Abstract
The importance of intracortical inhibitory circuits in setting the feature-selective spatial organization of primary sensory cortices remains controversial. To address this issue, we examined the strength of interneuron-to-pyramidal cell connections across the rat anterior piriform cortex (aPC) and found a pronounced gradient of increasing pyramidal cell inhibition along the aPC rostro-caudal axis. This functional heterogeneity could govern aPC spatial activation in response to varying odor identities and features.
Precise timing of inhibition has been proposed to be a key mechanism in primary sensory cortices for tuning pyramidal cell spike output to sensory-evoked inputs1,2,3,4,5. Whether local inhibitory circuits are also involved in establishing the feature-selective spatial organization of primary sensory cortices has been the subject of considerable debate6. At the crux of the controversy is the issue of how local inhibitory circuits are organized across cortical space. In order to test this idea, we examined the strength of inhibitory connections onto pyramidal cells along the aPC rostro-caudal axis.
To date, all studies that have investigated the intracortical connective organization of the aPC have been anatomical in nature and as such have not characterized functional synapses7. To assess functional interneuron-to-pyramidal cell connections, we uncaged glutamate focally over Layer 1, 2, or 3 interneurons and recorded the resulting GABAA receptor-mediated inhibitory postsynaptic currents (IPSCs) in Layer 2/3 pyramidal cells4 (Fig. 1a and Supplementary methods; see Supplementary Fig. 1 for uncaging beam spatial resolution). We then used IPSC charge as our measure for strength of connectivity (Fig. 1a). This approach allowed us to sample a large, spatially distributed pool of layer-specific inhibitory connections onto a single pyramidal cell (Fig. 1a,b).
We found that the location of an interneuron relative to a pyramidal cell dictates connective strength. Pyramidal cell IPSC charge gradually increased as we moved our uncaging beam from rostral to caudal locations relative to the cell soma (Fig. 1a,b and Supplementary Fig. 2). When we compared caudal uncaging spots (positions 50 to 450 μm in Fig. 1b) with their equidistant rostral counterparts (positions −50 to −450 μm in Fig. 1b), we found that IPSC charge evoked by caudal uncaging spots were significantly larger than those elicited by rostral spots (Fig. 1c). On average, the caudal/rostral ratio of IPSC charge (Fig. 1c) was 1.6±0.2 for Layer 1 (mean±s.e.m.; P=0.002; n=26), 1.8±0.2 for Layer 2 (P=0.0001), and 2.0±0.1 for Layer 3 (P=0.0002).
To determine whether inhibitory asymmetry is unique to pyramidal cells, we compared inhibitory connections onto Layer 2 bitufted interneurons and adjacent pyramidal cells (< 150 μm apart) in the same slice (Fig. 1d,e). We selected bitufted interneurons because, like pyramidal cells, their dendrites project across all aPC layers. We identified bitufted interneurons based on their distinct bipolar apical and basal dendrites8 and depolarized resting membrane potentials (−51±4 vs. −60±2 mV; Fig. 1d and Supplementary methods). In stark contrast to their adjacent pyramidal cells, we found that inhibition onto bitufted interneurons was highly symmetric in all aPC layers (Fig. 1d,e and Supplementary Fig. 2). This indicates that asymmetric rostro-caudal inhibition is a distinct pyramidal cell-specific mechanism. Further, it suggests that the differential inhibition is not an artifact of the brain slicing procedure.
Since inhibition of a pyramidal cell increases along the aPC rostro-caudal axis (Fig. 1), we hypothesized that pyramidal cells located caudally should receive greater inhibition than those located rostrally. We tested this possibility by comparing inhibitory inputs to pairs of pyramidal cells in the same slice (Fig. 2a). We found significant differences in inhibition across all aPC layers for cells separated by at least 200 μm (Fig. 2b). Layer 1 interneurons evoked a mean charge of 2.9±1.0 pA.sec in caudal versus 1.4±0.6 pA.sec in rostral pyramidal cells (P=0.02; n=8 cell pairs). Layer 2 interneurons evoked a mean charge of 9.6±2.5 pA.sec in caudal versus 4.9±1.6 in rostral cells (P=0.0001; n=8). Layer 3 interneurons evoked a mean charge of 6.2±1.5 pA.sec in caudal versus 2.2±0.8 in rostral cells (P=0.0001; n=8). Thus, the strength of inhibition onto pyramidal cells occurs as a gradient, increasing in magnitude along rostal-caudal space. Further, differences in inhibitory input strength over relatively short distances (~200 μm) suggest that local circuits differ widely within the aPC. Due to this functional spatial heterogeneity, discrete computational units—analogous to barrels and columns in other sensory cortices—could potentially be formed on the basis of significant differences in rostro-caudal inhibition.
Because pyramidal cells located at more caudal aPC regions receive greater inhibition than cells at rostral locations, caudal pyramidal cells should require greater olfactory bulb excitation than rostral cells to spike. Consequently, increases in olfactory bulb activity, as seen with increased odor concentration9, would be represented as a gradual recruitment of pyramidal cell spike activity from rostral to caudal aPC locations. Indeed, in vivo unit recordings have shown a spatial gradient of increasing spike activity from rostral to caudal aPC regions with increasing odor concentrations10. Thus, the asymmetric nature of pyramidal cell inhibition could be a fundamental organizational principle that governs how the aPC represents changes in odor features at both the single cell and population level.
Experiments focused on determining the mechanisms underlying differential rostro-caudal inhibition should lead to an even better understanding of its computational significance. Our findings provide a couple of important insights into the properties of these mechanisms. First, there must be a target cell-specific mechanism that restricts asymmetric inhibition to pyramidal cells (Fig. 1). Second, there must be a mechanism that allows pyramidal cell inhibition to differ greatly over short distances (~200 μm; Fig. 2). Increasing interneuron cell density along the aPC rostro-caudal axis is perhaps the simplest mechanism that could account for asymmetric inhibition. However, because of target cell-specificity (Fig. 1d,e and Supplementary Fig. 2) and because asymmetric inhibition occurs over narrow cortical areas (Fig. 2), it is unlikely that increased interneuron cell density underlies the differential inhibition of pyramidal cells. In fact, thorough anatomical studies indicate that any appreciable difference in interneuron density could only be observed along rostro-caudal distances of ~2000 μm11, an order of magnitude larger than our rostral-caudal gradient threshold of 200 μm (Fig. 2). More plausible mechanisms include, but are not restricted to: (1) marked differences in intrinsic interneuron excitability that allow caudal interneurons to fire more action potentials than rostral interneurons, (2) differences in interneuron presynaptic release machinery that allow caudal interneurons to release more GABA than rostral interneurons, and (3) skewed interneuron axon projections that make it possible for caudal interneurons to more robustly inhibit pyramidal cells. Further, multiple cellular and circuit mechanisms working independently or in concert may be in play for differential pyramidal cell inhibition to arise.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Drs. Adam Kohn, Scott Nawy, and Ellen Yang for helpful input on the manuscript. This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (NS 044399).
Footnotes
Author contributions
V.M.L performed and analyzed experiments. V.M.L and D.L.P. designed the experiments and prepared the manuscript.
References
- 1.Hirsch JA, Alonso JM, Reid RC, Martinez LM. J Neurosci. 1998;18:9517–9528. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-22-09517.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Wehr M, Zador AM. Nature. 2003;426:442–446. doi: 10.1038/nature02116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wilent WB, Contreras D. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1364–1370. doi: 10.1038/nn1545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Luna VM, Schoppa NE. J Neurosci. 2008;28:8851–8859. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2385-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Poo C, Isaacson JS. Neuron. 2009;62:850–861. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.05.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Priebe NJ, Ferster D. Neuron. 2008;57:482–497. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Neville KR, Haberly LB. In: The synaptic organization of the brain. 5. Shepherd GM, editor. Oxford Univ. Press; New York: 2004. pp. 415–454. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ekstrand JJ, Domroese ME, Feig SL, Illig KR, Haberly LB. J Comp Neurol. 2001;434:308–328. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Johnson BA, Leon M. J Comp Neurol. 2007;503:1–34. doi: 10.1002/cne.21396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Sugai T, Miyazawa T, Fukuda M, Yoshimura H, Onoda N. Neuroscience. 2005;130:769–781. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.09.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Löscher W, Lehmann H, Ebert U. Brain Res. 1998;800:21–31. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(98)00488-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.