Abstract
Most theories of visual masking focus prima-rily on the temporal aspects of visual information processing, strongly neglecting spatial factors. In recent years, however, we have shown that this position is not tenable. Spatial aspects cannot be neglected in metacontrast, pattern and un-masking. Here, we review these results.
Keywords: temporal processing, verniers, unmasking, metacontrast
INTRODUCTION
In visual backward masking, perception of a target is impeded by a trailing mask. Most research has focused on the phenomenon of B-type masking, in which the strongest deterioration of performance occurs for intermediate SOAs. In these investigations, usually metacontrast masks are used, i.e. masks not overlapping with the target. Deteriorated performance is often explained by neural inhibitory mechanisms such as lateral inhibition (e.g. Bridgeman, 1971; Growney & Weisstein, 1972), mask blocking (Francis, 2000), dual channel inhibition (e.g. Breitmeyer & Ganz, 1976; Öğmen, 1993), delayed facilitation (e.g. Bachman, 1994), contour elimination (e.g. Kolers, 1962; Werner, 1935), or object substitution (e.g. Di Lollo, Enns, & Rensink, 2000). For example, in the influential models by Breitmeyer and Ganz (1976) and Bachman (1994), target and mask processing occurs in two channels, a faster and a slower one, thereby allowing the mask signal in the faster channel to catch up with the target signal in the slower channel (Öğmen, 1993).
In A-type masking, performance improves mono-tonically when the ISI between the target and mask increases. The effects of the mask on the target are often explained in terms of contrast reduction (e.g. Eriksen, 1966) or camouflage (e.g. Coltheart & Arthur, 1972; Enns, 2004).
Almost all studies of both A and B-type masking have a common focus on the temporal characteristics of the target and mask, largely neglecting non-basic spatial dimensions (however, see Werner, 1935; Williams & Weisstein, 1981, 1984). Here, we review results suggesting that the spatial layout of the target and mask exerts a tremendous influence on backward masking that was largely neglected previously. In particular, spatial grouping seems to be a key factor for certain masking effects. We will argue that, for this reason, models have to incorporate explicit spatial processing components. Models employing temporal mechanisms only are not sufficient.
RESULTS
Pattern and A-type masking
In pattern masking (by structure), mask and target spatially overlap. Usually A-type masking is found, which is explained, in terms of integration masking, for example, as a result of luminance summation and contrast reduction (e.g. Eriksen, 1966), by camouflage and montage (recently, Enns, 2004), or a degraded target image (e.g. Scheerer, 1973). These factors are often assumed to occur at stages as early as the retina (e.g. Michaels & Turvey, 1979).
In a series of experiments using pattern masks, we have shown that these explanations are not sufficient (Herzog & Fahle, 2002; Herzog, Fahle, & Koch, 2001; Herzog & Koch, 2001). Figure 1 shows a typical example of these experiments. A vernier target is followed by a grating comprised of 25 aligned verniers; a moderate threshold elevation occurs compared to when the vernier is presented without the grating. This masking can be strongly potentiated if four single contextual lines are presented in addition to the grating: the vernier target can be rendered invisible and thresholds dramatically rise (Figure 1).
This interference is dominant in a temporal window of more than 100 ms and can hardly be explained with the classical accounts of integration masking. Luminance summation and contrast reduction may play a role if only the central grating follows the vernier (horizontal line in Figure 1). However, they cannot explain why adding four additional lines potentiates masking. This becomes even more evident when taking into account that adding 2*25 contextual lines, hence further increasing energy, undoes the masking of the four lines which are contained in the 2*25 lines (Figure 2). Camouflage or montage play no role since the four lines may even serve as a reference to localize the vernier (collinear lines above and beneath the central grating element also yield a strong performance deterioration; Herzog, Schmonsees, & Fahle, 2003b). Finally, the vernier is covered only by the central grating element in all conditions, which yields the same degree of image distortion in the near neighborhood of the vernier. Still, performance varies strongly with the spatial layout of the contextual elements. Taken together, classical explanations of integration masking fail to account for our masking results (Herzog, Dependahl, Schmonsees, & Fahle, 2004; Herzog & Fahle, 2002).
It is interesting that masking is not linear regarding the masking of the mask pieces. The four contextual lines themselves exert only weak masking (by a factor of about 1.5; Figure 3) while the grating presented without these lines causes a threshold elevation of a factor of 5.5 (Figure 1, horizontal line). However, if the grating and contextual lines are displayed together, the vernier is largely invisible and thresholds can be elevated by more than a factor of 31 (see Figure 2). These non-linear results show that the strength of a pattern mask cannot simply be explained by the masking of its parts.
Our results clearly show that explanations of pattern masking have to carefully consider aspects of the spatial layout of the target and mask. We could show that some of the above results can be reproduced with a simple but dynamic model of spatial information processing (Hermens & Ernst, this volume; Herzog, Ernst, Etzold, & Eurich, 2003).
It is very important to note that the dramatic changes of performance, caused by rather simple spatial manipulations, occur only in a short temporal window. Even only slightly longer vernier durations, as used above, yield weak or no masking independent of the spatial layout (e.g. Herzog, Schmonsees, & Fahle, 2003a). Hence, it seems that the above results reveal complex spatio-temporal effects at the very beginning of spatial information processing.
Unmasking
The previous subsection suggests an important role for the spatial layout of the mask in pattern masking. In this subsection, we show analogous results for unmasking. In unmasking, a target is followed by two masks. Under some conditions, performance is better in the two-mask condition compared to when only the first mask is presented. Hence, the second mask unmasks the first one in some way (e.g. Amassian, Cracco, Maccabee, Cracco, Rudell, & Eberle 1993; Breitmeyer, Rudd, & Dunn, 1981; Briscoe, Dember, & Warm, 1983; Öğmen, this volume; Robinson, 1966; Tenkink, 1983).
Using a feature fusion paradigm, we have shown how the spatial layout contributes to unmasking. We presented a vernier followed by a second vernier with the same duration and spatial parameters as the first vernier except for having an offset with opposite direction (Herzog, Parish, Koch, & Fahle, 2003). This “anti-vernier” serves as the first mask. With this condition, both verniers fuse yielding the percept of one single vernier. The anti-vernier dominates performance more strongly than the vernier, i.e. backward masking is stronger than forward masking (Figure 4a). When these two verniers are followed by an additional mask, dominance can reverse, i.e. the vernier dominates performance (Figure 4d-f; Herzog, Lesemann, & Eurich, 2006). However, this unmasking is present only for extended masks but not, for example, for a single aligned vernier, even though this single vernier is part of the 25-element grating which yields strong unmasking (Figure 4b,d. On the other hand, the single vernier is not part of the metacontrast grating which, however, yields unmasking like the 25-element grating. Hence, unmasking like pattern masking cannot be explained by the masking of its parts. This again suggests complex spatial processing.
Metacontrast and B-type masking
B-type masking is usually believed to be the most interesting phenomenon in backward masking. A later presented mask can catch up to an earlier presented target and dominate performance, thereby ruling out an ultra-fast feedforward processing as found in other domains of vision (e.g. Thorpe, Fize, & Marlot, 1996; VanRullen, this volume). It should be mentioned that B-type masking loses much of its mystery when it is accepted that the brain acts as a temporal low pass filter and some temporal non-linearities are involved (Francis, 2000; Francis, this volume; Francis & Herzog, 2004). Here, we show that temporal aspects are not the whole story but that B-type masking strongly depends on the spatial layout of the mask and target.
A vernier was presented for 20 ms and flanked by a line on each side, presented for 20 ms as well. Flank length was either the same as the vernier or twice as long. These metacontrast masks exerted B-type masking as expected (Figure 5; Duangudom, Francis, & Herzog, 2007; see also Otto, Öğmen, & Herzog, 2006; Otto, this volume). Surprisingly, for more flanking lines, A-type masking or flat masking functions were obtained depending on the length of flanks. Hence, we can change the masking function, e.g. from A to B, by changing the spatial layout of the mask. Surprisingly, the weakest masking was obtained for the mask with 6 lines on each side of the vernier being twice its length (Figure 5; Duangudom, Francis, & Herzog, 2007). This mask has the highest energy but still yields the weakest masking contrary to many models of masking (Breitmeyer & Öğmen, 2006, p. 48).
DISCUSSION
Visual masking has been explored for more than a century. Still, the underlying mechanisms are subject for debate. Most models try to explain masking from purely temporal grounds, ignoring spatial aspects (only the model by Francis, 1997, has a full 2-dimensional spatial representation). Here, we have provided strong support for the involvement of spatial aspects in pattern, un-, and metacontrast masking. These effects are only visible with backward masking in a very narrow time window. For example if an ISI of 10 ms only is inserted between the vernier and the standard grating, adding contextual lines raises thresholds only modestly. Hence, contextual modulation has vanished (Herzog et al., 2003a; see also Herzog, Koch et al., 2001). We believe that masking with the shine-through effect reveals aspects present only at the very beginning of spatial information processing.
Local contour interactions. B-type masking with metacontrast masks is often assumed to occur because the mask inner contour suppresses the processing of the target contours (e.g. Werner, 1935). In support of this hypothesis, it was found that the larger the space between the target and the inner contour of a metacontrast mask, the better was the performance (e.g. Growney, Weisstein, & Cox, 1977; Kolers, 1962; review: Breitmeyer & Öğmen, 2006, p. 56). Hence, local spatial interactions seem to be important.
However, contrary to this proposition, we could change the masking function qualitatively from B-type to A-type masking while leaving the inner contour of the metacontrast masks constant (Figure 5). Thus, local computations between the target and the neighboring masking elements are not sufficient to explain B-type masking.
In pattern masking, we used gratings. Performance changed greatly in the various conditions even though the standard grating was constant (see Figure 1). In unmasking, both the 25-element and the metacontrast grating yielded comparable results whereas the proximity of contours clearly differed in these conditions. For these reasons we deny an important role of local contour interactions, at least with our stimuli. Also, models based on simple lateral interactions may not be able to explain many of our results.
Energy ratio. Another aspect considered important to masking is the energy ratio between the target and mask. For example, it was often proposed that B-type masking occurs only when the target and mask have approximately the same energy or when the mask has weaker energy than the target (e.g. Breitmeyer & Öğmen, 2006, p.48). Energy is usually defined in terms of the product of luminance and duration of the elements of the mask. The mask blocking idea by Francis (2000) has provided an elegant mathematical description for this argument that can be sketched as follows: For SOA = 0 ms, a strong target “blocks” the mask completely. For intermediate SOAs, the target signal has decayed and the mask can influence performance. For longer SOAs, the mask arrives too late to influence target processing. Hence, masking is strongest for intermediate SOAs.
However, the target-to-mask energy does not play a role in our experiments either in pattern or metacontrast masking. Longer length flanks, i.e. more energy, can yield weaker masking than equal length flanks for metacontrast masks (Figure 5). 2*25 contextual elements yield better performance than four lines in pattern masking (Figure 2).
Spatial layout: Grouping. On a stimulus description level, we propose that the complex spatial effects we reported hitherto can be best explained in terms of spatial grouping. In pattern masking, mask elements exert no influence on the target if they are grouped within an entity different from the target. For example, single contextual lines exert interference on the vernier, whereas this interference vanishes when the lines are grouped within an extended contextual grating (Figures 1 and 2; Herzog et al., 2004; Herzog & Koch, 2001; Herzog, Schmonsees, & Fahle, 2003a, b). On a neural description level, we showed that the proposed grouping effects can be mathematically modelled with simple differential equations mimicking the spatial processing in early cortical areas such as V1 (Hermens & Ernst, this volume; Herzog, Ernst, et al., 2003; see also Zhaoping, 2003). These models do not contain an explicit grouping operation and were not proposed to explain masking. Computer simulations with these models show that redundant elements, e.g. inner lines of gratings, are removed from further processing by dynamic lateral inhibition. In this respect, masking may be viewed as redundancy reduction (Reeves, this volume). It is important to note that ‘‘grouping’’ is a term of perceptual organization and may therefore be explained by several types of neural network models. Hence, it has to be seen whether existing mathematical models of masking (e.g. Bridgeman, 1971; Di Lollo et al., 2000; Öğmen, 1993) can capture the above effects when extended by appropriate spatial processing components (for the 2D model of Francis, 1997, no simulation results are available because of limited spatial resolution).
We propose that grouping also plays an important role in metacontrast masking. For short SOAs, the vernier offset can hardly be discriminated when it can be grouped within the flanking lines – as the single contextual elements lose their power when grouped within contextual gratings (Figure 2; see also Malania, Herzog, & Westheimer, 2007; Sharikadze, Fahle, & Herzog, 2005). When SOA increases, grouping breaks down by temporal cues and performance improves. To the best of our knowledge, there are only a few visual masking studies taking complex spatial aspects into account beyond low level variations such as varying mask size or the distance between target and mask contours. Williams and Weisstein (1984) showed that B-type masking occurs when the target appears as part of a 3-dimensional structure but A-type when not. More recently, Moore and Lleras (2005, Lleras & Moore, 2003) argued that masking depends strongly on whether or not the target and mask can be processed separately (see also Kahan & Mathis, 2002).
It is surprising to see so few studies jointly investigating temporal and spatial vision even though the first goal of vision is the generation of a coherent spatial representation of the outer world that, as masking shows, is not created instantaneously. Spatial and temporal vision research belong together.
Acknowledgements
I would like to thank Cathleen Moore for her helpful comments on this manuscript. This work was supported by the Swiss National Fund (SNF).
References
- Amassian V. E., Cracco R. Q., Maccabee P. J., Cracco J. B., Rudell A. P., Eberle L. Unmasking human visual perception with the magnetic coil and its relationship to hemispheric asymmetry. Brain Research. 1993;605:312–316. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(93)91757-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bachmann T. Psychophysiology of visual masking. New York: Nova Science Publishers, Inc., Commack; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Breitmeyer B. G., Ganz L. Implications of sustained and transient channels for theories of visual pattern masking, saccadic suppression, and information processing. Psychological Review. 1976;83:1–36. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breitmeyer B. G., Öğmen H. Visual masking. Time slices through conscious and unconscious vision. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Breitmeyer B. G., Rudd M., Dunn K. Metacontrast investigations of sustained-transient channel inhibitory interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1981;7:770–779. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.7.4.770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bridgeman B. Metacontrast and lateral inhibition. Psychological Review. 1971;78:528–539. doi: 10.1037/h0031782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briscoe G., Dember W., Warm J. S. Target recovery in visual backward masking: no clear explanation in sight. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1983;9:898–911. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.9.6.898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coltheart M., Arthur B. Evidence for an integration theory of visual masking. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1972;24:262–269. doi: 10.1080/14640747208400279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Lollo V., Enns J. T., Rensink R. A. Competition for consciousness among visual events: the psychophysics of reentrant visual processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2000;129:481–507. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.129.4.481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duangudom V., Francis G., Herzog M. H. What is the strength of a mask in visual metacontrast masking? Journal of Vision. 2007;7(1) doi: 10.1167/7.1.7. Article 7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enns J. T. Object substitution and its relation to other forms of visual masking. Vision Research. 2004;44:1321–1331. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2003.10.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eriksen C. W. Independence of successive inputs and uncorrelated error in visual form perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1966;72:26–35. doi: 10.1037/h0023316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Francis G. What should a quantitative model of masking look like and why would we want it? Advances in Cognitive Psychology. 2007;3:21–31. doi: 10.2478/v10053-008-0011-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Francis G. Cortical dynamics of lateral inhibition: metacontrast masking. Psychological Review. 1997;104:572–594. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.104.3.572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Francis G. Quantitative theories of metacontrast masking. Psychologcial Review. 2000;107:1–18. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.107.4.768. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Francis G., Herzog M. H. Testing quantitative models of backward masking. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 2004;11:104–111. doi: 10.3758/bf03206468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Growney R., Weisstein N. Spatial characteristics of metacontrast. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 1972;62:690–696. doi: 10.1364/josa.62.000690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Growney R., Weisstein N., Cox S. I. Metacontrast as a function of spatial separation with narrow line targets and masks. Vision Research. 1977;17:1205–1210. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(77)90155-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hermens F., Ernst U. Visual backward masking: Modeling spatial and temporal aspects. Advances in Cognitive Psychology. 2007;3:93–105. doi: 10.2478/v10053-008-0017-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herzog M. H., Dependahl S., Schmonsees U., Fahle M. Valences in contextual vision. Vision Research. 2004;44:3131–3143. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.07.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herzog M. H., Ernst U., Etzold A., Eurich C. Local interactions in neural networks explain global effects in Gestalt processing and masking. Neural Computation. 2003;15:2091–2113. doi: 10.1162/089976603322297304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herzog M. H., Fahle M. Effects of grouping in contextual modulation. Nature. 2002;415:433–436. doi: 10.1038/415433a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herzog M. H., Fahle M., Koch C. Spatial aspects of object formation revealed by a new illusion, shine-through. Vision Research, 41, 2325-2335. Please read Erratum: Vision Research. 2001;42:271–271. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(01)00122-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herzog M. H., Koch C. Seeing properties of an invisible object: feature inheritance and shine-through. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA. 2001;98:4271–4275. doi: 10.1073/pnas.071047498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herzog M. H., Koch C., Fahle M. Shine-through: temporal aspects. Vision Research. 2001;41:2337–2346. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(01)00123-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herzog M. H., Lesemann E., Eurich C. W. Spatial interactions determine temporal feature integration as revealed by unmasking. Advances in Cognitive Psychology. 2006;2:77–85. [Google Scholar]
- Herzog M. H., Parish L., Koch C., Fahle M. Fusion of competing features is not serial. Vision Research. 2003;43:1951–1960. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(03)00278-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herzog M. H., Schmonsees U., Fahle M. Timing of contextual modulation in the shine-through effect. Vision Research. 2003a;43:2039–2051. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(03)00313-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herzog M. H., Schmonsees U., Fahle M. Collinear contextual suppression. Vision Research. 2003b;43:2915–2925. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2003.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahan T. A., Mathis K. M. Gestalt grouping and common onset masking. Perception & Psychophysics. 2002;64:1248–1259. doi: 10.3758/bf03194769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kolers P. A. Intensity and contour effects in visual masking. Vision Research. 1962;2:277–294. [Google Scholar]
- Lleras A., Moore C. M. When the target becomes the mask: using apparent motion to isolate the object-level component of object substitution masking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Performance and Perception. 2003;29:106–120. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malania M., Herzog M. H., Westheimer G. Grouping of contextual elements that affect vernier thresholds. Journal of Vision. 2007;7(2) doi: 10.1167/7.2.1. Article 1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore C. M., Lleras A. On the role of object representations in substitution masking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Performance and Perception. 2005;31:1171–1180. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.6.1171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michaels C. F., Turvey M. T. Central sources of visual masking: indexing structures supporting seeing at a single, brief glance. Psychological Research. 1979;41:1–61. doi: 10.1007/BF00309423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Öğmen H. A theory of moving form perception: Synergy between masking, perceptual grouping, and motion computation in retinotopic and non-retinotopic representations. Advances in Cognitive Psychology. 2007;3:67–84. doi: 10.2478/v10053-008-0015-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Öğmen H. A neural theory of retino-cortical dynamics. Neural Networks. 1993;6:245–273. [Google Scholar]
- Otto T. U. Grouping based feature attribution in metacontrast masking. Advances in Cognitive Psychology. 2007;3:107–109. doi: 10.2478/v10053-008-0018-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Otto T. U., Öğmen H., Herzog M. H. The flight path of the Phoenix – the visible trace of invisible elements in human vision. Journal of Vision. 2006;6:1079–1086. doi: 10.1167/6.10.7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reeves A. An analysis of visual masking, with a defense of ‘Stopped Processing’. Advances in Cognitive Psychology. 2007;3:57–65. doi: 10.2478/v10053-008-0014-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson D. N. Disinhibition of visually masked stimuli. Science. 1966;154:157–158. doi: 10.1126/science.154.3745.157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharikadze M., Fahle M., Herzog M. H. Attention and feature integration in the feature-inheritance effect. Vision Research. 2005;45:2608–2619. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.03.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scheerer E. Integration, interruption and processing rate in visual backward masking. Psychol. Forschung. 1973;36:71–93. doi: 10.1007/BF00424655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tenkink E. Recovery at short intervals between masking flashes. Vision Research. 1983;23:1693–1698. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(83)90185-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thorpe S., Fize D., Marlot C. Speed of processing in the human visual system. Nature. 1996;381:520–522. doi: 10.1038/381520a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- VanRullen R. The power of the feed-forward sweep. Advances in Cognitive Psychology. 2007;3:167–176. doi: 10.2478/v10053-008-0022-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werner H. Studies on contour: I. Qualitative analyses. American Journal of Psychology. 1935;47:40–64. [Google Scholar]
- Williams M. C., Weisstein N. Spatial frequency response and perceived depth in the time-course of object superiority. Vision Research. 1981;21:631–646. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(81)90071-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams M. C., Weisstein N. The effect of perceived depth and connectedness of metacontrast functions. Vision Research. 1984;24:1279–1288. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(84)90182-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhaoping L. V1 mechanisms and some figure-ground and border effects. Journal of Physiology, Paris. 2003;97:503–515. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2004.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]