Skip to main content
Upsala Journal of Medical Sciences logoLink to Upsala Journal of Medical Sciences
. 2010 Mar 10;115(1):2–10. doi: 10.3109/03009730903573246

Serotonin, genetic variability, behaviour, and psychiatric disorders - a review

Niklas Nordquist 1,, Lars Oreland 1
PMCID: PMC2853349  PMID: 20187845

Abstract

Brain monoamines, and serotonin in particular, have repeatedly been shown to be linked to different psychiatric conditions such as depression, anxiety, antisocial behaviour, and dependence. Many studies have implicated genetic variability in the genes encoding monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) and the serotonin transporter (5HTT) in modulating susceptibility to these conditions. Paradoxically, the risk variants of these genes have been shown, in vitro, to increase levels of serotonin, although many of the conditions are associated with decreased levels of serotonin. Furthermore, in adult humans, and monkeys with orthologous genetic polymorphisms, there is no observable correlation between these functional genetic variants and the amount or activity of the corresponding proteins in the brain. These seemingly contradictory data might be explained if the association between serotonin and these behavioural and psychiatric conditions were mainly a consequence of events taking place during foetal and neonatal brain development. In this review we explore, based on recent research, the hypothesis that the dual role of serotonin as a neurotransmitter and a neurotrophic factor has a significant impact on behaviour and risk for neuropsychiatric disorders through altered development of limbic neurocircuitry involved in emotional processing, and development of the serotonergic neurons, during early brain development.

Keywords: Behaviour, development, genetic polymorphism, limbic system, MAOA, psychiatric disorder, serotonin, sexual dichotomy, SLC6A4

Genetic variability affecting serotonin levels

Two of the most studied molecules involved in regulating levels of serotonin in the brain are the serotonin transporter (5HTT, also known as SLC6A4), which transports serotonin from the extracellular space, and monoamine oxidase A (MAOA), the key enzyme responsible for degrading serotonin. Both genes encoding these proteins harbour genetic polymorphisms in their promoter regions that have been shown, in vitro, to affect their transcriptional activity (18). These genetic variants have been studied for their possible involvement in a number of psychiatric conditions and behavioural traits. The serotonin transporter polymorphism (5HTTLPR) and the monoamine oxidase A polymorphism (MAOA-LPR) have been implicated in conditions and behaviours such as depression, anxiety, aggression, alcoholism, autism, suicidality, and impulsiveness (915). The relation between these traits and the genetic variability in these genes has not always been consistent, and has displayed both interactions with environmental factors, and shown different effects in males and females.

The 5HTTLPR consists of a complex structured length polymorphism region (9,16). The allelic variants are usually referred to as S (short, low-expressing variant) and L (long, high-expressing variant). The L-allele has further been shown to encompass a single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP rs25531) that affects a putative transcription factor binding site and renders the LG-variant functionally equivalent to the S-allele (7,17). Interestingly, the rhesus macaque (Macaca mulatta) has been shown to contain an orthologous polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene that has also been shown to have a functional effect on transcriptional activity (18).

The MAOA-LPR also constitutes a length polymorphism region upstream of the coding region (2). There are six allelic variants characterized in this locus, representing 2, 3, 3.5, 4, 5, or 6 copies of a repeat structure. Functional studies of their relative effect on transcriptional activity in vitro have unanimously shown that for the two most common variants, the 3-repeat allele displays lower transcriptional activity than the 4-repeat allele (2,3,8). The 2-repeat allele has been reported to have lower transcriptional activity than both the 3- and 4-repeat alleles (8), whereas the 3.5-repeat allele has been reported to be equal to the 4-repeat allele (23). The 5-repeat allele has given conflicting results (23), whereas the 6-repeat allele has not been functionally characterized. Most studies on this MAOA polymorphism have been focusing on the 3- and 4-repeat alleles since the other alleles are rare. Also for this polymorphism there is an orthologous equivalent in rhesus macaques with functionality shown in an in vitro assay (6).

Despite overwhelming amounts of data supporting a robust association between these polymorphisms and shaping of behavioural traits and vulnerability to develop psychiatric disorders, the molecular and biological events leading to the observed associations are still not fully understood. A consensus finding from association studies seems to hold that the low-expressing variants of these genes are risk factors for vulnerability to develop psychiatric disorders, conditions associated with reduced serotonergic activity, despite the fact that the low-expressing variants of 5HTTLPR and MAOA-LPR should intuitively promote increased serotonergic activity by keeping extracellular levels of serotonin high. This contradictory relation is further discussed below. Another seemingly paradoxical relation, or rather lack of relation, is based on results from in vivo studies on both humans and monkeys showing that the genotype in these loci does not correspond to levels of the corresponding proteins in the brain of adult individuals, as determined by positron emission tomography (PET), for either 5HTT (1923), or MAOA (2426), or from measurements in post-mortem brain tissues (27,28). There is a possibility, though, that an association actually exists between these polymorphisms and the abundance of the respective proteins, which might be concealed by variability in the number of expressing neurons, two measures that have not yet been simultaneously assessed. Furthermore, results from several genetic association studies suggest that these polymorphisms act differently, in some cases even in opposite directions, between the sexes (further discussed below).

Dual functions for serotonin

Serotonin acts as a neurotransmitter released from serotonergic neurons, which emanate from cell bodies concentrated in the raphe nuclei in the brain-stem. Serotonergic projections can be traced to literally all parts of the brain. The neurocircuits making up the limbic system, involved in regulation of mood and behaviour, are particularly rich in serotonergic projections, where serotonin has been postulated to act as an inhibitor on the activity of these neuronal circuits (29). During human embryonic development, serotonin is one of the first neurotransmitters to appear, with detectable CNS neurons by 5 weeks of gestation (30). An important role for serotonin also as a trophic factor during development has been described in neural crest cells (31), heart (32), and in CNS development, which is further discussed below. Different strategies have been used to manipulate the effects of serotonin during development to understand how and where it exerts its effects. From these experiments it has become evident that regulation of serotonin homeostasis is critical for normal development of the central nervous system, where dysregulation has been linked to anatomical, functional, and behavioural anomalies.

Serotonin excess during brain development

One of the first studies demonstrating the importance of MAOA, and consequently serotonin, in influencing human behaviour was based on an observation in a Dutch family with familial aggregation of impulsive aggression. The condition was shown to be caused by a nonsense mutation in the MAOA gene, located on the X chromosome, which caused male family members to become devoid of the enzyme (33). Both MAOA and 5HTT knock-out mouse models display elevated levels of brain serotonin during development and clearly show that this condition is associated with behavioural changes. The most prominent behavioural effect in MAOA-deficient mice is an increase in aggressiveness and impulsiveness, possibly combined with a reduction in stress response (3436). In 5HTT knock-out mouse and rat models, the behavioural outcome is rather different from the MAOA knock-out in that it induces higher anxiety and inhibitory control, combined with reduced aggressiveness (37,38). Despite the usefulness of these models, limitations in terms of specific spatio-temporal patterns of action for the target protein have to be taken into consideration when interpreting the effects resulting from complete absence of a specific protein in a whole organism throughout all developmental stages. Using pharmaceutical inhibitors against 5HTT has been shown to cause analogous depressive-like and anxious behavioural outcomes as in 5HTT knock-out mice, treating animals either prenatally (E8–18) (39) or postnatally (P4–21) (40), which corresponds approximately to the first and the third trimester, respectively, during human foetal development (41). Inhibition of 5HTT at later time points has no observable effect on behaviour (40). Also pharmacological inhibition of MAOA during foetal development has been shown to mimic the effect seen on aggressive behaviour in MAOA knock-out mice (42). These results indicate that consequences of excessive levels of serotonin in the brain on specific behaviours later in life are, at least to some extent, determined during development.

Serotonin deficiency during brain development

At the other end of the spectrum, clues to the involvement of serotonin in brain development and behaviour as a consequence of absence of serotonin have also been investigated. The development of serotonergic neurons is dependent on a transcriptional programme, where the ETS transcription factor Pet-1 (FEV in humans) has been shown to be a unique marker for these cells (43,44). In Pet-1-deficient mice, development of the serotonin system is abolished, rendering these animals almost completely devoid of serotonin neurons and with an 85%–90% reduction of 5HT, without evident alterations to the cytoarchitecture of the brain. Behavioural characterization of the Pet-1-deficient mice established an increased aggressive behaviour compared to wild-type control mice and also elevated anxiety-like behaviours (44). Serotonin is synthesized from the essential amino acid tryptophan by an enzyme, tryptophan hydroxylase (TPH), which is present in two forms in humans, where type 1 (TPH1) is the predominant form in peripheral tissues, whereas type 2 (TPH2) is the rate-limiting enzyme in CNS serotonin synthesis. In two recent studies, mice with either a mutant form of the Tph2 gene that caused an 80% reduction in enzyme activity, or with a disrupted gene resulting in absence of the enzyme, were characterized (4546). These genetic modifications to the Tph2 gene caused an 80% reduction of CNS serotonin in the mutant mice (45) and total absence in the Tph2 knock-out mice (46). Also in these animal models, the behavioural consequences of serotonin depletion during development resulted in elevated aggressive and anxiety-like behaviours. From these experiments it seems clear that disruption of serotonin homeostasis during embryonic and foetal brain development has an impact on behavioural traits, which have been fundamental during evolution. The question arises as to which neuronal circuits become affected and how the elevated or reduced levels of serotonin cause these effects.

Mechanisms of altered neuronal development by serotonin

A well characterized rodent brain region that displays anatomical alterations in response to elevated serotonin levels during development is the somatosensory cortex, which, in humans as well as rodents, has a well defined cytoarchitecture, corresponding to the afferent sensory projections from different areas of the body. Specifically the barrel fields in rodents, innervated by thalamocortical afferent neurons emanating from the whiskers, have been extensively studied due to the clear anatomical representation of barrel-like structures corresponding to individual snout whiskers in rodents (for review, see Killackey et al. (47)). Numerous studies in rodents have shown that both pharmacological and genetic alterations to serotonin levels during embryonic brain development affect the formation of barrels in the somatosensory cortex (4851). This effect on cortex patterning results from the inability of thalamocortical afferent neurons properly to form branching arbours at their terminal ends (52). These neurons, which are mainly glutamatergic, exhibit a transient serotonergic phenotype during a period of the foetal development, in that they transiently express 5HTT, MAOA, and VMAT2, and actively take up extracellular serotonin, which is stored in vesicles and released at the nerve terminals. Detailed studies have revealed that the normal development of these neurons is dependent on their ability to capture serotonin through uptake via 5HTT, but is not dependent on the vesicular release of serotonin (50). Interestingly, this phenotype, present in 5HTT and MAOA knock-out mice, was rescued by simultaneous depletion of the serotonin receptor 5HT-1B (53,54), which has been shown to inhibit the release of glutamate at the thalamocortical nerve terminals (55). This indicates that the transient serotonin uptake in these neurons could serve to regulate the receptor activity by altering the levels of available substrate. This regulation of neuron development provides a good model to understand how altered levels of serotonin could exert its effects.

Neuronal circuits affected by altered serotonin levels

In vivo functional studies of the human brain, using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), have provided important clues to which specific brain regions and functional connections between these that display serotonin-dependent activity differences (15,5662). A recurrent finding in these studies has been that the amygdala displays both morphometric changes and activity response to behavioural tasks as a function of genetic variability in the promoter of both the MAOA and the 5HTT genes. The amygdala has an important role in emotional learning and processing, such as adequate reactions to potentially harmful situations (63). Healthy subjects with the low-functioning alleles of MAOA-LPR and 5HTTLPR have been shown to display increased amygdala activation to fearful stimuli in facial expression recognition tasks and also a reduced volume of the amygdala. In a study by Pezawas et al. it was shown that both the perigenual anterior cingulate cortex (pACC) and amygdala showed reduced volume in subjects with the low-functioning allele of the 5HTTLPR. Furthermore, it was shown that these subjects had lower ‘functional connectivity’ between these regions, implying that differences in emotional processing of negative stimuli could, to some extent, be explained by alterations in a neural circuit connecting these regions (58). These results were supported by a recent study by Pacheco et al. that investigated the effect of the 5HTTLPR on differences to a white matter fibre tract (uncinate fasciculus) anatomically connecting amygdala to regions of the prefrontal cortex (62). Here it was shown that carriers of the low-functioning 5HTTLPR had a reduced white matter connection, as determined by diffusion MRI, which supports the previously described reduced functional connectivity between these regions. Interestingly, a recent study by Jedema et al., investigating the orthologous 5HTT polymorphism in rhesus macaques, identified brain morphometric differences associated with the rh5-HTTLPR, analogous to what has been observed in humans (23). In two studies focusing on the MAOA-LPR (59,60), brain regions that showed genotypic association in response to emotion-stimulating tasks overlapped to some extent with those identified using the 5HTTLPR. In carriers of the low-functioning allele of MAOA, both the amygdala and anterior cingulate cortex displayed reduced volume compared to carriers of the high-functioning allele. However, morphometric and functional analyses also identified other regions affected by MAOA genotype and, most importantly, that the associations were conditional on sex. In males, but not in females, the low-functioning allele of MAOA was associated with increased volume of the lateral orbitofrontal cortex (59). Furthermore, in a task promoting retrieval of aversive memories, this study showed that amygdala activation in relation to MAOA genotype displayed opposite effects between males and females, where the low-functioning allele in males was associated with increased amygdala response. Also the hippocampus displayed sex-dependent activity response to this task, where male carriers of the low-functioning allele of MAOA had higher activity compared to the low-functioning allele, whereas females did not differ with respect to genotype. In a no-go flanker task, used to measure inhibitory control, the low-functioning allele of MAOA in males was associated with lowered activity in the anterior cingulate cortex, whereas in females no significant difference was observed. A study by Buckholtz et al. identified a neural circuit comprised of the amygdala, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and anterior cingulate cortex. Male carriers of the low-functioning allele of MAOA showed an increased functional coupling between the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and the amygdala, mediated via the anterior cingulate cortex, in response to a facial expression recognition task (60). In summary, these fMRI studies clearly show that specific brain regions, previously implicated in emotion processing, and behaviour were functionally and anatomically affected by genetic variation in the genes for MAOA and 5HTT, indicating a potential effect of variable serotonin levels on these structures. The fact that levels and activity of the MAOA and 5HTT in the adult brain are poorly associated with genotype (1926) argues for a developmental effect of the functional polymorphisms on these neurocircuits by regulating embryonic/foetal serotonin levels. This interpretation is supported by recent studies showing disturbances to the same corticolimbic structures resulting from absence of the serotonin transporter during development (64,65) and also from the identification of non-serotonergic neurons in various limbic areas, transiently expressing the serotonin transporter (66). Surprisingly, the last-mentioned study did not observe 5HTT marker expression in the amygdala during any developmental period. This result could indicate that the wealth of data associating the low-functioning variant of 5HTTLPR to hyper-reactive amygdala response and lower amygdala volume is related to an activity-dependent effect from the neural projections to and from this region during development, rather than a developmental effect of serotonin on the amygdala itself.

Negative feedback on the serotonergic neurons by serotonin

Serotonin has been shown to modulate the outgrowth of terminals from serotonergic neurons in an auto-regulatory feedback loop both directly and indirectly (for review, see Whitaker-Azmitia (67)). The negative feedback loop seems to be dependent on the 5HT1A receptor, which is expressed early during development, both on serotonergic neurons and in target regions of the limbic circuitry. This means that excessive serotonin levels during brain development could have consequences not only for the neurocircuitry discussed above but also for the size and capacity of the serotonergic system itself. This could in part explain the paradoxical relation between genetic variants associated with increased levels of available serotonin (low-functioning alleles of MAOA and 5HTT) and the link to behavioural traits and psychiatric disorders associated with lower levels of serotonin in the brain and its metabolite 5HIAA in cerebrospinal fluid. Or, put another way, the low-functioning variants of MAOA and 5HTT could be associated with an increased risk for psychiatric disorders due to increased levels of serotonin during CNS development, causing functional alterations to neurocircuits critical for emotional processing while simultaneously inhibiting the outgrowth of the serotonergic system.

The effect of serotonin in adulthood

Although much of the discussion above focuses on the role of serotonin during brain development and the behavioural consequences, it also has an evident role in shaping mood and behaviour in adulthood. Two well studied experimental paradigms for exploring phenotypic effects of altered serotonin levels in adulthood are ‘tryptophan depletion’ and ‘tryptophan challenge’ (68). These methods for altering central serotonin levels have specifically been used to study depression, since this condition has been strongly associated with reduced serotonergic neurotransmission. The tryptophan depletion paradigm has repeatedly been associated with a lowering of mood (68,69), assumed to be a consequence of decreased serotonin synthesis. Recent studies, however, have provided results that support a more multi-faceted function of serotonin in adulthood. A study performed on healthy women exposed to tryptophan depletion showed reduction in mood compared to controls, but this effect was shown to be moderated by genotype in the 5HTTLPR. A stress-inducing task caused a significant increase in depressive symptoms and reduced sense of vigour among carriers of the low-functioning variant of 5HTTLPR in response to tryptophan depletion, whereas the opposite effect was observed for carriers of the high-functioning variant (70). These results are in line with the observations that the low-functioning variant of 5HTTLPR has been associated with increased amygdala reactivity and reduced stress-coping (71). Since there seem to be a poor correlation between genotype at the 5HTTLPR locus and availability of the serotonin transporter in the adult brain (1923), these differences from modulating serotonin levels in the adult brain, we hypothesize, are phenotypic consequences arising from a brain that has already been ‘wired’ by variable levels of serotonin during development. The situation seems to be even more complex considering that response to tryptophan depletion has been shown to be conditional on sex. In a study by Walderhaug et al., women reacted with a reduction in both mood and impulsiveness, whereas men showed no difference in mood but with increased impulsiveness (72). The lowering of mood among women was associated to 5HTTLPR genotype, where homozygosity for either the high- or the low-functioning variant predicted mood reduction. In this study, however, no stress-inducing task was used. The opposite paradigm, where healthy volunteers have been subjected to a tryptophan challenge, shows contradictory effects on depressive mood for females (73,74). Furthermore, also opposite genotypic effects among women were observed, where the low-functioning variant of 5HTTLPR either caused a lowering of mood (73), or an improved mood (74). In healthy men, tryptophan challenge induced a reduction in mood, which was observed to be modulated by 5HTTLPR genotype, in that carriers of the high-functioning variant scored higher on depression (73).

Serotonin and environment interactions

Brain structures involved in emotional regulation form the neuronal foundation for how we respond to external stimuli. Above, we have outlined how serotonin could be involved in modulating some of these neurocircuits with apparent consequences for shaping of different aspects of behaviour. From this follows that genetic variability leading to altered serotonergic neurotransmission will have an impact on how individuals interpret and respond to environmental cues, such as stressful or fearful stimuli for example. However, emotional processing in this sense is not a unilateral event, determined only by the shape and activity in these neuronal circuits. In a famous study by Caspi et al., it was shown that the phenotypic outcome was modulated through an interplay between environmental insult and genetic factors (10). In a sample of male adolescents, antisocial behaviour was shown to be more common among carriers of the low-functioning variant of the MAOA-LPR, given that they had also experienced childhood maltreatment. Similar results have been observed in depression, where carriers of the low-functioning variant of 5HTTLPR were particularly vulnerable to stressful life events (11). These results imply that the penetrance of a genetic factor on behaviour seems to be dependent on the environmental context. In consecutive studies of gene–environment interactions it has been observed that the effects of variable socio-environmental factors, in combination with genotype of the MAOA-LPR and 5HTTLPR, on behavioural outcomes also display sex differences. A study by Nilsson et al. replicated the finding that males exposed to an unfavourable psycho-social environment displayed increased antisocial behaviour, particularly evident among carriers of the low-functioning variant of the MAOA-LPR (12). In a follow-up study, it was shown that, among females, this genetic variant was associated with a lower degree of antisocial behaviour (75). The findings of opposite effects from genetic variability in the MAOA-LPR between the sexes have also been revealed for alcohol-related problems in relation to poor psycho-social environment (14,76). Furthermore, a similar pattern of sexual dichotomy has been observed for the 5HTTLPR in relation to depressive symptoms (13). Interestingly, these findings have been substantiated by studies in non-human primates, showing that early rearing conditions had sex-dependent effects on stress-related response, moderated by a 5HTTLPR-orthologous polymorphism in rhesus macaques (77). Plausible explanations for the emergence of these sexually dimorphic responses might be sought among a number of different mechanisms and during various stages in development. Historically, the impact of gonadal hormones has been a prevailing model for sexually dimorphic traits, but has over time developed to include a more complex pattern of interactions between hormones, sex chromosome-linked gene expression, and environmental influences through epigenetic processes (78,79). There is a clear discrepancy in prevalence for neuropsychiatric disorders between the sexes, where, for example, females more often tend to develop major depressive disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, and post-traumatic stress disorder, whereas, in males, antisocial personality disorder, childhood attention-defecit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and alcohol and drug dependence are more frequent (80). Based on recent literature, an emerging consensus seems to hold that the male and female brain differ in many aspects with regard to both anatomical and functional structures (81). These differences between the sexes might indicate that the aetiology and progression in neuropsychiatric disorders could follow somewhat different paths with regard to alterations in the CNS. Furthermore, though genetic variants that cause variable levels of CNS serotonin during brain development have been shown to regulate the response to early stressful life events, these aversive stimuli, irrespectively of genetic background, also seem to affect postnatal brain development. In a study on rhesus macaques that were reared either by their mother or peers, Spinelli et al. could show that the early life stress associated with peer-rearing led to stable morphological changes in brain regions previously shown to be affected in human neuropsychiatric disorders (82). Other studies in animal models have shown that early life stress can affect neurotrophic factors such as brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and nerve growth factor (NGF), known to be important for development of neurocircuits involved in emotional processing (83). Alterations to BDNF during postnatal brain development, in response to stress, are particularly interesting since BDNF seems to interact functionally with serotonin in brain morphogenesis (84), results which are supported by a human fMRI study where epistasis between BDNF and 5HTT was shown to affect both volume and functional connectivity within parts of the limbic circuitry (85). There are also studies in animal models showing that early life stress can affect serotonergic neurotransmission by causing expressional differences to 5HTT and serotonin receptors (86), and reducing the density of serotonin neuronal axons to specific brain regions (87).

Concluding remarks

Serotonin has been shown to be an important factor in regulating the neuro-development of brain regions critical for emotional processing. In animal models, dysregulation of serotonin during these early neuro-developmental stages have revealed behavioural outcomes with important parallels to human neuropsychiatric disorders. Functional genetic variability in the genes for MAOA and 5HTT has been firmly associated with anatomical and functional differences in the limbic system in humans, in the absence of an association to availability or activity of the corresponding proteins, arguing for an effect of these genetic factors during brain development. Furthermore, the functional genetic variants of MAOA and 5HTT have been shown to modulate the impact of environmental factors on behavioural traits, in terms of gene–environment interactions, which in many cases display apparent sex differences. It also appears as if these interactions are reciprocal at early ages, in the sense that genetic factors regulating serotonin levels result in developmental alterations to specific neurocircuits involved in emotional processing, causing different responses to external stimuli, which has been observed to have an effect on serotonergic neurotransmission, further affecting on-going neuronal development. Future studies on the causal mechanisms behind the role of serotonin during brain development will hopefully provide knowledge, not only of how the neurocircuits involved in emotional processing work, but also give an improved understanding of the biological aetiology for neuropsychiatric disorders.

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to Erika Comasco for critical reading of the manuscript. This work has been supported by grants from the Swedish Research Council (VR), AFA insurance (AFA försäkring), and Systembolaget Council for Alcohol Research (SRA).

Declaration of interest: The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of the paper.

References

  • 1.Little KY, McLaughlin DP, Zhang L, Livermore CS, Dalack GW, McFinton PR, et al. Cocaine, ethanol, and genotype effects on human midbrain serotonin transporter binding sites and mRNA levels. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155:207–13. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.2.207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Sabol SZ, Hu S, Hamer D. A functional polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase A gene promoter. Hum Genet. 1998;103:273–9. doi: 10.1007/s004390050816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Deckert J, Catalano M, Syagailo YV, Bosi M, Okladnova O, Di Bella D, et al. Excess of high activity monoamine oxidase A gene promoter alleles in female patients with panic disorder. Hum Mol Genet. 1999;8:621–4. doi: 10.1093/hmg/8.4.621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Denney RM, Koch H, Craig IW. Association between monoamine oxidase A activity in human male skin fibroblasts and genotype of the MAOA promoter-associated variable number tandem repeat. Hum Genet. 1999;105:542–51. doi: 10.1007/s004399900183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Bennett AJ, Lesch KP, Heils A, Long JC, Lorenz JG, Shoaf SE, et al. Early experience and serotonin transporter gene variation interact to influence primate CNS function. Mol Psychiatry. 2002;7:118–22. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000949. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Newman TK, Syagailo YV, Barr CS, Wendland JR, Champoux M, Graessle M, et al. Monoamine oxidase A gene promoter variation and rearing experience influences aggressive behavior in rhesus monkeys. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:167–72. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Hu XZ, Lipsky RH, Zhu G, Akhtar LA, Taubman J, Greenberg BD, et al. Serotonin transporter promoter gain-of-function genotypes are linked to obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Hum Genet. 2006;78:815–26. doi: 10.1086/503850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Guo G, Ou XM, Roettger M, Shih JC. The VNTR 2 repeat in MAOA and delinquent behavior in adolescence and young adulthood: associations and MAOA promoter activity. Eur J Hum Genet. 2008;16:626–34. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Lesch KP, Bengel D, Heils A, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, Petri S, et al. Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science. 1996;274:1527–31. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5292.1527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Caspi A, McClay J, Moffitt TE, Mill J, Martin J, Craig IW, et al. Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science. 2002;297:851–4. doi: 10.1126/science.1072290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, et al. Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science. 2003;301:386–9. doi: 10.1126/science.1083968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Nilsson KW, Sjoberg RL, Damberg M, Leppert J, Ohrvik J, Alm PO, et al. Role of monoamine oxidase A genotype and psychosocial factors in male adolescent criminal activity. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59:121–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Sjoberg RL, Nilsson KW, Nordquist N, Ohrvik J, Leppert J, Lindstrom L, et al. Development of depression: sex and the interaction between environment and a promoter polymorphism of the serotonin transporter gene. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2006;9:443–9. doi: 10.1017/S1461145705005936. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Nilsson KW, Wargelius HL, Sjoberg RL, Leppert J, Oreland L. The MAO-A gene, platelet MAO-B activity and psychosocial environment in adolescent female alcohol-related problem behaviour. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2008;93:51–62. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.08.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lau JY, Goldman D, Buzas B, Fromm SJ, Guyer AE, Hodgkinson C, et al. Amygdala function and 5-HTT gene variants in adolescent anxiety and major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65:349–55. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.08.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Nakamura M, Ueno S, Sano A, Tanabe H. The human serotonin transporter gene linked polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) shows ten novel allelic variants. Mol Psychiatry. 2000;5:32–8. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Kraft JB, Slager SL, McGrath PJ, Hamilton SP. Sequence analysis of the serotonin transporter and associations with antidepressant response. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58:374–81. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lesch KP, Meyer J, Glatz K, Flugge G, Hinney A, Hebebrand J, et al. The 5-HT transporter gene-linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) in evolutionary perspective: alternative biallelic variation in rhesus monkeys. Rapid communication. J Neural Transm. 1997;104:1259–66. doi: 10.1007/BF01294726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Willeit M, Stastny J, Pirker W, Praschak-Rieder N, Neumeister A, Asenbaum S, et al. No evidence for in vivo regulation of midbrain serotonin transporter availability by serotonin transporter promoter gene polymorphism. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50:8–12. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)01123-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Shioe K, Ichimiya T, Suhara T, Takano A, Sudo Y, Yasuno F, et al. No association between genotype of the promoter region of serotonin transporter gene and serotonin transporter binding in human brain measured by PET. Synapse. 2003;48:184–8. doi: 10.1002/syn.10204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Parsey RV, Hastings RS, Oquendo MA, Hu X, Goldman D, Huang YY, et al. Effect of a triallelic functional polymorphism of the serotonin-transporter-linked promoter region on expression of serotonin transporter in the human brain. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:48–51. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.1.48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Christian BT, Fox AS, Oler JA, Vandehey NT, Murali D, Rogers J, et al. Serotonin transporter binding and genotype in the nonhuman primate brain using [C-11]DASB PET. NeuroImage. 2009;47:1230–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Jedema HP, Gianaros PJ, Greer PJ, Kerr DD, Liu S, Higley JD, et al. Cognitive impact of genetic variation of the serotonin transporter in primates is associated with differences in brain morphology rather than serotonin neurotransmission. Mol Psychiatry. 2009. Sep 1, (Epub ahead of print) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 24.Fowler JS, Alia-Klein N, Kriplani A, Logan J, Williams B, Zhu W, et al. Evidence that brain MAO A activity does not correspond to MAO A genotype in healthy male subjects. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:355–8. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.08.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Alia-Klein N, Goldstein RZ, Kriplani A, Logan J, Tomasi D, Williams B, et al. Brain monoamine oxidase A activity predicts trait aggression. J Neurosci. 2008;28:5099–104. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0925-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Alia-Klein N, Kriplani A, Pradhan K, Ma JY, Logan J, Williams B, et al. The MAO-A genotype does not modulate resting brain metabolism in adults. Psychiatry Res. 2008;164:73–6. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Balciuniene J, Emilsson L, Oreland L, Pettersson U, Jazin E. Investigation of the functional effect of monoamine oxidase polymorphisms in human brain. Hum Genet. 2002;110:1–7. doi: 10.1007/s00439-001-0652-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Cirulli ET, Goldstein DB. In vitro assays fail to predict in vivo effects of regulatory polymorphisms. Hum Mol Genet. 2007;16:1931–9. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddm140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Soubrié P. Reconciling the role of central serotonin neurons in human and animal behavior. Behav Brain Sci. 1986;9:319–64. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Sundstrom E, Kolare S, Souverbie F, Samuelsson EB, Pschera H, Lunell NO, et al. Neurochemical differentiation of human bulbospinal monoaminergic neurons during the first trimester. Brain Res Dev Brain Res. 1993;75:1–12. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(93)90059-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Moiseiwitsch JR, Lauder JM. Serotonin regulates mouse cranial neural crest migration. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1995;92:7182–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.16.7182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Gustafsson BI, Tommeras K, Nordrum I, Loennechen JP, Brunsvik A, Solligard E, et al. Long-term serotonin administration induces heart valve disease in rats. Circulation. 2005;111:1517–22. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.0000159356.42064.48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Brunner HG, Nelen M, Breakefield XO, Ropers HH, van Oost BA. Abnormal behavior associated with a point mutation in the structural gene for monoamine oxidase A. Science. 1993;262:578–80. doi: 10.1126/science.8211186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Cases O, Seif I, Grimsby J, Gaspar P, Chen K, Pournin S, et al. Aggressive behavior and altered amounts of brain serotonin and norepinephrine in mice lacking MAOA. Science. 1995;268:1763–6. doi: 10.1126/science.7792602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Popova NK, Maslova LN, Morosova EA, Bulygina VV, Seif I. MAO A knockout attenuates adrenocortical response to various kinds of stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2006;31:179–86. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Scott AL, Bortolato M, Chen K, Shih JC. Novel monoamine oxidase A knock out mice with human-like spontaneous mutation. Neuroreport. 2008;19:739–43. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282fd6e88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Holmes A, Lit Q, Murphy DL, Gold E, Crawley JN. Abnormal anxiety-related behavior in serotonin transporter null mutant mice: the influence of genetic background. Genes Brain Behav. 2003;2:365–80. doi: 10.1046/j.1601-1848.2003.00050.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kalueff AV, Olivier JD, Nonkes LJ, Homberg JR. Conserved role for the serotonin transporter gene in rat and mouse neurobehavioral endophenotypes. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009. Aug 19, (Epub ahead of print) [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 39.Noorlander CW, Ververs FF, Nikkels PG, van Echteld CJ, Visser GH, Smidt MP. Modulation of serotonin transporter function during fetal development causes dilated heart cardiomyopathy and lifelong behavioral abnormalities. PloS One. 2008;3:e2782. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002782. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Ansorge MS, Morelli E, Gingrich JA. Inhibition of serotonin but not norepinephrine transport during development produces delayed, persistent perturbations of emotional behaviors in mice. J Neurosci. 2008;28:199–207. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3973-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Clancy B, Kersh B, Hyde J, Darlington RB, Anand KJ, Finlay BL. Web-based method for translating neurodevelopment from laboratory species to humans. Neuroinformatics. 2007;5:79–94. doi: 10.1385/ni:5:1:79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Mejia JM, Ervin FR, Baker GB, Palmour RM. Monoamine oxidase inhibition during brain development induces pathological aggressive behavior in mice. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;52:811–21. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01418-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Hendricks T, Francis N, Fyodorov D, Deneris ES. The ETS domain factor Pet-1 is an early and precise marker of central serotonin neurons and interacts with a conserved element in serotonergic genes. J Neurosci. 1999;19:10348–56. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-23-10348.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Hendricks TJ, Fyodorov DV, Wegman LJ, Lelutiu NB, Pehek EA, Yamamoto B, et al. Pet-1 ETS gene plays a critical role in 5-HT neuron development and is required for normal anxiety-like and aggressive behavior. Neuron. 2003;37:233–47. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01167-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Beaulieu JM, Zhang X, Rodriguiz RM, Sotnikova TD, Cools MJ, Wetsel WC, et al. Role of GSK3 beta in behavioral abnormalities induced by serotonin deficiency. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:1333–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0711496105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Alenina N, Kikic D, Todiras M, Mosienko V, Qadri F, Plehm R, et al. Growth retardation and altered autonomic control in mice lacking brain serotonin. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:10332–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0810793106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Killackey HP, Rhoades RW, Bennett-Clarke CA. The formation of a cortical somatotopic map. Trends Neurosci. 1995;18:402–7. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(95)93937-s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Cases O, Vitalis T, Seif I, De Maeyer E, Sotelo C, Gaspar P. Lack of barrels in the somatosensory cortex of monoamine oxidase A-deficient mice: role of a serotonin excess during the critical period. Neuron. 1996;16:297–307. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80048-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Vitalis T, Cases O, Callebert J, Launay JM, Price DJ, Seif I, et al. Effects of monoamine oxidase A inhibition on barrel formation in the mouse somatosensory cortex: determination of a sensitive developmental period. J Comp Neurol. 1998;393:169–84. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19980406)393:2<169::aid-cne3>3.0.co;2-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Persico AM, Mengual E, Moessner R, Hall FS, Revay RS, Sora I, et al. Barrel pattern formation requires serotonin uptake by thalamocortical afferents, and not vesicular monoamine release. J Neurosci. 2001;21:6862–73. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-17-06862.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Xu Y, Sari Y, Zhou FC. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor disrupts organization of thalamocortical somatosensory barrels during development. Brain Res Dev Brain Res. 2004;150:151–61. doi: 10.1016/j.devbrainres.2003.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Auso E, Cases O, Fouquet C, Camacho M, Garcia-Velasco JV, Gaspar P, et al. Protracted expression of serotonin transporter and altered thalamocortical projections in the barrelfield of hypothyroid rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2001;14:1968–80. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01815.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Salichon N, Gaspar P, Upton AL, Picaud S, Hanoun N, Hamon M, et al. Excessive activation of serotonin (5-HT) 1B receptors disrupts the formation of sensory maps in monoamine oxidase a and 5-ht transporter knock-out mice. J Neurosci. 2001;21:884–96. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-03-00884.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Rebsam A, Seif I, Gaspar P. Refinement of thalamocortical arbors and emergence of barrel domains in the primary somatosensory cortex: a study of normal and monoamine oxidase a knock-out mice. J Neurosci. 2002;22:8541–52. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-19-08541.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Laurent A, Goaillard JM, Cases O, Lebrand C, Gaspar P, Ropert N. Activity-dependent presynaptic effect of serotonin 1B receptors on the somatosensory thalamocortical transmission in neonatal mice. J Neurosci. 2002;22:886–900. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-03-00886.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Hariri AR, Mattay VS, Tessitore A, Kolachana B, Fera F, Goldman D, et al. Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala. Science. 2002;297:400–3. doi: 10.1126/science.1071829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Hariri AR, Drabant EM, Munoz KE, Kolachana BS, Mattay VS, Egan MF, et al. A susceptibility gene for affective disorders and the response of the human amygdala. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:146–52. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.2.146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Pezawas L, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Drabant EM, Verchinski BA, Munoz KE, Kolachana BS, et al. 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:828–34. doi: 10.1038/nn1463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Meyer-Lindenberg A, Buckholtz JW, Kolachana B, R Hariri A, Pezawas L, Blasi G, et al. Neural mechanisms of genetic risk for impulsivity and violence in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103:6269–74. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0511311103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Buckholtz JW, Callicott JH, Kolachana B, Hariri AR, Goldberg TE, Genderson M, et al. Genetic variation in MAOA modulates ventromedial prefrontal circuitry mediating individual differences in human personality. Mol Psychiatry. 2008;13:313–24. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Passamonti L, Cerasa A, Gioia MC, Magariello A, Muglia M, Quattrone A, et al. Genetically dependent modulation of serotonergic inactivation in the human prefrontal cortex. NeuroImage. 2008;40:1264–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Pacheco J, Beevers CG, Benavides C, McGeary J, Stice E, Schnyer DM. Frontal-limbic white matter pathway associations with the serotonin transporter gene promoter region (5-HTTLPR) polymorphism. J Neurosci. 2009;29:6229–33. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0896-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Paton JJ, Belova MA, Morrison SE, Salzman CD. The primate amygdala represents the positive and negative value of visual stimuli during learning. Nature. 2006;439:865–70. doi: 10.1038/nature04490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Wellman CL, Izquierdo A, Garrett JE, Martin KP, Carroll J, Millstein R, et al. Impaired stress-coping and fear extinction and abnormal corticolimbic morphology in serotonin transporter knock-out mice. J Neurosci. 2007;27:684–91. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4595-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Bearer EL, Zhang X, Janvelyan D, Boulat B, Jacobs RE. Reward circuitry is perturbed in the absence of the serotonin transporter. NeuroImage. 2009;46:1091–104. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Narboux-Neme N, Pavone LM, Avallone L, Zhuang X, Gaspar P. Serotonin transporter transgenic (SERTcre) mouse line reveals developmental targets of serotonin specific reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) Neuropharmacology. 2008;55:994–1005. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.08.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Whitaker-Azmitia PM. Behavioral and cellular consequences of increasing serotonergic activity during brain development: a role in autism? Int J Dev Neurosci. 2005;23:75–83. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.07.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Young SN, Leyton M. The role of serotonin in human mood and social interaction. Insight from altered tryptophan levels. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2002;71:857–65. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(01)00670-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Fusar-Poli P, Allen P, McGuire P, Placentino A, Cortesi M, Perez J. Neuroimaging and electrophysiological studies of the effects of acute tryptophan depletion: a systematic review of the literature. Psychopharmacology. 2006;188:131–43. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0493-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Firk C, Markus CR. Differential effects of 5-HTTLPR genotypes on mood, memory, and attention bias following acute tryptophan depletion and stress exposure. Psychopharmacology. 2009;203:805–18. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1428-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Munafo MR, Brown SM, Hariri AR. Serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) genotype and amygdala activation: a meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:852–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.08.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Walderhaug E, Magnusson A, Neumeister A, Lappalainen J, Lunde H, Refsum H, et al. Interactive effects of sex and 5-HTTLPR on mood and impulsivity during tryptophan depletion in healthy people. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:593–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Brummett BH, Muller CL, Collins AL, Boyle SH, Kuhn CM, Siegler IC, et al. 5-HTTLPR and gender moderate changes in negative affect responses to tryptophan infusion. Behav Genet. 2008;38:476–83. doi: 10.1007/s10519-008-9219-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Markus CR, Firk C. Differential effects of tri-allelic 5-HTTLPR polymorphisms in healthy subjects on mood and stress performance after tryptophan challenge. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:2667–74. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Sjoberg RL, Nilsson KW, Wargelius HL, Leppert J, Lindstrom L, Oreland L. Adolescent girls and criminal activity: role of MAOA-LPR genotype and psychosocial factors. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2007;144B:159–64. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Nilsson KW, Sjoberg RL, Wargelius HL, Leppert J, Lindstrom L, Oreland L. The monoamine oxidase A (MAO-A) gene, family function and maltreatment as predictors of destructive behaviour during male adolescent alcohol consumption. Addiction. 2007;102:389–98. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01702.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Barr CS, Newman TK, Schwandt M, Shannon C, Dvoskin RL, Lindell SG, et al. Sexual dichotomy of an interaction between early adversity and the serotonin transporter gene promoter variant in rhesus macaques. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:12358–63. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0403763101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Arnold AP. Sex chromosomes and brain gender. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5:701–8. doi: 10.1038/nrn1494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Weaver IC, Cervoni N, Champagne FA, D'Alessio AC, Sharma S, Seckl JR, et al. Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:847–54. doi: 10.1038/nn1276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Holden C. Sex and the suffering brain. Science. 2005;308:1574. doi: 10.1126/science.308.5728.1574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Cahill L. Why sex matters for neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:477–84. doi: 10.1038/nrn1909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Spinelli S, Chefer S, Suomi SJ, Higley JD, Barr CS, Stein E. Early-life stress induces long-term morphologic changes in primate brain. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:658–65. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Cirulli F, Francia N, Branchi I, Antonucci MT, Aloe L, Suomi SJ, et al. Changes in plasma levels of BDNF and NGF reveal a gender-selective vulnerability to early adversity in rhesus macaques. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:172–80. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.08.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Ren-Patterson RF, Cochran LW, Holmes A, Sherrill S, Huang SJ, Tolliver T, et al. Loss of brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene allele exacerbates brain monoamine deficiencies and increases stress abnormalities of serotonin transporter knockout mice. J Neurosci Res. 2005;79:756–71. doi: 10.1002/jnr.20410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Pezawas L, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Goldman AL, Verchinski BA, Chen G, Kolachana BS, et al. Evidence of biologic epistasis between BDNF and SLC6A4 and implications for depression. Mol Psychiatry. 2008;13:709–16. doi: 10.1038/mp.2008.32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Oreland S, Pickering C, Gokturk C, Oreland L, Arborelius L, Nylander I. Two repeated maternal separation procedures differentially affect brain 5-hydroxytryptamine transporter and receptors in young and adult male and female rats. Brain Res. 2009;(1305 Suppl):S37–49. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.08.069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Kuramochi M, Nakamura S. Effects of postnatal isolation rearing and antidepressant treatment on the density of serotonergic and noradrenergic axons and depressive behavior in rats. Neuroscience. 2009;163:448–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Upsala Journal of Medical Sciences are provided here courtesy of Upsala Medical Society

RESOURCES