4. Summary of recent advances
Multielectrode recording experiments let us measure correlations between the activity of individual neurons and the neural circuits in which they are embedded. Recently, multielectrode studies have been emphasizing how correlated neuronal activity is linked with behavior. Decisions are fundamental to voluntary behavior. Here, we discuss computations necessary to turn a decision into an action and review progress in studying correlated neural activity in areas of the brain which link sensory and motor representations. The themes that emerge are that correlated patterns of activity in populations of neurons can be revealed by measurements of field potential fluctuations and that these measurements can relate the activity of individual neurons to the activity of populations of neurons distributed across different regions of the brain. Investigations into patterns of neuronal correlation are helping us to understand how decisions and other cognitive processes result from the interactions between different brain systems that are responsible for controlling and regulating our behavior.
5. Introduction
Decisions have an almost universal appeal. We experience our own decisions and the sense we have of choosing. We speak of cells choosing to divide or to die, of algorithms that divide inputs into different outputs, and of rational markets. Even the neurophysiogical study of decision, with its emphasis on animal models and the simplifications that implies, brings many different influences such as sensation, reward, emotion, learning and memory to our understanding of making a decision.
Here, the focus is on how analyzing neuronal correlations can inform sensory-motor decisions: Decisions that involve a voluntary, flexible association between a set of sensory inputs and a motor output that is modifiable by experience. We will start by reviewing recent evidence that sensory-motor decisions can be viewed as the combination of two processes. One process assigns values to sensory stimuli and another process leads to the movement expressing the choice. Valuations involve making predictions about the outcomes of different choices, the potential rewards and costs of choices. Choice, in turn, involves weighing the valuations for different alternatives and selecting one to act on.
We then highlight how analyzing neuronal correlations may reveal how neural circuits perform distributed processes like valuation and choice. Understanding the distributed neural computations that underlie decision making and other cognitive processes represents a major challenge for systems neuroscience. A rapidly growing body of work is developing a new approach that uses the dynamics of neural activity to link the spiking activity of neurons to the neural circuits within which they are embedded. A notable direction in current research is the emerging spike-field approach which has the potential to resolve how information is transformed between different regions of the brain.
Sensory-motor decisions activate distributed brain networks
To allow voluntary, flexible associations, sensory-motor decisions must integrate incoming sensory information with prior experiences involving costs and rewards. An important issue in understanding voluntary behavior is understanding the neural circuit mechanisms by which information about decisions, such as costs and rewards, ultimately drives movements.
The frontal and parietal cortices comprise the classical sensory-motor association cortex and contain areas responsible for integrating sensory information to guide ongoing motor behavior. The functional roles of the frontal-parietal cortices involve sensory, motor and cognitive aspects and individual areas are specialized for representing different parts of the body such as the eye, arm and hand 1-3. In the context of decision making and preparation for choice, there is strong evidence that the valuation of an alternative modulates the firing of parietal neurons that have been implicated in planning eye and arm movements (for a review see, 4). The activity of these neurons is, in turn thought to bias processing in frontal regions and, ultimately, which movements are made. When movements are being selected there is increasing evidence that the process of selecting an alternative for action and preparing action do not occur sequentially in time. Instead, movement planning occurs concurrently across frontal and parietal areas for different currently available alternatives before one alternative is used to generate a movement 5. Choice information does not always emerge across frontal and parietal cortex at the same time. In top-down, experience-based tasks, frontal areas precede parietal areas in signaling selection 6-8. A useful conceptual framework for these results is that choices are made in the frontal-parietal networks through competition between different action representations.
Competition between different action representations in the frontal-parietal cortices may incorporate value signals that bias different outcomes by taking in inputs from other areas. Areas of the limbic cortex, prefrontal cortex and related sub-cortical structures could provide these inputs. These areas are believed to serve a modulatory role and provide information necessary to regulate on-going behavior, for example by monitoring performance and signaling rewards. Such areas are well-positioned to bias frontal-parietal action representations. An established line of work has studied how rewards are processed by the dopaminergic system. Current theories posit that reward processing involves the signaling of reward prediction errors by dopaminergic neurons which project to the ventral striatum.
Unlike rewards which can be objectively determined, costs and other biases are subjective in nature. How subjective choices are processed in the brain has historically received relatively little attention but costs are likely to play an important role in shaping many behaviors. In one recent study, costs were seen to drive choice behavior. Monkeys were presented with three alternatives and rewards were randomly assigned to each alternative with equal probability. Even though the reward distribution could not be learned, each animal adopted a specific strategy for performing the task which reflected the effort associated with moving to select each choice8. Several studies have examined subjective choice using an inter-temporal choice paradigm in which subjects discount delayed rewards9-11. Temporal discounting is inherently subjective and the tendency to discount larger, delayed rewards and prefer smaller, immediate rewards, differs between subjects and correlates with other personality traits. Brain imaging during inter-temporal choice reveals that activity in the ventral striatum, medial prefrontal cortex and posterior cingulate cortex track subjective valuations expressed by choices for the timing of primary and monetary rewards 12,10,11. In monkeys, electrical recordings of neurons in the striatum and anterior cingulate cortex support this picture and identify a network of areas specifically involved in representing the costs and rewards of actions 13-15. In addition to temporal delays, costs can depend on the effort required to obtain a reward. In rats, lesions to orbitofrontal cortex cause impulsive choices but not effort-based decisions while anterior cingulate cortex lesions lead to apathy but not impulsiveness 16. Evaluating the costs of time and effort may depend on distinct brain regions within frontal cortex and depend on different decision circuits.
These considerations can be conceptually summarized into two different systems, a sensory-motor choice system and decision theoretic valuation system17. These systems are distributed across different regions of the brain. A major challenge facing the neurobiological understanding of decision making is understanding how valuation drives choice to transform decisions into actions. Addressing this challenge requires the development of new techniques and concepts for studying neural circuits. These neural circuit tools will be broadly relevant to understanding many cognitive processes such as those involved in perception, action, attention, learning and memory.
Using correlated neural activity to understand neural circuits
Understanding the computational function of interactions across neural circuits, why brain regions communicate, involves eavesdropping on the activity of groups of neurons while computations are being performed. Neural ensemble recordings are increasingly focusing on characterizing the correlations in neural activity between brain regions. The premise is that the presence or absence of correlations in neural activity across a circuit reflects the underlying computational demands being placed on the circuit. Pair-wise correlations, higher dimensional subspaces and manifolds, information metrics, and model-based analyses of neural signals have all come to the fore in recent years with the goal of resolving correlations in neural circuits18-26.
The massive divergence and convergence of synaptic connectivity within the neuropil mean that individual neurons have measurable correlations mainly with neurons in local microcircuits27. A recent study reexamining V1 neuron correlations within local circuits finds pairs of individual neurons can be strikingly uncorrelated in their activity, although the role of stimulus size is likely to be important and remains to be accounted for28. The relative lack of correlation between spike trains from pairs of neurons makes sense when you consider that even the activity of a neuron firing at 50 Hz is relatively sparse point process. A barrage of synaptic events driven by correlated inputs may still fail to raise the membrane potential to the threshold for action potential initiation29.
The relative lack of correlation in the activity of pairs of neurons should not, however, be taken to imply that the activity of groups of neurons does not show strong correlations. In fact, there is strong evidence that populations of neurons which have only weak correlations between pairs of neurons can form correlated states with strong collective dynamics. Collective dynamics in the spiking activity of groups of neurons have been characterized in the retina and in neuronal cell cultures18. In vivo evidence for collective dynamics is also available using neural ensemble recordings made in rats, monkeys, and humans30,19. The available evidence points to strongly collective states that extend across long-range circuits. Truccolo et al. used model-based analyses of recordings of neural ensembles in frontal-parietal cortex to show that activity of neurons in frontal motor cortex was as strongly correlated with other motor cortical neurons as it was with another group of neurons in parietal cortex19. These reports do not tie correlated states to specific behavioral processes, like making decisions, but the signs point in the right direction.
Measuring neural correlations with local field potentials
Local field potentials (LFPs) offer another approach to study neural circuits through correlated neural activity. LFPs are measured by placing an electrode within or directly on the surface of the brain. Unlike action potentials which are generated by individual neurons, LFPs predominantly measure synaptic potentials pooled across groups of neurons near the recording electrode and are closely linked to trans-membrane currents31. As a result, LFP signals reflect the presence of correlations in the activity of many neurons. Although recording from individual neurons is challenging, measuring LFPs is easier. Crucially, while LFP signals are related to EEG signals measured at the scalp because both measure voltages from pooled synaptic potentials, LFP signals can reveal quite different activity patterns than EEG signals. For example, unlike EEG, response fields calculated from LFP signals can be as informative as response fields measured from the activity of individual neurons32,33. The relationship between LFP signals and action potentials is hard to disentangle as correlated firing in groups of neurons can be measured in both action potentials and LFPs. Therefore, there are reasons to be concerned that LFP signals are biased by action potential recordings34. Increased confidence that LFP signals are not significantly biased by action potentials comes from observations that the coupling between action potentials and LFP signals varies in a task-dependent manner33 and that informative LFP signals can be measured on electrodes even when the spiking activity of neurons cannot35.
Since LFPs are sensitive to correlations in neural activity and are relatively easy to record, LFPs may play an important role in understanding how information is processed between different regions of the brain. The study of LFPs has a long history in, for example, the olfactory bulb and the hippocampus and has played a fundamental role in our understanding of sleep and clinical states like epilepsy. In contrast, the use of LFPs to study how information is processed in other neural circuits remains at a relatively early stage. That said, more and more work shows that LFPs measured in cortex and in subcortical structures like the basal ganglia, amygdala, and thalamus have rich dynamics reflecting strongly correlated states36-39. In the sensory-motor areas of the frontal and parietal cortex, studies have implicated LFPs in plans to make saccadic movements of the eyes33, reaching movements of the arm40, grasping movements of the hand41, ongoing motor control and processing of visual-motor information42,43, the orientation of spatial attention7, working memory33,44, and decision making8. Recent work using MEG extends some of these results to human parietal cortex45.
The mechanism of LFP measurements involve a complex dependence between the electrode and the current sources in the brain46. At root, measuring the LFP voltage involves summing across many neural elements with particular spatial and temporal correlations. If the currents generated by neural activity flow in different directions and are uncorrelated across the spatial extent of tissue, LFP signals will be small. LFPs also selectively represent neural signals that share temporal correlations such as those found when many neurons fire together in synchrony. Consequently, LFPs are intrinsically measures of neural correlation. Getting to the heart of LFP measurements involves understanding how correlated neural activity is reflected in the LFP.
Progress on understanding how current sources generate different features of LFP activity is most advanced in the hippocampal formation where the mechanisms and properties of different rhythms in the theta and gamma frequency bands are being elucidated, most recently through studies of travelling waves and coherence between CA1, CA3 and entorhinal cortical regions47,48. In the neocortex, studies of the mechanism of LFP measurements have focused on understanding how LFP signals propagate passively within tissue and place limitations on how LFPs are passively filtered by the cortex. Two recent studies examine how far LFP signals passively spread in cortex using very different approaches49,50. The results are remarkably similar. Katzner et al. optically image orientation selectivity columns and fit LFP responses to visual stimuli by blurring the orientation maps49. They find a best fit between LFP tuning and orientation selectivity when blurring with a Gaussian with radius of ~250 um. Xing et al. use the retinotopy of V1 to infer the spread of LFP signals driven by visual input50. They analyze the difference between the sizes of response fields measured using multiunit and LFP activity and reason that if the increased size of LFP response fields compared with multiunit response fields can be purely attributed to passive current spread, the LFP spreads by no more than ~250 um. This is an upper bound on the degree of spread due to passive mechanisms because the difference in response field size likely includes a contribution from active neural correlations. How much LFP signals at different frequencies can be distorted by capacitive components in cortical tissue has also been measured. Measuring the impedance of cortical tissue reveals that, at the frequencies of interest, < 300 Hz, cortical tissue behaves like an anisotropic ohmic resistor51. As a result, the passive propagation of LFP signals is influenced by cortical layers but is relatively undistorted by capacitive neural elements.
While these studies do not directly speak to how LFP signals reflect correlated neural activity, they provide important bounds beyond which active neural processes must shape LFP signals. The message is that correlations in LFP signals that extend beyond several hundred microns are due to active neural mechanisms and the fact that LFP power is present at lower temporal frequencies (< 100 Hz) and reduced at higher temporal frequencies (> 100 Hz) is not due to capacitive cortical filtering.
Measuring neural interactions between distant neural populations
Understanding how neural correlations can inform us about interactions between neural populations in different brain regions is likely to be essential for understanding distributed computations like the transformation of value into choice. One promising approach to understanding long-range interactions is a spike-field approach that depends on the combined analysis of spiking and LFP activity.
There are several reasons why analyzing both spiking and LFP activity could offer advantages over studying either signal alone. The distinction between action potentials and synaptic potentials means spiking and LFP activity represent different yet complementary aspects of neural processing. In the context of a distributed network, spiking of a population of neurons will form the output of an area while LFP activity may be more sensitive to the local processes within the area and inputs to the area from other areas. Dissociations between the content of spiking and LFP signals within an area could be informative about how an area transforms inputs into outputs52. In FEF, this line of argument has been used to implicate FEF as the site where selection for eye movements first emerges7. Supporting evidence comes from the properties spike and LFP responses in MT and MST to visual motion. LFPs in MST were found to encode motion with properties similar to spiking in MT while LFPs in MT had properties more similar to neurons in V138. The implication of this work is that LFPs encode the feed-forward sensory afferents into each area.
Reasoning about how spiking and LFP activity differentially encode decision signals may be effective in understanding how areas transform values into choices. There are many studies of how single unit activity encodes value-based signals in a variety of frontal and parietal regions. Studies of LFP activity in monkeys performing decision tasks that parametrically manipulate value-based signals have not yet been reported. Establishing whether the spike-field approach can track transformations between the interlinked brain regions that form decision circuits is an exciting new direction.
Another advantage of the spike-field approach involves the analysis of correlations in simultaneous recordings between brain regions. When faced with the task of understanding multiple brain areas, LFP signals may provide a measure of correlated local activity that can be linked to the activity of nearby neurons in the local circuit as well as to the activity of neurons in other regions. Unlike correlations between individual neurons, LFP signals in different brain regions display rich patterns of correlation. Such correlations likely reflect the underlying computational demands being placed on the circuit. The role of gamma band synchronization has received particular emphasis53 and correlations at other frequencies may also play an important role.
A growing number of spike-field studies report task-dependent long-range correlations, indicating that synchronization at specific frequencies may reflect inter-area communication6,8,54,55. Buschman and Miller examine prefrontal interactions with parietal cortex during attentional processing and link bottom-up attention with synchronization at 22-35 Hz while synchronization at 35-55 Hz is linked to top-down attention6. Gregoriou et al examine gamma frequency band interactions between FEF and V4 during attention and find evidence for a driving influence of activity in FEF on activity in V454. Saalman et al, find correlations between parietal area LIP and visual area MT are consistent with parietal neurons selectively increasing activity in the sensory area during attention55. During a decision making task, we found activity at a lower 15 Hz frequency band was correlated between the dorsal premotor cortex and the parietal reach region, two interconnected reach-related areas of the frontal and parietal cortex, when monkeys were freely choosing reach targets more than when they followed externally-imposed instructions8. Interestingly, in this study, the neurons that exhibited long-range correlation predicted the outcome of the decision earlier than neurons that did not suggesting that a decision circuit was activated between frontal and parietal cortex. Since freely choosing targets required costs and not rewards to drive choices, frontal-parietal correlations may be modulated by inputs from valuation system.
Overall, these and other similar studies open the door to using neural correlations to understand how brain areas communicate with each other to guide behavior. At this point, processing in particular frequency bands seems to be linked to specific brain circuits and, perhaps, cognitive processes. How these patterns of correlation activity reflect the communication of information between brain regions that drive behavioral processes remains an important, challenging goal for the future.
7. Conclusions
Analyzing neural correlations between spiking activity and LFP signals to identify interactions between distant areas is a new approach that is already starting to bear fruit. Simultaneous neural ensemble recordings from different brain regions, while technically more demanding than spike-field measurements, also have the potential to reveal how correlated states reflect neural computations that are distributed across brain regions. In this review, integration of value and choice activity across distributed neuronal networks when making a decision has been highlighted and illustrates a potentially general approach to tackling the challenge of understanding how interactions between different brain regions can drive behavior.
Highlights.
• • Gregoriou GG, Gotts SJ, Zhou H, Desimone R. High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attention. Science. 2009;324(5931):1207-1210.
The most recent of a set of studies that examine, for the first time, the structure of inter-areal spike-field correlations and their relationship to behavior. The role of long-range gamma frequency coupling during attention is emphasized in this work.
• • Khawaja FA, Tsui JMG, Pack CC. Pattern motion selectivity of spiking outputs and local field potentials in macaque visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 2009;29(43):13702-13709.
An elegant study that leverages our knowledge of the progression of neural encoding in the dorsal visual stream to link information encoded by spiking activity at one level in the hierarchy with LFP activity at the next.
• • Monosov IE, Trageser JC, Thompson KG. Measurements of simultaneously recorded spiking activity and local field potentials suggest that spatial selection emerges in the frontal eye field. Neuron. 2008;57(4):614-625.
A clever analysis of the timing of selectivity in spike and LFP activity in the frontal eye fields during a popout search task. The analysis of signal timing is a classic approach to understanding the flow of information in the brain. This study suggests that comparisons of signal timing between spiking and LFP activity could be a powerful extension of studies of timing.
• Buschman TJ, Miller EK. Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science. 2007;315(5820):1860.
This study measures LFP coherence between the frontal and parietal cortices to disambiguate bottom-up and top-down attentional processing. The authors highlight the role that neural activity at different frequencies may play in attentional processing.
• Xing D, Yeh C, Shapley RM. Spatial spread of the local field potential and its laminar variation in visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 2009;29(37):11540-11549.
An ingenious approach that uses a simple model to infer the spatial spread of LFP activity. The authors drive the inputs to V1 with visual stimuli and use the cortical magnification factor to transform the visual spread of neural signals to the corresponding spread in cortex.
• Kable JW, Glimcher PW. The neurobiology of decision: consensus and controversy. Neuron. 2009;63(6):733-745.
A recent review the presents a broad survey of the latest thinking on the neural mechanisms of decision.
8. Acknowledgements
Work in the author’s lab is supported by a McKnight Scholar award, a Career Award in the Biomedical Sciences from the Burroughs Wellcome Fund, a Sloan Research Fellowship, a Watson Program Investigator Award from NYSTAR, a CRCNS award from the NIMH, and the Swartz Foundation.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
10. References
- 1.Cui H, Andersen RA. Posterior parietal cortex encodes autonomously selected motor plans. Neuron. 2007;56(3):552–559. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Freedman DJ, Assad JA. Distinct encoding of spatial and nonspatial visual information in parietal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2009;29(17):5671–5680. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2878-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ipata AE, Gee AL, Gottlieb J, Bisley JW, Goldberg ME. LIP responses to a popout stimulus are reduced if it is overtly ignored. Nat. Neurosci. 2006;9(8):1071–1076. doi: 10.1038/nn1734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Sugrue LP, Corrado GS, Newsome WT. Choosing the greater of two goods: neural currencies for valuation and decision making. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2005;6(5):363–375. doi: 10.1038/nrn1666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Cisek P. Cortical mechanisms of action selection: the affordance competition hypothesis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci. 2007;362(1485):1585–1599. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Buschman TJ, Miller EK. Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science. 2007;315(5820):1860. doi: 10.1126/science.1138071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Monosov IE, Trageser JC, Thompson KG. Measurements of simultaneously recorded spiking activity and local field potentials suggest that spatial selection emerges in the frontal eye field. Neuron. 2008;57(4):614–625. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.12.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Pesaran B, Nelson MJ, Andersen RA. Free choice activates a decision circuit between frontal and parietal cortex. Nature. 2008;453(7193):406–409. doi: 10.1038/nature06849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hwang J, Soyoun K, Lee D. Temporal discounting and inter-temporal choice in rhesus monkeys. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2009;3:9. doi: 10.3389/neuro.08.009.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kable JW, Glimcher PW. The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10(12):1625–1633. doi: 10.1038/nn2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.McClure SM, Ericson KM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD. Time discounting for primary rewards. J. Neurosci. 2007;27(21):5796–5804. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4246-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Gregorios-Pippas L, Tobler PN, Schultz W. Short-term temporal discounting of reward value in human ventral striatum. J. Neurophysiol. 2009;101(3):1507–1523. doi: 10.1152/jn.90730.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hayden BY, Pearson JM, Platt ML. Fictive reward signals in the anterior cingulate cortex. Science. 2009;324(5929):948–950. doi: 10.1126/science.1168488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Lau B, Glimcher PW. Action and outcome encoding in the primate caudate nucleus. J. Neurosci. 2007;27(52):14502–14514. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3060-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Samejima K, Ueda Y, Doya K, Kimura M. Representation of Action-Specific Reward Values in the Striatum. Science. 2005;310(5752):1337–1340. doi: 10.1126/science.1115270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Rudebeck PH, Walton ME, Smyth AN, Bannerman DM, Rushworth MFS. Separate neural pathways process different decision costs. Nat. Neurosci. 2006;9(9):1161–1168. doi: 10.1038/nn1756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kable JW, Glimcher PW. The neurobiology of decision: consensus and controversy. Neuron. 2009;63(6):733–745. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Schneidman E, Berry MJ, Segev R, Bialek W. Weak pairwise correlations imply strongly correlated network states in a neural population. Nature. 2006;440(7087):1007–1012. doi: 10.1038/nature04701. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Truccolo W, Hochberg LR, Donoghue JP. Collective dynamics in human and monkey sensorimotor cortex: predicting single neuron spikes. Nat. Neurosci. 2010;13(1):105–111. doi: 10.1038/nn.2455. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Broome BM, Jayaraman V, Laurent G. Encoding and decoding of overlapping odor sequences. Neuron. 2006;51(4):467–482. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.07.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Yu BM, Cunningham JP, Santhanam G, et al. Gaussian-process factor analysis for low-dimensional single-trial analysis of neural population activity. J. Neurophysiol. 2009;102(1):614–635. doi: 10.1152/jn.90941.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Pillow JW, Shlens J, Paninski L, et al. Spatio-temporal correlations and visual signalling in a complete neuronal population. Nature. 2008;454(7207):995–999. doi: 10.1038/nature07140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Truccolo W, Eden UT, Fellows MR, Donoghue JP, Brown EN. A point process framework for relating neural spiking activity to spiking history, neural ensemble, and extrinsic covariate effects. J. Neurophysiol. 2005;93(2):1074–1089. doi: 10.1152/jn.00697.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Mitra PP, Pesaran B. Analysis of dynamic brain imaging data. Biophys. J. 1999;76(2):691–708. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(99)77236-X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Lüdtke N, Panzeri S, Brown M, et al. Information-theoretic sensitivity analysis: a general method for credit assignment in complex networks. J R Soc Interface. 2008;5(19):223–235. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2007.1079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Shlens J, Field GD, Gauthier JL, et al. The structure of multi-neuron firing patterns in primate retina. J. Neurosci. 2006;26(32):8254–8266. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1282-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kohn A, Smith MA. Stimulus dependence of neuronal correlation in primary visual cortex of the macaque. J. Neurosci. 2005;25(14):3661–3673. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5106-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Ecker AS, Berens P, Keliris GA, et al. Decorrelated neuronal firing in cortical microcircuits. Science. 2010;327(5965):584–587. doi: 10.1126/science.1179867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Renart A, de la Rocha J, Bartho P, et al. The asynchronous state in cortical circuits. Science. 2010;327(5965):587–590. doi: 10.1126/science.1179850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Harris KD, Csicsvari J, Hirase H, Dragoi G, Buzsáki G. Organization of cell assemblies in the hippocampus. Nature. 2003;424(6948):552–556. doi: 10.1038/nature01834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Frost JD., Jr. EEG-intracellular potential relationships in isolated cerebral cortex. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1968;24(5):434–443. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(68)90103-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Mehring C, Rickert J, Vaadia E, et al. Inference of hand movements from local field potentials in monkey motor cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2003;6(12):1253–1254. doi: 10.1038/nn1158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Pesaran B, Pezaris JS, Sahani M, Mitra PP, Andersen RA. Temporal structure in neuronal activity during working memory in macaque parietal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2002;5(8):805–811. doi: 10.1038/nn890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.David SV, Malaval N, Shamma SA. Decoupling action potential bias from cortical local field potentials. Comput Intell Neurosci. 2010:393019. doi: 10.1155/2010/393019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Bokil HS, Pesaran B, Andersen RA, Mitra PP. A method for detection and classification of events in neural activity. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 2006;53(8):1678–1687. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2006.877802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Popescu AT, Popa D, Pare D. Coherent gamma oscillations couple the amygdala and striatum during learning. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12(6):801–807. doi: 10.1038/nn.2305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.DeCoteau WE, Thorn C, Gibson DJ, et al. Learning-related coordination of striatal and hippocampal theta rhythms during acquisition of a procedural maze task. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2007;104(13):5644–5649. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0700818104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Khawaja FA, Tsui JMG, Pack CC. Pattern motion selectivity of spiking outputs and local field potentials in macaque visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 2009;29(43):13702–13709. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2844-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Wilke M, Mueller K, Leopold DA. Neural activity in the visual thalamus reflects perceptual suppression. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2009;106(23):9465–9470. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900714106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Scherberger H, Jarvis MR, Andersen RA. Cortical local field potential encodes movement intentions in the posterior parietal cortex. Neuron. 2005;46(2):347–354. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Spinks RL, Kraskov A, Brochier T, Umilta MA, Lemon RN. Selectivity for grasp in local field potential and single neuron activity recorded simultaneously from M1 and F5 in the awake macaque monkey. J. Neurosci. 2008;28(43):10961–10971. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1956-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Rubino D, Robbins KA, Hatsopoulos NG. Propagating waves mediate information transfer in the motor cortex. Nat Neurosci. 9(12):1549–1557. doi: 10.1038/nn1802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.O’Leary JG, Hatsopoulos NG. Early visuomotor representations revealed from evoked local field potentials in motor and premotor cortical areas. J. Neurophysiol. 2006;96(3):1492–1506. doi: 10.1152/jn.00106.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Siegel M, Warden MR, Miller EK. Phase-dependent neuronal coding of objects in short-term memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2009;106(50):21341–21346. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908193106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Van Der Werf J, Jensen O, Fries P, Medendorp WP. Neuronal synchronization in human posterior parietal cortex during reach planning. J. Neurosci. 2010;30(4):1402–1412. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3448-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Pesaran B. Uncovering the mysterious origins of local field potentials. Neuron. 2009;61(1):1–2. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Lubenov EV, Siapas AG. Hippocampal theta oscillations are travelling waves. Nature. 2009;459(7246):534–539. doi: 10.1038/nature08010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Colgin LL, Denninger T, Fyhn M, et al. Frequency of gamma oscillations routes flow of information in the hippocampus. Nature. 2009;462(7271):353–357. doi: 10.1038/nature08573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Katzner S, Nauhaus I, Benucci A, et al. Local origin of field potentials in visual cortex. Neuron. 2009;61(1):35–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.11.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Xing D, Yeh C, Shapley RM. Spatial spread of the local field potential and its laminar variation in visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 2009;29(37):11540–11549. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2573-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Logothetis NK, Kayser C, Oeltermann A. In vivo measurement of cortical impedance spectrum in monkeys: implications for signal propagation. Neuron. 2007;55(5):809–823. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Nielsen KJ, Logothetis NK, Rainer G. Dissociation between local field potentials and spiking activity in macaque inferior temporal cortex reveals diagnosticity-based encoding of complex objects. J. Neurosci. 2006;26(38):9639–9645. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2273-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Fries P. Neuronal gamma-band synchronization as a fundamental process in cortical computation. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2009;32:209–224. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Gregoriou GG, Gotts SJ, Zhou H, Desimone R. High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attention. Science. 2009;324(5931):1207–1210. doi: 10.1126/science.1171402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Saalmann YB, Pigarev IN, Vidyasagar TR. Neural mechanisms of visual attention: how top-down feedback highlights relevant locations. Science. 2007;316(5831):1612–1615. doi: 10.1126/science.1139140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]