Abstract
The convergent and discriminant validity of Five Factor Model (FFM) personality traits with concurrent and prospective social, work, and recreational dysfunction was assessed in a large, longitudinal clinical sample. Consistent with five factor theoretical expectations, neuroticism is broadly related to dysfunction across domains; extraversion is primarily related to social and recreational dysfunction; openness to recreational dysfunction; agreeableness to social dysfunction; and conscientiousness to work dysfunction. Findings support five factor theory and the clinical assessment of normative personality traits.
Five-Factor Model (FFM: Neuroticism = N, Extraversion = E, Openness to Experience = O; Agreeableness = A; Conscientiousness = C) personality traits have demonstrated validity in predicting a range of important behaviors such as physical health (Caspi, Roberts, & Shiner, 2005; Grucza & Goldberg, 2007; Goodwin & Friedman, 2006), social dysfunction (Bornstein & Huprich, 2006; Donnellan, Conger, & Bryant, 2004; Eisenberg, Fabes, Guthrie, & Reiser, 2000; Karney & Bradbury, 1995; Schmitz, Hartkamp, Baldini, Rollnik, & Tress, 2001; Watson, Hubbard, & Weise, 2000), work performance (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Judge, Higgins, Thoreson, & Barrick, 1999), and recreational dysfunction (Burnett, 2006; Kircaldy, 1990; Wild, Kuiken, & Schoopflocker, 1995) even after controlling for factors such as intelligence and socioeconomic status (Roberts et al., 2007) and psychiatric diagnosis (Hopwood, Morey, Shea et al., 2007). FFM traits have also demonstrated utility predicting clinical outcomes. For example, FFM traits relate to clinically relevant outcomes such as suicidal (Kerby, 2003) and aggressive behavior (Skeem, Miller, Mulvey, Tiemann, & Monahan, 2005), substance use (Hopwood, Morey, Skodol et al., 2007; Ruiz, Pincus, & Schinka, 2008), and the effectiveness of treatment for other disorders (Bottlender & Soyka, 2005). Increasing evidence for the clinical utility of the FFM has led some to argue for their routine use in clinical assessment (e.g., Costa, 2008; Widiger & Trull, 2007).
Demonstrations that FFM traits bear specific relations to domains of dysfunction predicted by five factor theory (FFT; McCrae & Costa, 1996; 1999) would strengthen the argument for including personality traits in clinical assessment. FFT proposes that the FFM traits represent the broadest level of basic tendencies in a comprehensive and universal description of human personality. These basic tendencies are thought to predispose characteristic adaptations (e.g., attitudes and self-concept). For example, the basic tendency to be agreeable may lead to adaptations such as forgiveness and an inclination to cooperate in social circumstances; the basic tendency to be open to experience may lead to adaptations such as travelling or having many hobbies (McCrae & Costa, 1999).
The functionality of characteristic adaptations is a critical concern of clinical assessment because maladaptations are often therapeutic targets. Maladaptivity can be organized into broad domains that denote when and where dysfunction occurs. For example, social dysfunction, which occurs between people, is a major potential source of stress for the clinically impaired, both because psychopathology disturbs relationships and interventions designed to ameliorate psychiatric distress are often interpersonal (i.e., occur in the context of a therapeutic relationship) or focus on outside relationships. Occupational dysfunction is an important domain because work engagement provides both material and psychological resources toward greater adaptation. Recreational dysfunction denotes the extent to which a person struggles to participate in pleasurable aspects of life, and thus potentially indicates the lack of positive, rather than the existence of negative, behaviors.
Social, occupational, and recreational dysfunction can be considered broad categories of characteristic adaptations, thus linking them to FFT. FFT supports hypotheses about specific relations between FFM traits and each of these sets of adaptations. For instance, neuroticism would be expected to globally and positively relate to all three kinds of dysfunction because it indicates generalized distress and dysfunction. As extraversion and agreeableness capture interpersonal content, they would be anticipated to have a specific and inverse relationship to social dysfunction. Extraversion should relate negatively to recreational dysfunction as well because of its association with the reward-sensitive system of personality. Openness to Experience, which taps interest in a broad domain of activities (e.g., the arts), should also negatively relate to recreational dysfunction. Finally, conscientiousness indicates responsibility and dutifulness, and thus could be expected to negatively predict occupational dysfunction.
Although initial results support the FFM in clinical predictions, these data are often not considered with regard to FFT, have rarely been longitudinal, have tended to address a limited variety of functional domains, and have often not included all five FFM traits. To address this gap, we investigated the validity of FFM traits in predicting concurrent and prospective social, occupational, and recreational dysfunction using data from the Collaborative Longitudinal Personality disorder Study (CLPS). Several previous CLPS studies have investigated the validity of FFM traits with regard to dysfunction. Skodol, Oldham, et al. (2005) showed that the FFM traits, as a group, incremented DSM-IV personality disorder symptom counts in two of seven assessed domains, whereas the personality disorders incremented FFM traits in five of seven domains. Morey et al. (2007) extended these findings by testing the validity of FFM traits, again considered as a group, to predict retrospective, concurrent, and prospective outcomes and using a wider range of outcome variables, including both interview and self-report assessed social, recreational, and occupational functioning. Importantly, Morey et al. showed that FFM predictive validity was more stable than that of personality disorders, and that, in contrast to the baseline results reported by Skodol et al., it tended to substantially increment the personality disorders in predicting prospective functioning. Hopwood, Morey, Shea, et al. (2007) showed that FFM traits, considered individually, significantly predicted concurrent and prospective GAF, whether or not participants had a personality disorder. However, no previous CLPS study has examined the relations between each of the FFM traits and specific domains of dysfunction, as a formal test of FFT-based hypotheses would require, and as the current study does.
METHOD
PARTICIPANTS
Participants were recruited from multiple clinical sites for the CLPS project (Gunderson et al., 2000). For this report, the original sample of 653 individuals with complete baseline data attenuated over time and across some measures because of attrition and incomplete data. These factors reduced the sample to 455 individuals at two years and 308 at five years. The sample included individuals with personality disorder or major depressive disorder with no personality disorder by study design, although cooccurring Axis I and II disorders were common (see McGlashan et al., 2000 for CLPS baseline diagnostic characteristics). At baseline, 62% of participants were women; 68% were Caucasian, 14% African-American, 13% Hispanic, 2% Asian, and the rest reported another or no ethnicity. Baseline ages ranged from 18 to 45, with the average age being 32.50 (SD = 8.11).
MEASURES
NEO Personality Inventory, Revised (NEO-PI-R, Costa & McCrae, 1992). The NEO-PI-R was designed to comprehensively assess the five factors of the FFM. Internal consistency reliabilities for the five domains in this sample ranged from .87 to .92.
Longitudinal Interval Follow-up Examination (LIFE; Keller et al., 1987). The LIFE is a structured interview that measures, among other variables, social, occupational, and recreational dysfunction. The occupational and recreational dysfunction markers are derived from single items. Social functioning was indexed by averaging ratings across several kinds of relationships (i.e., romantic partner, friends, and family members).
Social Adjustment Scale—Self-report (SAS-SR; Weissman & Bothwell, 1976). The SAS-SR is a 54-item self-report measure of clinically relevant functioning with scales that measure work, recreational, and social functioning. As with the LIFE, SAS-SR social functioning in this study represents a composite from scales measuring functioning in the family, with romantic relationships, and with friends. The median baseline internal consistency across SAS-SR scales was .70 in the current sample. All LIFE and SAS-SR indicators were scaled such that higher scores meant greater dysfunction.
Principal axis factor scores were computed to collapse self-report and interview measured scores for each of the three domains of dysfunction at baseline, two-year, and five-year follow-ups. To compute these scores, scales depicting the same functional domains from different instruments were factored, and the score was retained. Summing standardized factor scores across domains at each assessment interval created a global dysfunction score. The use of two measures of the same domains of dysfunction, one based on interview data and one on self-report, likely yields more reliable estimates than would observed scores on a single instrument. In particular, applying measures that used two methods limits the potential that results could be explained by shared method variance between the NEO-PI-R and self-reported dysfunction.
ANALYSES
Initial analyses were conducted to assess the influence of time on study variables. To test study hypotheses, baseline NEO-PI-R domain scores were correlated with dysfunction scores across domains of dysfunction at each assessment interval. Partial coefficients representing the contribution of each FFM score independent of the remaining four FFM scores in multiple regression models, as well as overall model validity, were then computed for each dysfunction score. Effect sizes and statistical significance at .01 and .001 type I error rates were computed for all analyses.
RESULTS
Individuals who only had baseline data had lower neuroticism scores than others and participants who provided data at all assessment intervals had higher levels of work functioning than those who only provided data through two years (both p< .01). There were no significant differences on other baseline variables.
Results depicted in Table 1 suggest that FFM traits are broadly associated with concurrent and prospective functioning in this clinical sample. Bivariate correlations between traits and functioning indicators were significant (p< .01) for 84% of nonredundant indicators (i.e., not including the overall composite) and were in the small to moderate range (Cohen, 1988). Moreover, as described in previous papers (Hopwood, Morey, Shea, et al., 2007; Morey, Hopwood, Gunderson, et al., 2007), trait validity tended to drop only slightly from baseline to five-year assessment, consistent with the notion that these traits reflect fairly stable dispositions to dysfunction.
TABLE 1.
N | E | O | A | C | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Overall | |||||
Baseline | .41** | −.34** | −.22** | −.12 | −.28** |
2 years | .31** | −.34** | −.25** | −.18** | −.27** |
5 years | .36** | −.34** | −.16* | −.12 | −.18** |
Social | |||||
Baseline | .40** | −.25** | −.12* | −.19** | −.22** |
2 years | .24** | −.28** | −.15** | −.19** | −.21** |
5 years | .32** | −.34** | −.21** | −.19** | −.21** |
Work | |||||
Baseline | .28** | −.18** | −.04 | −.02 | −.30** |
2 years | .24** | −.24** | −.10 | −.15* | −.30** |
5 years | .32** | −.20** | −.15 | −.06 | −.19** |
Recreation | |||||
Baseline | .37** | −.34** | −.28** | −.09 | −.24** |
2 years | .28** | −.32** | −.32** | −.15* | −.19** |
5 years | .25** | −.36** | −.23** | −.06 | −.15* |
Note.
p <.01;
p <.001.
Regression analyses that control for the influence of other traits clarify trait-dysfunction relations. These results, given in Table 2, show that FFM traits have differential patterns of validity that are clearly consistent with FFT. At a type I error rate of .01, the percentage of significant hypothesized regression coefficients was 79% (19/24), whereas the percentage of hypothesized nonsignificant coefficients was 90% (19/21; overall hit rate = 84%). To further assess the validity of FFM traits, we recomputed these regression coefficients after statistically controlling for the number of PD symptoms. Notably, the overall pattern of results continued to support the convergent and discriminant validity of FFM traits, even after controlling for personality pathology symptoms (results are available from the authors upon request).
TABLE 2.
N | E | O | A | C | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
R2 | Partial Coefficients | |||||
Overall | ||||||
Baseline | .24 | .32** | −.14** | −.16** | −.08** | |
2 years | .20 | .19** | −.19** | −.17** | −.10* | |
5 years | .19 | .29** | −.20** | |||
Social | ||||||
Baseline | .19 | .34** | −.11* | |||
2 years | .12 | .15* | −.18** | |||
5 years | .19 | .22** | −.19** | −.12* | −.13* | |
Work | ||||||
Baseline | .12 | .17** | −.21** | |||
2 years | .13 | −.14* | −.20** | |||
5 years | .13 | .30** | ||||
Recreation | ||||||
Baseline | .23 | .29** | −.14** | −.23** | ||
2 years | .20 | .22** | −.16** | −.26** | ||
5 years | .16 | −.27** | −.14* |
Notes.
Grayed areas were predicted to be significant.
p < .01;
p < .001.
With regard to the validity of specific FFM traits, higher neuroticism was generally and moderately associated with worse functioning. It correlated significantly with every indicator of dysfunction and incremented the other traits for all but two: 2-year work and 5-year recreational dysfunction.
Extraversion was negatively and specifically linked to social and recreational dysfunction. Like neuroticism, this trait was moderately correlated with dysfunction indicators, though relations were smaller with work functioning and all were negative. Further, these relations tended to remain significant, though small to moderate, when controlling other traits in models predicting global, social, and recreational dysfunction. In contrast, two of three coefficients were no longer significant in work dysfunction models.
Openness to experience was moderately and negatively associated with dysfunction in general, and was somewhat specifically associated with recreational dysfunction. Correlations between openness and dysfunction were in the moderate range for recreational dysfunction, whereas other correlations were small. In regression models predicting recreational dysfunction, all three recreation coefficients remained significant. Conversely, in models predicting work and social dysfunction, five of six openness coefficients were not significant.
Agreeableness demonstrated small and uniformly negative correlations to dysfunction, suggesting it was the least predictive of the four traits. It did not increment the other traits in models predicting work and recreational functioning. However, the association of agreeableness with social dysfunction was consistent, if small, before and after controlling the influence of other traits.
Conscientiousness was negatively and specifically related to work dysfunction. Again, bivariate correlations were significant in suggesting a general, negative relation of this trait to dysfunction. However, all but three of these relations remained significant in regression analyses controlling for the influence of other traits; two of these were in models predicting baseline and two-year work dysfunction.
DISCUSSION
The specific relations of personality traits to functional domains constitute an important clinical concern, and the correspondence of these relations to theoretical hypotheses is an important test of five-factor theory. The current study supports FFT in suggesting that FFM traits can predict concurrent and prospective characteristic maladaptations in fairly specific ways. In particular, neuroticism positively relates to global dysfunction, extraversion negatively predicts social and recreational dysfunction, openness is negatively associated with recreational dysfunction, agreeableness negatively predicts social dysfunction, and conscientiousness is negatively related to work dysfunction.
These findings reinforce recommendations to weave personality trait assessment into the diagnostic system (e.g., Krueger, Skodol, Livesley, Shrout, & Huang, 2007; Widiger & Trull, 2007) by demonstrating that it can provide specific information about patient functioning that might yield pragmatic clinical predictions. If a patient is extremely vulnerable and distressed, as indicated by a high neuroticism score, the clinician should expect pervasive difficulties in a range of areas, although they may also have certain strengths associated with elevations on one or more of the other four traits. A disagreeable and introverted patient will most likely have functional difficulties related to social behavior. Introverts may also demonstrate recreational difficulties. A patient with a low conscientiousness score may have difficulties at work but fare relatively well socially and engage in a variety of enjoyable, if poorly-planned, behaviors. The patient who is closed to experience may lead an overly-structured life devoid of leisure, but work effectively and maintain an adequate social network.
The current findings also suggest treatment targets and perhaps a framework for treatment matching in practice and research (Anderson, 1998; Singer, 2005). Therapies designed to treat individuals with the same diagnosis often target dysfunction in a particular domain. For example, interpersonal therapies (e.g., Benjamin, 1993; Klerman, Weissman, Rounsaville, & Chevron, 1984) focus on social behavior, cognitive-behavioral therapy (Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979) on internal thoughts and behaviors, and pharmacology on symptomatic aspects of functioning. Results from this study may suggest individuals with varying personality styles could differentially benefit from treatments that target the manner in which their psychopathology is expressed (Barber & Muenz, 1996; Horowitz, Rosenberg, & Bartholomew, 1993; Pincus et al., 2005). Therapist variables may also interact with patient personality to predict outcome, independent of treatment technique (e.g., Tracey, 1993). Future research should continue to explore the relevance of FFM personality traits for treatment matching.
Future research also needs to test finer-grained aspects of dysfunction. Although the current study extends previous investigations by depicting relationships between three broad domains of living and the FFM in a large and longitudinal clinical sample, this study was limited insofar as each of these domains comprises an array of more specific areas of dysfunction. For example, it is possible that the predictive validity of traits for work performance interacts with the kind of work being done (Barrick & Mount, 1991). Five factor theory has linked specific characteristic adaptations to basic personality tendencies (i.e., FFM traits), providing a theoretical foundation for such future investigations (McCrae & Costa, 1996, 1999).
Research should also continue to investigate the optimal integration of FFM data with other diagnostic information. Current views on the relevance of normative traits in clinical assessment vary, even among researchers who support consideration of the FFM. For instance, those who see personality disorders as maladaptive variants of FFM traits suggest replacing the DSM personality disorders with the FFM (e.g., Costa, 2008; Widiger & Trull, 2007). This view is buttressed by evidence that personality disorders are associated with varying constellations of FFM traits (Samuel & Widiger, 2008). Others see FFM traits and DSM personality disorders as different (Skodol, Gunderson, et al., 2005). This view is supported by findings that (a) traits are more stable than personality disorders (Morey et al., 2007); (b) traits and disorders increment one another in the prediction of functioning, with traits being particularly incremental for prospective functioning (Morey et al., 2007); (c) whereas trait change predict changes in personality disorder severity, changes in personality disorders do not lead to changes in traits (Lenzenweger & Willett, 2007; Warner et al., 2004); and (d) disorders such as borderline personality can be differentiated from others based on their trait instability (Hopwood et al., in press). These alternative views have important implications for clinical practice: If traits and personality disorders are the same, then assessment should only comprise one or the other, whereas if they are different and both are important, assessment should comprise both. Importantly, results from this study support consideration of FFM traits in clinical assessment, regardless of one’s views about the relationship of the FFM with personality disorders.
Acknowledgments
Funding for this study was provided by NIMH Grants MH 50837, 50838, 50839, 50840, 50850; MH75543 (Hopwood); MH01654; (McGlashan); MH073708 (Sanislow).
REFERENCES
- Anderson KW. Utility of the five-factor model of personality in psychotherapy aptitude-treatment interaction research. Psychotherapy Research. 1998;8:54–70. [Google Scholar]
- Barber JP, Muenz LR. The role of avoidance and obsessiveness in matching patients to cognitive and interpersonal psychotherapy: Empirical findings from the treatment for de pression collaborative research program. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1996;64:951–958. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.64.5.951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrick MR, Mount MK. The big-five personality dimensions and job performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology. 1991;44:1–26. [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, Rush AJ, Shaw BF, Emery G. Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford; 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Benjamin LS. Interpersonal diagnosis and treatment of personality disorders. New York: The Guilford Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Bornstein RF, Huprich SK. Construct validity of the relationship profile test: Three-year retest reliability and links with core personality traits, object relations, and interpersonal problems. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2006;86:162–171. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa8602_05. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bottlender M, Soyka M. Impact of different personality dimensions (NEO-five factor inventory) on the outcome of alcohol-dependent patients 6-and 12-months after treatment. Psychiatry Research. 2005;136:61–67. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burnett LA. Accounting for leisure preferences from within: Relative contributions of gender, race or ethnicity, personality, affective style, and motivational orientation. Journal of Leisure Research. 2006;38:445–474. [Google Scholar]
- Caspi A, Roberts BW, Shiner R. Personality development: Stability and change. Annual Review of Psychology. 2005;56:453–484. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen JJ. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Costa PT., Jr . Just do it: Replace Axis II with a diagnostic system based on the five-factor model of personality. In: Widiger TA, Simonsen E, Sirovatka PJ, Regier DA, editors. Dimensional models of personality disoroders: Refining the research agenda for DSM-V. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2008. pp. 195–198. [Google Scholar]
- Costa PT, Jr, McCrae RR. NEO Personality Inventory Revised professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Donnellan MB, Conger RD, Bryant CM. The big five and enduring marriages. Journal of Research in Personality. 2004;38:481–504. [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Guthrie IK, Reiser M. Dispositional emotionality and regulation: their role in predicting quality of social functioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;78:136–157. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.1.136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodwin RD, Friedman HS. Health status and personality traits in nationally-representative sample. Journal of Health Psychology. 2006;11:643–654. doi: 10.1177/1359105306066610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grucza RA, Goldberg LA. The comparative validity of 11 modern personality inventories: Predictions of be, havioral acts, informant reports, and clinical indicators. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2007;89:167–187. doi: 10.1080/00223890701468568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunderson JG, Shea MT, Skodol AE, McGlashan TH, Morey LC, Stout RL, et al. The collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study: Development, aims, design, and sample characteristics. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2000;14:300–315. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2000.14.4.300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopwood CJ, Morey LC, Skodol AE, Stout RL, Yen S, Ansell EB, et al. Five-vactor model personality traits associated with alcohol-related diagnoses in a clinical sample. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2007;68:455–460. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2007.68.455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopwood CJ, Morey LC, Shea MT, McGlashan TH, Sanislow CA, Grilo CM, et al. Personality traits predict current and future functioning comparably for individuals with major depressive and personality disorders. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2007;195:266–269. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000253747.44836.61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopwood CJ, Newman DA, Markowitz JC, Grilo CM, Sanislow CA, McGlashan TH, et al. The stability of personality traits in individuals with borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. doi: 10.1037/a0016954. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horowitz LM, Rosenberg SE, Barthlomew K. Interpersonal problems, attachment styles, and outcome in brief dynamic psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1993;61:549–560. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.61.4.549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Judge TA, Higgins CA, Thoreson CJ, Barrick MR. The big five personality traits, general mental ability, and career success across the life span. Personnel Psychology. 1999;52:621–652. [Google Scholar]
- Karney BR, Bradbury TN. The longitudinal course of marital quality and stability: A review of theory, method, and research. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;118:3–34. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.118.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keller MB, Lavori PW, Friedman B, Nielsen E, Endicott J, McDonald-Scott P, Andreason NC. The longitudinal interval follow-up evaluation: A comprehensive method for assessing outcome in prospective longitudinal studies. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1987;44:540–548. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800180050009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kerby DS. CART analysis with unit-weighted regression to predict suicidal ideation from big five traits. Personality and Individual Differences. 2003;35:249–261. [Google Scholar]
- Kircaldy BD. Gender and personality determinants of recreational interrest. Studia Psychologica. 1990;62:115–121. [Google Scholar]
- Klerman G, Weissman MM, Rounsaville B, Chevron E. Interpersonal psychotherapy of depression. New York: Basic Books; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Krueger RG, Skodol AE, Livesley WJ, Shrout PE, Huang Y. Synthesizing categorical and dimensional approaches to personality disorders: Refining the research agenda for DSM-V, Axis II. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 2007;16:65–73. doi: 10.1002/mpr.212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lenzenweger MF, Willett JF. Predicting individual change in personality disorder features by simultaneous individual change in personality dimensions linked to neurobehavioral systems: The longitudinal study of personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2007;116:684–700. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.4.684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCrae RR, Costa PT., Jr . Toward a new generation of personality theories: Theoretical contexts for the five-factor model. In: Wiggins J, editor. The five-factor model of personality: Theoretical perspectives. New York: Guilford Press; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- McCrae RR, Costa PT., Jr . A five factor theory of personality. In: Pervin LA, John OP, editors. Handbook of Personality. 2nd ed. New York: The Guilford Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- McGlashan TH, Grilo CM, Skodol AE, Gunderson JG, Shea MT, Morey LC, Zanarini MC, Stout RL. The collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study: Baseline axis I/II diagnostic co-occurence. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2000;102:256–264. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2000.102004256.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morey LC, Hopwood CJ, Gunderson JG, Skodol AE, Shea MT, Yen S, et al. A comparison of personality disorder models. Psychological Medicine. 2007;37:983–994. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706009482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pincus AL, Przeworski A, Yamasaki A, Kasoff MB, Newman MG, Caston-guay LG, Borkovec TD. Interpersonal pathoplasticity in generalized anxiety disorder; Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Interpersonal Theory and Research (SITAR); 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts BW, Kuncel NR, Shiner R, Caspi A, Goldberg LR. The power of personality: The comparative validity of personality traits, socioeconomic status, and cognitive ability for predicting important life outcomes. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2007;2:313–345. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00047.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruiz A, Pincus A, Schinka J. Externalizing disorders and the fivefactor model. A meta-analysis of personality traits associated with antisocial personality disorder, substance abuse disorders and their co-occurance. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2008;22:365–388. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2008.22.4.365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samuel DB, Widiger TA. A meta-analytic review of the relationships between the five-factor model and DSM-IV-TR personality disorders: A facet level analysis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2008;28:1326–1342. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmitz N, Hartkamp N, Baldini C, Rollnik J, Tress W. Psychometric properties of the german version of the NEO-FFI in psychosomatic outpatients. Personailty and Individual Differences. 2001;31:713–722. [Google Scholar]
- Singer JA. Personality and psychotherapy: Treating the whole person. New York: The Guilford Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Skeem JL, Miller JD, Mulvey ED, Tiemann J, Monahan J. Using a five-factor lens to explore the relations between personality traits and violence in psychiatric patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2005;73:454–465. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skodol AE, Gunderson JG, Shea MT, McGlashan TH, Morey LC, Sanislow CA, et al. The collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study (CLPS): Overview and implications. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2005;19:487–504. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2005.19.5.487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skodol AE, Oldham JM, Bender DS, Dyck IR, Stout RL, Morey LC, et al. Dimensional representations of DSM-IV personality disorders: Relationships to functional impairment. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162:1919–1925. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tracey TJ. An interpersonal stage model of therapeutic process. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 1993;40:396–409. [Google Scholar]
- Warner MB, Morey LC, Finch JF, Gunderson JG, Skodol AE, Sanislow CA. The longitudinal relationship of personality traits and disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2004;113:217–227. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.2.217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson D, Hubbard B, Wiese D. General traits of personality and affectivity as predictors of satisfaction in intimate relationships. Evidence from self- and partner-ratings. Journal of Personality. 2000;68:413–449. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weissman M, Bothwell S. The assessment of social adjustment by patient self-report. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1976;33:1111–1115. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1976.01770090101010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Widiger TA, Trull TJ. Plate tectonics in the classification of personality disorder: Shifting to a dimensional model. American Psychologist. 2007;62:71–83. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.62.2.71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wild TC, Kuiken D, Schoopflocker D. The role of absorption in experiential involvement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1995;69:569–579. [Google Scholar]