Abstract
Wide-ranging conceptual and diagnostic approaches to defining mild cognitive impairment (MCI) have led to highly variable prevalence and progression rates. We sought to examine whether bilateral hippocampal volumes and cerebrovascular risk factors in individuals characterized by two different neuropsychological definitions of MCI subtypes would also differ. Participants were 65 nondemented, community-dwelling, older adults, ages 62–91 years, drawn from a larger group of individuals enrolled in a longitudinal study of normal aging. A comprehensive neuropsychological definition of MCI that required the presence of more than one impaired score in a cognitive domain resulted in expected anatomical results; hippocampal volumes were significantly smaller in the aMCI group as compared to cognitively normal or nonamnestic MCI participants. However, a typical definitional scheme for classifying MCI based only on the presence of one impaired score within a cognitive domain did not result in hippocampal differences between groups. Global stroke risk factors did not differ between the two definitional schemes, although the relationship between stroke risk variables and neuropsychological performance did vary by diagnostic approach. The comprehensive approach demonstrated associations between stroke risk and cognition, whereas the typical approach did not. Use of more sophisticated clinical decision-making and diagnostic approaches that incorporate comprehensive neuropsychological assessment techniques is supported by this convergence of neuropsychological, neuropathological, and stroke risk findings.
Keywords: Mild cognitive impairment, Neuroimaging, Neuropsychology, Older adults, Memory, Executive functioning, Stroke risk, Diagnosis, Amnestic, Nonamnestic
INTRODUCTION
The concept of mild cognitive impairment (MCI), as originally conceived, was thought to represent a borderland between normal age-related cognition and the early but unambiguous signs and symptoms of dementia, particularly Alzheimer's disease (AD) (Petersen et al., 1999). It required an objective memory impairment, a lack of impairment in other areas of cognition, and an absence of appreciable declines in activities of daily living. In more recent years, however, the concept of MCI has evolved to include multiple clinical subtypes that allow for impairments in domains of cognition aside from memory (Petersen & Morris, 2005), although the etiology and course of different MCI subtypes remain unclear.
Petersen and Morris (2005) also offered the possibility that the various subtypes of MCI may relate to distinct neuropathological substrates, although such evidence is only beginning to emerge. Particularly in the amnestic form of MCI (aMCI), converging evidence supports that entorhinal cortex and hippocampal volumes may be predictive of progression from normal cognition to MCI (Martin et al., in press) and from MCI to AD (Whitwell et al., 2007). Hippocampal volumes of those with aMCI are more similar to those with early AD than to cognitively healthy older adults (Stoub et al., in press), and hippocampal volumes in aMCI generally are intermediate between those in normal aging and with AD (Mariani et al., 2007).
When comparing different MCI subtypes, Bell-McGinty et al. (2005) found that an aMCI group had greater volume loss in the left entorhinal cortex and inferior parietal lobe as compared with multidomain MCI. In another study, a multidomain MCI group exhibited neuropathological changes in other areas, including smaller right inferior frontal gyrus, right middle temporal gyrus, and bilateral superior temporal gyrus as compared to aMCI (Becker et al., 2006). In a sample comprising both amnestic and nonamnestic MCI subtypes, hippocampal volumes have been able to predict progression from MCI to dementia, particularly the volume of the left hippocampus (Eckerström et al., 2008). However, others have found that hippocampal volumes in those with multidomain MCI were not statistically different from those of control groups but were significantly larger than both the aMCI and AD groups (Becker et al., 2006).
Petersen and Morris (2005) suggested that the multidomain subtypes of MCI may reflect a vascular etiology. Thus, it could be that cerebrovascular disease (CVD) risk factors provide supplementary information among those with MCI to better characterize this heterogeneous group. Recent research has shown that multidomain or nonamnestic MCI subtypes may be more likely to have CVD risk factors than either those with single-domain amnestic presentations or those without MCI (Di Carlo et al., 2007; Verghese et al., 2008; Zanetti et al., 2006). In contrast, there is also compelling evidence that CVD risk factors pose a risk for MCI in general (Delano-Wood et al., in press), not specifically for nonamnestic presentations, though the specific cardiovascular risk factors most strongly related to MCI remain unclear. Some have reported that hypertension is a general risk factor for all subtypes of MCI (Reitz et al., 2007). Others have found a relationship between MCI and some CVD risk factors, such as smoking and cholesterol, but not among other risk factors such as hypertension, stroke, or diabetes (Solfrizzi et al., 2004).
Complicating the outcomes of any investigation of MCI subtypes is the lack of a universally accepted approach to the objective identification of cognitive impairment in MCI. As an example, a wide range of conceptual and diagnostic approaches to MCI have led to highly variable prevalence rates from 1 to 30% and annual rates of progression from MCI to dementia that vary drastically from 1 to 72% (Tuokko & McDowell, 2006). In one recent study, Jak et al. (in press) demonstrated that widely varying prevalence rates result from the same subject pool, depending on the diagnostic scheme used. These challenges to diagnosing MCI also lead to difficulty interpreting with certainty findings regarding differences between MCI subtypes. In other words, differences may simply be due to varying diagnostic strategies and not due to any inherent neuropathological or etiologic distinctions.
Because of these uncertainties, we sought to evaluate potential anatomical support for two different and common neuropsychological diagnostic approaches to MCI subtypes. There is emerging evidence that supports distinct neuropathological etiologies for amnestic versus nonamnestic MCI. Hippocampal volume reductions are a robust finding in early AD and are predictive of the likelihood of cognitive decline, particularly in amnestic presentations. Therefore, we examined bilateral hippocampal volumes in individuals characterized by two different neuropsychological definitions of objective cognitive impairment. We hypothesized that those identified as aMCI via a comprehensive approach, which requires impairment on more than one neuropsychological measure within a domain (see Jak et al., in press, for discussion), would have expected hippocampal volumes across groups, namely, smaller hippocampi in the amnestic group. However, we predicted that the diagnoses would be less reliable with a typical approach used in many published studies that relies on impairment on only one test within a cognitive domain and, therefore, would not result in the expected relationship between hippocampal volume and group. Finally, since CVD risks may represent nonspecific factors that increase risk for any MCI or dementia subtype, we also examined magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)-derived hippocampal volumes in amnestic and nonamnestic MCI in concert with stroke risk factors to better characterize brain structural correlates of different MCI subtypes. We hypothesized that expected relationships, where higher CVD risk is related to poorer executive functioning, in particular, would emerge in the groups identified using a comprehensive diagnostic approach but would be less clear in groups determined by a typical diagnostic approach.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Subjects
Participants were community-dwelling volunteers drawn from a larger group of individuals enrolled in a longitudinal study of normal aging who were consecutively accrued and selected because they had both undergone an MRI and received a comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation. All participants provided informed consent, and the research was conducted in accordance with the Institutional Review Board of the University of California, San Diego. Sixty-five nondemented older adults, ages 62–91 (mean age = 76) years, were assessed. Participants were determined to be nondemented based on consensus diagnosis utilizing all available neurological, neuropsychological, and functional data. As shown in Table 1, all participants were free of functional impairment [Independent Living Scales (Loeb, 1996) T scores >39] and exhibited normal global cognitive functioning [Dementia Rating Scale (DRS: Mattis, 1988) total score ≥ 129, mean = 139; SD = 3.9]. Those with a history of alcoholism, drug abuse, learning disability, neurological, or major psychiatric illness were excluded.
Table 1.
Normal, mean (SD) | Amnestic, mean (SD) | Nonamnestic, mean (SD) | p Value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
(a) Comprehensive criteria | n = 29 | n = 16 | n = 20 | |
Age | 75.5 (8.0) | 76.0 (7.3) | 77.3 (6.5) | .71 |
Education | 15.9 (2.6) | 16.3 (2.6) | 16.3 (2.4) | .86 |
Gender (M/F) | 12/17 | 11/5 | 8/12 | .15 |
DRS total T score | 55.6 (4.3) | 48.0 (6.0) | 53.0 (5.7) | <.001* |
ILS managing money T score | 58.1 (3.8) | 52.6 (5.9) | 55.8 (4.4) | .004 |
ILS health and safety T score | 58.3 (5.4) | 54.2 (6.2) | 53.1 (7.2) | .02 |
APOE (ε4–/ε4+) | 19/7 | 9/6 | 13/5 | .65 |
(b) Typical criteria | n = 31 | n = 15 | n = 19 | |
Age | 75.7 (7.8) | 75.9 (7.4) | 77.3 (6.5) | .75 |
Education | 15.8 (2.7) | 17.0 (2.2) | 15.9 (2.4) | .31 |
Gender (M/F) | 13/18 | 11/4 | 7/12 | .07 |
DRS total T-score | 54.9 (4.9) | 49.0 (5.7) | 52.9 (5.5) | .003* |
ILS managing money T score | 57.2 (4.7) | 52.2 (5.0) | 57.5 (4.0) | .003 |
ILS health and safety T score | 56.5 (6.9) | 54.1 (6.2) | 55.8 (6.2) | .55 |
APOE (ε4–/ε4+) | 19/9 | 9/5 | 13/4 | .74 |
Note. F, female; ILS, Independent Living Scales; M, male; APOE, apolipoprotein E.
Statistically significant.
Neuropsychological Assessment
All participants underwent a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment. The tests of interest included measures from five cognitive domains (memory, attention, language, visuospatial functioning, and executive functioning) with at least three measures from each domain. These tests were selected from the larger assessment to be used in the diagnoses because they covered multiple cognitive domains, are widely employed in the clinical assessment of older adults, and were judged to be psychometrically sound. Memory was measured by the Logical Memory Subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised (Wechsler, 1987) [WMS-R; immediate and delayed free recall, normative data drawn from Mayo's Older Americans Normative Studies (MOANS: Ivnik et al., 1992)], the Visual Reproduction subtest of the WMS-R [immediate and delayed free recall, normative data drawn from the MOANS (Ivnik et al., 1992)], and the California Verbal Learning Test [(Delis et al., 1987); Trials 1–5 total recall and long delay free recall, normative data drawn from Norman et al., 2000]. Attention was assessed with the attention subscale of the DRS [published norms (Mattis, 1988)], the Digit Span subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised [(Wechsler, 1981); normative data drawn from the MOANS (Ivnik et al., 1992)], and Trail Making Test, Part A [(Reitan & Wolfson, 1985); normative data drawn from the MOANS (Ivnik et al., 1992)]. Language was measured with the Boston Naming Test [BNT (Kaplan et al., 1983); normative data drawn from the MOANS (Ivnik et al., 1992)] and letter fluency and category fluency (Gladsjo et al., 1999). Visuospatial functioning was measured with the Block Design subtest of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Revised [(Wechsler, 1974); age- and education-adjusted norms drawn from local unpublished data derived from the University of California, San Diego, Alzheimer Disease Research Center (UCSD ADRC)], the Visual Scanning condition of the Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System Trail Making Test (D-KEFS) and the D-KEFS Design Fluency Test [empty and filled dot conditions; published norms (Delis et al., 2001)], DRS construction [published norms (Mattis, 1988)], and draw-a-clock. Executive functioning was measured with the modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test [(Lineweaver et al., 1999); WCST-48-card version; categories achieved and perseverative errors], Trail Making Test, Part B [(Reitan & Wolfson, 1985); normative data drawn from the MOANS (Ivnik et al., 1992)], D-KEFS Color-Word Interference Test (inhibition and inhibition/switching), and D-KEFS fluency switching conditions (visual and verbal).
MCI Classification
Each participant was classified as normal or MCI on the basis of two sets of neuropsychologically based criteria for MCI that differed in their characterization of objective cognitive impairment. For all criteria, participants were labeled as Single-Domain aMCI if only the memory domain was impaired, as Single-Domain Nonamnestic MCI if only one nonmemory domain was impaired, as Multiple-Domain aMCI if memory and at least one other domain showed impairment, and as Multiple-Domain Nonamnestic MCI if more than one nonmemory domain was impaired.
Two distinct diagnostic approaches were employed. Variations on cutoffs for impairment and number of tests required to be in the impaired range are delineated as follows. The “comprehensive criteria” impairment objectively required that at least two performances within a cognitive domain fell greater than one standard deviation (SD) below normative expectations in order for that domain to contribute to the MCI classification. By this approach, individuals were classified as normal if, at most, performance on one measure within one or two cognitive domains fell more than 1 SD below age-appropriate norms. The “typical criteria,” adapted from the most recent criteria outlined by Petersen and Morris (2005), operationally defined objective cognitive impairment for multiple subtypes of MCI. Individuals were classified as normal if no neuropsychological measure fell greater than 1.5 SD below age-appropriate norms in any cognitive domain. Impairment required scores to fall more than 1.5 SD below age-appropriate norms on any test within a domain (for additional information on the diagnostic approaches, see Jak et al., in press).
According to the comprehensive criteria, 29 individuals were characterized as cognitively normal, 16 as aMCI (5 single and 11 multiple domain), and 20 as nonamnestic MCI (15 single and 5 multiple domain). No significant differences between groups on any demographic factors, including presence of the apolipoprotein epsilon 4 allele (APOE ε4), were noted (Table 1, Panel a). DRS total T scores were within normal limits for all subjects, although as expected there was a significant main effect of group on DRS total T scores (F2,62 = 12.5, p < .0001, ). Tukey post hoc comparisons of the three groups indicated that the amnestic group (M = 48.0) had significantly lower scores than either the normal group (M = 55.6, p < .0001) or the nonamnestic group (M = 53.1, p = .008). Comparisons between the nonamnestic and the normal groups were not statistically significant (p = .19).
When the typical criteria were applied, 31 individuals were characterized as cognitively normal, 15 as aMCI (6 single and 9 multiple domain), and 19 as nonamnestic MCI (16 single and 3 multiple domain). Despite the overall samples sizes remaining similar, 15 participants changed diagnostic category depending on the neuropsychological approach applied. Again, no significant differences between groups on any demographic factors, including presence of the APOE ε4allele, were noted with the application of the typical criteria (Table 1, Panel b). DRS total T scores were within normal limits for all subjects, although again there was a significant main effect of group on DRS total T scores (F2,62 = 6.4, p = .003, ). Tukey post hoc comparisons of the three groups indicated that the amnestic group (M = 49.0) had significantly lower scores than the normal group (M = 54.9, p < .002). Comparisons between the nonamnestic (M = 52.9) and the normal or amnestic groups were not statistically significant (p values >.08).
CVD Risk Factors
Validated health risk appraisal functions used for the basis of the evaluation of stroke risk [Framingham Stroke Risk Profile (FSRP); D'Agostino et al., 1994] were available for 53 participants. The following stroke risk factors were included: age, systolic blood pressure, diabetes mellitus, cigarette smoking, prior cardiovascular disease, atrial fibrillation, left ventricular hypertrophy by electrocardiogram, and the use of antihypertensive medication. Using the methods previously described by D'Agostino et al. (1994), a total stroke risk score summing the assigned number of points related to each of the individual stroke risk factors was calculated for each participant.
Structural Imaging Acquisition and Regions of Interest Protocol
Participants were scanned either on a 3.0-Tesla General Electric (GE) Medical Systems EXCITE whole-body imager or on a 1.5-Tesla GE Signa imager (General Electric Medical Systems, Milwaukee, WI). Hippocampal volumes were obtained (bilaterally) via visual inspection and manual outlining performed in the coronal plane. Images were realigned perpendicular to the anterior–posterior commissure line but not transformed into standard space coordinates. Regions of interest were delineated using Analysis of Functional Neuro-Images software and completed by an experienced operator (A.J.J.), who was blind to participant identity and group. High levels of intra- and inter-rater reliability for the procedure were established on a separate set of images not among those studied presently (intraclass correlation coefficients >.90). Hippocampal regions of interest were delineated using a stereotactic approach using methods published previously (Jak et al., 2007). Briefly, the anterior bound of the hippocampus was chosen as the coronal slice through the fullest portion of the mammillary bodies, and the posterior boundary was traced on the last coronal slice on which the superior colliculi could be fully visualized. Whole-brain images were also skull-stripped and segmented into gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid compartments. Scans were manually edited when necessary to remove any residual non-brain material. Whole-brain volume was derived and used in normalizing hippocampal volumes (Bigler & Tate, 2001).
Statistical Analyses
Data were analyzed using multiple one-way analyses of variance, where diagnostic group represented the independent variable and imaging, neuropsychological, or stroke risk variables represented the dependent variables. Tukey honestly significant difference tests were applied for post hoc comparisons. Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons were performed, and for the hippocampal volume and stroke risk analyses, an α level of .025 was considered significant; for neuropsychological variables, an α level of .003 was considered significant. Pearson correlations or partial correlations were used to determine the relationship between neuropsychological, stroke risk, and imaging variables in each of the three diagnostic groups.
RESULTS
When groups were created based on the comprehensive neuropsychological criteria and compared, normalized hippocampal volumes were significantly different between groups bilaterally. There was a significant group effect for the left hippocampus (F2,62 = 5.46, p = .007, ) and a trend for right-sided hippocampal volumes (F2,62 = 3.84, p = .03, ). Tukey post hoc comparisons of the three groups indicated that the amnestic group (M = 0.21) had significantly smaller left hippocampal volumes than did the normal group (M = 0.25, p = .01). Comparisons between the amnestic (M = 0.21) and the nonamnestic group (M = 0.24) and between the normal and the nonamnestic groups were not statistically significant (all p values >.14). Similarly, on the right side, post hoc comparisons of the three groups indicated that the amnestic group (M = 0.23) had a trend toward smaller right hippocampal volumes than did the normal group (M = 0.27, p = .03). Comparisons between the amnestic and the nonamnestic groups (M = 0.25) and between the normal and the nonamnestic groups were not statistically significant (all p values >.28). These results are in the context of no group dif ferences in either whole-brain volume (F2,62 = 0.58, p = .56, ) or total white matter volume (F2,62 = 0.60, p = .55, ).
In contrast, when the typical diagnostic approach was applied, normalized hippocampal volumes were not significantly different between groups (left hippocampus: F2,62 = 2.43, p = .10, ; right hippocampus: F2,62 = 2.4, p = .10, ). There were also no group differences in either whole-brain volume (F2,62 = 1.4, p = .27, ) or total white matter volume (F2,62 = 2.32, p = .11, ).
Neuropsychological differences between groups are depicted in Table 2. Regardless of diagnostic criteria, there was a significant group effect for the DRS Memory subscale (comprehensive criteria: F2,62 = 19.9, p < .0001, ; typical criteria: F2,62 = 15.8, p < .0001, ). With the comprehensive criteria, Tukey post hoc comparisons of the three groups indicated that the amnestic group (M = 40.3) had significantly lower DRS memory scores than did the normal group (M = 55.3, p < .0001) or the nonamnestic group (M = 52.6, p < .0001). The nonamnestic and normal groups were not significantly different (p = .44). With the typical criteria, Tukey post hoc comparisons of the three groups indicated that the amnestic group (M = 40.6) had significantly lower DRS memory scores than did the normal group (M = 54.9, p < .0001) or the nonamnestic group (M = 52.1, p < .0001). The nonamnestic and normal groups were not statistically different (p = .46). This measure was not part of the neuropsychological diagnosis.
Table 2.
Normal, mean (SD) | Amnestic, mean (SD) | Nonamnestic, mean (SD) | p Value | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(a) Comprehensive criteria | ||||||
WAIS-R digit span MOANS SS | 13.7 (3.3) | = | 11.4 (2.9) | = | 12.3 (3.0) | .06 |
WISC-R block design T score | 57.3 (12.7) | = | 49.3 (18.2) | = | 42.8 (15.0) | .008* |
DRS memory T score | 55.3 (3.2) | > | 40.3 (12.9) | < | 52.6 (7.3) | <.001* |
BNT MOANS SS | 14.4 (2.3) | = | 11.4 (2.8) | = | 13.4 (3.2) | .005 |
WCST categories T score | 54.8 (5.1) | = | 50.3 (12.5) | = | 45.8 (11.5) | .007 |
WCST perseverative errors T score | 49.9 (5.3) | = | 46.7 (11.3) | = | 43.9 (11.3) | .08 |
Trails A MOANS SS | 12.3 (2.8) | = | 10.8 (3.4) | = | 10.1 (3.1) | .04 |
Trails B MOANS SS | 12.9 (2.6) | > | 10.1 (1.9) | = | 11.1 (2.6) | .001* |
Verbal fluency T score | 56.5 (11.1) | = | 48.3 (10.0) | = | 50.3 (10.3) | .03 |
Category fluency T score | 53 (10.1) | = | 43.8 (7.6) | = | 50.6 (16.3) | .05 |
D-KEFS Color-Word Interference SS | 12.8 (2.2) | > | 10.1 (2.6) | = | 11.1 (2.2) | .001* |
WMS-R Logical Memory I MOANS SS | 13.0 (2.9) | > | 7.9 (3.2) | < | 12.4 (3.8) | <.001* |
WMS-R Logical Memory II MOANS SS | 13.5 (2.8) | > | 7.1 (3.6) | < | 12.6 (3.1) | <.001* |
WMS-R Visual Reproduction I MOANS SS | 13.2 (2.8) | = | 11.9 (3.3) | = | 12.7 (3.0) | .40 |
WMS-R Visual Reproduction II MOANS SS | 12.7 (3.3) | = | 9.0 (4.2) | = | 12.5 (3.1) | <.005 |
CVLT 1–5 total T score | 53.9 (9.4) | > | 36.1 (6.6) | < | 53.4 (10.2) | <.001* |
ILS managing money T score | 58.1 (3.8) | = | 52.6 (5.9) | = | 55.8 (4.4) | .004 |
ILS health and safety T score | 58.3 (5.4) | = | 54.2 (6.2) | = | 53.1 (7.2) | .02 |
(b) Typical criteria | ||||||
WAIS-R digit span MOANS SS | 14.0 (3.2) | = | 11.3 (2.8) | = | 11.7 (2.9) | .007 |
WISC-R block design T-score | 56.1 (11.8) | = | 48.7 (18.3) | = | 43.7 (17.8) | .003 |
DRS memory T score | 55.0 (4.3) | > | 40.6 (13.4) | < | 52.1 (7.7) | <.001* |
BNT MOANS SS | 14.3 (2.4) | = | 11.5 (3.0) | = | 13.3 (3.2) | .008 |
WCST categories T score | 55.1 (5.3) | = | 46.6 (9.9) | > | 43.7 (12.3) | <.001 |
WCST perseverative errors T score | 50.2 (5.3) | = | 46.6 (10.9) | = | 42.7 (11.4) | .02 |
Trails A MOANS SS | 12.3 (2.4) | = | 10.7 (3.5) | = | 10.3 (3.8) | .10 |
Trails B MOANS SS | 13.0 (2.7) | > | 10.1 (1.9) | = | 10.7 (2.2) | .001* |
Verbal fluency T score | 57.0 (10.6) | = | 47.1 (9.2) | = | 49.5 (10.5) | .005 |
Category fluency T score | 53.0 (9.5) | = | 43.1 (8.0) | = | 50.5 (16.5) | .03 |
D-KEFS Color-Word Interference SS | 12.8 (2.0) | > | 10.3 (2.7) | = | 10.7 (2.4) | .001* |
WMS-R Logical Memory I MOANS SS | 12.7 (3.4) | > | 8.2 (3.8) | < | 12.5 (3.1) | <.001* |
WMS-R Logical Memory II MOANS SS | 13.4 (3.0) | > | 7.2 (4.1) | < | 12.3 (2.6) | <.001* |
WMS-R Visual Reproduction I MOANS SS | 12.8 (3.0) | = | 11.7 (2.7) | = | 13.4 (3.1) | .29 |
WMS-R Visual Reproduction II MOANS SS | 12.8 (3.6) | > | 8.5 (3.7) | < | 12.9 (2.6) | <.001 |
CVLT 1–5 total T score | 54.4 (9.1) | > | 36.3 (7.0) | < | 51.5 (11.2) | <.001* |
ILS managing money T score | 57.2 (4.7) | = | 52.2 (5.0) | = | 57.5 (4.0) | .003 |
ILS health and safety T score | 56.5 (6.9) | = | 54.1 (6.2) | = | 55.8 (6.2) | .55 |
Note. CVLT, California Verbal Learning Test; ILS, Independent Living Scales; WAIS-R, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised; WISC-R, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Revised; SS – Scaled Score; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.
Statistically significant.
When creating groups based on the comprehensive criteria, Pearson correlations revealed that normalized hippocampal volumes (bilaterally) were related to DRS Memory performance in the amnestic group but not the normal or nonamnestic group (right: r = .55, p = .03; left: r = .61, p = .01). Delayed memory on WMS-R Logical Memory was related to hippocampal volumes bilaterally in the amnestic group (right: r = .66, p = .006; left: r = .51, p = .04) and on the left on the nonamnestic group (r = .45, p = .05). Delayed memory on the WMS-R Visual Reproduction was also related to hippocampal volumes bilaterally in the amnestic group only (right: r = .85, p < .001; left: r = .65, p = .009). Left hippocampal volumes were related to immediate memory on the Logical Memory subtest in the nonamnestic group only (r = .46, p = .04).
When the typical criteria were applied, relationships between the amnestic group and neuropsychological functioning were similar. Pearson correlations revealed that normalized hippocampal volumes (bilaterally) were related to DRS Memory performance in the amnestic group but not the normal or nonamnestic group (right: r = .68, p = .005; left: r = .64, p = .01). Delayed memory on WMS-R Logical Memory was related to hippocampal volumes bilaterally in the amnestic group (right: r = .71, p = .003; left: r = .71, p = .003). Delayed memory on the WMS-R Visual Reproduction was also related to hippocampal volumes bilaterally in the amnestic group only (right: r = .79, p < .001; left: r = .60, p = .02). There were no significant correlations between hippocampal volumes and neuropsychological variables in the nonamnestic or cognitively normal group, as defined by the typical criteria.
Use of the comprehensive criteria revealed no significant differences between groups in FSRP percent risk (F2,50 = 0.76, p = .47, ) or in blood pressure between groups (F2,50 = 0.74, p = .48, ). Results were similar for the main effect of group produced through application of the typical criteria for both FSRP percent risk (F = 0.07, p = .93, ) and blood pressure (F = 1.21, p = .30, ).
Relationships between stroke risk and cognitive performances differed depending on the diagnostic strategy applied. When using the comprehensive criteria, Pearson partial correlation coefficients controlling for age indicated that higher stroke risk was related to lower performance on block design in cognitively healthy individuals (r = −.57, p = .002) and in the amnestic group (r = −.75, r = .01). In the cognitively healthy group, higher stroke risk was related to poorer performance on WCST perseverative errors (r = −.40, p = .04) and Trails B performance (r = −.51, p = .007). In the amnestic group, higher stroke risk was associated with lower BNT (r = −.74, p = .01). No significant correlations were noted between FSRP stroke risk percent and cognitive performances in the nonamnestic group. In contrast, no significant relationships between stroke risk and neuropsychological functioning emerged in any diagnostic group when the typical criteria were used.
DISCUSSION
Neuropsychological definitions of MCI that require the presence of more than one impaired score in a cognitive domain resulted in expected anatomical results, where hippocampal volumes were significantly smaller in the aMCI group as compared to cognitively normal or nonamnestic MCI participants. However, diagnoses based only on the presence of one impaired score within a cognitive domain did not result in this expected anatomical difference. Specifically, those diagnosed with aMCI via the comprehensive criteria have significantly smaller left-sided hippocampal volumes than cognitively healthy individuals, whereas those with nonamnestic MCI had volumes intermediate between aMCI and normal groups. A similar trend was noted for right-sided hippocampal volumes. However, there were no differences in hippocampal volumes between any of the groups when diagnoses were arrived at using the typical criteria. These differential neuroanatomical findings depending on the diagnostic criteria applied were present despite the fact that, neuropsychologically, the groups were generally similar, regardless of the diagnostic strategy applied. That is, the diagnostic groups differed on the same tests of memory and executive functioning, irrespective of the diagnostic strategy applied. Yet, the typical criteria lacked the expected difference in hippocampal volumes between groups that was evident when the comprehensive criteria to group classification were applied.
Global stroke risk factors did not differ between diagnostic groups for either the comprehensive or the typical criteria. However, the relationship between stroke risk variables and neuropsychological functioning did vary by diagnostic approach. When the comprehensive criteria were applied, higher stroke risk was related to poorer executive functioning and visuoconstruction in cognitively healthy and aMCI participants. However, when the typical criteria were applied, no relationships between stroke risk factors and cognitive performances emerged for any group. Again, the literature tends to support stroke risk as a general risk factor for MCI or cognitive weaknesses, and the failure to find any relationship between stroke risk factors and neuropsychological test performances when using the typical criteria further calls into question the validity of this method of objectively defining impairment in MCI.
The alignment of anatomical and stroke risk variables in the comprehensive neuropsychological approach but not in the typical approach is noteworthy as it further highlights the role that diagnostic rigor can play in studies of MCI. Not only do different diagnostic strategies result in different prevalence estimates of MCI, but they can also suggest very different neuropathological substrates. The group differences in hippocampal volumes noted with use of the comprehensive diagnostic criteria add support to the concept of multiple MCI subtypes, which appear to be associated with different neuropathological processes and disease risk factors (see also Busse et al., 2006; Storandt et al., 2006).
It is also of note that stoke risk profiles did not correspond to cognitive functioning in the nonamnestic subtype, despite suggestions in the literature that this particular subtype of MCI may have a more vascular etiology (Petersen & Morris, 2005). The results of the current study are more supportive of CVD factors as more general risks for cognitive changes with age or even more preferentially in amnestic forms of MCI. Many have begun to highlight the strong relationship between CVD and AD (Cechetto et al., 2008; Hachinski, 2008), and our findings are consistent with stroke risk factors as potentially more salient in amnestic versus nonamnestic presentations.
The composition of the MCI subtype groups may have also contributed to some of the differential findings related to CVD risk. The amnestic group had a majority of multidomain presentations, while the nonamnestic group was predominantly single-domain presentations. Some evidence suggests that those with multidomain aMCI appear to be at greatest risk for future dementia (Di Carlo et al., 2007; Palmer et al., 2008; Tabert et al., 2006), while others indicate that aMCI places one at highest risk for progression to dementia (Ravaglia et al., 2006; Yaffe et al., 2006). It may be that the relationship of CVD risk and cognition in the amnestic group reflects the greater general risk factor burden of multidomain presentations, as opposed to relating specifically to amnestic versus nonamnestic distinctions. Future investigations with a larger sample size allowing for separation into four MCI groups are warranted to further examine these issues.
Additional limitations of the study include the fact that, despite being a community sample, the group is highly educated and therefore may not generalize to lower education levels, although the use of age- and education-corrected norms in the current study represents an improvement from most decision-making methods in the existing literature.
Because there is no “gold standard” operational criterion for diagnosis of MCI and its subtypes, and none of our sample has yet progressed to AD, it is impossible to determine with certainty which diagnostic strategy is ultimately the most valid or has the highest sensitivity or specificity. The convergence of neuropsychological, neuropathological, and stroke risk findings within the groups identified by the comprehensive criteria nonetheless offers strong support for the use of diagnostic approaches that consider impairments on more than one neuropsychological test as the criteria for objective cognitive impairment in MCI subtypes.
These study results and the larger research literature are generally supportive of the MCI construct. However, the work presented here highlights the importance of utilizing comprehensive neuropsychological data in MCI determinations as well examining multiple subtype presentations. It seems imperative that investigations of MCI consistently examine multiple cognitive domains, not just memory, and continue to explore additional neuroanatomical correlates of clinical subtypes of MCI. While hippocampal volumes were the only neuroanatomical structure examined in the current study, a more detailed examination of other neuroanatomy, particularly white matter, in relation to MCI diagnosis and cerebrovascular risk factors is certainly warranted. Clinical outcomes of those with MCI should also be a focus of continued investigation. Future research focused on rigorous objective definitions and cognitive characterization of clinical subtypes of MCI will continue to add to the clinical utility of the construct.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This work was supported by National Institute on Aging grants R01 AG012674 (MWB), K24 AG026431 (MWB), and T35AG026757 (SU), by a VA Career Development Award (AJJ), by an Alzheimer's Association New Investigator (AJJ) and Investigator Initiated (MWB) Research Grants. The authors gratefully acknowledge the assistance of Heather Alviso; Gali Weissberger; the VA/UCSD Laboratory of Cognitive Imaging; as well as the staff, patients, and volunteers of the UCSD ADRC.
REFERENCES
- Becker JT, Davis SW, Hayashi KM, Meltzer CC, Toga AW, Lopez OL, Thompson PM. Three-dimensional patterns of hippocampal atrophy in mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology. 2006;63:97–101. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.1.97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell-McGinty S, Lopez OL, Meltzer CC, Scanlon JM, Whyte EM, DeKosky ST, Becker JT. Differential cortical atrophy in subgroups of mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology. 2005;62:1393–1397. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.9.1393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bigler ED, Tate DF. Brain volume, intracranial volume, and dementia. Investigative Radiology. 2001;36:539–546. doi: 10.1097/00004424-200109000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Busse A, Hensel A, Guhne U, Angermeyer MC, Riedal-Heller SG. Mild cognitive impairment: Long-term course of four clinical subtypes. Neurology. 2006;67:2176–2185. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000249117.23318.e1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cechetto DF, Hachinski V, Whitehead SN. Vascular risk factors and Alzheimers disease. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics. 2008;8:743–750. doi: 10.1586/14737175.8.5.743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D'Agostino RB, Wolf PA, Belanger AJ, Kannel WB. Stroke risk profile: Adjustment for antihypertensive medication. The Framingham study. Stroke. 1994;25:40–43. doi: 10.1161/01.str.25.1.40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delano-Wood L, Bondi MW, Jak AJ, Stricker NR, Schweinsburg BC, Frank LR, Wierenga CE, Delis DC, Theilmann RJ, Salmon DP. Stroke risk modifies regional white matter differences in mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiology of Aging. 2009 doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.09.013. in press, epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delis DC, Kaplan E, Kramer JH. Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. The California Verbal Learning Test. Psychological Corporation; New York, NY: 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Di Carlo A, Lamassa M, Baldereschi M, Inzitari M, Scafato E, Farchi G, Inzitari D. CIND and MCI in the Italian elderly: Frequency, vascular risk factors, progression to dementia. Neurology. 2007;68:1909–1916. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000263132.99055.0d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eckerström C, Olsson E, Borga M, Ekholm S, Ribbelin S, Rolstad S, Starck G, Edman A, Wallin A, Malmgren H. Small baseline volume of left hippocampus is associated with subsequent conversion of MCI into dementia: The Göteborg MCI study. Journal of the Neurological Sciences. 2008;272:48–59. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2008.04.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gladsjo JA, Schuman CC, Evans JD, Peavy GM, Miller SW, Heaton RK. Norms for letter and category fluency: Demographic corrections for age, education, and ethnicity. Assessment. 1999;6:147–178. doi: 10.1177/107319119900600204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hachinski V. Shifts in thinking about dementia. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2008;300:2172–2173. doi: 10.1001/jama.2008.525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivnik RJ, Malec JF, Smith GE, Tangalos EG, Petersen RC, Kokmen E, Kurland LT. Mayo's Older Americans Normative Studies: WMS-R norms for ages 56-94. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 1992;6:49–82. [Google Scholar]
- Jak AJ, Bondi MW, Delano-Wood L, Wierenga C, Corey-Bloom J, Salmon DP, Delis D. Quantification of five neuropsychological approaches to defining mild cognitive impairment. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2009;17 doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31819431d5. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jak AJ, Houston WS, Nagel BJ, Corey-Bloom J, Bondi MW. Differential cross-sectional and longitudinal impact of APOE genotype on hippocampal volumes in nondemented older adults. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 2007;23:282–289. doi: 10.1159/000101340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaplan EF, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. The Boston Naming Test. Lea & Febiger; Philadelphia, PA: 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Lineweaver TT, Bondi MW, Thomas RG, Salmon DP. A normative study of Nelson's (1976) modified version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in healthy older adults. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 1999;13:328–347. doi: 10.1076/clin.13.3.328.1745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loeb PA. ILS: Independent Living Scales Manual. Psychological Corp, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich; San Antonio, TX: 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Mariani E, Monastero B, Mecocci P. Mild cognitive impairment: A systematic review. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2007;12:23–35. doi: 10.3233/jad-2007-12104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin SB, Smith CD, Collins HR, Schmitt FA, Gold BT. Evidence that volume of anterior medial temporal lobe is reduced in seniors destined for mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiology of Aging. 2009 doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.08.010. in press, epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mattis S. Dementia Rating Scale: Professional Manual. Psychological Assessment Resources; Odessa, FL: 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Norman MA, Evan JD, Miller WS, Heaton RK. Demographically corrected norms for the California Verbal Learning Test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2000;22:80–95. doi: 10.1076/1380-3395(200002)22:1;1-8;FT080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmer K, Backman L, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. Mild cognitive impairment in the general population: Occurrence and progression to Alzheimer disease. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2008;16:603–611. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181753a64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen RC, Morris JC. Mild cognitive impairment as a clinical entity and treatment target. Archives of Neurology. 2005;62:1160–1163. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.7.1160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome. Archives of Neurology. 1999;56:303–308. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.3.303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ravaglia G, Forti P, Maioli F, Martelli M, Servadei L, Brunetti N, Pantieri G, Mariani E. Conversion of mild cognitive impairment to dementia: Predictive role of mild cognitive impairment subtypes and vascular risk factors. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 2006;21:51–58. doi: 10.1159/000089515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reitan RM, Wolfson D. The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery. Neuropsychology Press; Tucson, AZ: 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Reitz C, Tang M, Manly J, Mayeux R, Luchsinger JA. Hypertension and the risk of mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology. 2007;64:1734–1740. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.12.1734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solfrizzi V, Panza F, Colacicco AM, D'Introno A, Capurso C, Torres F, Grigoletto F, Maggi S, DelParigi A, Reiman EM, Caselli RJ, Scafato E, Farichi G, Capurso A. Vascular risk factors, incidence of MCI, and rates of progression to dementia. Neurology. 2004;63:1882–1891. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000144281.38555.e3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Storandt M, Grant EA, Miller JP, Morris JC. Longitudinal course and neuropathologic outcomes in original vs. revised MCI and in pre-MCI. Neurology. 2006;67:467–473. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000228231.26111.6e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoub TR, Rogalski EJ, Leurgans S, Bennett DA, deToledo-Morrell L. Rate of entorhinal and hippocampal atrophy in incipient and mild AD: Relation to memory function. Neurobiology of Aging. 2009 doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.08.003. in press, epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tabert MH, Manly JJ, Liu X, Pelton GH, Rosenblum S, Jacobs M, Zamora D, Goodkind M, Bell K, Stern Y, Devanand DP. Neuropsychological prediction of conversion to Alzheimer disease in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63:916–924. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.8.916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tuokko HA, McDowell I. An overview of mild cognitive impairment. In: Tuokko HA, Hultsch DF, editors. Mild cognitive impairment: International perspectives. Taylor and Francis; New York: 2006. pp. 3–28. [Google Scholar]
- Verghese J, Robbins M, Holtzer R, Zimmerman ME, Wang C, Xue X, Lipton RB. Gait dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment syndromes. Journal of the American Geriatric Society. 2008;56:1–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01758.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Revised. Psychological Corporation; New York: 1974. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised Manual. Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised. Psychological Corporation; New York: 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Whitwell JL, Przybelski SA, Weigand SD, Knopman D, Boeve BF, Petersen RC, Jack CR. 3D maps from multiple MRI illustrate changing atrophy patterns as subjects progress from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer's disease. Brain. 2007;130:1777–1786. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yaffe K, Petersen RC, Lindquist K, Kramer J, Miller B. Subtype of mild cognitive impairment and progression to dementia and death. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 2006;22:312–319. doi: 10.1159/000095427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zanetti M, Ballabio C, Abbate C, Cutaia C, Vergani C, Bergamaschini L. Mild cognitive impairment subtypes and vascular dementia in community-dwelling elderly people: A 3-year follow-up study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2006;54:580–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2006.00658.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]