly different from each other at p < .05.

Measures

Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV (SCID-I)

The SCID is a semi-structured clinical interview used to assign diagnoses for mood disorders, psychotic disorders, substance abuse and dependence, anxiety disorders, somatoform disorders, eating disorders, and adjustment disorders (First, Gibbons, & Williams, 1997). The purpose of administering the SCID was to diagnose DSM Axis I disorders. In this study, kappa values for the most frequent lifetime Axis I diagnoses were excellent (e.g., κ = .85 for Mood Disorder, κ = .79 for Anxiety Disorder; κ = .93 for Substance Dependence Disorder).

Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality (SID-P)

The SID-P was employed to diagnose DSM-IV personality disorders (Pfohl, Blum, & Zimmerman 1997). In this study, the inter-rater reliability for the diagnosis of personality disorder was excellent at κ = .83.

Intermittent Explosive Disorder Interview (IED Interview)

The IED interview (E.F. Coccaro, on request) is a semi-structured clinical interview that was used to diagnose IED by DSM-IV and by Research Criteria (IED-IR, Coccaro, Schmidt, Samuels & Nestadt, 2004). The IED interview enables the interviewer to obtain quantitative (e.g., frequency) and qualitative (e.g., description of most severe events) information for verbal aggression, aggression against property, and aggression against others, as well as aggression-related distress and psychosocial impairment and potential exclusionary information (i.e., aggressive acts occurring solely within the context of another Axis I disorder, substance use, or a medical condition). The Inter-rater reliability of the IED-IR with the IED-M in this study was excellent (κ = .82).

Buss Perry Aggression Questionnaire (BPAQ; Buss & Perry, 1992)

Participants also complete the BPAQ (described in Study 1). The physical aggression subscale of the BPAQ includes 9 items (e.g., “Given enough provocation, I may hit another person”) scored on a five-point scale ranging from 1 (“extremely uncharacteristic of me”) to 5 (“extremely characteristic of me”). Items for each subscale were summed to yield a total physical aggression score.

Procedure

All participants completed the 14 items of the SRASBM validated in Study 1 (as described above) and the BPAQ. Participants then completed a structured clinical interview including the SCID, SID-P, and IED assessment. Fifty-nine participants underwent the SRASBM twice to assess test-retest reliability in this patient-control sample. Axis I and Axis II Personality Disorder (PD) diagnoses were made according to DSM-IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). The diagnosis of Intermittent Explosive Disorder (IED) was made by using Integrated Research Criteria (IED-IR; Coccaro, et al., 2004) which differ from DSM-IV criteria in that they require: a) one month (or more) period of aggressive outbursts (including verbal outbursts only, or outbursts in which property is not destroyed) occurring twice a week on average, or at least three episodes of serious assaultive or destructive behavior in one year (even when there are not recurrent aggressive outbursts within the one-month time frame); b) aggressive outbursts to be, primarily, impulsive in nature, c) aggressive outbursts to be associated with significant subjective distress or psychosocial impairment. Final diagnoses were assigned by team best-estimate consensus procedures involving at least two research psychiatrists and two clinical psychologists as previously described (Klein, Ouimette, Kelly Ferro, & Riso, 1994). This methodology has previously been shown to enhance the accuracy of diagnosis over direct interview alone (Leckman, Sholomskas, Thompson, Belanger, & Weissman, 1982; Kosten & Rounsaville 1992).

Results

Sample Characteristics

Axis I and Axis II disorder diagnoses for the fifty IED+ and fifty psychiatric control participants are presented in Table 7. Among the IED+ patients, all had a current and/or lifetime history of an Axis I and Axis II disorder. The distribution of Axis I or II diagnoses between the IED+ and Psychiatric Control groups did not differ except the IED patients exhibited heightened incidence of IED (by definition; Fisher Exact p < .001 corrected for multiple comparisons), Paranoid PD (Fisher Exact p = .006 corrected), Antisocial PD (Exact p = .013, corrected), and Borderline PD (Exact p = .02). Analysis of variance analyses indicated that participants diagnosed with Paranoid PD [M = 1.26, SD = .88 versus M = .64, SD = .63, F(1, 147) = 6.17, p < .05, η2 = .04], Antisocial PD [M = 1.28, SD = .60 versus M = .63, SD = .64, F(1, 147) = 8.83, p < .01, η2 = .06], and Borderline PD [M = 1.11, SD = .46 versus M = .63, SD = .65, F(1, 147) = 6.14, p < .05, η2 = .04] exhibited heightened levels of total relational aggression when compared to their peers. By definition, the fifty Healthy Control participants had no current or past history of any Axis I or Axis II disorder.

Table 7.

Number (and percentage) of IED/PD and Psychiatric Control Participants with Axis I and Axis II Diagnoses for Study 2

Axis I Disorders
IED/PD Participants (n = 50) Psych Control Participants (n = 25)

Current Diagnosis Lifetime Diagnosis Current Diagnosis Lifetime Diagnosis

Mood Disorders
 Major Depression 9 (18%) 21 (42%) 4 (16%) 14 (56%)
 Dysthymia 6 (12%) 7 (14%) 3 (12%) 4 (16%)
 Depressive Disorder-NOS 2 (4%) 4 (8%) 1 (4%) 4 (16%)
 Any Mood Disorder 13 (26%) 28 (56%) 7 (28%) 17 (68%)
Anxiety Disorders
 Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder 4 (8%) 6 (12%) 0 (0%) 2 (8%)
 Specific Phobia 4 (8%) 5 (10%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
 Panic Disorder 3 (6%) 4 (8%) 1 (4%) 1 (4%)
 Generalized Anxiety Disorder 3 (6%) 3 (6%) 1 (4%) 1 (4%)
 Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder 2 (4%) 2 (4%) 0 (0%) 1 (4%)
 Agoraphobia 2 (4%) 2 (4%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
 Social Phobia 1 (2%) 1 (2%) 2 (8%) 2 (8%)
 Anxiety Disorder-NOS 1 (2%) 3 (6%) 3 (12%) 3 (12%)
 Any Anxiety Disorder 15 (30%) 19 (38%) 5 (20%) 8 (32%)
Impulse Control Disorders
 IED by Research Criteria 37 (74%) 50 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
 Pathological Gambling 1 (2%) 1 (2%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
 Trichotillomania 1 (2%) 1 (2%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
Eating Disorders 3 (6%) 7 (14%) 0 (0%) 4 (16%)
Adjustment Disorder 0 (0%) 3 (6%) 0 (0%) 2 (8%)
Somatoform Disorders 0 (0%) 1 (2%) 1 (4%) 1 (4%)
Substance Use Disorders
 Alcohol Dependence 0 (0%) 11 (22%) 0 (0%) 13 (52%)
 Drug Dependence 0 (0%) 7 (14%) 0 (0%) 4 (16%)

Axis II Disorders
IED/PD Participants (n = 50) Psych Control Participants (n = 25)

Paranoid 11 (22%) 0 (0%)
Schizoid 0 (0%) 1 (4%)
Borderline 12 (24%) 1 (4%)
Antisocial 10 (20%) 0 (0%)
Narcissistic 11 (22%) 5 (20%)
Histrionic 2(4%) 0 (0%)
Obsessive-Complusive 11 (22%) 5 (20%)
Avoidant 5 (10%) 3 (12%)
Dependent 1 (2%) 1 (4%)
PD-NOS 17 (34%) 12 (48%)

Note. A number of participants had more than one diagnosis; thus, participants could be classified in more than one diagnostic category.

Basic Psychometrics Properties of the SRASBM in Patient and Control Participants

Alpha coefficients for Total Relational Aggression, Proactive Relational Aggression, Reactive Relational Aggression, and Romantic Relational Aggression in the patient-control sample (N = 150) were .91, .80, .83, and .78, respectively. Alpha coefficients were similar in the patients and controls examined separately. Test-retest data on these variables was obtained on 59 of 75 participants selected at random for administration of the SRASBM a second time separated by about nine months (mean 9.1 ± 0.9 months, range = 6–11 months). Pearson correlations for the SRASBM scales at retest were as follows: r = .92 for Total Relational Aggression, .91 for Proactive Relational Aggression, .84 for Reactive Relational Aggression, and .88 for Romantic Relational Aggression (each p < .001).

Patient-Control Differences

IED participants demonstrated clear differences from the two control groups with all relational aggression variables. A three (Group: IED, Psychiatric Controls, Healthy Controls) by two (Gender) ANOVA with Total Relational Aggression serving as the dependent variable revealed a significant effect for Group [F(2,144) = 20.70, p < .001, partial η2 = .22], but not for Gender [F(1,144) = 0.00, p = .98] or Group × Gender [F(1,144) = 0.39, p = .68]. Post-hoc analyses (Tukey HSD) revealed that IED participants had significantly (p < .001) higher Total Relational Aggression scores than either Psychiatric or Healthy controls whose Relational Aggression scores, in turn, were not different from each other. This result was not altered when GAF scores (which differed significantly among the groups; see Table 6) were added to the model. The same results were found when the SRASBM subscales were examined (Figure 1). In addition, since Paranoid PD, Antisocial PD, and Borderline PD were associated with IED status and with relational aggression scores, analyses were re-run controlling for these diagnoses; all significant effects were replicated. Finally, the analyses were re-run controlling for BPAQ Physical Aggression scores. All significant effects were replicated with this additional control.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Relational aggression in adults with IED, healthy controls, and psychiatric controls for Study 2.

*p < .001

Discussion

The first goal of the present study was to examine the psychometric properties of a self-report measure of relational aggression in adulthood. We replicated previous work suggesting adequate internal consistency of the SRASBM (e.g., Bailey & Ostrov, 2008; Linder et al, 2002). The test-retest reliability of the measure was also confirmed in two short-term longitudinal studies. In addition, this is the first study to provide support for the internal structure of the SRASBM. That is, factor analysis suggested the presence of the three hypothesized factors (i.e., proactive, reactive, and romantic relational aggression). The inter-correlations between the factors indicated moderate to large levels of association between the variables, which is consistent with past studies investigating proactive and reactive physical aggression (e.g., Dodge & Coie, 1987; Fite et al., 2006).

As expected, only reactive relational aggression was uniquely associated with history of abuse, hostile attribution biases, and feelings of distress for relational provocation situations. In addition, although both proactive and reactive relational aggression were uniquely associated with hostility and anger, reactive relational aggression was more strongly associated with these outcomes than proactive relational aggression. This research replicates and extends past findings for physical aggression with younger samples. For example, the finding that only reactive relational aggression was associated with a history of abuse mirrors those found with reactive physical aggression in past studies with younger samples (Dodge et al., 1997). Overall, then, these results are consistent with previous research demonstrating the distinct correlates of proactive and reactive physical and relational aggression in samples of children (e.g., Crick & Dodge, 1996) and young adults (e.g., Bailey & Ostrov, 2008). However, this study is the first to provide empirical support for the distinction between proactive, reactive, and romantic relational aggression in an adult sample. In particular, the results of the CFA showed that the three-factor model (separate latent factors for proactive, reactive, and romantic relational aggression) fit the data quite well. Moreover, when compared to competing models in which the factors were collapsed in various ways, the three-factor model provided the best fit for the data.

The results also suggest that aggression researchers and clinicians should attend to the context of relationally aggressive behavior among adults. Specifically, consistent with the importance of romantic relationships in late adolescence and adulthood, romantic relational aggression was uniquely associated with anger, hostility, impulsivity, abuse, hostile attribution biases, and feelings of distress regarding relational provocation, even when controlling for peer-directed relational aggression. Although previous studies have assessed romantic relational aggression (e.g., Linder et al., 2002), this is the first study to demonstrate that relational aggression in this context predicts important outcomes above and beyond peer-directed relational aggression. As such, a complete understanding of relational aggression in adults requires inclusion of relational aggression in the context of both peer and romantic relationships.

The second goal was to examine gender differences in relational aggression in adulthood. Consistent with hypotheses, men and women did not differ in their overall use of relational aggression. These findings are consistent with previous research with young adults (Bailey & Ostrov, 2008; Basow et al., 2007; Burton et al., 2007; Loudin, Loukis, & Robinson, 2003) and suggest that as males and females increasingly interact in adolescence and adulthood, males may learn how to effectively employ relationally aggressive strategies from female friends and romantic partners. Interestingly, gender differences in engagement in subtypes of relational aggression were observed. Specifically, females were more likely than males to engage in romantic relational aggression whereas males were more likely than females to engage in peer-directed proactive and reactive relational aggression. Gender differences in specific subtypes of relational aggression may help explain some inconsistent findings regarding gender differences in relational aggression among adults (e.g., Storch et al., 2004). These findings also suggest that as females get older, they may transfer relationally aggressive behaviors learned in the peer context to interactions with romantic partners. Moreover, although men are as likely as women to use relational aggression, they are more likely than women to engage in such behaviors in the peer context. It is possible that males learn and use relational aggression when interacting with female peers. Further research with more diverse adult samples is warranted to replicate these findings and to explore potential reasons for gender differences in subtypes of relational aggression.

The findings can be interpreted in light of a recent study which provides additional detail regarding the nature of female-to-male aggression (Nelson, Springer, Nelson, & Bean, 2008). In this study, emerging adults’ normative perceptions of aggression were assessed across dyads in which the perpetrator and target varied by gender (male-to-male, female-to-female, male-to-female, female-to-male). Although romantic relational aggression was not expressly considered (participants simply were asked to list what women generally do when they want to be hurtful to men), the findings nonetheless show the prominent place of relational aggression in the actions of females against males. Moreover, much of this aggression is expected to occur in romantic relationships. Emerging adults perceive that females are particularly likely to engage in variants of relational aggression when the target is male. In contrast, male-to-female aggression was predominantly characterized by verbal aggression, according to the perceptions of respondents. The results of the present study with adults ranging in age from 25 to 45 are thus consistent with young adults’ perceptions regarding the typical interpersonal contexts of relational aggression.

Finally, our results from the second study indicated that adults with IED exhibited heightened levels of relational aggression when compared to healthy controls and psychiatric controls. These results held even when controlling for general functioning and physical aggression. The findings suggest that relational aggression provides important information regarding clinical symptoms during adulthood. In fact, clinicians may benefit from including assessments of relational aggression when gathering information about participants’ aggressive conduct since relational aggression was associated with IED even when controlling for physically aggressive conduct. In other words, assessment of relational aggression may provide clinicians with a more comprehensive picture of patients’ aggressive problems than focusing exclusively on physical aggression. We hope that this paper provides a first step in the development of a reliable and valid method for clinicians to assess relational aggression.

Despite the large sample and novel nature of the study, there were several limitations which should be addressed. First, our findings may not generalize to samples that are non-twin (for Study 1) or from other cultural, economic, or ethnic backgrounds (Study 1 and Study 2). For example, it is possible that the unique experiences of being a twin may affect engagement in relational aggression. For instance, evidence suggests that twins experience higher levels of intimacy with their co-twin relative to their closest friends (e.g., Foy, Vernon, & Yang, 2001), and high levels of intimacy have been related to involvement in relational aggression (Murray-Close, Ostrov, & Crick, 2007). As a result, twins may be at especially high risk for engagement in relational aggression. However, since the focus of this study is on the psychometric properties of the measure of relational aggression and the association between relational aggression and outcome measures (rather than on mean levels of relational aggression), we believe that the findings will generalize to non-twin populations as well. In particular, although twins might exhibit higher levels of relational aggression than their peers, we would expect that the unique correlates of proactive and reactive relational aggression would be similar across samples.

Future research should also focus on the development of appropriate measures of relational aggression among groups differing in socioeconomic status and ethnicity (see Crick et al., 2007 for a discussion of age, gender, and cultural considerations in the study of relational aggression). Although some research has documented the presence of physical and relational forms of aggression among children in a variety of cultures (e.g., French, Jansen, & Pidada, 2002; Hart et al., 1998; Russell, Hart, Robinson, and Olsen, 2003; Tomada & Schneider, 1997), it is possible that the correlates of involvement in relational aggression may differ across cultural contexts; for example, relational aggression may be associated with greater levels of maladjustment in cultures such as Japan that place a relative emphasis on harmony in interpersonal relationships (see Crick et al., 2007). In addition, it is important that future researchers develop and validate measures of relational aggression using culturally-sensitive approaches (Crick et al., 2007).

An additional limitation is that only self-report methods were used in the present study and some of the associations may be due to shared-method variance. All methods that were used here have demonstrated reliability and validity and have been widely used in the psychology and psychiatry literatures. However, future research should replicate the findings using additional methods of assessment. The present analyses were all concurrent and longitudinal designs are needed to demonstrate the development of subtypes of relational aggression across the lifespan. In addition, the subscale assessing romantic relational aggression included items assessing proactive and reactive functions of aggression. Future research would benefit from the development of a measure of separate proactive and reactive subtypes of romantic relational aggression so that the unique correlates of each can be assessed. Finally, future research would benefit from further examination of the association between clinical problems and relational aggression among adults. For example, research assessing the association between relational aggression and adjustment, controlling for clinical disorders such as IED, would be very important. In addition, studies exploring the role of relational aggression in the context of clinical disorders such as bipolar disorder are sorely needed.

In conclusion, this study demonstrated the utility of a recently introduced measure of proactive, reactive, and romantic relational aggression in a large adult sample. Overall, this instrument demonstrated good reliability and validity. The present findings suggest that we should not eliminate the proactive and reactive function types from our investigations of aggressive behavior problems in adulthood. Moreover, researchers and clinicians interested in relational aggression in adulthood should examine such behaviors in the context of both peer and romantic relationships. Future research should examine the development of gender differences in subtypes of relational aggression in adult samples. Finally, research should further explore the association between clinical problems and relational aggression. Given the ease of use of this self-report measure, we hope that future researchers will include assessments of these subtypes of relational aggression to better capture a wider array of adult behavior problems.

Acknowledgments

Special thanks to study participants. This study was funded in part by NIMH Grants RO1MH63262 AND RO1MH80109 (Dr. Coccaro).

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV. 4. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bagner DM, Storch EA, Preston AS. Romantic relational aggression: What about gender? Journal of Family Violence. 2007;22:19–24. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bailey CA, Ostrov JM. Differentiating forms and functions of aggression in emerging adults: Associations with hostile attribution biases and normative beliefs. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2008;37:713–722. [Google Scholar]
  4. Basow SA, Cahill KF, Phelan JE, Longshore K, McGillicuddy-DeLisi A. Perceptions of relational and physical aggression among college students: Effects of gender of perpetrator, target, and perceiver. Social Behavior and Personality. 2007;31:85–95. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bernstein DP, Fink L. Childhood Trauma Questionnaire: A retrospective self-report manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bernstein DP, Fink L, Handelsman L, Foote J, Lovejoy M, Wenzel K, et al. Initial reliability and validity of a new retrospective measure of child abuse and neglect. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1994;151:1132–1136. doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.8.1132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bjorkqvist K. Sex differences in physical, verbal, and indirect aggression: A review of recent research. Sex Roles. 1994;30:177–187. [Google Scholar]
  8. Burton LA, Hafetz J, Henninger D. Gender differences in relational and physical aggression. Social Behavior and Personality. 2007;35:41–50. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bushman BJ, Anderson CA. Is it time to pull the plug on the hostile versus instrumental aggression dichotomy? Psychology Review. 2001;108:273–279. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.1.273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Buss AH, Perry M. The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1992;63:452–459. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.63.3.452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Card NA, Little TD. Proactive and reactive aggression in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analysis of differential relations with psychosocial adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2006;30:466–480. [Google Scholar]
  12. Coccaro EF, Berman ME, Kavoussi RJ. Assessment of life history of aggression: development and psychometric characteristics. Psychiatry Research. 1997;73:147–157. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(97)00119-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Coccaro EF, Jacobson KC. PennTwins: A population based cohort for twin studies. Twin Research. 2006;9:998–1005. doi: 10.1375/183242706779462633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Coccaro EF, Noblett KL, McCloskey MS. Attributional and Emotional Responses to Socially Ambiguous Cues: Validation of a New Assessment of Social/Emotional Information Processing in Healthy Adults and Impulsive Aggressive Patients. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2009;43:915–925. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Coccaro EF, Schmidt CS, Samuels J, Nestadt G. Lifetime and one-month prevalence rates of Intermittent Explosive Disorder in a community sample. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2004;65:820–824. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v65n0613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cohen J, Cohen P. Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  17. Crick NR. Relational aggression: The role of intent attributions, provocation type, and feelings of distress. Development and Psychopathology. 1995;7:313–322. [Google Scholar]
  18. Crick NR, Dodge KA. A review and reformulation of social information- processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;115:74–101. [Google Scholar]
  19. Crick NR, Dodge KA. Social information-processing mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child Development. 1996;67:993–1002. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Crick NR, Grotpeter JK. Relational aggression, gender, and social-psychological adjustment. Child Development. 1995;66:710–722. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00900.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Crick NR, Grotpeter JK, Bigbee MA. Relationally and physically children’s intent attributions and feelings of distress for relational and instrumental peer conflicts. Child Development. 2002;73:1134–1142. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Crick NR, Ostrov JM, Kawabata Y. Relational aggression and gender: An overview. In: Flannery DJ, Vazsonyi AT, Waldman ID, editors. The Cambridge handbook of violent behavior and aggression. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2007. pp. 245–259. [Google Scholar]
  23. Crick NR, Werner NE, Casas JF, O’Brien KM, Nelson DA, Grotpeter JK, Markon K. Childhood aggression and gender: A new look at an old problem. In: Bernstein D, editor. Nebraska Symposium On Motivation. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press; 1999. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Day DM, Bream LA, Pal A. Proactive and reactive aggression: An analysis of subtypes based on teacher perceptions. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1992;21:210–217. [Google Scholar]
  25. Dillman DA. Mail and Internet surveys: The tailored design method, 2nd ed. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  26. Dodge KA. The structure and function of reactive and proactive aggression. In: Pepler DJ, Rubin KH, editors. The Development and Treatment of Childhood Aggression. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  27. Dodge KA, Coie JD. Social information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children’s playgroups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1987;53:1146–1158. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.53.6.1146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Dodge KA, Coie JD, Lynam D. Aggression and antisocial behavior in youth. Handbook of Child Psychology. In: Damon W, Eisenberg N, editors. Social, Emotional, and Personality Development. 6. Vol. 3. New York: Wiley; 2006. pp. 719–788. [Google Scholar]
  29. Dodge KA, Lochman JE, Harnish JD, Bates JE, Pettit GS. Reactive and proactive aggression in school children and psychiatrically impaired chronically assaultive youth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997;53:1146–1158. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.106.1.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Bates JE, Valente E. Social information-processing patterns partially mediate the effect of early physical abuse on later conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1995;104:632–643. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.104.4.632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. First MB, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID) New York: Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  32. Fite PJ, Colder CR, Pelham WE. A factor analytic approach to distinguish pure and co-occurring dimensions of proactive and reactive aggression. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2006;35:578–582. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3504_9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Fite PJ, Stauffacher K, Ostrov JM, Colder CR. Replication and extension of Little et al.’s (2003) forms and functions of aggression measure. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2008;32:238–242. [Google Scholar]
  34. Fitzgerald P, Asher SR. Aggressive-rejected children’s attributional biases about liked and disliked peers. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association; New York. 1987. Aug, [Google Scholar]
  35. Foy AK, Vernon PA, Jang K. Examining the dimensions of intimacy in twin and peer relationships. Twin Research. 2001;4:443–452. doi: 10.1375/1369052012777. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. French DC, Jansen EA, Pidada S. United States and Indonesian children’s and adolescents’ reports of relational aggression by disliked peers. Child Development. 2002;73:1143–1150. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Furman W, Buhrmester D. Children’s perceptions of the personal relationships in their social networks. Developmental Psychology. 1985;21:1016–1024. [Google Scholar]
  38. Goldstein SE, Chesir-Teran D, McFaul A. Profiles and correlates of relational aggression in young adults’ romantic relationships. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2008;37:251–265. [Google Scholar]
  39. Grotpeter JK, Crick NR. Relational aggression, overt aggression, and friendship. Child Development. 1996;67:2328–2338. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hart CH, Nelson DA, Robinson CC, Olsen SF, McNeilly-Choque MK. Overt and relational aggression in Russian nursery-school-age children: Parenting style and marital linkages. Developmental Psychology. 1998;34:687–697. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.34.4.687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hubbard JA, Dodge KA, Cillessen AHN, Coie JD, Schwartz D. The dyadic nature of social information processing in boys’ reactive and proactive aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;80:268–80. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hubbard JA, Parker EH, Ramsden SR, Flanagan KD, Relyea N, Dearing KF, Smithmyer CM, Simons RF, Hyde CT. The relations among observational, physiological, and self-report measures of children’s anger. Social Development. 2004;13:14–39. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Hubbard JA, Smithmyer CM, Ramsden SR, Parker EH, Flanagan KD, Dearing KF, Relyea N, Simons RF. Observational, physiological, and self-report measures of children’s anger: Relations to reactive versus proactive aggression. Child Development. 2002;73:1101–1118. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Keenan K, Coyne C, Lahey BB. Should relational aggression be included in DSM-V? Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2008;47:86–93. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e31815a56b8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Klein DN, Ouimette PC, Kelly HS, Ferro T, Riso LP. Test-retest reliability of team consensus best-estimate diagnoses of axis I and II disorders in a family study. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1994;151:1043–1047. doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.7.1043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kosten TA, Rounsaville BJ. Sensitivity of psychiatric diagnosis based on the best estimate procedure. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1992;149:1225–1227. doi: 10.1176/ajp.149.9.1225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Leckman JF, Sholomskas D, Thompson WD, Belanger A, Weissman MM. Best estimate of lifetime psychiatric diagnosis: A methodological study. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1982;39:879–883. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1982.04290080001001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lento-Zwolinski J. College students’ self-report of psychosocial factors in reactive forms of relational and physical aggression. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2007;24:407–421. [Google Scholar]
  49. Linder JR, Crick NR, Collins WA. Relational aggression and victimization in young adults’ romantic relationships: Associations with perceptions of parent, peer, and romantic relationship quality. Social Development. 2002;11:69–86. [Google Scholar]
  50. Little TD, Jones SM, Henrich CC, Hawley PH. Disentangling the “whys” from the “whats” of aggressive behavior. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2003;27:122–133. [Google Scholar]
  51. Little TD, Brauner J, Jones SM, Nock MK, Hawley PH. Rethinking aggression: A typological examination of the functions of aggression. Merrill Palmer Quarterly. 2003b;49:343–372. [Google Scholar]
  52. Loudin JL, Loukas A, Robinson S. Relational aggression in college students: Examining the roles of social anxiety and empathy. Aggressive Behavior. 2003;29:430–439. [Google Scholar]
  53. Maccoby EE. Gender and relationships: A developmental account. American Psychologist. 1990;45:513–520. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.45.4.513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Miller JD, Lynam DR. Psychopathy and the five-factor model of personality: A replication and extension. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2003;81:168–178. doi: 10.1207/S15327752JPA8102_08. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Morales JR, Crick NR. Self-report measure of aggression and victimization. University of Minnesota; Twin Cities Campus: 1998. Unpublished measure. [Google Scholar]
  56. Murray-Close D, Ostrov JM, Crick NR. A short-term longitudinal study of growth of relational aggression during middle childhood: Associations with gender, friendship intimacy, and internalizing problems. Development & Psychopathology. 2007;19:187–203. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407070101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Nelson DA, Mitchell C, Yang C. Intent attributions and aggression: A study of children and their parents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36:793–806. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9211-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Nelson DA, Springer MM, Nelson LJ, Bean NH. Normative beliefs regarding aggression in emerging adulthood. Social Development. 2008;17:638–660. [Google Scholar]
  59. Ohan JL, Johnston C. Gender appropriateness of symptom criteria for attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder, oppositional-defiant disorder, and conduct disorder. Child Psychiatry & Human Development. 2005;35:359–381. doi: 10.1007/s10578-005-2694-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Orobio de Castro B, Merk W, Koops W, Veerman JW, Bosch JD. Emotions in social information processing and their relations with reactive and proactive aggression in referred aggressive boys. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2005;34:105–116. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3401_10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Orobio de Castro BO, Veerman JW, Koops W, Bosch JD, Monshouwer HJ. Hostile attribution of intent and aggressive behavior: A meta-analysis. Child Development. 2002;73:916–934. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Ostrov JM, Crick NR. Forms and functions of aggression during early childhood: A short-term longitudinal study. School Psychology Review. 2007;36:22–43. [Google Scholar]
  63. Ostrov JM, Houston RJ. The utility of forms and functions of aggression in emerging adulthood: Association with personality disorder symptomatology. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2008;37:1147–1158. [Google Scholar]
  64. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1995;51:768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Pfohl B, Blum N, Zimmerman M. Structured interview for DSM-IV personality: SIDP-IV. Washington D.C.: American Psychiatric Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  66. Poulin F, Boivin M. Reactive and proactive aggression: Evidence of a two- factor model. Psychological Assessment. 2000;12:115–122. doi: 10.1037//1040-3590.12.2.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Price JM, Dodge KA. Reactive and proactive aggression in childhood: Relations to peer status and social context dimensions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1989;17:455–71. doi: 10.1007/BF00915038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Prinstein MJ, Cillessen AHN. Forms and functions of adolescent peer aggression associated with high levels of peer status. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 2003;49:310–342. [Google Scholar]
  69. Pulkkinen L. Proactive and reactive aggression in early adolescence as precursors to anti- and prosocial behaviors in young adults. Aggressive Behavior. 1996;22:241–257. [Google Scholar]
  70. Raine A, Dodge K, Loeber R, Gatzke-Kopp L, Lynam D, Reynolds C, Stouthamer-Loeber M, Liu J. The reactive-proactive aggression questionnaire: Differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent boys. Aggressive Behavior. 2006;32:159–171. doi: 10.1002/ab.20115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Russell A, Hart CH, Robinson C, Olsen SF. Children’s sociable and aggressive behavior with peers: A comparison of the U.S. and Australia, and contributions of temperament and parenting styles. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2003;27:74–86. [Google Scholar]
  72. Salmivalli C, Nieminen E. Proactive and reactive aggression among school bullies, victims, and bully-victims. Aggressive Behavior. 2002;28:30–44. [Google Scholar]
  73. Schad MM, Szwedo DE, Antonishak J, Hare A, Allen JP. The broader context of relational aggression in adolescent romantic relationships: Predictions from peer pressure and links to psychosocial functioning. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2008;37:346–358. doi: 10.1007/s10964-007-9226-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Schwartz D, Dodge KA, Coie JD, Hubbard JA, Cillessen AHN, Lemerise EA, Bateman H. Social-cognitive and behavioral correlates of aggression and victimization in boys’ play groups. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1998;26:431–440. doi: 10.1023/a:1022695601088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Spielberger CD. State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  76. Standford MS, Houston RJ, Mathias CW, Villemarette-Pittman NR, Helfritz LE, Conklin SM. Characterizing aggressive behavior. Assessment. 2003;10:183–190. doi: 10.1177/1073191103010002009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Storch EA, Bagner DM, Geffken GR, Baumeister AL. Association Between Overt and Relational Aggression and Psychosocial Adjustment in Undergraduate College Students. Violence & Victims. 2004;19:689–700. doi: 10.1891/vivi.19.6.689.66342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Tomada G, Schneider BH. Relational aggression, gender, and peer acceptance: Invariance across culture, stability over time, and concordance among informants. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33:601–609. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.33.4.601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Vitaro F, Gendreau PL, Tremblay RE, Oligny P. Reactive and proactive aggression differentially predict later conduct problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1998;39:377–385. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Waschbusch DA, Pehlam WE, Jr, Jennings JR, Greiner AR, Tarter RE, Moss HB. Reactive aggression in boys with disruptive behavior disorders: Behavior, physiology, and affect. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2002;30:641–656. doi: 10.1023/a:1020867831811. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Waschbusch DA, Willoughby MT, Pelham WE. Criterion validity and the utility of reactive and proactive aggression: Comparisons to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, and other measures of functioning. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1998;27:396–405. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2704_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Werner NE, Crick NR. Relational aggression and social-psychological adjustment in a college sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1999;108:615–623. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.108.4.615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Zalecki CA, Hinshaw SP. Overt and relational aggression in girls with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2004;33:125–137. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES