Abstract
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity is a childhood-onset disorder that can persist into adult life. Traditional family, twin and adoption studies have shown that ADHD defined both categorically and dimensionally is familial and heritable. Twin studies are now being used to examine ways of defining the ADHD phenotype, to investigate gender differences, the effects on genes on continuity and comorbidity and to consider gene-environment interplay. Molecular genetic findings on ADHD have mainly arisen from functional candidate gene association studies and a number of pooled and meta-analyses have now been conducted. There is consistent evidence of association between ADHD and a dopamine D4 receptor gene VNTR and a dopamine D5 receptor gene microsatellite marker. More recent evidence from different studies and a pooled analysis suggests that conduct problems in those with ADHD is influenced by the COMT val158/108 met variant. Linkage studies suggest that there are no genes of moderate effect size and findings from large scale whole genome association studies are currently awaited. Overall the evidence to date, suggests that examining gene-phenotype links and testing whether gene variants have modifying effects on the ADHD phenotype are important. The contribution of gene-environment interplay (G × E) to psychopathology is becoming increasingly recognised, although for ADHD little is known on causal environmental risk factors.
Keywords: ADHD, Twin studies, Molecular genetic findings
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a childhood-onset disorder that is characterised by developmentally inappropriate, severe, impairing inattention, overactivity and impulsiveness. It is increasingly recognised that ADHD can persist into adolescence and adult life. The reported prevalence of ADHD is between 1.4-5%. Although ADHD is considered as a diagnostic category for clinical purposes, it can also be viewed as a continuously distributed dimension. ADHD is commonly accompanied by other psychiatric and behavioural disorders, notably conduct disorder as well as developmental conditions (e.g. dyslexia, autism), tic disorders, depression and anxiety (Taylor & Sonuga-Barke, 2008). In this review we provide an overview on genetic studies of ADHD. We begin by considering traditional family, twin and adoption study designs. These types of study inform us about the ADHD phenotype that is crucial for informing molecular genetic studies and provideinsights into the origins and development of ADHD. We then move onto review the molecular genetics literature and finally consider future developments.
Family, twin and adoption studies
These research designs have traditionally been used to examine whether ADHD runs in families and is genetically influenced. More recently twin and family studies have been utilised to examine the genetic validity of different phenotype definitions, gender, developmental continuity and change, comorbidity and gene-environment interplay (Thapar & Rutter, 2008).
Evidence of a genetic contribution to ADHD
Family studies show that the biological relatives of probands with ADHD display higher rates of ADHD than relatives of controls. The relative risk of ADHD in first degree relatives is between 4.0 and 9.0 (Faraone et al, 2000; Chen et al, 2008); thus the familial risk of ADHD is higher than for rheumatoid arthritis but lower than for schizophrenia. Disorders can cluster in families because of shared environment as well as genes. Thus twin and adoption designs are needed to separate these effects.
Twin studies have primarily focused on dimensionally defined ADHD. There have been numerous published studies from across the world showing that ADHD symptoms are highly heritable (Thapar et al, 2006; Faraone et al, 2005); that is most of the individual variation in ADHD scores is attributable to genetic influences. Shared environmental influences that result in greater twin similarity for a given phenotype do not appear to be important. Non-inherited influences must however contribute, as genetic factors do not account for 100% of the phenotypic variation in ADHD. This “left over” non-shared environment variance could be explained by measurement error, random effects including those that are biological (e.g. epigenetic effects) as well as environmental factors that make twins different. The findings of all the published adoption studies are consistent with those of twin studies in demonstrating the importance of genetic influences in ADHD. All of these studies show that adopted children are more similar to their biological relatives than to their adoptive relatives on measures of ADHD.
In summary, there is consistent evidence of a strong genetic contribution to ADHD from family, twin and adoption studies and it is clear that ADHD is also influenced by non-inherited factors.
Defining the ADHD phenotype
1. Which informant and measure?
Most twin studies have obtained maternal ratings of children’s ADHD symptoms using questionnaire measures. Mother’s reports of ADHD scores have been found to be highly heritable with heritability estimates of between 60% - 91% (Thapar et al, 2007). Interestingly many of these studies have found low, near zero or even negative DZ (dizygotic; non identical) twin correlations, particularly where measures with fewer ADHD items have been used. This is thought to arise from rater contrast effects whereby mothers tend to exaggerate twin differences in ADHD symptom levels. This would explain low/negative DZ twin correlations and why DZ/MZ twin variances in ADHD scores differ in some studies. Non additive genetic effects (either genetic dominance or gene-gene interaction) and gene-environment interaction could also account for MZ twin correlations that are much higher than DZ twin correlation coefficients, although it is difficult to distinguish these from rater contrast effects.
Teacher reports of ADHD are also heritable. Twin studies find that most of the phenotypic overlap in terms of mother and teacher reports is due to shared genetic factors. Where ADHD is defined using both parent and teacher reports, this appears to result in a more reliable, heritable phenotype (Thapar et al, 2006).
Given the increased interest in adolescent and adult ADHD, self reports need to be considered. Twin studies of adolescents and adults show that most of the variation in self reported ADHD scores is attributable to non-shared environmental variance that includes measurement error and heritability estimates are much lower than those found in studies of children where parent and teacher reports have been used (Thapar et al, 2006). Retrospective measures of ADHD in adults have also yielded lower heritability estimates (around 30%) and most of the variance again is accounted for by non shared environmental factors (e.g. Haberstick et al, 2007).
In summary, twin evidence supports the practice of using both mother and teacher reports for defining ADHD in children. In adolescents and adults, the evidence suggests another informant is advisable given some concerns about self reports.
2. ADHD Subtypes
Although DSM-IV allows for subdividing ADHD into combined, hyperactive-impulsive and inattentive subtypes, it is not clear to what extent these are distinct. Family and sibling studies yield mixed findings. Overall results from a meta-analysis and subsequent studies suggest that there is only a small familial effect on subtype distinction and there is evidence for some overlap in terms of shared familial factors (Stawicki et al, 2006). Twin studies have been used to examine whether covariation of hyperactive-impulsive and inattentive symptoms is attributable to shared genetic liability. Most studies have found that there is shared genetic liability. However, others have suggested that there are also distinct genetic and environmental effects on the different symptom groups (Thapar et al, 2006; McLouglin et al, 2007). There is also evidence to suggest that the subtypes are not stable over time (Todd et al, 2008).
Another approach to defining subtypes is to undertake latent class analysis to generate different groups of individuals. A large twin study in Missouri, US derived eight latent classes that were found to be familial and heritable (Todd et al, 2008). A key issue is the extent to which these same latent classes can be derived in other populations. These latent classes have broadly been replicated in other populations and appear to be more consistently independent than DSM-IV subtypes. As a result, some groups have used these classes in molecular genetic studies.
In summary, the genetic evidence in favour of distinguishing ADHD subtypes is mixed.
3. Category or dimension
For clinical diagnostic purposes it is useful to consider ADHD as a categorical entity. However, most twin studies of ADHD have investigated ADHD defined dimensionally. Although fewer twin studies have examined categorical ADHD, those published show that broadly defined categorical ADHD is also heritable. A number of studies have further tested whether there is discontinuity in the genetic aetiology of ADHD scores at the extreme end of the dimension (i.e. higher scores). The genetic contribution to normal variation in ADHD scores seems to be the same as that for high or extreme scores and overlaps with genetic influences on the DSM-IV diagnosis of ADHD (Thapar et al, 2006; Chen et al, 2008; Derks et al, 2008). In summary, it appears that ADHD whether defined dimensionally or categorically is highly heritable and that there is genetic overlap in these constructs.
Gender effects
It is well established that the rate of ADHD is much higher in males than females. It is not clear why and how this male excess arises. One possibility is that there are different risk factors for males and females. Twin and family studies suggest this is not the case. Family studies show that ADHD in females as well as males is familial. Overall most twin studies do not find major differences in the magnitude of genetic influences on ADHD in males and females (Rhee et al, 1999) and also have been unable to demonstrate that there are different sets of male and female genetic influences.
Developmental change and continuity
Longitudinal studies of ADHD show that there is considerable diagnostic persistence and symptom continuity over time. There has been one family study that suggests that persistent ADHD may define a more strongly familial subtype (Faraone et al, 2000). Longitudinal twin studies show that most symptom continuity over time (in early, mid-childhood and adolescence) is attributable to genetic influences (e.g. Kuntsi et al, 2005; Larsson et al, 2006). Some have also found that there are additional separate genetic influences that contribute to ADHD symptoms in older children and adolescents. In summary, genetic factors not only contribute to the origins of ADHD but also contribute to continuity and persistence.
Comorbidity
ADHD and conduct problems commonly co-occur, in clinical populations (conduct disorder or oppositional defiant disorder) and at a symptom level in the general population and twin studies show that this arises because of shared genetic liability for the two phenotypes (Faraone et al, 2005; Thapar et al, 2006). A somewhat different question relates to whether or not children with categorically defined ADHD who also have conduct problems are different to those without conduct problems in terms of familial and genetic loading especially as they are known to have worse outcomes. Family studies suggest that ADHD and conduct disorder is a more strongly familial category than ADHD alone (Faraone et al, 2000). Findings from a subsequent twin study have also shown that comorbid conduct problems indexes higher genetic loading in ADHD (Thapar et al, 2001). Recent work suggests that ADHD with conduct disorder problems vs. ADHD alone are not qualitatively distinct entities (Rhee et al, 2008). As a result of these findings, a number of molecular genetic studies have utilised conduct disorder/ODD as an index of ADHD heterogeneity. ADHD also co-occurs with reading disability (the overlap appears to be stronger with inattention symptoms) and autistic traits. Twin studies suggest that these phenotypic overlaps are also attributable to shared genetic influences (Wilcutt et al, 2007; Ronald et al, 2007; Reiersen et al, 2007). Overall these findings suggest that the same genetic risk variants may contribute liability to ADHD and these different phenotypes.
Gene-environment interplay
Genes and environmental influences work together in complex ways (Plomin et al, 2008; Rutter 2007). There is increasing evidence that gene-environment correlation and gene-environment interaction are important in the aetiology of psychopathology.
1. Gene-environment correlation
Here, genetic and environment risk factors co-occur in a non-random fashion because heritable characteristics of the individual or parents create risk for exposure to certain environmental risk factors. This means that the risk effects of genes and environment are not necessarily distinct and also that genes may indirectly increase risk for ADHD by influencing exposure to risk or protective environments. This is certainly important for other psychopathology such as depression and emerging evidence suggests the likely importance of gene-environment correlation for ADHD (Lifford, Harold & Thapar, in preparation).
2. Gene-environment interaction
Gene-environment interaction occurs where the risk or protective effects of genetic factors depends upon the presence of a specific environmental factor (Thapar et al, 2007b; Rutter, 2007). Gene-environment interaction (GXE) can be examined using twin or adoption study designs or through molecular genetic study designs. These methods examine gene-environment interaction in different ways. So far, there have been no published twin studies examining gene-environment interaction for ADHD. However more knowledge is needed on likely causal environmental risk factors for ADHD. Risk factors that are associated are not necessarily causal (Rutter, 2007). Molecular genetic studies testing for GXE are described later.
Molecular genetics of ADHD
ADHD is a complex disorder and as such is most likely influenced by a large number of genes as well as environmental factors. Thus far, the quest of identifying susceptibility genes has been based on three main approaches (Thapar & Rutter, 2008). The first strategy involves genotyping many genetic markers across the genome in families where multiple members are affected (usually sibling pairs). This method highlights chromosomal regions that are shared more often than expected by affected relatives. Regions identified by these whole genome linkage studies are usually large and fine mapping strategies are required to identify genes (Plomin et al, 2008). The second approach requires an a prior hypothesis about the likely involvement of a candidate gene in the disorder. There are two types of these candidate gene studies: case-control studies, where the allele frequencies are compared between ADHD patients and healthy controls and family-based studies, where parents serve as controls. Functional candidate genes are selected because they are considered to be involved in disease pathophysiology. Positional candidates are selected because of their chromosomal positions (for example, informed by linkage studies). Finally and most recently a new generation of genetic studies has become feasible. Whole genome association (WGA) studies examine thousands or even millions of markers across the genome and they promise to yield novel, hypothesis-free results. Initial findings for other disorders appear promising (e.g., Wellcome Trust Case Consortium, 2007). Currently, three WGA studies for ADHD are under way and should be completed by the end of 2008. To date, most molecular genetic findings for ADHD have arisen from functional candidate gene association studies.
Whole genome linkage studies
Whole genome linkage scans have been based either on the affected sibling pair (ASP) or the extended pedigree approach. At the time of writing, results of seven linkage studies of ADHD have been published (Table 1); five using ASPs and two using multiplex families. As table 1 suggests, there is some overlap in significant linkage peaks. Evidence for at least nominally significant linkage has been found more than once for chromosomal regions 5p13, 16p13 and 17p11.
Table 1. Summary of ADHD whole genome linkage studies published so far.
Study | Size and design | Sample origin | Chromosomal regions | Evidence of linkage |
---|---|---|---|---|
Fisher et al. 2002 | 126 ASPs | USA | No evidence of linkage | - |
| ||||
Smalley et al. 2002 | 203 ASPs | USA | 16p13 | 4.2 |
| ||||
Ogdie et al. 2003 | 270 ASPs | USA | 16p13 | 4 |
| ||||
Ogdie et al. 2004 | 308 ASPs | USA | 5p13 | 2.55 |
6q12 | 3.3 | |||
16p13 | 3.73 | |||
17p11 | 3.63 | |||
| ||||
Ogdie et al. 2006 | 424 ASPs | USA and Dutch | 5p13 | 3.67 |
| ||||
Bakker et al. 2003 | 164 ASPs | Dutch | 15q15 | 3.54 |
7p13 | 3.04 | |||
5p13 | 1.43 | |||
| ||||
Arcos-Burgos et al. 2004 | 16 extended pedigrees | Colombian | 17p11 | MLS=1.42 |
| ||||
Hebebrand et al. 2006 | 155 ASPs | German | 5p(at 17cM) | 2.59 |
17p | 3.37(for inattention only) Nominally significant |
|||
|
||||
Faraone et al. 2007 | 601 ASPs | USA | No significant or suggestive linkage | - |
| ||||
Romanos et al. 2008 | 8 extended pedigrees | German | 5q13 | 4.16 |
14q12 | 4.5 | |||
2q35 | 3.4 | |||
| ||||
Asherson et al. 2008 | 142 ASPs | IMAGE sample | 16q23 | 3.1 |
9q22 | 2.13 |
ADHD: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; ASPs: Affected Sibling Pairs; MLS: Maximum Multipoint LOD; IMAGE: International Multi-centre ADHD Gene
A study of 126 ASPs from USA provided weak evidence for linkage at 16p13 (Fisher et al. 2002), which has also been implicated in autism. Stronger evidence for 16p13 [LOD (logarithm of the odds) score 4.2] came from an expanded sample of 203 ASPs (Smalley et al, 2002). The same region also came up in a study of 308 American ASPs with an MLS (Maximum Multipoint LOD) of 3.73 (Ogdie et al, 2004).
The chromosomal region at 5p13 also came up in the latter study (Ogdie et al, 2004). Modest evidence for linkage for the same region (MLS=1.43) appeared in a study of 164 Dutch ASPs (Bakker et al, 2003). Pooled analysis of the American and Dutch samples (Ogdie et al, 2006) yielded significant evidence of linkage (MLS=3.67) for 5p13, although the signal appeared to be coming mainly from the American families suggesting sample heterogeneity had an impact on the results. Finally, Hebebrand et al. (2006) studied 155 ASPs from Germany and found weak evidence of linkage (MLS=2.59) on chromosome 5p at 17cM. Another interesting region is 17p11, as there is evidence of linkage in both the study of 308 ASPs from USA (Ogdie et al, 2004) with an MLS of 3.63, as well as from the study of German ASPs (Hebebrand et al, 2006). A study with a different design using 16 multigenerational and extended pedigrees from Colombia also reported weak evidence of linkage at 17p11 (Arcos-Burgos et al, 2004).
In summary, whole genome linkage studies have yielded some interesting results for chromosomal regions that need to be further investigated. It seems difficult to achieve replication of genome-wide significant results, suggesting that there are no susceptibility genes of large effect for ADHD. For this reason, an association approach is likely to be more suitable, since it can identify genes of smaller effect.
Functional candidate gene association studies
Due to the large number of such studies, this review will only focus on genes where significant findings have stood up to meta-analyses or pooled analyses or where there have been replications. Many of these functional candidate genes have been selected because they code for proteins or enzymes involved in the dopamine pathway. Interest in the dopaminergic system in ADHD has come from pharmacological, animal and imaging studies. First, stimulant medication, such as methylphenidate, reduces ADHD symptoms and inhibits the reuptake of dopamine thus increasing its extracellular concentration (DiMaio et al, 2003). In addition, imaging studies of patients with ADHD provide evidence of changes in brain regions where dopaminergic systems are more active (Spencer et al, 2005). Finally, animal studies, such as work on the DAT knockout mouse which manifests ADHD-like behaviour, have also implicated dopaminergic systems (Gainetdinov, 2007). All this evidence has led to the investigation of DRD4, DRD5, SLC6A4 and COMT which will be discussed. Other genes such as those encoding dopamine beta-hydroxylase, monoamine oxidase A, the dopamine D2 and dopamine D3 receptors have also been investigated but the results are not yet conclusive. Nearly all the published molecular genetic studies of ADHD have been based on clinical cases where ADHD has been defined according to DSM-IV diagnostic criteria. A few studies have examined ADHD defined dimensionally but the association findings here have generally been weaker (Mill et al, 2005).
1. Dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4)
The DRD4 gene is on chromosome 11p15.5. The D4 receptor binds both dopamine and noradrenaline. Most studies have focused on a variable number tandem repeat (VNTR) polymorphism in exon III of the gene. The number of repeats ranges from 2-11 with different populations having different alleles. This polymorphism is supposed to be functional, since the 7-repeat allele reduces the ability of the receptor to bind dopamine according to in vitro studies. The first meta-analysis of the DRD4 gene in ADHD (Faraone et al, 2001) found significant association between the 7-repeat allele and ADHD in both case-control studies [OR (odds ratio) =1.9, 95% CI (confidence interval) 1.4-2.2] and family-based studies (OR=1.4, 95% CI 1.1-1.6). More recently, the same group conducted a pooled analysis and found that the association with ADHD was still significant in both case-control studies (OR=1.45, 95% CI 1.27-1.65) and family-based studies (OR=1.16, 95% CI 1.03-1.31) (Faraone et al, 2005). Li et al. (2006) included 33 association studies in their meta-analysis and also obtained strong evidence that the 7-repeat allele (P=2 × 10−12, OR=1.34, 95% CI 1.23-1.45) is associated with ADHD. They also suggested that the 5-repeat allele (P=0.005, OR=1.68, 95% CI 1.17-2.41) confers increased risk for ADHD and concluded that the 4-repeat allele has a protective role (P=0.004, OR=0.90, 95% CI 0.84-0.97) (Li et al, 2006) but that evidence is not clear-cut. Since this last meta-analysis, further association studies of DRD4 have been published. A longitudinal study from Germany replicated the association between ADHD and the 7-repeat allele (El-Faddagh et al, 2004). However, Brookes et al. (2006a) in a study of 776 ADHD cases from the International Multi-centre ADHD Gene (IMAGE) project failed to replicate this association.
The DRD4 7-repeat allele has also been reported to influence cognitive performance but findings here are mixed with some studies showing that those with the 7-repeat allele perform worse on measures of accuracy and cognitive ability and other groups finding better performance in those with the risk allele, so no conclusions can yet be drawn (Thapar et al, 2007a). There have also been longitudinal studies suggesting that those with ADHD who possess the DRD4 7 repeat allele show a poorer outcome (Mill et al, 2005; El Faddagh et al, 2004; Langley et al, 2008). However again, findings have not been entirely consistent (Shaw et al, 2007).
2. Dopamine D5 receptor gene (DRD5)
Another dopamine receptor gene, DRD5, on chromosome 4p15.1-15.3 also appears to be important. The associated polymorphism is a microsatellite (a dinucleotide repeat with variable number of copies) mapping 18.5 kb away from the 5′ end of the gene (Daly et al, 1999). The first meta-analysis of DRD5 included 5 studies and showed a significant association with the 148-bp allele (Maher et al, 2002). Significant association (OR=1.24, 95% CI 1.1-1.4) with the same allele was also reported by a joint analysis of 14 independent studies resulting in approximately 2000 ADHD cases (Lowe et al, 2004). Association was considered to be stronger in those with ADHD inattentive type. Finally, Li et al. (2006) included 9 association studies of DRD5 and also concluded that the 148-bp allele of DRD5 confers risk for ADHD. These authors also suggested that the 136-bp allele of DRD5 has a protective role. However, a study of 329 male twins examining a trait measure of ADHD, found association with the same variant but in the opposite direction, that is, the 148-bp allele had a protective role (Mill et al, 2005). Association with ADHD has also been shown for other polymorphisms in DRD5 including two microsatellites at the 5′ end of the gene and a SNP in the 3′ UTR (Untranslated Region) of DRD5 but these have been less widely studied.
3. Dopamine transporter gene (SLC6A3 or DAT1)
The dopamine transporter gene on chromosome 5p13.3 was initially considered the most likely candidate gene for ADHD. The major reason being that it is responsible for the reuptake of dopamine in the presynaptic cleft and is the target of stimulant medication (DiMaio et al, 2003). Another reason for considering SLC6A3 an important candidate gene for ADHD is because the DAT knockout mouse model exhibits hyperactivity and deficits in inhibitory behaviour. Treating these mice with stimulants reduces symptoms (Gainetdinov et al., 2007).
Despite these findings, association studies have not been able to produce clear evidence about the involvement of SLC6A3 in ADHD. The best studied polymorphism is a VNTR in the 3′ UTR of the gene. The first meta-analysis of nine SLC6A3 studies in ADHD reported an OR of 1.27 with a trend for association with the 480-bp allele (p=0.063) (Maher et al, 2002). Similar results were produced by Curran et al. (2005) based on 11 studies (two more than the previous meta-analysis). Nevertheless, both studies found evidence of heterogeneity across samples. In an updated meta-analysis by Faraone et al. (2005) the association of the 480-bp allele was significant, although the OR=1.13 (95% CI 1.03-1.24) was small. Another meta-analysis of 12 family-based studies the same year failed to find any association of the SLC6A3 with ADHD (Purper-Ouakil et al, 2005). No association of the 480-bp allele of SLC6A3 with ADHD was also found in the meta-analysis by Li et al. (2006). The most recent meta-analysis showed a small but significant association (p=0.004, OR=1.17, 95% CI 1.05-1.30) for TDT studies but not for haplotype-based studies or case-control studies (Yang et al, 2007). However, the number of haplotype-based and case- control studies was small (Yang et al, 2007). Apart from the 3′ UTR VNTR, association has been reported for a SNP (rs40184) in the IMAGE sample (Brookes et al, 2006a). Haplotypes that include the 3′ UTR VNTR and other microsatellite repeats have also been found to be associated with increased risk for ADHD (Asherson et al, 2007; Brookes et al, 2006b).
The evidence overall from an abundance of meta-analytic studies is inconsistent. One possibility is that the effect size of this variant is very small. Another possibility is that the polymorphism in question is not directly responsible for increasing risk for ADHD but is in linkage disequilibrium with another functional polymorphism. After all, it is in the 3′ UTR of the gene, which as the name suggests, is transcribed to mRNA but not translated into protein, thus its function is questionable. It is worth noting that most of the meta-analyses have found evidence of sample heterogeneity.
Another possibility for non-replication is gene-environment interaction (Thapar et al, 2007b). One study of ADHD found evidence of interaction between the haplotype containing the 3′ UTR VNTR and exposure to maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy (Brookes et al, 2006b) but this requires replication. Two studies suggested that those with the 480bp (10 repeat) allele who were exposed to maternal smoking in pregnancy showed higher levels of ADHD symptoms. However another study found evidence of GXE for the 9 repeat allele in relation to exposure to maternal smoking in pregnancy (Neuman et al, 2007) and a different group failed to replicate these findings (Langley et al, 2007).
4. Catechol-O-Methyltransferase (COMT)
The gene encoding COMT, an enzyme catalyzing the degradation of dopamine, adrenaline and noradrenaline, is on chromosome 22q11.2. Interest in COMT comes from its involvement in dopaminergic pathways. The most studied polymorphism in COMT is a SNP resulting in a valine to methionine substution. This polymorphism is functional with the val/val genotype increasing enzyme activity.
Virtually no individual studies and two pooled analyses (Cheuk and Wong 2006; Faraone et al. 2005) have found evidence of associated between this variant and ADHD. Interestingly the, COMT Val allele yielded an almost significant result in the male group of the Cheuk and Wong (2006) meta-analysis. Thus, it could potentially be involved in male susceptibility to ADHD (Cheuk and Wong 2006).
In contrast there is evidence that the COMT val/val genotype is associated with conduct disorder symptoms in patients with ADHD (Thapar et al. 2005). Since this first report, the same genotype was subsequently found to be associated with antisocial behaviour in those with ADHD (but not in those without ADHD) in two independent populations from a UK and a New Zealand birth cohort (Caspi et al. 2008). A pooled analysis of four studies also showed significant association with the val/val genotype (Caspi et al, 2008). These results suggest that some gene variants operate by modifying the ADHD phenotype rather than by increasing risk.
5. Synaptosomal-associated protein of 25kD (SNAP25)
SNAP25 is a neuron specific protein involved in the regulation of neurotransmitter release. It emerged as a candidate gene for ADHD when it was discovered that the coloboma mouse mutant, which lacks a chromosomal region containing SNAP25 as well as other genes, exhibits hyperactivity that could be reduced by the use of stimulant medication D-amphetamine.
Currently there is only one meta-analysis including this gene which reported a significant association with the T1065G SNP at the 3′ end of the gene (Faraone et al, 2005). Nominally significant association with SNAP25 was also reported using the IMAGE sample, although the group tested different markers (Brookes et al. 2006a). A study of 12 SNPs in SNAP25 using two independent samples also yielded evidence of association but in only one of the samples (Feng et al, 2005). Finally, Kim et al. (2007) reported a modestly significant association with two SNPs that have not been studied before in a TDT analysis. Interestingly, in this study, a stronger association with SNAP25 was found in patients with ADHD and comorbid depression highlighting the importance of taking into account psychiatric comorbidity in association studies (Kim et al, 2007).
To summarize, the most robust evidence is for an association between the DRD4 VNTR and DRD5 microsatellite marker in ADHD. (Table 2 summarizes all the meta-analyses for the genes discussed). It is still unclear whether the DRD4 VNTR is causal. The DRD5 148-bp microsatellite is 18.5kb away from the gene but that does not mean it does not influence DRD5 in a yet unknown fashion. As for SLC6A3, most studies have been negative or inconclusive, although it initially appeared as the strongest candidate gene. One explanation could be that the SLC6A3 480-bp VNTR is not responsible for the association but is tagging the functional polymorphism which is yet to be found. The evidence that COMT val/val genotype has a modifying effect on antisocial behaviour in ADHD is also fairly strong. Further studies on SNAP25 are needed before conclusions can be drawn.
Table 2. Meta-analyses and pooled studies for candidate gene association studies of ADHD.
Gene | Reference | Type of studies included | OR | 95% CI | p value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
DRD4 7-repeat allele | Faraone et al. 2001 | Case-control | 1.9 | 1.4-2.2 | <0.001 |
Family-based | 1.4 | 1.1-1.6 | 0.02 | ||
| |||||
Faraone et al. 2005 | Case-control | 1.45 | 1.27-1.65 | - | |
Family-based | 1.16 | 1.03-1.31 | - | ||
| |||||
Li et al. 2006 | Case-control and family-based |
1.34 | 1.23-1.45 | 2 × 10−12 | |
| |||||
DRD4 5-repeat allele | Li et al. 2006 | Case-control and family-based |
1.68 | 1.17-2.41 | 0.005 |
| |||||
DRD5 148-bp microsatellite repeat |
Maher et al. 2005 | Family-based | 1.57 | 1.25-1.96 | 0.00008 |
| |||||
Lowe et al. 2004a | Family-based | 1.24 | 1.1-1.4 | 0.00005 | |
| |||||
Li et al. 2006 | Case-control and family-based |
1.34 | 1.21-1.50 | 8 × 10−8 | |
| |||||
SLC6A3 480-bp VNTR | Maher et al. 2002 | Family-based | 1.27 | 0.99-1.62 | 0.063 |
| |||||
Curran et al. 2005 | Family-based | 1.15 | - | 0.06 | |
| |||||
Faraone et al. 2005 | Family-based | 1.13 | 1.03-1.24 | - | |
| |||||
Purper-Ouakil et al. 2005 | - | 1.19 | 0.99-1.41 | 0.21 | |
| |||||
Li et al. 2006 | Case-control and family-based |
1.04 | 0.98-1.11 | 0.20 | |
| |||||
Yang et al. 2007 | Family-based | 1.7 | 1.05-1.30 | 0.004 | |
Case-control | 0.95 | 0.8-1.12 | 0.54 | ||
Haplotype-based | 1.5 | 0.97-2.33 | 0.07 | ||
| |||||
COMT Val158Met | Faraone et al. 2005 | Case-control | 1.0 | - | - |
Family-based | |||||
| |||||
Cheuk and Wong, 2006 | Case-control and family-based |
0.99 | 0.88-1.12 | 0.87 | |
| |||||
SNAP25 T1065G | Faraone et al. 2005 | Case-control and family-based |
1.19 | 1.03-1.38 | - |
ADHD: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; OR: Odds Ratio; CI: Confidence Interval
Future
This review has highlighted that a number of consistent genetic findings have emerged in relation to ADHD. So what are the likely future directions? Traditional family, twin and adoption designs are no longer needed to test whether ADHD is genetically influenced. That is now known. However, these methods are still useful for examining important clinical and developmental questions that remain unanswered. These designs also provide evidence that can guide molecular genetic studies. For example, research on phenotype definition, phenotypic overlap and intermediate phenotypes are likely to be important. Genetically sensitive designs, such as twin studies will also be useful for investigating causal environmental risk factors for ADHD. This is important now we are entering the era of testing for gene-environment interaction. Such analyses must be guided by prior knowledge as to whether a given environmental risk factor is truly causal. Another example of gene-environment interaction is the study of genetic variants influencing drug response known as pharmacogenetics. Identifying factors that influence treatment response may be helpful although thus far, there have been no consistent findings and risk effects may be too small to have immediate impact on clinical practice.
Laboratory and statistical genetic methods will undoubtedly develop even further in the next few years. Published results from WGA studies of ADHD are currently awaited. It is already clear, however, from WGAS of other phenotypes that very large sample sizes are needed to detect genetic risk variants of small effect size and that international collaboration is necessary to achieve this aim. There is increasing interest in the potential role of sub-microscopic chromosomal changes (copy number variants) to complex diseases and the contribution of rare variants to neurodevelopmental disorders (Abrahams & Geschwind, 2008). Thus it is also important to consider these possibilities for ADHD.
Examining gene-phenotype links will remain important. In recent years, there has also been increasing interest in cognitive and neuroimaging phenotypes. Although ADHD is defined according to reported clinical symptoms, it is also characterised by a range of cognitive deficits, as evaluated by different cognitive tasks and differences in brain structure and function compared to typically developing children. Cognitive and imaging measures are not used to make a diagnosis of ADHD but are of interest to researchers who are attempting to examine the pathways from risk factor to disorder and identify intermediate phenotypes. Due to phenotyping costs, it is difficult to undertake genomic and cognitive imaging work on a very large scale. Nevertheless, such studies, although not used for gene discovery, will still be useful for examining the phenotypic effects of identified gene variants. Although there are many criticisms of current clinical diagnostic criteria, there is still the need to understand the aetiology of a phenotype that is relevant to clinicians and patients. Finally, one of the key goals of genetic studies of ADHD is to understand its aetiology and mechanisms. The challenge undoubtedly will not only be in identifying risk factors but more importantly testing causal mechanisms. Proving causality of genetic risk variants at a biological and phenotypic level undoubtedly represents a challenge but an important and interesting one that will need to be tackled by researchers in innovative ways.
References
- Abrahams BS, Geschwind DH. Advances in autism genetics: on the threshold of a new neurobiology. Nat Rev Genet. 2008 May;9(5):341–55. doi: 10.1038/nrg2346. 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arcos-Burgos M, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a population isolate: linkage to loci at 4q13.2, 5q33.3, 11q22, and 17p11. Am J Hum Genet. 2004;75(6):998–1014. doi: 10.1086/426154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asherson P, et al. Confirmation that a specific haplotype of the dopamine transporter gene is associated with combined-type ADHD. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(4):674–677. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.4.674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bakker SC, et al. A whole-genome scan in 164 Dutch sib pairs with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: suggestive evidence for linkage on chromosomes 7p and 15q. Am J Hum Genet. 2003;72(5):1251–1260. doi: 10.1086/375143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brookes K, et al. The analysis of 51 genes in DSM-IV combined type attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: association signals in DRD4, DAT1 and 16 other genes. Mol Psychiatry. 2006a;11(10):934–953. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brookes KJ, Mill J, Guindalini C, Curran S, Xu X, Knight J, Chen CK, Huang YS, Sethna V, Taylor E, Chen W, Breen G, Asherson P. A common haplotype of the dopamine transporter gene associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and interacting with maternal use of alcohol during pregnancy. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006 Jan;63(1):74–81. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.1.74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brookes KJ, et al. A common haplotype of the dopamine transporter gene associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and interacting with maternal use of alcohol during pregnancy. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006b;63(1):74–81. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.1.74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caspi A, et al. A replicated molecular genetic basis for subtyping antisocial behavior in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(2):203–210. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen W, Zhou K, Sham P, Franke B, Kuntsi J, Campbell D, Fleischman K, Knight J, Andreou P, Arnold R, Altink M, Boer F, Boholst MJ, Buschgens C, Butler L, Christiansen H, Fliers E, Howe-Forbes R, Gabriëls I, Heise A, Korn-Lubetzki I, Marco R, Medad S, Minderaa R, Müller UC, Mulligan A, Psychogiou L, Rommelse N, Sethna V, Uebel H, McGuffin P, Plomin R, Banaschewski T, Buitelaar J, Ebstein R, Eisenberg J, Gill M, Manor I, Miranda A, Mulas F, Oades RD, Roeyers H, Rothenberger A, Sergeant J, Sonuga-Barke E, Steinhausen HC, Taylor E, Thompson M, Faraone SV, Asherson P. DSM-IV combined type ADHD shows familial association with sibling trait scores: A sampling strategy for QTL linkage. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008 Jan 11; doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheuk DK, Wong V. Meta-analysis of association between a catechol-O-methyltransferase gene polymorphism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Behav Genet. 2006;36(5):651–659. doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9076-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curran S, et al. The serotonin transporter gene as a QTL for ADHD. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2005;134B(1):42–47. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daly G, et al. Mapping susceptibility loci in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: preferential transmission of parental alleles at DAT1, DBH and DRD5 to affected children. Mol Psychiatry. 1999;4(2):192–196. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Derks EM, Hudziak JJ, Dolan CV, van Beijsterveldt TC, Verhulst FC, Boomsma DI. Genetic and environmental influences on the relation between attention problems and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Behav Genet. 2008 Jan;38(1):11–23. doi: 10.1007/s10519-007-9178-8. Epub 2007 Dec 12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DiMaio S, et al. Dopamine genes and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a review. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2003;28(1):27–38. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- El-Faddagh M, et al. Association of dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) gene with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in a high-risk community sample: a longitudinal study from birth to 11 years of age. J Neural Transm. 2004;111(7):883–889. doi: 10.1007/s00702-003-0054-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faraone SV, Biederman J, Friedman D. Validity of DSM-IV subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a family study perspective. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000 Mar;39(3):300–7. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200003000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faraone SV, Biederman J, Monuteaux MC. Attention-deficit disorder and conduct disorder in girls: evidence for a familial subtype. Biol Psychiatry. 2000 Jul 1;48(1):21–9. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00230-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faraone SV, Perlis RH, Doyle AE, Smoller JW, Goralnick JJ, Holmgren MA, Sklar P. Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2005 Jun 1;57(11):1313–23. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.024. Epub 2005 Jan 21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faraone SV, et al. Meta-analysis of the association between the 7-repeat allele of the dopamine D(4) receptor gene and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(7):1052–1057. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.7.1052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feng Y, et al. The SNAP25 gene as a susceptibility gene contributing to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10(11):998–1005. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001722. 1973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher SE, et al. A genomewide scan for loci involved in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Am J Hum Genet. 2002;70(5):1183–1196. doi: 10.1086/340112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gainetdinov RR. Dopamine transporter mutant mice in experimental neuropharmacology. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol. 2007 doi: 10.1007/s00210-007-0216-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haberstick BC, Timberlake D, Hopfer CJ, Lessem JM, Ehringer MA, Hewitt JK. Genetic and environmental contributions to retrospectively reported DSM-IV childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Medicine. 2008;38:1057–1066. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hebebrand J, et al. A genome-wide scan for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in 155 German sib-pairs. Mol Psychiatry. 2006;11(2):196–205. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim JW, et al. Investigation of variation in SNAP-25 and ADHD and relationship to co-morbid major depressive disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2007;144B(6):781–790. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuntsi J, Rijsdijk F, Ronald A, Asherson P, Plomin R. Genetic influences on the stability of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms from early to middle childhood. Biol Psychiatry. 2005 Mar 15;57(6):647–54. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langley K, Fowler TA, Grady DL, Moyzis RK, Holmans PA, van den Bree MB, Owen MJ, O’Donovan MC, Thapar A. Molecular genetic contribution to the developmental course of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2008 doi: 10.1007/s00787-008-0698-4. Online June. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langley K, Turic D, Rice F, Holmans P, van den Bree MB, Craddock N, Kent L, Owen MJ, O’Donovan MC, Thapar A. Testing for gene x environment interaction effects in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and associated antisocial behavior. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008 Jan 5;147B(1):49–53. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li D, et al. Meta-analysis shows significant association between dopamine system genes and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Hum Mol Genet. 2006;15(14):2276–2284. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddl152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lifford KJ, Harold GT, Thapar A. Do family relationship factors have environmentally mediated risk effects on ADHD symptoms? Evidence from two community studies. 2008 In preparation. [Google Scholar]
- Lowe N, et al. Joint analysis of the DRD5 marker concludes association with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder confined to the predominantly inattentive and combined subtypes. Am J Hum Genet. 2004;74(2):348–356. doi: 10.1086/381561. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maher BS, et al. Dopamine system genes and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychiatr Genet. 2002;12(4):207–215. doi: 10.1097/00041444-200212000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLoughlin G, Ronald A, Kuntsi J, Asherson P, Plomin R. Genetic support for the dual nature of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: substantial genetic overlap between the inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive components. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2007 Dec;35(6):999–1008. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9149-9. Epub 2007 Aug 10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mill J, Caspi A, Williams BS, Craig I, Taylor A, Polo-Tomas M, Berridge CW, Poulton R, Moffitt TE. Prediction of heterogeneity in intelligence and adult prognosis by genetic polymorphisms in the dopamine system among children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: evidence from 2 birth cohorts. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006 Apr;63(4):462–9. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.4.462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mill J, et al. Quantitative trait locus analysis of candidate gene alleles associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in five genes: DRD4, DAT1, DRD5, SNAP-25, and 5HT1B. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2005;133B(1):68–73. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neuman RJ, Lobos E, Reich W, Henderson CA, Sun LW, Todd RD. Prenatal smoking exposure and dopaminergic genotypes interact to cause a severe ADHD subtype. Biol Psychiatry. 2007 Jun 15;61(12):1320–8. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.08.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ogdie MN, et al. Pooled genome-wide linkage data on 424 ADHD ASPs suggests genetic heterogeneity and a common risk locus at 5p13. Mol Psychiatry. 2006;11(1):5–8. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ogdie MN, et al. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: fine mapping supports linkage to 5p13, 6q12, 16p13, and 17p11. Am J Hum Genet. 2004;75(4):661–668. doi: 10.1086/424387. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plomin R, DeFries JC, McClearn GE, McGuffin P. Behavioural Genetics (Book) 5th Edition Worth Publishers; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Price TS, Simonoff E, Asherson P, Curran S, Kuntsi J, Waldman I, Plomin R. Continuity and change in preschool ADHD symptoms: longitudinal genetic analysis with contrast effects. Behav Genet. 2005 Mar;35(2):121–32. doi: 10.1007/s10519-004-1013-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Purper-Ouakil D, et al. Meta-analysis of family-based association studies between the dopamine transporter gene and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatr Genet. 2005;15(1):53–59. doi: 10.1097/00041444-200503000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rhee SH, Willcutt EG, Hartman CA, Pennington CO, DeFries FC. Test of alternative hypotheses explaining the comorbidity between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and conduct disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2008 Jan;36(1):29–40. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9157-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rhee SH, Waldman ID, Hay DA, Levy F. Sex differences in genetic and environmental influences on DSM-III-R attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 1999 Feb;108(1):24–41. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.108.1.24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ronald A, Simonoff E, Kuntsi J, Asherson P, Plomin R. Evidence for overlapping genetic influences on autistic and ADHD behaviours in a community twin sample. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008 May;49(5):535–42. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01857.x. Ronald, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reiersen AM, Constantino JN, Volk HE, Todd RD. Autistic traits in a population-based ADHD twin sample. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007 May;48(5):464–72. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01720.x. 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M. Gene-environment interdependence. Dev Sci. 2007 Jan;10(1):12–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00557.x. 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaw P, Gornick M, Lerch J, Addington A, Seal J, Greenstein D, Sharp W, Evans A, Giedd JN, Castellanos FX, Rapoport JL. Polymorphisms of the dopamine D4 receptor, clinical outcome, and cortical structure in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007 Aug;64(8):921–31. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.8.921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smalley SL, et al. Genetic linkage of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder on chromosome 16p13, in a region implicated in autism. Am J Hum Genet. 2002;71(4):959–963. doi: 10.1086/342732. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spencer TJ, et al. In vivo neuroreceptor imaging in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a focus on the dopamine transporter. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(11):1293–1300. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.03.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stawicki JA, Nigg JT, von Eye A. Family psychiatric history evidence on the nosological relations of DSM-IV ADHD combined and inattentive subtypes: new data and meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006 Sep;47(9):935–45. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01628.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor E, Sonuga-Barke E. Rutter’s Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 5th edition 2008. Disorders of Attention and Activity; pp. 521–542. [Google Scholar]
- Thapar A, Rutter M. Rutter’s Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 5th edition 2008. Genetics; pp. 339–358. [Google Scholar]
- Thapar A, Langley K, Owen MJ, O’Donovan MC. Advances in genetic findings on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychol Med. 2007a Dec;37(12):1681–92. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707000773. Epub 2007 May 17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thapar A, Harold G, Rice F, Langley K, O’donovan M. The contribution of gene-environment interaction to psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol. 2007b Fall;19(4):989–1004. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407000491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thapar A, Langley K, O’donovan M, Owen M. Refining the attention deficit hyperactivity disorder phenotype for molecular genetic studies. Mol Psychiatry. 2006 Aug;11(8):714–20. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001831. Epub 2006 May 16. Review. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thapar A, Langley K, Fowler T, Rice F, Turic D, Whittinger N, Aggleton J, Van den Bree M, Owen M, O’Donovan M. Catechol O-methyltransferase gene variant and birth weight predict early-onset antisocial behavior in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005 Nov;62(11):1275–8. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.11.1275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thapar A, Harrington R, McGuffin P. Examining the comorbidity of ADHD-related behaviours and conduct problems using a twin study design. Br J Psychiatry. 2001 Sep;179:224–9. doi: 10.1192/bjp.179.3.224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Todd RD, Huang H, Henderson CA. Predictors of stability of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder subtypes from childhood to young adulthood. J AM Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008 Jan;47(1):76–85. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e31815a6aca. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willcutt EG, Pennington BF, Olson RK, DeFries FC. Understanding comorbidity: a twin study of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2007 Sep 5;144B(6):709–14. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium Genome-wide association study of 14,000 cases of seven common diseases and 3,000 shared controls. Nature. 2007;447(7145):661–78. doi: 10.1038/nature05911. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang B, et al. A meta-analysis of association studies between the 10-repeat allele of a VNTR polymorphism in the 3′-UTR of dopamine transporter gene and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2007;144B(4):541–550. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]