Abstract
On average, language and communication characteristics of individuals with Down syndrome (the most common genetic cause of intellectual disability) follow a consistent profile. Despite considerable individual variability, receptive language is typically stronger than expressive language, with particular challenges in phonology and syntax. We review the literature on language and literacy skills of individuals with Down syndrome, with emphasis on the areas of phonology, vocabulary, syntax, and pragmatics. We begin by describing the hearing, oral-motor, cognitive, social, and prelinguistic and early nonverbal communication characteristics of individuals with Down syndrome. We conclude with a discussion of clinical implications and research directions.
Keywords: Communication, Down syndrome, Language, Literacy, Neurodevelopmental Disorders, Speech
Despite considerable individual variability, individuals with Down syndrome have a characteristic profile of language and communication strengths and difficulties. Receptive language is typically stronger than expressive language, with phonology, syntax, and some aspects of pragmatics presenting particular developmental challenges. In this article, we review the research on phonology, vocabulary, syntax, pragmatics, and literacy skills of individuals with Down syndrome. We focus on receptive and expressive language in individuals of all ages. We begin by describing several foundations of language and communication development, including hearing, oral-motor, cognitive, social, and prelinguistic and early nonverbal communication skills. We conclude by describing implications for practice and directions for research.
Since Down syndrome is a genetic disorder, the following review is largely consistent with a categorical, medical model of language development. Accordingly, we aim to show how language and literacy competence may be affected by the cognitive-behavioral phenotype associated with a diagnosis of Down syndrome. However, we recognize the theoretical and practical importance of other prevalent models such as the social model, which stresses the role of social or environmental factors in language learning. Indeed, we consider social skills to be one of the foundations of language and communication development. Moreover, our sections on assessment and intervention are more socially-situated, with attention to family-centered and integrated service models. For further discussion of language development models, see Paul (2007), Hixson (1993), and Staskowski and Rivera (2005).
ETIOLOGY OF DOWN SYNDROME
Down syndrome is the most common genetic cause of intellectual disability, occurring in approximately 1 in 700 live births (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2006). Ninety-eight percent of cases of Down syndrome are caused by an extra copy of chromosome 21 (Trisomy 21). Translocation, occurring when part of chromosome 21 attaches to another chromosome, and mosaicism, occurring when some cells—but not all—include an extra copy of chromosome 21, are less common. A person of any race, socioeconomic status, or geographic location can have a child with Down syndrome. The only etiological factor undoubtedly linked to Down syndrome is increasing maternal age (Hassold & Sherman, 2002). See Patterson and Lott (2008) for further details about etiology.
FOUNDATIONS OF LANGUAGE AND COMMUNICATION
For all individuals, language and communication skills are related to skills in other areas of development. In this section, we review the literature on hearing, oral-motor, cognitive, social, and prelinguistic and early nonverbal communication skills of individuals with Down syndrome. The areas discussed represent a subset of domains that have a potential impact on language development. We chose to focus on these domains given their relevance for individuals with Down syndrome, the breadth of literature available for review, and limits on article length. However, our choice should not be construed as a claim that other areas are not important. For instance, problem-solving or attention can clearly have important effects on language and communication competence even though they are not covered in the section on cognitive skills.
Hearing skills
Approximately two-thirds of children with Down syndrome experience conductive hearing loss, sensorineural hearing loss, or both (Roizen, 2007). Hearing loss can affect one or both ears and range from mild to profound (Roizen, Wolters, Nicol, & Blondis, 1993). Otitis media is one cause of mild to moderate fluctuating conductive hearing loss when accompanied by middle ear fluid. Children with Down syndrome may be particularly susceptible to otitis media, possibly due to narrow auditory canals and cranial facial differences seen in this population (Roizen, 2007). Otitis media has been found to occur in 96% of young children with Down syndrome, with 83% requiring tympanotomy tubes (Shott, Joseph, & Heithaus, 2001). Whereas the association between early otitis media with effusion (OME) and language development is mild at best for typically developing children, OME-associated hearing loss possibly presents an extra risk factor for children with Down syndrome because they are already at risk for language difficulties (American Academy of Pediatrics [AAP], 2004; Roberts et al., 2004). In fact, hearing loss is related concurrently to difficulties in comprehension of grammatical morphemes and vocabulary for individuals with Down syndrome (Miolo, Chapman, & Sindberg, 2005; Chapman, Schwartz, & Kay-Raining Bird, 1991). However, data on the early effects of OME on language development of children with Down syndrome are lacking. Longitudinal studies are needed to address the impact of OME during the early years of development on later language performance for this population.
Oral-motor skills
Speech production of individuals with Down syndrome may be related to differences in oral structure and function (Miller & Leddy, 1998; Stoel-Gammon, 1997). Structural differences include a small oral cavity with a relatively large tongue and a narrow, high arched palate. Missing, poorly differentiated, or additional muscles characterize facial structures, and differences in nerve innervation have been found as well (Miller & Leddy, 1998). These differences are thought to account, in part, for poor speech intelligibility through dysarthric factors such as reduced speed, range of motion, and coordination of the articulators. In addition, compared with typically developing children, boys with Down syndrome show differences in the structure of the lips, tongue, and velopharynx, and are less skilled at speech motor functions and coordinated speech movements involving the lips, tongue, velopharynx, and larynx (Barnes, Roberts, Mirrett, Sideris, & Misenheimer, 2006). Symptoms of childhood apraxia of speech also have been reported (Rupela & Manjula, 2007; Kumin, 2006; Kumin & Adams, 2000).
Cognitive skills
About 80% of individuals with Down syndrome have moderate intellectual disability, although some have severe intellectual disability and others have IQ scores in the average range (Pueschel, 1995; Roizen, 2007). Visuo-spatial processing and perception are generally viewed as relative strengths in individuals with Down syndrome (Fidler, Hepburn, & Rogers, 2006; Jarrold, Baddeley, & Hewes, 1999; Klein & Mervis, 1999). Although visual long-term memory appears to be impaired (Jarrold, Baddeley, & Phillips, 2007), this impairment may be restricted to visual-object learning tasks, and visual-spatial tasks may constitute an area of relative strength (Vicari, Bellucci, & Carlesimo, 2005). Substantial evidence points to verbal short-term memory impairments that cannot be explained by hearing loss or speech problems (Jarrold & Baddeley, 2001; Laws, 2002). Impaired phonological memory skills (measured with nonword repetition) may be associated with poorer language comprehension, reduced mean length of utterance (MLU), and reading difficulty in children and adolescents with Down syndrome (Laws, 1998, 2004).
Social skills
Social interaction is considered by many to be an important precursor to language acquisition. Social skills appear to be commensurate with mental age in the early development of young children (0-4 years) with Down syndrome (Dykens, Hodapp, & Evans, 1994). Children with Down syndrome are typically characterized as highly social, engaging, and affectionate (Moore, Oates, Hobson, et al., 2002; Wishart & Johnston, 1990). In fact, socialization and daily living skills appear to be relative strengths compared with communication (Dykens, Hodapp, & Evans, 2006; Fidler, Hepburn, & Rogers, 2006). Children with Down syndrome form interpersonal relationships in much the same way as typically developing peers (Freeman & Kasari, 2002), and adults with Down syndrome appear to show lower aggression and antisocial behavior than other adults with learning disabilities (Collacott, Cooper, Branford, & McGrother, 1998).
On the other hand, social skills may be impaired for some individuals with Down syndrome. Older studies indicate that at least 5%–7% of individuals with Down syndrome meet diagnostic criteria for autism (Ghaziuddin, Tsai, & Ghaziuddin, 1992; Kent, Evans, Paul, & Sharp, 1999). More recently, Hepburn, Philofsky, Fidler, and Rogers (2008) found that 15% of young children with Down syndrome met criteria for an autism spectrum disorder. Behavioral and psychiatric disorders, such as depression, appear to be less common in Down syndrome than in other types of intellectual disability; however, these difficulties may be more common in individuals with Down syndrome than in the general population (Antonarakis & Epstein, 2006; Roizen & Patterson, 2003). Moreover, social skills may decline and maladaptive behaviors increase with the common onset of dementia in adults with Down syndrome, which occurs at high rates at ages 45 years and older (Urv, Zigman, & Silverman, 2008).
Prelinguistic vocal development and early nonverbal communication skills
The frequency and variety of consonants and vowels produced, as well as the age of onset of repeated consonant-vowel combinations (reduplicated, canonical babbling), are reportedly similar for infants with Down syndrome and typically developing infants (Dodd, 1972; Smith & Oller, 1981). The onset of canonical babbling, however, was found to be delayed by about two months in a longitudinal study of children with Down syndrome (Lynch et al., 1995). Children with Down syndrome seem to use gestures at a comparable rate as language-matched children (Iverson, Longobardi, & Caselli, 2003), although the communicative functions of the gestures may differ (Mundy, Kasari, Sigman, & Ruskin, 1995; Mundy, Sigman, Kasari, and Yirmiya, 1988). Specifically, Mundy and colleagues (1988) reported that 18–48 month-old children with Down syndrome produced fewer nonverbal requests for objects or for help with objects than mental age-matched typically developing children. In fact, more frequent nonverbal requests for objects or for repetition of events may be associated with better expressive language development later in children with Down syndrome (Mundy et al., 1995). Requests for shared attention, however, may constitute another area of relative strength. The frequency of indicating gestures (e.g., pointing to toys within reach, showing) is reportedly similar for young children with Down syndrome and mental age-matched typically developing children (Mundy et al., 1988).
LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT
Expressive language skills present particular challenges and generally are more impaired than receptive skills in young individuals with Down syndrome (Caselli et al., 1998; Chapman, Hesketh, & Kistler, 2002; Laws & Bishop, 2003). In this section, we describe the receptive and expressive language of children, adolescents, and young adults with Down syndrome in the areas of phonology, vocabulary, syntax, and pragmatics.
Phonology
In their preschool and school-age years, children with Down syndrome exhibit phonological errors as a common feature. Although errors resemble those made by younger typically developing children (Dodd, 1976; Rosin et al., 1988), inconsistency of errors may be particularly characteristic of the phonological disorder in Down syndrome (Dodd & Thompson, 2001). Furthermore, children with Down syndrome continue to use phonological processes (systematic sound error and simplification patterns) for longer periods than typically developing children would do (Bleile & Schwartz, 1984; Dodd, 1976; Roberts et al., 2005; Smith & Stoel-Gammon, 1983; Stoel-Gammon, 1980). Based on a standardized test of single word articulation, Roberts and colleagues (2005) found that boys with Down syndrome produced fewer consonants correctly and more syllable structure phonological processes (e.g., cluster reduction, final consonant deletion) than younger typically developing boys of similar nonverbal mental age. Barnes et al. (in press) reported similar findings for connected speech samples.
A consistent finding is that children with Down syndrome have poorer speech intelligibility than younger typically developing children of similar nonverbal mental age (Chapman, Seung, Schwartz, & Kay-Raining Bird, 1998; Barnes et al., in press). In fact, nearly all individuals with Down syndrome may be difficult to understand at least some of the time (Kumin, 1994). The phonological factors described above, in addition to factors such as apraxia of speech, dysarthria, and voice quality, may impact speech intelligibility. It is also possible that poor speech intelligibility affects productive language performance (Bray & Woolnough, 1988; Miller & Leddy, 1998), which could help to explain the discrepancy between expressive and receptive language levels seen in individuals with Down syndrome. For example, Bray and Woolnough (1988) reported that intelligibility decreased with increasing syntactic complexity for 11 children with Down syndrome. Perhaps using a simpler sentence structure and communicating mainly via key words is a more successful strategy for children with Down syndrome, making syntactic simplification an adaptive process. Price and Kent (2008) offer more details about speech intelligibility in individuals with Down syndrome.
Vocabulary
Conflicting findings have been reported regarding the receptive vocabulary skills of individuals with Down syndrome. Several studies using standardized assessments suggest that children and adolescents with Down syndrome comprehend spoken words at levels similar to mental age-matched typically developing children (Chapman et al., 1991; Miller, 1995; Laws & Bishop, 2003). In one study, vocabulary comprehension exceeded nonverbal cognitive ability for adolescents and young adults with Down syndrome (Glenn & Cunningham, 2005). However, in several other studies, children with Down syndrome scored lower than younger nonverbal mental age-matched typically developing children on standardized measures of receptive vocabulary (Caselli, Monaco, Trasciani, & Vicari, 2008; Hick, Botting, & Conti-Ramsden, 2005; Price, Roberts, Vandergrift, & Martin, 2007; Roberts, Price et al., 2007). Future research should determine whether conflicting findings are due to the different measures used or to variations in the ages and, possibly, to the hearing status of participants.
The acquisition of first words is delayed in children with Down syndrome, and subsequent growth in early expressive vocabulary is slow compared with expectations for typically developing children (Berglund, Eriksson, & Johansson, 2001; Caselli et al., 1998; Mervis & Robinson, 2000). For example, only 12% of one year-old Swedish children with Down syndrome included in a large-scale survey study produced at least one word (Berglund et al., 2001). The prognosis for spoken language acquisition is good, however. Ninety percent of three year-olds and 94% of five year-olds produced one or more words (with 73% of five year-olds having 50 or more words).
Some evidence suggests that the productive vocabularies of adolescents with Down syndrome are similar to those of younger typically developing children matched for nonverbal mental age (Laws & Bishop, 2003). Still, children with Down syndrome scored lower than mental age-matched typically developing children on standardized assessments of expressive vocabulary in several studies (Caselli et al., 2008; Hick et al., 2005; Roberts, Price et al., 2007). Additionally, Chapman and colleagues (1998) found that children and adolescents with Down syndrome (ages 5-20 years) produced fewer total and a smaller number of different words during connected speech (conversation and narration) than nonverbal mental age-matched typically developing children. In summary, most evidence suggests that expressive vocabulary is delayed beyond expectations for mental age in young individuals with Down syndrome.
Syntax
Syntax appears to present a particular linguistic challenge for individuals with Down syndrome. Several studies have shown that it is more impaired than vocabulary in both receptive and expressive domains (Abbeduto et al., 2003; Berglund & Eriksson, 2000; Chapman et al., 1991; Laws & Bishop, 2003; Berglund et al., 2001). Syntax comprehension skills of children, adolescents, and young adults with Down syndrome are lower than expected given nonverbal cognitive ability (Abbeduto et al., 2003; Caselli et al., 2008; Chapman et al., 1991; Joffe & Varlokosta, 2007; Laws & Bishop, 2003; Price et al., 2007; Rosin, Swift, Bless, & Vetter, 1988). Price and colleagues (2007) reported that a group of 45 boys with Down syndrome scored lower on comprehension of grammatical morphology (prepositions and bound morphemes) and syntax (e.g., active or passive voice, direct or indirect objects) than younger typically developing boys of similar nonverbal mental age. Moreover, syntax comprehension may be characterized by slower growth or decline in late adolescence and early adulthood (Chapman et al., 2002; Laws & Gunn, 2004).
Considerable evidence points to productive syntax deficits in young individuals with Down syndrome that cannot be explained by cognitive level. The emergence of two-word combinations is delayed in young children with Down syndrome (Iverson et al., 2003), and children and adolescents with Down syndrome continue to produce shorter and less complex utterances than typically developing children of the same nonverbal mental age as they get older (Caselli et al., 2008; Chapman et al., 1998; Price et al., 2008; Rosin et al., 1988). For example, Price and colleagues (2008) found that a group of 31 boys with Down syndrome produced less complex noun phrases, verb phrases, sentence structures, and less complex questions and negations during conversation with an examiner than younger typically developing boys of similar nonverbal mental age. In addition, production of grammatical morphemes is impaired in individuals with Down syndrome compared to MLU-matched typically developing children (Eadie, Fey, Douglas, & Parsons, 2002; Hesketh & Chapman, 1998). Specifically, Eadie and colleagues (2002) reported that children with Down syndrome scored lower than typically developing children on tense (past tense –ed, third person singular –s, and modals) and non-tense (articles, present progressive -ing) morphemes. Importantly, unlike syntax comprehension, MLU and syntax complexity may not plateau for individuals with Down syndrome, but may continue to grow into late adolescence and young adulthood, making expressive syntax a fertile area for intervention (Chapman et al., 2002; Thordardottir, Chapman, & Wagner, 2002).
Pragmatics
With the exception of requests, children with Down syndrome use the same variety of communicative functions (i.e., comments, answers, and protests) as younger typically developing children matched for language or developmental level (Beeghly, Weiss-Perry, & Cicchetti, 1990; Coggins, Carpenter, & Owings, 1983). Children with Down syndrome may stay on topic for a similar number of exchanges as mental age-matched children (Tannock, 1988), and for even more turns than younger MLU-matched children (Beeghly et al., 1990). On the other hand, some areas of pragmatics may benefit from intervention. Children with Down syndrome may initiate topics less often than younger mental age-matched children (Tannock, 1988). In addition, Abbeduto and Hesketh (1997) pointed out that measures of topic maintenance should not be limited to measures of topic length, which may contain minimal contributions, but also reflect the quality of contributions to the topic. More recently, Roberts, Martin, et al. (2007) reported that a group of 29 boys with Down syndrome were less elaborative when maintaining topics, and produced more turns that were simply adequate in quality (e.g., simple responses and acknowledgments), than younger typically developing boys of similar nonverbal mental age.
Adolescents with Down syndrome may be less likely than typically developing children of the same mental age to signal noncomprehension of another’s message. Abbeduto et al. (2008) found that adolescents with Down syndrome signaled noncomprehension less often than typically developing children matched on nonverbal mental age during a task that required participants to request clarification or additional information. Another study showed that adolescents and young adults with Down syndrome expressed messages that were less clear when describing novel shapes during a non-face-to-face task than mental age-matched typically developing children (Abbeduto et al., 2006). However, responding to requests for clarification may be an area of strength. In an early study of four children with Down syndrome, Coggins and Stoel-Gammon (1982) found that all four responded to all clarification requests in order to repair communication breakdowns.
Conveying the content of stories may be another relative strength for individuals with Down syndrome when using visual supports. Boudreau and Chapman (2000) reported that children, adolescents, and young adults with Down syndrome (ages 12-26 years) included a similar number of plot elements in their narratives of a wordless film as mental age-matched typically developing children. Moreover, young individuals with Down syndrome may recall more plot components and theme references than typically developing children matched on MLU or expressive language level (Boudreau & Chapman, 2000; Miles & Chapman, 2002). With only auditory presentation of stories, however, adolescents with Down syndrome have been found to recall less information than younger mental age-matched children (Kay-Raining Bird, Chapman, & Schwartz, 2004). This discrepancy in findings is likely related to the strengths in visual processing discussed previously. Even with visual support, young individuals with Down syndrome use fewer cohesive devices (“intersentential connections”) than mental age-matched children (but not fewer than language-matched children) (Boudreau & Chapman, 2000).
In summary, young individuals with Down syndrome display a complex profile of strengths and weaknesses in pragmatic aspects of language. Challenges may include initiation and elaboration of topics, initiation of communicative repairs, and some linguistic aspects of narratives. Strengths tend to include use of a variety of communicative functions, ability to stay on topic, responses to requests for clarification, and storytelling with sufficient content when visual supports are used.
LITERACY DEVELOPMENT
Little is known about the percentage of individuals with Down syndrome who learn to read and the level of mastery they achieve. Many are able to develop some degree of literacy with guidance and exposure (Byrne, MacDonald, & Buckley, 2002; Kay-Raining Bird, Cleave & McConnell 2000; Kay-Raining Bird, Cleave, White, Pike, & Helmkay, 2008; Shepperdson, 1994). Still, many others do not master reading. Accordingly, most available evidence focuses on emergent skills, such as phonological awareness and single word-decoding.
Relationships between literacy, language, and cognition
The ability to extract meaning from text is dependent on the possession of lexical and syntactic knowledge (Catts & Kamhi, 1999). Consequently, oral language skills have been shown to predict performance on a variety of literacy measures in typically developing children (Paul, Murray, Clancy, & Andrews, 1997; Scarborough & Dobrich, 1994; Storch & Whitehurst, 2002) and in children and adolescents with Down syndrome (Boudreau, 2002). Therefore, receptive and expressive language impairments (and areas of relative strength) are likely to extend to reading and writing skills. In children and adolescents with Down syndrome, receptive vocabulary skills predict performance on both word attack (phonological decoding) and word identification measures (Kay-Raining Bird et al., 2000) and measures of written language (Kay-Raining Bird et al., 2008). The relationship between language and literacy may be bi-directional; some evidence suggests that literacy acquisition may foster language development in children with Down syndrome by playing to their relative visual strengths (Buckley, 2003; Laws, Buckley, Bird, MacDonald & Broadley, 1995; Laws & Gunn, 2002). For example, Laws and Gunn (2002) found that early word reading skills of individuals with Down syndrome significantly correlated with MLU five years later.
Cardoso-Martins, Peterson, Olson, and Pennington (2009) administered a variety of reading measures (including single word recognition, reading comprehension, pseudoword reading accuracy, and single word spelling) to 20 adolescents with Down syndrome and found that average reading ability was lower than what would be predicted by IQ, although skill level varied among participants. However, other evidence suggests that children with Down syndrome have reading abilities above expectations given cognitive ability. Byrne, Buckley, Macdonald and Bird (1995) found that a group of 24 children with Down syndrome performed significantly more poorly on IQ measures compared with typically developing children matched on reading age. In other words, the children with Down syndrome were at the same reading level as younger typically developing children, despite significantly lower IQ scores.
Emergent literacy skills
Emergent literacy skills, including word identification skills in particular, have been identified as a relative strength for individuals with Down syndrome. In fact, not only do young individuals with Down syndrome perform similarly on measures of print, letter sounds, and letter identification as mental age-matched typically developing children, they perform significantly better on word identification tasks (Boudreau, 2002). Moreover, they perform significantly better than reading-matched typically developing children on frequent-word reading tasks (Cardoso-Martins, Michalick, & Pollo, 2002). This relative strength in word identification may be a function of individuals with Down syndrome being older than typically developing children who were matched for reading ability and therefore having had more exposure to common words (Fidler, Most & Guiberson, 2005). It is also consistent with the research that has supported relative strengths in the area of vocabulary knowledge (e.g., Glenn & Cunningham, 2005).
Strength in word identification has been linked to whole-word reading processes in individuals with Down syndrome. When compared with typically developing reading age-matched children (matched on number of words read correctly and rapidly), individuals with Down syndrome perform similarly on tasks requiring recognition of orthographic patterns, but perform more poorly on nonword reading tasks, suggesting that individuals with Down syndrome rely on whole-word (versus phonological) processes in word-decoding (Verucci, Menghini, & Vicari, 2006). Whole-word versus phonological reading has been much debated in Down syndrome literacy research. The hypothesis that children with Down syndrome rely on whole-word processes to decode words was first introduced by Buckley (1985). This hypothesis has since been well-documented (see Boudreau, 2002; Cardoso-Martins et al., 2002; Fidler et al., 2005) and is appealing from a theoretical perspective in that strengths in visuo-spatial memory may facilitate whole-word processing. In fact, word identification is positively associated with visual processing skills in children and adolescents with Down syndrome, even after controlling for chronological age (Fidler et al., 2005).
Phonological analysis skills
As visual processing strengths may aid whole-word recognition, weaknesses in phonological (auditory) memory are thought to hinder phonological decoding skills in this population. Phonological memory predicts variation in reading ability above what can be explained by general cognitive ability (Fowler, Doherty & Boyton, 1995), suggesting that it would be a mistake to focus on whole word recognition to the exclusion of phonological awareness in intervention. Kay-Raining Bird and colleagues (2000) also found that phonological memory predicted word attack ability 4.5 years later in a small group of children with Down syndrome after controlling for chronological and mental age.
Phonological awareness skills, such as phoneme segmentation and rhyme awareness, are strongly correlated with the ability to “sound out” words (phonological decoding) in typically developing children (Scarborough, 1998) and also in children with Down syndrome (Boudreau, 2002; Carduso-Martins, & Frith, 2001; Cossu, Rossini, & Marshall, 1993; Cupples & Iacono, 2000; Fletcher & Buckley, 2002; Gombert, 2002; Kay-Raining Bird et al., 2000; Snowling, Hulme, & Mercer, 2002). Several studies suggest that individuals with Down syndrome face particular challenges in phoneme and syllable segmentation, sound deletion, and rhyming (Cossu et al., 1993; Fowler et al., 1995, Cardoso-Martins & Frith, 2001, Verucci et al., 2006). Still, reading skills in this population are stronger than would be predicted by phonological awareness skills, suggesting reliance on other processes in word identification tasks (Cossu et al., 1993, Kay-Raining Bird et al., 2000). Evidence does support that individuals with Down syndrome are capable of utilizing and improving phonological-decoding for word identification (Cupples & Iacono, 2000; Kennedy & Flynn, 2003; Van Bysterveldt, Gillon, & Moran, 2006; Goetz, Hulme, Brigstocke, Carroll, Nasir, & Snowling, 2008). In fact, after controlling for chronological age and intellectual ability, phonological awareness is significantly related to reading and writing abilities in individuals with Down syndrome (Cupples & Iacono, 2000, Cardoso-Martins & Frith, 2001, Fowler et al., 1995).
Complex literacy skills
Few studies have focused on complex literacy skills in individuals with Down syndrome, although available evidence suggests difficulties with these advanced skills. Bryne and colleagues (2002) charted the literacy development of 24 children with Down syndrome over a two-year period. Although the children with Down syndrome made significant improvements in single-word reading, no change in reading comprehension was observed. Verucci and colleagues (2006) also found impaired passage comprehension in 17 individuals with Down syndrome, as compared to a typically developing control group matched on accuracy and speed of single word reading.
Summary
In conclusion, many individuals with Down syndrome are capable of achieving some level of literacy competence given instruction and exposure to print. Individuals with Down syndrome have relatively strong whole-word recognition skills, despite impairments in phonological awareness. Literacy deserves attention from researchers and clinicians, as even basic literacy skills can improve quality of life for individuals with Down syndrome by promoting independence and the ability to be employed (Miller, Leddy, & Leavitt, 1999).
LANGUAGE INTO ADULTHOOD
Compared with the research on young individuals with Down syndrome, fewer studies have characterized the language and communication of adults. In a review article, Rondal and Comblain (1996) concluded that morphosyntax and phonology continue to be areas of relative weakness for adults with Down syndrome, whereas semantics and pragmatics remain areas of relative strength. However, receptive and expressive morphosyntax and lexical skills may remain stable from late adolescence through at least about 50 years (Rondal & Comblain, 2002). Declines in pragmatic skills have been reported with the progression of suspected Alzheimer’s disease, and brain atrophy may also be associated with declines in receptive vocabulary (Nelson, Orme, Asann, & Lott, 2001). Fifty to seventy percent of adults with Down syndrome may evidence symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease by 60-70 years of age (Zigman & Lott, 2007), and dementia has been found to be associated with declines in adaptive behavior for individuals with Down syndrome (Prasher & Chung, 1996). See Rondal and Comblain (1996) and Chapman and Hesketh (2000) for further details about language in adults with Down syndrome, and see Zigman and Lott (2007) for a review of Alzheimer’s disease in Down syndrome.
CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS
The preceding review provides information regarding common language and communication characteristics of individuals with Down syndrome. Though an etiological category by itself may not be very helpful for directing clinical practice (Paul, 2007), awareness of the characteristic profile of strengths and weaknesses associated with a particular diagnosis may help a clinician to focus assessment and intervention efforts. Similarly, to maximize the usefulness of this short review, we have focused in this section on areas likely to benefit from intervention given the characteristic profile of language in Down syndrome. In practice, of course, clinical procedures should ultimately reflect the particular strengths and needs of individual clients and their families.
Assessment
Because middle ear problems and associated hearing loss are common among individuals with Down syndrome, routine screenings in these areas are recommended (Roberts et al., 2004). Hearing should be tested when OME lasts three or more months (AAP, 2004), consistent with recommended procedures of the ASHA Audiologic Assessment Panel (1996). According to clinical practice guidelines, at-risk children may benefit from the use of tympanotomy tubes when OME lasts longer than four months (AAP, 2004). In addition, speech and language should be monitored while OME is being medically managed. Amplification devices such as low gain hearing aids and classroom FM sound field systems may be helpful (ASHA, 2002). For additional strategies to optimize the language-learning environment for children with OME-related hearing loss, see AAP (2004), Roberts and colleagues (2004), and Roberts and Wallace (1997).
Each child with Down syndrome should have his or her language assessed to identify strengths and needs in phonology, vocabulary, syntax, pragmatics, and literacy. As with all children, families should be involved from the beginning of the assessment process. They can provide valuable information regarding child interests, general and medical history, interactions with siblings and other communication partners, and their own perspectives on strengths and needs (Crais, 1996; Crais, Roy, & Free, 2006; Hixson, 1993; Paul, 2007).
Given the common speech intelligibility and phonological problems of children with Down syndrome, speech production should be assessed in connected speech in addition to single words to determine sound accuracy and the occurrence of phonological processes. Importantly, speech assessment should focus on other potential causes of poor intelligibility such as oral-motor skills and vocal quality. In addition to instruments like the Test of Language Development-3 Primary (Newcomer & Hammill, 1997), which help a clinician to gain an understanding of a child’s language skills across language components, the characteristic profile of language in Down syndrome supports the use of standardized tests that focus on particular areas. For example, results from the Expressive Vocabulary Test 2 (Williams, 2007) for productive vocabulary and the Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test 3 (Dawson, Stout, & Eyer, 2003) for productive syntax and morphology may be useful with the aim of obtaining a detailed individual language profile.
Assessment of vocabulary and syntax should utilize a variety of language samples in addition to standardized measures. For example, narration may elicit more complex language (longer MLU, more word tokens, more word types) compared with conversation in individuals with Down syndrome (Chapman et al., 1998; Miles, Chapman, & Sindberg, 2006). Knowledge of the characteristic language challenges in Down syndrome, in addition to results from standardized testing, can inform a clinician’s choice of areas to assess in-depth with language sampling. For example, computer-assisted language sample analyses such as the Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT; Miller & Chapman, 2008) may be used to look at various aspects of productive syntax and morphology, an area of likely need for many children with Down syndrome. Pragmatic skills, particularly the ability to initiate topics, elaborate on topics, and initiate communicative repairs, may also be assessed from the conversation samples. Finally, language assessment should occur while the child interacts with a variety of communication partners (family, teachers, peers) and in a variety of contexts (home, classroom, community).
Literacy assessment should not be delayed until the school years, as foundational literacy skills begin to develop at birth, and early emergent literacy skills lay the foundation for more conventional forms of literacy. Assessment of early literacy skills can help to identify children who are at risk for difficulties with later, conventional literacy skills during the school years. Thus, early literacy assessment is essential.
Literacy assessment might include direct assessment of literacy skills (such as written language awareness, phonological awareness, letter name knowledge, grapheme-phoneme conversion) or parent-reported measures, such as information regarding home literacy practices or the child’s literacy motivation. Assessment of the child’s literacy environment, such as availability of books and writing materials and the availability of literacy role-models within the household can provide information regarding the child’s given opportunities to develop foundational literacy skills.
Intervention
Language intervention for individuals with Down syndrome should aim to improve functioning in communication, academic, social, and vocational areas (ASHA, 2005). Prioritization of intervention targets should take into consideration family priorities, severity of the deficit, and importance for functionality in academic and social contexts (Crais, 1996; Dodge, 2004; McCauley & Fey, 2006; Paul, 2007). Knowledge of the cognitive-behavioral phenotype of Down syndrome, such as the neurocognitive profile and developmental trajectory, may also guide intervention practices (Fidler, 2005). For example, intervention strategies that capitalize on strengths in visual memory, such as the use of visually-oriented pictures and storybooks, may enhance learning in individuals with Down syndrome (Chapman, 2003; Hick et al., 2005; Roberts, Chapman, Martin, & Moskowitz, 2008).
Regardless of the particular intervention used, generalization of targeted skills should be considered from the beginning of any treatment program. Generalization is promoted by providing multiple opportunities to practice targeted skills in a variety of natural settings, such as the home, classroom, and community, and with a variety of communication partners, such as family members, teachers, and peers. Thus, collaboration among educators, speech-language pathologists, and families will be of utmost importance for treatment success. For more details about the intervention strategies that follow see Paul (2007) and McCauley and Fey (2006), and for specific information regarding integrated models of language intervention see Hixson (1993).
Target early communication
Early intervention is more effective than later intervention for young children with Down syndrome (Aparicio & Balana, 2002), even when intervention is delayed by only two months (Sanz & Menendez, 1996). Prelinguistic skills training and parent education may be effective interventions for young children with Down syndrome who produce few or no words. Responsivity Education/Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching (RE/PMT) targets prelinguistic communication skills by teaching young children to use coordinated eye gaze, vocalizations, and gestures through prompting, arranging the environment, and teaching parents to be responsive to their children’s verbal and nonverbal behaviors (Warren et al., 2006). For children with low rates of prelinguistic commenting and canonical vocalizations pre-treatment, RE/PMT improved rates of growth in prelinguistic commenting and lexical density, respectively, in one study that included 17 children with Down syndrome (39 children overall; Yoder & Warren, 2002). However, RE/PMT improved rate of growth in requesting only for children without Down syndrome. Later, Fey et al. (2006) conducted a randomized clinical trial of 24-33 month-old children with developmental disabilities (26 of 51 with Down syndrome). Children who received six months of PMT used more intentional communication than controls, and having Down syndrome did not affect child outcomes. See Brady, Bredin-Oja, and Warren (2008) for further review of RE/PMT and other prelinguistic language interventions for young children with Down syndrome.
Target speech skills
For the child with Down syndrome who talks but is difficult to understand, speech intervention should target the specific phonological processes and sound errors that are problematic. Although speech intervention studies are greatly lacking, there is some indication that the speech accuracy of young children with Down syndrome improves with parent-implemented treatment focusing on listening and production practice (Cholmain, 1994; Dodd, McCormack, & Woodyatt, 1994). Consistency of word production might be targeted before specific phonological targets in children with Down syndrome who show inconsistent error patterns (Dodd & Thompson, 2001). Given the common speech profile of individuals with Down syndrome, interventions that focus on the reduction of syllable structure processes (Hodson & Paden, 1991) may be successful in improving intelligibility. The cycles remediation approach (see Hodson, 2006; Hodson & Paden, 1991) may be particularly useful for highly unintelligible speakers. For individuals with Down syndrome who are severely unintelligible, initial treatment targets may include more functional vocabulary, such as words for basic needs and names of family members (Roberts, Stoel-Gammon, & Barnes, 2008). For specific strategies for improving speech production skills, see Bauman-Waengler (2004) and Smit (2004).
Target more complex language
Given that many individuals with Down syndrome have deficits in expressive syntax, syntax production is likely to be a focus of intervention. In fact, Chapman and colleagues (2002) argue that intervention goals should continue to address expressive syntax in adolescents and young adults with Down syndrome, as this area continues to develop into adulthood. In general, language input should be based on a child’s receptive rather than expressive ability (Chapman et al., 1998). Speech-language pathologists can educate parents and teachers regarding the appropriate complexity of language based on results of the formal language assessment and on how to check children’s comprehension of academic and social language, with the goal of extending the child’s abilities.
Conversational recasting is one method for helping children develop more complex syntax. In this approach, the child’s utterance is reshaped by the communication partner (clinician, teacher, or family member) to include additional grammatical information. For example, if a child says “doggy sleep,” the communication partner could elaborate “the doggy is sleeping.” (See Camarata and Nelson, 2006, for more details about the recast procedure.) It is also possible to support children in elaborating their syntax by commenting on their ideas and expressing interest in how their ideas fit together while they are engaging in storytelling using spoken or written language (Nelson, Roth, & Van Meter, 2009). In addition, the use of books that include repetitive examples of complex syntax has been recommended (Roberts, Chapman et al., 2008), and may capitalize on relative strengths in visual processing. Clinicians may also utilize books to target developmentally appropriate expressive vocabulary, taking into account age-appropriate themes, academic and social needs, and current interests of the child (Roberts, Chapman et al., 2008). Moreover, partnering with teachers to identify key vocabulary from the curriculum will also be important for many children (Paul, 2007).
Pragmatic impairments such as the tendency to maintain a topic without adding additional meaningful information or the failure to initiate repairs to communication breakdowns are also important areas for intervention. Strategies to increase elaborative language include using topics and materials of interest to the child, allowing additional processing time, and using open-ended questions (Roberts, Chapman, et al., 2008). Individuals with Down syndrome may be taught to request clarification through the use of barrier games, in which the clinician intentionally gives unclear messages in order to create opportunities for the child to request clarification (Paul, 2007). In the writing lab approach to language intervention (see Nelson, Bahr, & Van Meter, 2004), children learn to ask for clarification during peer conferencing and author chair activities where students with diverse abilities have the opportunity to ask each other about their work. Parents and teachers may also be educated to respond to all requests for clarification, so that such behaviors are naturally reinforced and more likely to continue.
Group treatment approaches that take place within the classroom (see Dodge, 2004, for specific suggestions) may succeed in improving pragmatic and other language skills by taking advantage of strengths in social interaction. For other strategies to develop complex language skills of individuals with Down syndrome across language domains, see Kumin (2008).
Target literacy skills
Historically, it was thought that children with Down syndrome would not benefit from phonological awareness instruction as a consequence of syndrome-specific impairments in phonological memory (see Jarrold, Baddeley & Phillips, 1999, for a review). As a result, a whole-word approach to literacy instruction has been recommended in the past (Hoddap & Fidler, 1999). An obvious weakness of this approach is that it does not explicitly teach strategies to decode untrained words. Fortunately, recent research suggests that children with Down syndrome are able to make improvements in phonological awareness skills (Kennedy & Flynn, 2003; Van Bysterveldt et al., 2006) and utilize phonological awareness skills to aid in word-decoding (Cupples & Iacono, 2000; Fidler et al., 2005). Cupples and Iacono (2002) found that children with Down syndrome who received phonological reading instruction generalized learning to untrained words, whereas children who received whole-word instruction did not. Therefore, despite known weaknesses in phonological memory, it seems that children with Down syndrome benefit from phonological skills training to support literacy acquisition.
More recently, it has been recommended that literacy intervention for children with Down syndrome should start with sight words (whole-word training) and then progress to include phonological awareness training (Buckley, 2003; Cupples & Iacono, 2002; Goetz et al., 2008; Oelwein, 1995). This comprehensive approach has also been supported for children with reading delays (Hatcher, Hulme, & Ellis, 1994; Hatcher, Hulme, Miles, Carroll, Hatcher, & Gibbs, 2006; Hatcher, Hulme, & Snowling, 2004). In any case, intervention strategies for children with Down syndrome should use strengths in whole-word recognition to foster the development of phonological decoding skills. Activities can be designed to emphasize phonological patterns in word sets with which the child has had previous success. For example, if the child has had whole-word decoding success with several words that begin with the phoneme /p/, targeting these words as a group and emphasizing their phonological similarity may raise the child’s phonological awareness, as well as serve as a confidence-building activity.
Later, intervention may target advanced literacy skills such as passage comprehension. Playing to the visual and narrative strengths seen in Down syndrome may reduce frustration when targeting advanced literacy skills. For example, a lesson aimed at improving plot structure knowledge or narrative comprehension might incorporate illustrations to support written text. Additional research is needed to evaluate the efficacy of specific intervention tactics. For further review and details about approaches to develop literacy skills in individuals with Down syndrome, see Buckley and Johnson-Glenberg (2008).
Consider augmentative or alternative communication
Augmentative or alternative communication (AAC) systems, such as sign language, visual schedules, pictures, object symbols, or computerized speech production devices, may improve the communicative competence of individuals with Down syndrome who are delayed in speech onset or have markedly reduced speech intelligibility. In fact, children with Down syndrome commonly use sign language to communicate (Kumin, 2003), and there is general agreement that AAC use does not hinder the development of spoken language but may actually promote its development (Brady, 2008; Millar, Light, & Schlosser, 2006). Moreover, the use of AAC systems could facilitate access to books and literacy experiences for some users. See Brady (2008) for more details about AAC use with individuals with Down syndrome.
CONCLUSION
Despite considerable individual variability, the language and communication characteristics of individuals with Down syndrome follow a common profile. Receptive language is typically stronger than expressive language, and vocabulary is stronger than syntax. Strong evidence suggests that phonology, expressive vocabulary, receptive and expressive syntax, and some pragmatic aspects of language are impaired beyond expectations for nonverbal cognitive level. Specifically, syllable structure phonological processes, such as cluster reduction and final consonant deletion, appear to be common in children with Down syndrome. Children and adolescents with Down syndrome produce shorter and less complex utterances than would be expected based on nonverbal mental age, although advances in syntax complexity may continue into late adolescence and young adulthood. They also may have difficulty initiating and elaborating on conversational topics, and initiating repairs of communicative breakdowns. These areas of impairment coexist with areas of relative strength, such as the ability to stay on conversational topic, content-related narrative skills, and the ability to respond to requests for clarification in order to repair communicative breakdowns. This pattern of relative strengths and weaknesses remains apparent in adulthood. Later on, dementia in older adults with Down syndrome may compromise various aspects of language and communication. Many individuals with Down syndrome can achieve some level of literacy competence given exposure and instruction. In fact, individuals with Down syndrome have relatively strong whole-word recognition skills (yet impaired phonological awareness skills). Intervention that is appropriate to each individual’s profile and reflects family priorities should begin early and continue throughout adolescence and adulthood.
Future research should continue to clarify the language profile of individuals with Down syndrome. For example, it will be important to examine whether discrepant findings for receptive vocabulary relate to differences in method of assessment, ages of participants, or hearing status (current or early) of participants. Future analyses should follow children longitudinally, examining whether language skills change over time and which factors predict change. The preceding review of the literature identified several underlying factors of language development in individuals with Down syndrome. For example, hearing loss has been shown to be related to difficulties in comprehension of grammatical morphemes and vocabulary (Miolo et al., 2005; Chapman et al., 1991), phonological memory to language comprehension, MLU, and reading (Laws, 1998, 2004), and nonverbal requesting to later expressive language development (Mundy et al., 1995). Future studies should continue to investigate predictors of language and communication in individuals with Down syndrome, paying attention to other cognitive (e.g., attention) and social (e.g., autistic characteristics, communication partner interaction style) aspects of development. Finally, well-designed intervention studies are needed to determine the efficacy of commonly recommended approaches for developing language and literacy, with attention to outcomes in communication as well as academic and social domains.
Acknowledgments
This article was prepared with the support of grants from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (5 R01 HD038819-07, 2 R01 HD044935-06A1, & 5 T32 HD40127) and the Ireland Family Foundation. We also appreciate the assistance of Kristin Cooley in manuscript preparation.
Biographies
Gary E. Martin, Ph.D., is an Investigator at the Frank Porter Graham Child Development Institute at UNC and an Adjunct Instructor of Speech and Hearing Sciences at UNC. He is a certified member of the American-Speech-Language-Hearing Association, as well as a certified speech-language pathologist in the state of North Carolina. Dr. Martin studies the speech and language development of children with fragile X syndrome, Down syndrome, and autism (with a particular emphasis on pragmatics). He received his Ph.D. in Speech and Hearing Sciences from UNC, and his B.A. and M.A. from the University of Pittsburgh.
Jessica Klusek is a doctoral student at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, and works at the Frank Porter Graham Child Development Institute, where she is researching language characteristics in children with developmental disorders. Ms. Klusek’s research interests stem from her training as a speech-language pathologist; she received her master’s degree from the University of Pittsburgh. Currently, Ms. Klusek is collaborating on several projects focusing on the pragmatic language skills of parents of children with fragile X syndrome and autism spectrum disorders.
Bruno Estigarribia, Ph.D., is a NIH Postdoctoral Fellow at the FPG Child Development Institute and the Neurodevelopmental Disorders Research Center at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. He has a Ph.D. and an M.A. in Linguistics from Stanford University, as well as a Maîtrise (M.A. equivalent) in Language Sciences from the Université Paris V - René Descartes - Sorbonne. Dr. Estigarribia is interested in the processes implicated in typical and atypical language development, models of adult and child language competence, and statistical models and research methodology for linguistics and language acquisition studies.
Until her passing on November 1, 2008, Joanne Roberts, Ph.D., was a Senior Scientist at the Frank Porter Graham Child Development Institute (FPG) at UNC and a Professor of Speech and Hearing Sciences and Research Professor of Pediatrics at UNC. She was also a certified member of the American-Speech-Language-Hearing Association, as well as an ASHA Fellow. Dr. Roberts studied the speech and language development of at-risk children and children with fragile X syndrome, Down syndrome, and autism. She authored more than 125 articles and book chapters. Dr. Roberts received her Ph.D. in Speech Pathology and Audiology from Indiana University.
References
- Abbeduto L, Hesketh LJ. Pragmatic development in individuals with mental retardation: Learning to use language in social interactions. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 1997;3:323–333. [Google Scholar]
- Abbeduto L, Murphy MM, Cawthon SW, Richmond EK, Weissman MD, Karadottir S, et al. Receptive language skills of adolescents and young adults with Down or fragile X syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2003;108:149–160. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2003)108<0149:RLSOAA>2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abbeduto L, Murphy MM, Richmond E, Amman A, Beth P, Weissman MD, et al. Collaboration in referential communication: Comparison of youth with Down syndrome or fragile X syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2006;111:170–183. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2006)111[170:CIRCCO]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abbeduto L, Murphy MM, Kover ST, Karadottir S, Amman A, Bruno L. Signaling noncomprehension of language: A comparison of fragile X syndrome and Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2008;113:214–230. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2008)113[214:SNOLAC]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Academy of Pediatrics. Clinical practice guideline: Otitis media with effusion. Pediatrics. 2004;113:1412–1429. [Google Scholar]
- American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. Guidelines for fitting and monitoring FM systems [Guidlelines] 2002 Available from http://www.asha.org/policy.
- American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. Roles and responsibilities of speech-language pathologists serving persons with mental retardation/developmental disabilities [Guidlelines] 2005 Available from http://www.asha.org/policy.
- American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Audiologic Assessment Panel. Guidelines for audiologic screening. Rockville, MD: Author; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Antonarakis SE, Epstein C. The challenge of Down syndrome. Trends in Molecular Medicine. 2006;12:473–479. doi: 10.1016/j.molmed.2006.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aparicio MTS, Balana JM. Early language stimulation of Down syndrome babies: A study on the optimum age to begin. Early Child Development & Care. 2002;172:651–656. [Google Scholar]
- Barnes E, Roberts J, Long SH, Martin GE, Berni MC, Mandulak KC, Sideris J. Phonological accuracy and intelligibility in connected speech of boys with fragile X syndrome or Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0001). in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes EF, Roberts JE, Mirrett PL, Sideris J, Misenheimer J. A comparison of oral motor structure and function in young males with fragile X syndrome and Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2006;49:903–917. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/065). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bauman-Waengler J. Articulatory and phonological impairments: A clinical focus. 2. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Beeghly M, Weiss-Perry B, Cicchetti D. Beyond sensorimotor functioning: Early communicative and play development of children with Down syndrome. In: Cicchetti D, Beeghly M, editors. Children with Down syndrome: A developmental perspective. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1990. pp. 329–368. [Google Scholar]
- Bergland M, Eriksson M. Communicative development in Swedish children 16-28 months old: The Swedish Early Communicative Inventory--words and sentences. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 2000;41:133–144. doi: 10.1111/1467-9450.00181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berglund E, Eriksson M, Johansson I. Parental reports of spoken language skills in children with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2001;44:179–191. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2001/016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bleile K, Schwartz I. Three perspectives on the speech of children with Down Syndrome. Journal of Communication Disorders. 1984;17:87–94. doi: 10.1016/0021-9924(84)90014-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boudreau D, Chapman R. The relationship between event representation and linguistic skill in narratives of children and adolescents with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2000;43:1146–1159. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4305.1146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boudreau D. Literacy skills in children and adolescents with Down syndrome. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal. 2002;15:497–525. [Google Scholar]
- Boudreau DM, Hedberg NL. A comparison of early literacy skills in children with specific language impairment and their typically developing peers. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 1999;8:249–260. [Google Scholar]
- Brady NC. AAC for children with Down syndrome and children with fragile X syndrome. In: Roberts JE, Chapman RS, Warren SF, editors. Speech and language development and intervention in Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome. Baltimore: Brookes; 2008. pp. 255–274. [Google Scholar]
- Brady NC, Bredin-Oja S, Warren SF. Prelinguistic and early language interventions for children with Down syndrome or fragile X syndrome. In: Roberts JE, Chapman RS, Warren SF, editors. Speech and language development and intervention in Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome. Baltimore: Brookes; 2008. pp. 173–192. [Google Scholar]
- Bray M, Woolnough L. The language skills of children with Down’s Syndrome aged 12 to 16 years. Child Language Teaching and Therapy. 1988;4:311–324. [Google Scholar]
- Buckley S. Attaining basic educational skills: Reading, writing and number. In: Lane D, Stratford B, editors. Current approaches to Down’s syndrome. New York: Praeger Press; 1985. pp. 315–343. [Google Scholar]
- Buckley S. Literacy and language. In: Ronald JA, Buckley S, editors. Speech and language intervention in Down syndrome. London: Whurr; 2003. pp. 132–153. [Google Scholar]
- Buckley S, Johnson-Glenberg MC. Increasing literacy learning for individuals with Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome. In: Roberts JE, Chapman RS, Warren S, editors. Speech and language development and intervention in Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome. Baltimore: Brookes; 2008. pp. 233–254. [Google Scholar]
- Byrne A, MacDonald J, Buckley S. Reading language and memory skills: a comparative longitudinal study of children with Down syndrome and their mainstream peers. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 2002;72:513–529. doi: 10.1348/00070990260377497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bryne A, Buckley S, MacDonald J, Bird G. Investigating the literacy, language, and memory skills of children with Down’s syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice. 1995;3:53–58. [Google Scholar]
- Camarata S, Nelson K. Conversational Recast Intervention with Preschool and Older Children. In: McCauley R, Fey M, editors. Treatment of language disorders in children. Baltimore: Brookes; 2006. pp. 237–264. [Google Scholar]
- Cardoso-Martins C, Frith U. Can individuals with Down syndrome acquire alphabetic literacy skills in the absence of phoneme awareness? Reading and Writing. 2001;14:361–375. [Google Scholar]
- Cardoso-Martins C, Michalick MF, Pollo TC. In sensitivity to rhyme a developmental precursor to sensitivity to phoneme?: Evidence from individuals with Down syndrome. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal. 2002;15:439–454. [Google Scholar]
- Cardoso-Martins C, Peterson R, Olson R, Pennington B. Component reading skills in Down Syndrome. Reading and Writing. 2009;22:277–292. [Google Scholar]
- Caselli M, Vicari S, Longobardi E, Lami L, Pizzoli C, Stella G. Gestures and words in early development of children with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1998;41:1125–1135. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4105.1125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caselli MC, Monaco L, Trasciani M, Vicari S. Language in Italian children with Down syndrome and with specific language impairment. Neuropsychology. 2008;22:27–35. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.1.27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catts HW, Kamhi AG. Language and reading disabilities: A developmental perspective. Boston: Allyn & Bacon; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Cardoso-Martins C, Frith U. Can individuals with Down syndrome acquire alphabetic literacy skills in the absence of phoneme awareness? Reading and Writing. 2001;14:361–375. [Google Scholar]
- Cardoso-Martins C, Michalick MF, Pollo TC. In sensitivity to rhyme a developmental precursor to sensitivity to phoneme?: Evidence from individuals with Down syndrome. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal. 2002;15:439–454. [Google Scholar]
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Improved national prevalence estimates for 18 selected major birth defects – United States, 1999 – 2001. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 2006;54:1301–1305. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman RS. Language and communication in individuals with Down syndrome. In: Glidden L, Abbeduto L, editors. Language and communication in mental retardation. Vol. 27. San Diego: Academic Press; 2003. pp. 1–35. [Google Scholar]
- Chapman RS, Hesketh LJ. Behavioral phenotype of individuals with Down syndrome. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research. 2000;6:84–95. doi: 10.1002/1098-2779(2000)6:2<84::AID-MRDD2>3.0.CO;2-P. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman RS, Hesketh LJ, Kistler DJ. Predicting longitudinal change in language production and comprehension in individuals with Down syndrome: Hierarchical linear modeling. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2002;45:902–915. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2002/073). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman RS, Schwartz SE, Kay-Raining Bird E. Language skills of children and adolescents with Down Syndrome: I. Comprehension. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1991;34:1106–1120. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3405.1106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman RS, Seung HK, Schwartz SE, Kay-Raining Bird E. Language skills of children and adolescents with Down syndrome. II. Production deficits. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1998;41:861–873. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4104.861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cholmain CN. Working on phonology with young children with Down syndrome. Journal of Clinical Speech and Language Studies. 1994;1:14–35. [Google Scholar]
- Coggins TE, Stoel-Gammon C. Clarification strategies used by four Down syndrome children for maintaining normal conversational interaction. Education and Training in Mental Retardation. 1982;17:65–67. [Google Scholar]
- Coggins T, Carpenter R, Owings N. Examining early intentional communication in Down’s syndrome and nonretarded children. British Journal of Disorders of Communication. 1983;18:98–106. doi: 10.3109/13682828309019827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collacott RA, Cooper SA, Branford D, McGrother C. Behaviour phenotype for Down’s syndrome. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1998;172:85–89. doi: 10.1192/bjp.172.1.85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cossu G, Rossini F, Marshall JC. When reading is acquired but phoneme awareness is not: A study of literacy on Down’s syndrome. Cognition. 1993;46:129–138. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(93)90016-o. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crais ER. Applying family-centered principles to child assessment. In: McWilliam PJ, Winton P, Crais ER, editors. Practical strategies for family-centered early intervention. San Diego, CA: Singular; 1996. pp. 69–96. [Google Scholar]
- Crais ER, Roy VP, Free K. Parents’ and professionals’ perceptions of the implementation of family-centered practices in child assessments. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2006;15:365–377. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2006/034). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cupples L, Iacono T. Phonological awareness and oral reading skill in children with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2000;43:595–608. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4303.595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cupples L, Iacono T. The efficacy of “whole word” versus “analytic” reading instruction for children with Down syndrome. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal. 2002;15:549–574. [Google Scholar]
- Dawson J, Stout C, Eyer J. Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test. Third Edition. DeKalb, IL: Janelle Publications; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Dodd B. Comparison of babbling patterns in normal and Down syndrome infants. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research. 1972;16:35–40. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.1972.tb01569.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodd B. A comparison of the phonological systems of mental age matched, normal, severely subnormal and Down’s syndrome children. British Journal of Disorders of Communication. 1976;11:27–42. doi: 10.3109/13682827609011289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodd B, McCormack P, Woodyatt G. Evaluation of an intervention program: Relation between children’s phonology and parents’ communicative behavior. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 1994;98:632–645. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodd B, Thompson L. Speech disorder in children with Down’s syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disabilities Research. 2001;45(4):308–316. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.2001.00327.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodge EP. Communication skills: The foundation for meaningful group intervention in school-based programs. Topics in Language Disorders. 2004;24(2):141–150. [Google Scholar]
- Dykens EM, Hodapp RM, Evans DW. Profiles and development of adaptive behavior in children with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 1994;98:580–587. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dykens EM, Hodapp RM, Evans DW. Profiles and development of adaptive behavior in children with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice. 2006;9:45–50. doi: 10.3104/reprints.293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eadie PA, Fey ME, Douglas JM, Parsons CL. Profiles of grammatical morphology and sentence imitation in children with specific language impairment and Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2002;45:720–732. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2002/058). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fey M, Warren S, Brady N, Finestack LH, Bredin-Oja SL, Fairchild M, et al. Early effects of prelinguistic milieu teaching and responsivity education for children with developmental delays and their parents. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2006;49:526–547. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/039). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fidler DL. The emerging Down syndrome behavioral phenotype in early childhood: Implications for practice. Infants and Young Children. 2005;18:83–103. doi: 10.1097/00001163-200507000-00007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fidler DJ, Most DE, Guiberson MM. Neuropsychological correlates of word identification in Down syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2005;26:487–501. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2004.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fidler DJ, Hepburn S, Rogers S. Early learning and adaptive behaviour in toddlers with Down syndrome: Evidence for an emerging behavioural phenotype? Down Syndrome Research and Practice. 2006;9:37–44. doi: 10.3104/reports.297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher H, Buckley S. Phonological awareness in children with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice. 2002;8:11–18. doi: 10.3104/reports.123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fowler AE, Doherty BJ, Boyton L. The basis of reading skills in young adults with Down syndrome. In: Nadel L, Rosethal D, editors. Down syndrome: Living and learning in the community. New York: Wiley-Liss; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Freeman SFN, Kasari C. Characteristics and qualities of the play dates of children with Down syndrome: Emerging or true friendships? American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2002;107:16–31. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2002)107<0016:CAQOTP>2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghaziuddin M, Tsai LY, Ghaziuddin N. Autism in Down’s syndrome: Presentation and diagnosis. Journal of Intellectual Disabilities Research. 1992;36:449–456. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.1992.tb00563.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glenn S, Cunningham C. Performance of young people with Down syndrome on the Leiter-R and British Picture Vocabulary Scales. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2005;9:239–244. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2005.00643.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goetz K, Hulme C, Brigstocke S, Carroll JM, Nasir L, Snowling M. Training reading and phoneme awareness skills in children with Down syndrome. Reading and Writing. 2008;21:395–412. [Google Scholar]
- Gombert JE. Children with Down syndrome use of phonological knowledge in reading. Reading and Writing. 2002;15:455–469. [Google Scholar]
- Hassold T, Sherman S. The origin and etiology of trisomy 21. In: Cohen WI, Nadel L, Madnick ME, editors. Down syndrome: Visions for the 21st century. New York: Wiley-Liss; 2002. pp. 295–301. [Google Scholar]
- Hatcher PJ, Hulme C, Ellis AW. Ameliorating early reading failure by integrating the teaching of reading and phonological skills – the phonological linkage hypothesis. Child Development. 1994;65:41–57. [Google Scholar]
- Hatcher PJ, Hulme C, Miles JN, Carroll JM, Hatcher J, Gibbs S, et al. Efficacy of small group reading intervention for beginning reader with reading-delay: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychiatry. 2006;47:820–826. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01559.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hatcher PJ, Hulme C, Snowling MJ. Explicit phoneme training combined with phonic reading instruction helps young children at risk of reading failure. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45:338–358. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00225.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hepburn S, Philofsky A, Fidler DJ, Rogers S. Autism symptoms in toddlers with Down syndrome: a descriptive study. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities. 2008;21:48–57. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-3148.2007.00368.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hesketh LJ, Chapman RS. Verb use by individuals with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 1998;103:288–304. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(1998)103<0288:VUBIWD>2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hick RF, Botting N, Conti-Ramsden G. Short-term memory and vocabulary development in children with Down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2005;47:532–538. doi: 10.1017/s0012162205001040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hixson PK. An integrated approach to program development. Topics in Language Disorders. 1993;14(1):41–57. [Google Scholar]
- Hodapp RM, Fidler DJ. Special education and genetics: Connections for the 21st century. Journal of Special Education. 1999;33:130–137. [Google Scholar]
- Hodson BW. Identifying phonological patterns and projecting remediation cycles: Expediting intelligibility gains of a 7 year old Australian child. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2006;8:257–264. [Google Scholar]
- Hodson B, Paden E. Targeting intelligible speech: A phonological approach to remediation. 2. Austin: Pro-Ed; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Iverson J, Longobardi E, Caseli MC. Relationship between gestures and words in children with Down’s syndrome and typically developing children in the early stages of communicative development. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 2003;38:179–197. doi: 10.1080/1368282031000062891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarrold C, Baddeley AD, Hewes AK. Genetically dissociated components of working memory: evidence from Downs and Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 1999;37:637–651. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(98)00128-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarrold C, Baddeley AD. Short-term memory in Down syndrome: Applying the working memory model. Down Syndrome Research and Practice. 2001;7:17–23. doi: 10.3104/reviews.110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarrold C, Baddeley AD, Phillips C. Down syndrome and the phonological loop: the evidence for, and the importance of, a specific verbal short-term memory deficit. Down Syndrome Research and Practice. 1999;6:61–71. doi: 10.3104/reviews.97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarrold C, Baddeley AD, Phillips C. Long-term memory for verbal and visual information in Down syndrome and Williams syndrome: Performance on the doors and people test. Cortex. 2007;43:233–247. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70478-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joffe V, Varlokosta S. Patterns of syntactic development in children with Williams syndrome and Down’s syndrome: Evidence from passives and wh-questions. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics. 2007;21:705–727. doi: 10.1080/02699200701541375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kay-Raining Bird E, Chapman RS, Schwartz SE. Fast mapping of words and story recall by individuals with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2004;47(6):1286–1300. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/097). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kay-Raining Bird E, Cleave P, McConnell L. Reading and phonological awareness in children with Down syndrome: A longitudinal study. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2000;9:319–990. [Google Scholar]
- Kay-Raining Bird E, Cleave P, White D, Pike H, Helmkay A. Written and oral narratives of children and adolescents with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2008;51:436–450. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/032). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kennedy EJ, Flynn MC. Training phonological awareness skills in children with Down syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2003;24:44–57. doi: 10.1016/s0891-4222(02)00168-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kent L, Evans J, Paul M, Sharp M. Comorbidity of autistic spectrum disorders in children with Down syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 1999;41:153–158. doi: 10.1017/s001216229900033x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein BP, Mervis CB. Contrasting patterns of cognitive abilities of 9-and 10-year-olds with Williams syndrome or Down syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology. 1999;16:177–196. [Google Scholar]
- Kumin L. Intelligibility of speech in children with Down syndrome in natural settings: parents’ perspective. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1994;78:307–313. doi: 10.2466/pms.1994.78.1.307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumin L. Early communication skills for children with Down syndrome: A guide for parents and professionals. Bethesda, MD: Woodbine House; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Kumin L. Speech intelligibility and childhood verbal apraxia in children with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice. 2006;10:10–22. doi: 10.3104/reports.301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumin L. Language intervention to encourage complex use: A clinical perspective. In: Roberts JE, Chapman RS, Warren S, editors. Speech and language development and intervention in Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome. Baltimore: Brookes; 2008. pp. 193–218. [Google Scholar]
- Kumin L, Adams J. Developmental apraxia of speech and intelligibility in children with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome Quarterly. 2000;5:1–6. [Google Scholar]
- Laws G. The use of nonword repetition as a test of phonological memory in children with Down syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 1998;39:1119–1130. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laws G. Working memory in children and adolescents with Down syndrome: Evidence from a colour memory experiment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2002;43:353–364. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laws G. Contributions of phonological memory, language comprehension and hearing to the expressive language of adolescents and young adults with Down syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2004;45:1085–1095. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.t01-1-00301.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laws G, Bishop DVM. A comparison of language abilities in adolescents with Down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2003;46:1324–1339. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2003/103). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laws G, Buckley S, MacDonald J, Broadley I. The influence of reading instruction on language and memory development in children with Down’s syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice. 1995;3:59–64. [Google Scholar]
- Laws G, Gunn D. Relationships between reading, phonological skills and language development in individuals with Down syndrome: A five year follow-up study. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal. 2002;15:527–548. [Google Scholar]
- Laws G, Gunn D. Phonological memory as a predictor of language comprehension in Down syndrome: A five-year follow-up study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45:326–337. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00224.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch MP, Oller DK, Steffens ML, Levine SL, Bassinger DL, Umbel V. Onset of speech-like vocalizations in infants with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 1995;100:68–86. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCauley RJ, Fey ME. Treatment of language disorders in children. Baltimore: Brooks; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Mervis C, Robinson B. Expressive vocabulary ability of toddlers with Williams syndrome or Down syndrome: A comparison. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2000;17:111–126. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN1701_07. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miles S, Chapman RS. Narrative content as described by individuals with Down syndrome and typically developing children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 2002;45:175–189. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2002/013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miles S, Chapman R, Sindberg H. Sampling context affects MLU in the language of adolescents with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2006;49:325–337. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/026). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Millar DC, Light JC, Schlosser RW. The impact of augmentative and alternative communication intervention on the speech production of individuals with developmental disabilities: A research review. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2006;49:248–264. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller JF. Individual differences in vocabulary acquisition in children with Down syndrome. In: Epstein C, Hassold T, Lott I, Nadel L, Patterson D, editors. Etiology and pathogenesis of Down syndrome: Proceedings of the international Down syndrome research conference. New York: Wiley-Liss; 1995. pp. 93–103. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller JF, Chapman RS. Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) 2008 (computer software) Madison, WI: Language Analysis Laboratory, Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Miller JF, Leddy M, Leavitt L, editors. Improving the communication of people with Down syndrome. Baltimore: Brookes; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Miller JF, Leddy M. Down syndrome: The impact of speech production on language development. In: Paul R, editor. Exploring the Speech-Language Connection. Baltimore: Brookes; 1998. pp. 163–177. [Google Scholar]
- Miolo G, Chapman RS, Sindberg HA. Sentence comprehension in adolescents with Down syndrome and typically developing children: Role of sentence voice, visual context, and auditory-verbal short-term memory. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2005;48:172–188. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore DG, Oates JM, Hobson RP, Goodwin J. Cognitive and social factors in the development of infants with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice. 1988;8:43–52. doi: 10.3104/reviews.129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mundy P, Kasari C, Sigman M, Ruskin E. Nonverbal communication and early language acquisition in children with Down syndrome and in normally developing children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1995;38:157–167. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3801.157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mundy P, Sigman M, Kasari C, Yirmiya N. Nonverbal communication skills in Down syndrome children. Child Development. 1988;59:235–249. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson LD, Orme D, Osann K, Lott IT. Neurological changes and emotional functioning in adults with Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2001;45:450–456. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.2001.00379.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson NW, Bahr CM, Van Meter AM. The writing lab approach to language instruction and intervention. Baltimore, MD: Paul H Brookes Publishing Co; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Nelson NW, Roth FP, Van Meter AM. Written composition instruction and intervention for students with language impairment. In: Troia GA, editor. Instruction and assessment for struggling writers: Evidence-based practices. New York: Guilford; 2009. pp. 187–212. [Google Scholar]
- Newcomer P, Hammill D. Test of Language Development – 3 Primary. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Oelwein PL. Teaching reading to children with Down syndrome: A guide for parents and teachers. Bethesda, MD: Woodbine House; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Patterson D, Lott I. Etiology, diagnosis, and development in Down syndrome. In: Roberts JE, Chapman RS, Warren S, editors. Speech and language development and intervention in Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome. Baltimore: Brookes; 2008. pp. 3–26. [Google Scholar]
- Paul R. Language disorders from infancy through adolescence: Assessment and intervention. 3. Philadelphia: Elsevier; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Paul R, Murray C, Clancy K, Andrews D. Reading and metaphonological outcomes in late talkers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1997;40:1037–1047. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4005.1037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prasher VP, Chung MC. Causes of age-related decline in adaptive behavior of adults with Down syndrome: Differential diagnoses of dementia. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 1996;101:175–183. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price JR, Kent RD. Increasing speech intelligibility in Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome. In: Roberts JE, Chapman RS, Warren S, editors. Speech and language development and intervention in Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome. Baltimore: Brookes; 2008. pp. 219–232. [Google Scholar]
- Price JR, Roberts JE, Hennon EA, Berni MC, Anderson KL, Sideris J. Syntactic complexity during conversation of boys with fragile X syndrome and Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2008;112:1–17. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price JR, Roberts JE, Vandergrift N, Martin G. Language comprehension in boys with fragile X syndrome and boys with Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2007;51:318–326. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00881.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pueschel SM. Down syndrome. In: Parker S, Zuckerman B, editors. Behavioral and developmental pediatrics: A handbook for primary care. New York: Little Brown; 1995. pp. 116–119. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts JE, Chapman RS, Martin GE, Moskowitz L. Language of preschool and school-age children with Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome. In: Roberts JE, Chapman RS, Warren SF, editors. Speech and language development and intervention in Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome. Baltimore: Brookes; 2008. pp. 77–115. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts J, Hunter L, Gravel J, Rosenthal R, Berman S, Haggard M, et al. Otitis media, hearing loss, and language learning: Controversies and current research. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2004;25:1–13. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200404000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts JE, Long SH, Malkin C, Barnes E, Skinner M, Hennon EA. A comparison of phonological skills of boys with fragile X syndrome and Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2005;48:980–995. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/067). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts JE, Martin GE, Moskowitz L, Harris AA, Foreman J, Nelson L. Discourse skills of boys with fragile X syndrome in comparison to boys with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2007;50:475–492. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/033). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts JE, Price JR, Barnes EF, Nelson L, Burchinal M, Hennon E, et al. Receptive vocabulary, expressive vocabulary, and speech production of boys with fragile X syndrome and Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2007;112:177–193. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2007)112[177:RVEVAS]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts JE, Stoel-Gammon C, Barnes EF. Phonological characteristics of children with Down syndrome or fragile X syndrome. In: Roberts JE, Chapman RS, Warren SF, editors. Speech and language development and intervention in Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome. Baltimore: Brookes; 2008. pp. 143–169. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts JE, Wallace IF. Otitis media and language. In: Roberts JE, Wallace IF, Henderson FW, editors. Otitis media, language and learning in young children: Medical, developmental, and educational considerations. Baltimore: Brookes; 1997. pp. 133–162. [Google Scholar]
- Roizen NJ. Down syndrome. In: Batshaw ML, Pellegrino L, Roizen NJ, editors. Children with disabilities. 6. Baltimore: Brookes; 2007. pp. 263–273. [Google Scholar]
- Roizen NJ, Patterson D. Down’s syndrome. Lancet. 2003;361:1281–1289. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)12987-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roizen NJ, Wolters C, Nicol T, Blondis TA. Hearing loss in children with Down syndrome. Journal of Pediatrics. 1993;23:9–12. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(05)81588-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rondal JA, Comblain A. Language in adults with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice. 1996;4:3–14. [Google Scholar]
- Rondal JA, Comblain A. Language in aging persons with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice. 2002;8:1–9. doi: 10.3104/reports.122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosin M, Swift E, Bless D, Vetter D. Communication profiles in adolescents with Down syndrome. Journal of Childhood Communication Disorders. 1988;12:49–64. [Google Scholar]
- Rupela V, Manjula R. Phonotactic patterns in the speech of children with Down syndrome. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics. 2007;21:605–622. doi: 10.1080/02699200701416784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanz MT, Menendez J. A study of the effect of age of treatment on observed development of Down syndrome babies. Early Child Development and Care. 1995;118:93–101. [Google Scholar]
- Scarborough HS. Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities: Phonological awareness and some promising predictors. In: Shapiro BK, Pas-quale PJ, Capute AJ, editors. Specific reading disability: A view of the spectrum. Timonium, MD: York; 1998. pp. 75–119. [Google Scholar]
- Scarborough HS, Dobrich W. On the efficacy of reading to preschoolers. Developmental Review. 1994;14:245–302. [Google Scholar]
- Shott SR, Joseph A, Heithaus D. Hearing loss in children with Down syndrome. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology. 2001;61:199–205. doi: 10.1016/s0165-5876(01)00572-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smit AB. Articulation and Phonology Resource Guide for School-Age Children and Adults. Clifton Park, NY: Delmar Learning; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Smith BL, Oller DK. A comparative study of pre-meaningful vocalizations produced by normally developing and Down’s syndrome infants. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1981;46:46–51. doi: 10.1044/jshd.4601.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith BL, Stoel-Gammon C. A longitudinal study of the development of stop consonant production in normal and Down’s syndrome children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1983;48:114–118. doi: 10.1044/jshd.4802.114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shepperdson B. Attainments in reading and number of teenagers and young adults with Down’s syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice. 1994;2:97–101. [Google Scholar]
- Snowling MJ, Hulme C, Mercer RC. A deficit in rime awareness in children with Down syndrome. Reading and Writing. 2002;15:471–495. [Google Scholar]
- Staskowski M, Rivera EA. Speech-language pathologists’ involvement in responsiveness to intervention activities: A complement to curriculum-relevant practice. Topics in Language Disorders. 2005;25(2):132–147. [Google Scholar]
- Stoel-Gammon C. Phonological analysis of four Down’s syndrome children. Applied Psycholinguistics. 1980;1:31–48. [Google Scholar]
- Stoel-Gammon C. Phonological development in Down syndrome. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Review. 1997;3:300–306. [Google Scholar]
- Storch SA, Whitehurst GJ. Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology. 2002;38:934–947. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tannock R. Mothers’ directiveness in their interactions with their children with and without Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 1988;93:154–165. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thordardottir ET, Chapman RS, Wagner L. Complex sentence production by adolescents with Down syndrome. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2002;23:163–183. [Google Scholar]
- Urv TK, Zigman WB, Silverman W. Maladaptive behaviors related to dementia status in adults with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2008;113:73–86. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2008)113[73:MBRTDS]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Bysterveldt AK, Gillon GT, Moran C. Enhancing phonological awareness and letter knowledge in preschool children with Down syndrome. International Journal of Disability, Development, and Education. 2006;53:301–329. [Google Scholar]
- Vicari S, Bellucci S, Carlesimo GA. Visual and spatial long-term memory: differential pattern of impairments in Williams and Down syndromes. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2005;47:305–311. doi: 10.1017/s0012162205000599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verucci L, Menghini D, Vicari S. Reading skills and phonological awareness acquisition in Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2006;50:477–491. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00793.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang PP, Bellugi U. Evidence from two genetic syndromes for a dissociation between verbal and visual-spatial short-term memory. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1994;16:317–322. doi: 10.1080/01688639408402641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warren SF, Bredin-Oja SL, Fairchild M, Finestack LH, Fey ME, Brady NC. Responsivity education/prelinguistic milieu teaching. In: McCauley RJ, Fey M, editors. Treatment of language disorders in children. Baltimore: Brookes; 2006. pp. 47–75. [Google Scholar]
- Williams KT. Expressive Vocabulary Test. Second Edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Wishart JG, Johnston FH. The effects of experience on attribution of a stereotyped personality to children with Down’s syndrome. Journal on Mental Deficiency Research. 1990;34:409–420. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.1990.tb01551.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoder P, Warren S. Effects of prelinguistic milieu teaching and parent responsivity education on dyads involving children with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2002;45:1158–1175. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2002/094). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zigman WB, Lott IT. Alzheimer’s disease in Down syndrome: Neurobiology and risk. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2007;13:237–246. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]