Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Apr 29.
Published in final edited form as: Pediatrics. 2008 Jun 9;122(1):e83–e91. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-2826

Effects of Prenatal Cocaine Exposure on Special Education in School-Aged Children

Todd P Levine a, Jing Liu a, Abhik Das b, Barry Lester a, Linda Lagasse a, Seetha Shankaran c, Henrietta S Bada d, Charles R Bauer e, Rosemary Higgins f
PMCID: PMC2861352  NIHMSID: NIHMS175433  PMID: 18541617

Abstract

OBJECTIVE

The objective of this study was to evaluate the effects of prenatal cocaine exposure on special education at age 7 with adjustment for covariates.

METHODS

As part of the prospective, longitudinal, multisite study of children with prenatal cocaine exposure (Maternal Lifestyle Study), school records were reviewed for 943 children at 7 years to determine involvement in special education outcomes: (1) individualized education plan; (2) special education conditions; (3) support services; (4) special education classes; and (5) speech and language services. Logistic regression was used to examine the effect of prenatal cocaine exposure on these outcomes with environmental, maternal, and infant medical variables as covariates, as well as with and without low child IQ.

RESULTS

Complete data for each analysis model were available for 737 to 916 children. When controlling for covariates including low child IQ, prenatal cocaine exposure had a significant effect on individualized education plan. When low child IQ was not included in the model, prenatal cocaine exposure had a significant effect on support services. Male gender, low birth weight, white race, and low child IQ also predicted individualized education plan. Low birth weight and low child IQ were significant in all models. White race was also significant in speech and language services. Other covariate effects were model specific. When included in the models, low child IQ accounted for more of the variance and changed the significance of other covariates.

CONCLUSIONS

Prenatal cocaine exposure increased the likelihood of receiving an individualized education plan and support services, with adjustment for covariates. Low birth weight and low child IQ increased the likelihood of all outcomes. The finding that white children were more likely to get an individualized education plan and speech and language services could indicate a greater advantage in getting educational resources for this population.

Keywords: prenatal exposure, cocaine, education, schools


Increased use of cocaine in the United States in the 1980s turned the scientific community toward studying the children of mothers who used cocaine during pregnancy. Initial legal and social stigma attached to mothers who abused cocaine during pregnancy and their “crack kids” who were feared to be “brain damaged”1,2 has been tempered by evidence that the risk for serious congenital malformations or medical complications in newborns with prenatal cocaine exposure (PCE) is minimal3; however, little is known about the potential long-term neurodevelopmental effects of PCE. Previous studies revealed varying effects of PCE on behavior46 and cognitive outcomes.713 Longitudinal follow-up studies of the intelligence of these children suggested that cocaine effects are apparent but more subtle than originally feared.14 Inconsistencies in the cocaine literature have been described and may be attributable to methodologic issues such as small sample size; confounding of cocaine exposure with exposure to other drugs; lack of biochemical verification for exposure status and levels; and lack of adequate control for demographic variables such as prenatal care, socioeconomic status (SES), and out-of-home placement.14,15

There are few studies of school function in children with PCE. Teacher rating of school behavior suggested increased behavior problems in children with PCE in some5,6,16 but not all studies.17 Studies have reported adverse effects of PCE on language in children aged 2.5 to 9.5 years,14,1824 whereas others have not25,26; however, there are no studies of use of school-based speech and language services in this group. One study of school performance showed no effects of PCE on grade progression, grade point average, or standardized test.27 We provide the first report of enrollment in special education of children with PCE. These services require significant school funding and teacher resources. On the basis of these findings, we hypothesized that PCE is associated with higher rates of enrollment in special education and the need for support services at 7 years, especially speech and language services.

METHODS

The Maternal Lifestyle Study is a large, multisite, longitudinal investigation of PCE being conducted at 4 geographically diverse, collaborating university centers (Wayne State University, University of Tennessee at Memphis, University of Miami, and Brown University). Each participating center had approval for the study from the institutional review board and a certificate of confidentiality from the National Institute on Drug Abuse. Informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Between May 1993 and May 1995, mothers at these centers were enrolled in the study within 24 hours after delivery.3,28,29 Initial screening included the mother’s labor and delivery chart, the newborn admission chart, and a meconium sample. A drug use questionnaire that addressed the mother’s use of nicotine, alcohol, marijuana, cocaine, opiates, and other illicit drugs was given by research staff who were trained and certified in the reliable administration of all of the study interviews. Exposure was determined on the basis of a mother’s admitting cocaine use during pregnancy and/or a positive meconium assay for cocaine metabolites including gas chromatography/mass spectrometry confirmation. Nonexposed children were those who were born to mothers who denied cocaine use, confirmed by negative meconium test results. As previously reported,30 participants for the longitudinal follow-up were recruited at a 1-month visit. The sample included a cohort of exposed infants (n = 658) who were group matched within site with a group of nonexposed comparison children (n = 730) by gestational age categories (<32, 33–36, and >36 weeks) and child gender, race, and ethnicity. At the 1-month visit, the biological mother was interviewed for a detailed inventory of her legal and illegal drug use during pregnancy using the Maternal Interview of Substance Use (MISU). Prenatal cocaine use was categorized as high, some, or none on the basis of standard criteria.31 “High” cocaine use referred to ≥3 times per week in the first trimester. Any other use was referred to as “some” cocaine use. MISU reports of the frequency and quantity of these substances per trimester were averaged to produce indices of the number of tobacco cigarettes (heavy use: ≥10 cigarettes per day), the amount of absolute alcohol (heavy use: ≥0.5 oz/day), and the number of marijuana joints (heavy use: ≥0.5 joints per day) consumed during the pregnancy.

Measures

Medical characteristics were collected at birth (Table 1).

TABLE 1.

Sample Characteristics According to Attrition

Characteristic With 7-y Data
(n= 943)
Without 7-y
Data
(n= 445)
P
Maternal characteristics
  Race, n (%) .000
    White 112 (11.9) 108 (24.3)
    Black 769 (81.5) 294 (66.1)
    Hispanic 52 (5.5) 36 (8.1)
    Other 10 (1.1) 7 (1.6)
  Low SES (Hollingshead 5), 1 mo,
  n (%)
230 (25.2) 79 (18.9) .010
  Marital status, single, n (%) 769 (81.6) 350 (79.0) .250
  Insurance, Medicaid, n (%) 774 (82.1) 357 (80.2) .410
  Education <12y, n (%) 373 (39.6) 172 (38.8) .780
  IQ, mean (SD) 73.00 (17.39) 76.98 (17.06) .001
  No prenatal care, n (%) 107 (11.3) 61 (13.7) .210
  Age, mean (SD), y 28.21 (5.65) 28.63 (6.18) .210
Prenatal drug use, n (%)a
  Cocaine use 398 (42.2) 202 (45.4) .263
  Heavy cocaine use 92 (10.6) 40 (10.4) .745
  Alcohol use 573 (60.8) 252 (56.6) .140
  Heavy alcohol use 119 (12.6) 44 (9.9) .140
  Tobacco use 487 (51.6) 261 (58.7) .020
  Heavy tobacco use 182 (19.3) 112 (25.2) .013
  Marijuana use 213 (22.6) 111 (24.9) .330
  Heavy marijuana use 35 (3.7) 15 (3.4) .751
Postnatal caregiving environment,
  n (%)
  Postnatal drug useb
    Cocaine use 85 (9.0) 33 (8.6) .810
    Heavy cocaine use 73 (7.8) 20 (5.4) .138
    Alcohol use 684 (72.8) 253 (66.1) .015
    Heavy alcohol use 269 (28.7) 75 (20.4) .002
    Tobacco use 551 (58.6) 241 (62.9) .150
    Heavy tobacco use 369 (39.3) 158 (42.9) .233
    Marijuana use 209 (22.2) 61 (15.9) .010
    Heavy marijuana use 68 (7.2) 12 (3.3) .007
  Caregiver depression 104 (11.1) 35 (10.0) .570
  Low quality of HOME, low 20% 199 (21.6) 46 (14.4) .005
  Primary caregiver change, 1 mo to
  7 y
289 (30.7) 132 (30.1) .840
  Low SES (Hollingshead 5), 7 y 164 (17.4) 62 (14.2) .140
  Child abuse, 1 mo to 7 y 29 (3.1) 15 (3.4) .770
  Domestic violence, 4–7 y 75 (8.0) 15 (3.4) .002
Newborn medical characteristics and
  child intelligence
  Gestational age, mean (SD), wk 36.15 (4.08) 36.48 (3.91) .140
  Birth weight, mean (SD), g 2616 (829) 2659 (796) .360
  Length, mean (SD), cm 46.68 (5.10) 46.88 (4.83) .470
  Head circumference, mean (SD), cm 32.06 (3.07) 32.24 (2.95) .290
  LBW, <1500g, n (%) 111 (11.8) 45 (10.1) .360
  SGA, n (%) 221 (23.5) 107 (24.1) .800
  Apgar score, 1 min, median (range) 8 (0–10) 8 (1–10) .740
  Apgar score, 5 min, median (range) 9 (3–10) 9 (1–10) .350
  Male, n (%) 491 (52.1) 236 (53.0) .740
  First born, n (%) 189 (20.0) 109 (24.5) .060
  Child IQ 85.08 (14.80) 87.90 (14.90) .060
  Low child IQ, <85, n (%) 380 (47.6) 50 (43.9) .460

SGA indicates small for gestational age.

a

Heavy use determined by MISU at 1 month from mothers who reported any useat initial screening.

b

Reported by caregiver via Caretaker Inventory of Substance Use at any point from 4 months to 7 years.

Demographics

Parent/caregiver age, race, marital status, education level, and Medicaid insurance status were collected at 1 month.

Maternal/Caregiver IQ

Maternal/caregiver IQ was measured with the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test at 30 months or 5 years.

Postnatal Substance Use

Postnatal substance use was measured at 4 months, 8 months, then yearly using the Caretaker Inventory of Substance Use, which quantifies frequency and amounts of cocaine, opiates, marijuana, tobacco, and alcohol with the same indices for heavy use as the MISU.

Socioeconomic Status

SES was measured with the Hollingshead Index of Social Position32,33 at 1 month, then yearly.

Home

Quality of the home environment was measured with the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME)34 at 10 months and 5.5 years.

Depression

Depression was measured with the Beck Depression Inventory, a proxy for caregiver depression assessed at 4 months, 30 months, and 5.5 years.

Domestic Violence

Domestic violence was assessed yearly between 4 and 7 years using a questionnaire (via Caretaker Inventory of Substance Use) on any experience of domestic violence by the caregiver, including physical or sexual abuse.

Child Abuse

Child abuse was defined by removal of the child from the home as a result of suspicion of physical and/or sexual abuse or medical examination findings suggestive of physical or sexual abuse.

Primary Caregiver

Changes in primary caregivers (child with biological mother or other caregiver) were recorded at 1 month, then yearly.

IQ

Child IQ was measured at 7 years using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children III.

School Performance

>Each child’s school performance data were gathered directly from school records at age 7 by research staff who were trained for interrater reliability and included the presence of the following for the current school year: (1) individualized education plan (IEP); (2) special education conditions (SEs) including mental retardation, learning disabilities, behavioral/emotional impairment, orthopedic impairment, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, speech/language impairment, and physical or other health impairment; (3) special education classes (SECs) including math, language arts, social studies, science, and conduct (work effort, interpersonal skills); (4) support services (SSs) including occupational therapy, physical therapy, speech and language, counseling/behavior management, reading tutoring/assistance, math tutoring/assistance, and gifted and talented enrichment; and (5) speech and language services (SLSs), which include any service or designation from the previous categories that involves speech, language, or reading. All examiners were blind to prenatal and caregiver substance use.

Statistical Methods

One-way analysis of variance and x2 were used for continuous and categorical variables, respectively. Logistic regression was used to examine the effect of PCE on 7-year special education outcomes, including the presence of an IEP, SE, SEC, SS, and SLS, controlling for covariates. Covariates included a priori were gender, low birth weight (LBW) (<1500 g), and study site. Additional characteristics were examined in preliminary analyses as candidate covariates (Table 1 and Table 2). Candidate covariates that were correlated with PCE and the outcome measures (P ≤ .10) were included in the logistic regression analysis. Measures that met this criterion included small for gestational age, defined as gender-specific weight < 10th percentile for gestational age; ethnicity (white versus nonwhite); low SES at 7 years (Hollingshead Index of Social Position level 5); any primary caregiver change (≥1); poor quality of the home environment as assessed by using the HOME total averaged at 10 months and 5.5 years and recoded into a dichotomous variable (lowest 20% cutoff); Medicaid insurance status; prenatal tobacco use; low caregiver IQ (<85); caregiver marital status at 1 month; maternal age at birth (in years); caregiver depression as indicated by average Beck Depression Inventory assessed at 4 months, 30 months, and 5.5 years and recoded into a dichotomous variable (score of >17, indicating moderate to severe depression); and any child abuse.

TABLE 2.

Sample Characteristics According to Cocaine Exposure

Characteristic PCE
(n= 398
[42.2%])
Comparison
(n= 545
[57.8%])
P
Maternal characteristics
  Race, n (%) .270
    White 38 (9.5) 74 (13.6)
    Black 334 (83.9) 435 (79.8)
    Hispanic 21 (5.3) 31 (5.7)
    Other 5 (1.3) 5 (0.9)
  Low SES (Hollingshead 5), 1 mo, n (%) 109 (29.1) 121 (22.4) .020
  Marital status, single, n (%) 360 (90.7) 409 (75.0) .000
  Insurance, Medicaid, n (%) 347 (87.2) 427 (78.3) .000
  Education <12y, n (%) 201 (50.5) 172 (31.6) .000
  IQ, mean (SD) 70.98 (15.97) 74.47 (18.22) .003
  No prenatal care, n (%) 87 (21.9) 20 (3.7) .000
  Age, mean (SD), y 30.28 (4.61) 26.70 (5.85) .000
Prenatal drug use, n (%)a
  Cocaine use 398 (100)
  Heavy cocaine use 92 (23.1)
  Alcohol use 302 (75.9) 271 (49.7) .000
  Heavy alcohol use 92 (23.1) 27 (5.0) .000
  Tobacco use 325 (81.7) 162 (29.7) .000
  Heavy tobacco use 121 (30.4) 61 (11.2) .000
  Marijuana use 162 (40.7) 51 (9.4) .000
  Heavy marijuana use 26 (6.5) 9 (1.7) .000
Postnatal caregiving environment,
  n (%)
  Postnatal drug useb
    Cocaine use 83 (20.9) 2 (0.4) .000
    Heavy cocaine use 69 (17.4) 4 (0.7) .000
    Alcohol use 313 (78.8) 371 (68.3) .000
    Heavy alcohol use 164 (41.3) 105 (19.4) .000
    Tobacco use 320 (80.6) 231 (42.5) .000
    Heavy tobacco use 203 (51.1) 166 (30.7) .000
    Marijuana use 119 (30.0) 90 (16.6) .000
    Heavy marijuana use 39 (9.8) 29 (5.4) .009
  Caregiver depression 42 (10.7) 62 (11.4) .720
  Low quality of HOME, low 20% 88 (22.2) 111 (20.9) .600
  Primary caregiver change, 1 mo to 7 y 216 (54.4) 73 (13.4) .000
  Low SES (Hollingshead 5), 7 y 97 (24.4) 67 (12.3) .000
  Child abuse, 1 mo to 7 y 16 (4.0) 13 (2.4) .151
  Domestic violence, 4–7 y 44 (11.1) 31 (5.7) .003
Newborn medical characteristics and
  child intelligence
  Gestational age, mean (SD), wk 36.09 (4.04) 36.19 (4.10) .700
  Birth weight, mean (SD), g 2554 (756) 2661 (876) .050
  Length, mean (SD), cm 46.40 (4.81) 46.88 (5.30) .160
  Head circumference, mean (SD), cm 31.92 (2.84) 32.16 (3.22) .230
  LBW, <1500g, % 46 (11.6) 65 (11.9) .860
  SGA, n (%) 113 (28.5) 108 (19.9) .002
  Apgar score, 1 min, median (range) 8 (7–9) 8 (1–10) .840
  Apgar score, 5 min, median (range) 9 (8–9) 9 (5–10) .170
  Male, n (%) 208 (52.3) 283 (51.9) .919
  First born, n (%) 33 (8.3) 156 (28.6) .000
  Child IQ, mean (SD) 83.63 (14.32) 86.17 (15.08) .016
  Low child IQ, <85, n (%) 179 (52.3) 201 (44.0) .019

SGA indicates small for gestational age.

a

Heavy use determined by MISU at 1 month from mothers who reported any use at initial screening.

b

Reported bycaregiver via Caretaker Inventory ofSubstance Use at any point from4 months to 7 years.

Regressions for each outcome variable used stepwise elimination of the covariates that made the least contribution to the models and had the least effect on other parameters. The eliminated covariates were then added back to the model in a stepwise manner to test for confounding effects. All covariates in the final models were required to have significant contribution (P < .10) to the outcomes except for the 3 selected a priori (gender, LBW, and study site).

Two sets of logistic regression analyses were performed: with and without low child IQ (score of <85 on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children III) as a covariate. The IEP evaluation process involves a multi-disciplinary approach35 and often uses several diagnostic tools. Because IQ scores are a predictor of educational achievement and are often reviewed when assessing a student for the necessity of an IEP and other special education services, we thought that it was important to investigate the interactions of the hypothesized cocaine effects with low child IQ on special education.

RESULTS

Retention

Of the 1388 infants (658 PCE/opiate exposed and 730 comparison children) seen at the 1-month visit, 1023 had 7-year school outcome data. Among the 1023 children, 79 were opiate users and 1 had incomplete data and were not included in this study, resulting in a final sample of 943 children. Compared with children who were not included in the study, included children were more likely to be black, have lower SES, be from mothers with lower IQ, and have less prenatal tobacco exposure (any or heavy; Table 1). In addition, those with 7-year data had a higher percentage of caregivers who had postnatal alcohol (any or heavy) and marijuana use (any or heavy). Included children were also more likely to have a low HOME score and to have lived in environments with domestic violence. There were no significant differences in newborn medical characteristics or child IQ between the 2 groups.

Sample Description According to Prenatal Cocaine Exposure

Children in this study with PCE were more likely than comparison children to be born to families with low SES at 1 month; come from single-caregiver households; be on Medicaid; and have mothers who had less education, lower IQ, and no prenatal care and were older (Table 2). They were also more likely to have prenatal exposure to alcohol (any and heavy), tobacco (any and heavy), and marijuana (any and heavy). They also were exposed to more postnatal cocaine (any and heavy), alcohol (any and heavy), tobacco (any and heavy), and marijuana (any and heavy); had greater probability of primary caretaker change and low SES; and had a greater likelihood of living in a home with domestic violence. Children with PCE were also more likely to be small for gestational age, less likely to be the first born child, and more likely to have lower IQ.

Of the 267 mothers who reported prenatal cocaine use, 47.9% reported use during all trimesters, 14.6% during the first and second trimesters only, and 14.6% during the first trimester only. Overall reported use declined progressively over the trimesters (81.3%, 75.4%, and 66.7% in the first, second, and third trimesters, respectively) as did heavy use (23.1%, 16.8%, and 11.8%, respectively), whereas some use remained relatively constant (31.7%, 33.9%, and 32.4%, respectively). These trends are similar to those reported previously.30 Of 92 women who reported heavy use in the first trimester, 22.8% continued heavy use in the first 2 trimesters and 32.6% continued heavy use throughout all 3 trimesters. Of 126 who reported some use during the first trimester, 50% continued throughout all 3 trimesters.

Special Education Outcomes

In unadjusted logistic regression analysis, children with PCE (16.5%) were more likely than children in the comparison group (11%) to have an IEP (odds ratio: 1.599 [95% confidence interval: 1.091–2.344]; P = .016). There were no effects of PCE on SEs, SSs, SECs, or SLSs.

Complete data for each outcome model controlling for covariates were available for 737 to 916 children. Logistic regression analyses with low child IQ as a covariate (Table 3) showed that children with PCE were more likely to have an IEP. Increased likelihood of an IEP was also related to male gender, LBW, white race, low child IQ, and site. No PCE effect was found on SEs, SSs, SECs, and SLSs. LBW and low child IQ were consistently related to the presence of all outcomes. Site was related to all outcomes except SECs. White race was associated with more SLSs.

TABLE 3.

Odds Ratio (95% CI) for Factors That Predict Education Outcomes in Logistic Regression Models

Factor IEP
SE
SS
SEC
SLS
With IQ
[n=781
[12.7%])
Without IQ
[n =831
[13.8%])
With IQ
[n =781
[13.3%])
Without IQ
[n =898
[14.0%])
With IQ
[n =790
[17.8%])
Without IQ
[n =909
[18.2%])
With IQ
[n =737
[7.6%])
Without IQ
[n =839
[8.1%])
With IQ
[n =784
[17.9%])
Without IQ
(n = 916
[17.8%])
PCE 179 (1.14–2.83) 1.23 (0.77–1.98) 1.49 (0.96–2.32) 1.21 (0.77–1.92) 1.35 (0.91–2.00) 1.49 (1.04–2.12) 1.18 (0.67–2.07) 0.987 (0.589–1.65) 1.13 (0.77–1.68) 1.31 (0.92–1.87)
    P .010 .380 .080 .410 .130 .030 .580 .960 .530 .130
Male gender 1.69 (1.06–2.70) 2.44 (1.57–3.78) 1.48 (0.94–2.32) 2.09 (1.39–3.16) 1.44 (0.97–2.13) 1.62 (1.13–2.33) 1.33 (0.75–2.38) 1.64 (0.97–2.75) 142 (0.96–2.10) 1.66 (1.16–2.37)
    P .030 .000 .090 .000 .070 .010 .330 .064 .080 .010
LBW 2.25 (1.22–4.13) 2.58 (1.49–4.48) 2.21 (1.21–4.04) 2.48 (1.46–4.21) 2.42 (140–4.20) 2.79 (1.73–4.50) 2.67 (1.36–5.21) 2.75 (1.50–5.05) 1.90 (1.10–3.30) 2.35 (146–3.80)
    P .010 .001 .010 .001 .002 .000 .004 .001 .020 .000
SGA 0.64 (0.40–1.01) 0.67 (0.43–1.06)
    P .060 .084
White race 2.06 (1.02–4.13) 2.60 (1.36–4.97) 1.67 (0.93–3.01) 1.70 (0.94–3.08) 1.57 (0.92–2.69) 1.90 (1.04–3.46) 1.68 (0.99–2.87)
    P .040 .004 .090 .080 .100 .040 .060
Low caregiver IQ 1.37 (1.10–1.69) 1.29 (0.96–1.73) 1.40 (1.07–1.84)
    P .004 .090 .020
Caregiver married 2.21 (0.91–5.37)
    P .080
HOME scale (lowest 20%) 1.66 (1.03–2.68) 1.82 (1.16–2.87) 1.53 (1.02–2.31) 1.83 (1.23–2.72)
    P .040 .010 .040 .003
Any caregiver change 1.75 (1.07–2.86) 1.50 (0.94–2.40)
    P .030 .090
Low SES, 7 y 1.54 (0.94–2.52)
    P .090
Low child IQ(<85) 4.99 (2.90–8.59) 3.78 (2.31–6.20) 2.76 (1.80–4.23) 3.55 (1.81–6.97) 3.33 (2.17–5.10)
    P .000 .000 .000 .000 .000
Site P .000 .060 .000 .001 .000 .000 .146 .580 .000 .001
Total R2 0.199 0.135 0.182 0.125 0.190 0.127 0.129 0.076 0.176 0.111

SGA indicates small for gestational age; —, indicate the factor was not included in the model due to lack of statistically significant correlation (P< .10) with the outcome measure.

In logistic regression analyses without low child IQ as a covariate (Table 3), PCE increased the likelihood of SSs but not IEPs, SEs, SECs, or SLSs. Having an IEP was related to male gender, LBW, white race, low caregiver IQ, low HOME score, and changes in caregiver. SEs were predicted by male gender, LBW, low HOME score, and site. Along with PCE, SSs were related to male gender, LBW, low HOME score, and site. SECs were predicted by LBW and low caregiver IQ. Increased SLSs were related to male gender, LBW, low HOME score, and site.

Our preliminary, univariate analyses did not find any correlations between the levels of prenatal cocaine use and any special education outcomes. We further tested the effect of heavy prenatal cocaine by controlling for the effects of heavy prenatal tobacco, alcohol, and marijuana use, as well as the dosage-response effects of these drugs (number of cigarettes per day, number of absolute alcohol drinks per day, and number of marijuana joints per day); however, no significant effects of heavy cocaine use were found.

DISCUSSION

PCE and Special Education Outcomes

We found that PCE increased the risk for receiving an IEP and SS with adjustment for covariates. Our findings compliment previously reported deficits in intelligence7,14,3639 and component academic skills, including learning disabilities, poor sustained attention, visual-motor integration and visuospatial memory, and more disorganized and less abstract thinking in this population5,36,38,4046 and suggest that these deficits generalize to the school setting. The rate of services received through IEPs for both those with PCE (16.5%) and comparison children (11%) was significantly higher than the national average of 6.8% in 2003 for children in kindergarten through third grade,47 reflecting the high-risk nature of this sample and their need for continued educational services.

The cocaine effect on IEPs was observed with IQ in the model in addition to other covariates, indicating that the IEP referral was not simply attributable to lower IQ in children with PCE. The cocaine effect on SSs was observed without but not with IQ in the model, suggesting that in this case, IQ could mediate the effects of PCE on SSs. Conversely, children with PCE were not more likely to receive SE, SEC, or SLS services. This could suggest that the effects of PCE on school function are not in specific academic domains. These children may need more special education services in general but not any specific type of service. We had expected more SLS referrals for the PCE children on the basis of previous work14,1824; however, within the IEP group, 95.3% of the PCE group and 91.4% of comparison children also had SLSs, indicating that SLSs are a major area of recognized resource necessity in special education for this population regardless of PCE.

Effects of Other Covariates

LBW was a consistent predictor of all special education outcomes in all models. This supports previous work suggesting that LBW is associated with multiple, adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes, including increased need for special education.48 Similarly, that low child IQ was a predictor of all outcome models is to be expected, because IQ is often used in the assessment of special education. In addition, in all models, the inclusion of IQ explained a greater percentage of the variance and significantly changed the contributions of the other covariates (Table 3). Factors such as caregiver IQ, HOME score, and caregiver change were statistically significant only when IQ was excluded from the models, possibly suggesting complex interactions among intelligence, environmental, and genetic factors.49,50 For example, a problematic, highly stressed home environment can adversely affect IQ scores, and low child IQ scores could mediate the effects of poor home functioning on special education outcomes.

We also found that boys were more likely to receive an IEP than girls, supporting previous reports.47 Male gender also increased the likelihood of receiving SEs, SSs, and SLSs when IQ was not included in the models. This could be explained by the significantly greater percentage of boys (56.6%) versus girls (43.4%) in the low child IQ group (P = .03).

Our study also indicates that white race increases the likelihood of having an IEP and SLS, independent of other factors, including low SES and low child IQ. On closer inspection, we found that white students were more likely to receive an IEP at the Detroit site than nonwhite students (29% versus 11.9%; P = .007). This finding may also help to explain site effects (Table 3). None of the other sites had statistically significant differences in rates of IEPs or SLSs in white versus nonwhite samples. Enrollment in private versus public school was not a factor in IEPs or SLSs, because only 3 students with IEPs and 4 with SLSs were enrolled in private school. That race plays a role in determining special education services either as a proxy for other factors or as a causal factor requires additional investigation.

Study Limitations

There are several limitations to this study. First, the data are based on school record review. Although our staff was trained in how to abstract the data, we cannot verify the accuracy of records. Second, the data were gathered from several schools in 4 different states that may have had different thresholds for enrollment in special education. Although federal legislation mandates that each school must provide appropriate services for each child with a disability,35 states and school systems have flexibility about information that they require in an IEP. There is state-to-state variation on documentation of standards and assessments on IEP forms, including differences between the states included in this study.51 These forms are a primary source of information to guide decisions during IEP team meetings. Ultimately, a group of qualified professionals and the parents look at the child’s evaluation results and together decide whether the child is a “child with a disability,” as defined by Individuals With Disabilities Education Act,52 which is not a standardized process.

We also recognize that the special education outcomes are correlated; however, we believed that it was important to distinguish specific services because they have different educational implications for each child. Another possible limitation is that mothers were recruited at delivery and could represent a different population than women who are recruited during pregnancy. Although that they were asked to report their drug use over the entire pregnancy could be affected by recall bias, there is evidence that postnatal self-report measures of maternal cocaine use are as effective as antenatal measures in predicting neurobehavioral outcomes.53 Finally, it is possible that we underestimated cocaine effects by adjusting for variables such as low IQ and LBW that are on the “causal pathway” between cocaine and the special education outcomes54; however, we believed that it would be too difficult to interpret special education findings without considering these factors.

Policy Implications

These findings have policy implications. There were 4 112 052 births in the United States in 200455; 3.9% of pregnant mothers reported illicit drug use in 2004–2005 in the past month.56 Although estimates vary, even a conservative estimate of 45 000 children born with PCE per year would suggest, on the basis of our study, that 12.7% will receive an IEP. Enrollment in special education in the United States costs an additional $8080 per year per student57 compared with nonenrolled students. The risk attributable to cocaine for an IEP is 1.79 – 1.00 = 0.79 (Table 3). Thus, additional cost per year for special education services as a result of PCE alone would total 45 000 cocaine births per year × 12.7/100 (baseline IEP rate) × 0.79 (excess risk attributable to cocaine) × $5918 (additional cost per child) = $26 718 882. This yearly cost would then have to be multiplied by the number of years the child receives special education services in school. “Investing” in early intervention might not only relieve long-term suffering in these children but also be cost-effective.

Illicit drugs, such as cocaine, may not be the only prenatal exposures that lead to an increased use of special education resources. Legal substances such as tobacco and alcohol, which were controlled in our study, may have effects on these outcomes. Findings of special education use in children with prenatal alcohol exposure but not fetal alcohol syndrome (comparable to our study children) have varied.5860 We found no studies that examined the effects of prenatal tobacco use on special education outcomes. These are important areas for future investigation.

What’s Known on This Subject.

Previous studies have reported deficits in intelligence, academic skills, language, sustained attention, visual motor integration, visuospatial memory, and abstract thinking in children with PCE. Only 1 study has examined school outcomes in this population.

What This Study Adds.

No previous studies have examined enrollment in special education in this population, which is vulnerable to cognitive and academic problems. We studied this in a large, prospective, multisite study that examined neurodevelopmental outcomes in children with PCE.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This study was conducted with support from a research award sponsored by the Elaine Schlosser Lewis Fund of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry as well as the National Institute of Mental Health Institutional Research Training Grant to Rhode Island Hospital (T32MH19927, principal investigator: Gregory Fritz, MD).

Support was also received from the National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development through cooperative agreements and interagency agreement with the National Institute on Drug Abuse; Administration on Children, Youth and Families; and Center for Substance Abuse Treatment. Participating institutions, grant awards, investigators, and key research personnel include Brown University, U10 HD 27904, N01-HD-2-3159 (Barry M. Lester, PhD, Cindy Loncar, PhD, Linda LaGasse, PhD, and Jean Twomey, PhD); University of Miami, U10 HD 21397 (Charles R. Bauer, MD, Wendy Griffin, RN, and Elizabeth Jacque, RN); University of Tennessee, U10 HD 21415 (Henrietta S. Bada, MD, Charlotte Bursi, MSSW, Marilyn Williams, MSW, Deloris Lee, MSW, Lillie Hughey, MSW, and Kimberly Yolton, PhD); Wayne State University, U10 HD 21385 (Seetha Shankaran, MD, Eunice Woldt, MSN, and Jay Ann Nelson, BSN); RTI International, U01 HD 36790 (W. Kenneth Poole, PhD, Abhik Das, PhD, and Jane Hammond, PhD); National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (Linda L. Wright, MD, and Rosemary Higgins, MD); and National Institute on Drug Abuse (Vincent L. Smeriglio, PhD).

Abbreviations

PCE

prenatal cocaine exposure

SES

socioeconomic status

MISU

Maternal Interview of Substance Use

HOME

Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment

IEP

individualized education plan

SE

special education conditions

SEC

special education classes

SS

support services

SLS

speech and language services

LBW

low birth weight

Footnotes

The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Zuckerman B, Frank DA. “Crack kids”: not broken. Pediatrics. 1992;89(2):337–339. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Harris LH, Paltrow LMSJAMA. The status of pregnant women and fetuses in US criminal law. JAMA. 2003;289(13):1697–1699. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.13.1697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Bauer CR, Langer JC, Shankaran S, et al. Acute neonatal effects of cocaine exposure during pregnancy. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2005;159(9):824–834. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.159.9.824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bada HS, Das A, Bauer CR, et al. Impact of prenatal cocaine exposure on child behavior problems through school age. Pediatrics. 2007;119(2) doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1404. Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/119/2/e348. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 5.Delaney-Black V, Covington C, Templin T, et al. Teacher-assessed behavior of children prenatally exposed to cocaine. Pediatrics. 2000;106(4):782–791. doi: 10.1542/peds.106.4.782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Delaney-Black V, Covington C, Nordstrom B, et al. Prenatal cocaine: quantity of exposure and gender moderation. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2004;25(4):254–263. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200408000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Singer LT, Minnes S, Short E, et al. Cognitive outcomes of preschool children with prenatal cocaine exposure. JAMA. 2004;291(20):2448–2456. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.20.2448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Frank DA, Rose-Jacobs R, Beeghly M, Wilbur M, Bellinger D, Cabral H. Level of prenatal cocaine exposure and 48-month IQ: importance of preschool enrichment. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2005;27(1):15–28. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2004.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Nulman I, Rovet J, Altmann D, Bradley C, Einarson T, Koren G. Neurodevelopment of adopted children exposed in utero to cocaine. CMAJ. 1994;151(11):1591–1597. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Hurt H, Malmud E, Betancourt L, Braitman LE, Brodsky NL, Giannetta J. Children with in utero cocaine exposure do not differ from control subjects on intelligence testing. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 1997;151(12):1237–1241. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.1997.02170490063011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kilbride H, Castor C, Hoffman E, Fuger KL. Thirty-six-month outcome of prenatal cocaine exposure for term or near-term infants: impact of early case management. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2000;21(1):19–26. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200002000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Wasserman GA, Kline JK, Bateman DA, et al. Prenatal cocaine exposure and school-age intelligence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1998;50(3):203–210. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(98)00037-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Chasnoff IJ, Anson A, Hatcher R, Stenson H, Iaukea K, Randolph LA. Prenatal exposure to cocaine and other drugs: outcome at four to six years. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1998;846:314–328. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Lester BM, LaGasse LL, Seifer R. Cocaine exposure and children: the meaning of subtle effects. Science. 1998;282(5389):633–634. doi: 10.1126/science.282.5389.633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Frank DA, Augustyn M, Knight WG, Pell T, Zuckerman B. Growth, development, and behavior in early childhood following prenatal cocaine exposure: a systematic review. JAMA. 2001;285(12):1613–1625. doi: 10.1001/jama.285.12.1613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Nordstrom Bailey B, Sood BG, Sokol RJ, et al. Gender and alcohol moderate prenatal cocaine effects on teacher-report of child behavior. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2005;27(2):181–189. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2004.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Richardson GA, Conroy ML, Day NL. Prenatal cocaine exposure: effects on the development of school-age children. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1996;18(6):627–634. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(96)00121-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bandstra ES, Morrow CE, Vogel AL, et al. Longitudinal influence of prenatal cocaine exposure on child language functioning. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2002;24(3):297–308. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(02)00192-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bandstra ES, Vogel AL, Morrow CE, Xue L, Anthony JC. Severity of prenatal cocaine exposure and child language functioning through age seven years: a longitudinal latent growth curve analysis. Subst Use Misuse. 2004;39(1):25–59. doi: 10.1081/JA-120027765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Beeghly M, Martin B, Rose-Jacobs R, et al. Prenatal cocaine exposure and children’s language functioning at 6 and 9.5 years: moderating effects of child age, birthweight, and gender. J Pediatr Psychol. 2006;31(1):98–115. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsj028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Lewis BA, Singer LT, Short EJ, et al. Four-year language outcomes of children exposed to cocaine in utero. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2004;26(5):617–627. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2004.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Mentis M. In utero cocaine exposure and language development. Semin Speech Lang. 1998;19(2):147–164. doi: 10.1055/s-2008-1064042. quiz 165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Morrow CE, Bandstra ES, Anthony JC, Ofir AY, Xue L, Reyes MB. Influence of prenatal cocaine exposure on early language development: longitudinal findings from four months to three years of age. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2003;24(1):39–50. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200302000-00009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Lewis BA, Kirchner HL, Short EJ, et al. Prenatal cocaine and tobacco effects on children’s language trajectories. Pediatrics. 2007;120(1) doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-2563. Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/120/1/e78. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 25.Hurt H, Malmud E, Betancourt L, Brodsky NL, Giannetta J. A prospective evaluation of early language development in children with in utero cocaine exposure and in control subjects. J Pediatr. 1997;130(2):310–312. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(97)70361-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Hawley TL, Halle TG, Drasin RE, Thomas NG. Children of addicted mothers: effects of the ‘crack epidemic’ on the caregiving environment and the development of preschoolers. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 1995;65(3):364–379. doi: 10.1037/h0079693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Hurt H, Brodsky NL, Roth H, Malmud E, Giannetta JM. School performance of children with gestational cocaine exposure. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2005;27(2):203–211. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2004.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Bauer CR, Shankaran S, Bada HS, et al. The Maternal Lifestyle Study: drug exposure during pregnancy and short-term maternal outcomes. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2002;186(3):487–495. doi: 10.1067/mob.2002.121073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Lester BM, ElSohly M, Wright LL, et al. The Maternal Lifestyle Study: drug use by meconium toxicology and maternal self-report. Pediatrics. 2001;107(2):309–317. doi: 10.1542/peds.107.2.309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Lester BM, Tronick EZ, LaGasse L, et al. The maternal lifestyle study: effects of substance exposure during pregnancy on neurodevelopmental outcome in 1-month-old infants. Pediatrics. 2002;110(6):1182–1192. doi: 10.1542/peds.110.6.1182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Jacobson SW, Jacobson JL, Sokol RJ, Martier SS, Chiodo LM. New evidence for neurobehavioral effects of in utero cocaine exposure. J Pediatr. 1996;129(4):581–590. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(96)70124-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hollingshead A. Four Factor Index of Social Status. New Haven, CT: Yale University; 1975. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Cirino PT, Chin CE, Sevcik RA, Wolf M, Lovett M, Morris RD. Measuring socioeconomic status: reliability and preliminary validity for different approaches. Assessment. 2002;9(2):145–155. doi: 10.1177/10791102009002005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Caldwell B, Bradley R. Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME) Administration Manual. Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas Press; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.American Academy of Pediatrics, Committee on Children With Disabilities. The pediatrician’s role in development and implementation of an Individual Education Plan (IEP) and/or an Individual Family Service Plan (IFSP) Pediatrics. 1999;104(1 pt 1):124–127. doi: 10.1542/peds.104.1.124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Richardson GA. Prenatal cocaine exposure: a longitudinal study of development. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1998;846:144–152. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.LaGasse L, Lester B, Seifer R, Bauer CR, et al. Prenatal cocaine exposure and cognitive development at school age [abstract] Pediatr Res. 2004;55(5):69A. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Arendt RE, Short EJ, Singer LT, et al. Children prenatally exposed to cocaine: developmental outcomes and environmental risks at seven years of age. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2004;25(2):83–90. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200404000-00002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Bennett DS, Bendersky M, Lewis M. Children’s intellectual and emotional-behavioral adjustment at 4 years as a function of cocaine exposure, maternal characteristics, and environmental risk. Dev Psychol. 2002;38(5):648–658. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.38.5.648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Loebstein R, Koren G. Pregnancy outcome and neurodevelopment of children exposed in utero to psychoactive drugs: the Motherisk experience. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 1997;22(3):192–196. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Noland JS, Singer LT, Short EJ, et al. Prenatal drug exposure and selective attention in preschoolers. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2005;27(3):429–438. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2005.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Schroder MD, Snyder PJ, Sielski I, Mayes L. Impaired performance of children exposed in utero to cocaine on a novel test of visuospatial working memory. Brain Cogn. 2004;55(2):409–412. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.02.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bandstra ES, Morrow CE, Anthony JC, Accornero VH, Fried PA. Longitudinal investigation of task persistence and sustained attention in children with prenatal cocaine exposure. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2001;23(6):545–559. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(01)00181-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Delaney-Black V, Covington C, Templin T, Ager J, Martier S, Sokol R. Prenatal cocaine exposure and child behavior. Pediatrics. 1998;102(4 pt 1):945–950. doi: 10.1542/peds.102.4.945. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Savage J, Brodsky NL, Malmud E, Giannetta JM, Hurt H. Attentional functioning and impulse control in cocaine-exposed and control children at age ten years. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2005;26(1):42–47. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Morrow CE, Culbertson JL, Accornero VH, Xue L, Anthony JC, Bandstra ES. Learning disabilities and intellectual functioning in school-aged children with prenatal cocaine exposure. Dev Neuropsychol. 2006;30(3):905–931. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn3003_8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Child Trends DataBank. [Accessed April 15];Individualized education plan. Available at: www.childtrendsdatabank.org/indicators/98IndividualizedEducationPlan.cfm.
  • 48.Rodrigues MC, Mello RR, Fonseca SC. Learning difficulties in schoolchildren born with very low birth weight[in Portuguese] J Pediatr (Rio J) 2006;82(1):6–14. doi: 10.2223/JPED.1429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Turkheimer E, Haley A, Waldron M, D’Onofrio B, Gottesman II. Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychol Sci. 2003;14(6):623–628. doi: 10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Scarr S, Weinberg RA. The Minnesota Adoption Studies: genetic differences and malleability. Child Dev. 1983;54(2):260–267. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Thompson SJ, Thurlow ML, Quenemoen RF, Esler A, Whetstone P. [Accessed December 4, 2007];Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota, National Center on Educational Outcomes; Addressing Standards and Assessments on State IEP Forms (Synthesis Report 38) 2001 Available at: http://education.umn.edu/NCEO/OnlinePubs/Synthesis38.html.
  • 52.Office of Special Education and Rehabilitative Services. A guide to the individualized education program. [Accessed December 4, 2007]; Available at: www.ed.gov/parents/needs/speced/iepguide/index.html.
  • 53.Jacobson SW, Chiodo LM, Sokol RJ, Jacobson JL. Validity of maternal report of prenatal alcohol, cocaine, and smoking in relation to neurobehavioral outcome. Pediatrics. 2002;109(5):815–825. doi: 10.1542/peds.109.5.815. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Tu YK, West R, Ellison GT, Gilthorpe MS. Why evidence for the fetal origins of adult disease might be a statistical artifact: the “reversal paradox” for the relation between birth weight and blood pressure in later life. Am J Epidemiol. 2005;161(1):27–32. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwi002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Martin JA, Hamilton BE, Sutton PD, Ventura SJ, Menacker F, Kirmeyer S. Births: final data for 2004. Natl Vital Stat Rep. 2006;55(1):1–101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. [Accessed July 20, 2007];Rockville, MD: Office of Applied Studies; Results From the 2005 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: National Finding. 2006 NSDUH Series H-30, DHHS publication SMA 06-4194 Available at: www.oas.samhsa.gov/NSDUH/2k5NSDUH/2k5results.htm.
  • 57.Chambers JG, Parrish TB, Harr JJ. What are we spending on special education services in the United States, 1999–2000? [Accessed May 7, 2008];Special Education Expenditure Project, Center for Special Education Finance. 2004 June; Available at: http://csef.air.org/publications/seep/national/advrpt1.pdf.
  • 58.Autti-Rämö I. Twelve-year follow-up of children exposed to alcohol in utero. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2000;42(6):406–411. doi: 10.1017/s0012162200000748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Greenbaum R, Nulman I, Rovet J, Koren G. The Toronto experience in diagnosing alcohol-related neurodevelopmental disorder: a unique profile of deficits and assets. Can J Clin Pharmacol. 2002;9(4):215–225. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Howell KK, Lynch ME, Platzman KA, Smith GH, Coles CD. Prenatal alcohol exposure and ability, academic achievement, and school functioning in adolescence: a longitudinal follow-up. J Pediatr Psychol. 2006;31(1):116–126. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsj029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES