Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 May 10.
Published in final edited form as: Clin Neuropsychol. 2009 Aug;23(6):944–961. doi: 10.1080/13854040802681664

LEUKOARAIOSIS SEVERITY AND LIST-LEARNING IN DEMENTIA

Catherine C Price 1, Kelly Davis Garrett 2, Angela L Jefferson 3, Stephanie Cosentino 4, Jared J Tanner 1, Dana L Penney 5, Rodney Swenson 6, Tania Giovannetti 7, Brianne Magouirk Bettcher 7, David J Libon 8
PMCID: PMC2866111  NIHMSID: NIHMS129557  PMID: 19370451

Abstract

In patients with dementia, leukoaraiosis (LA) was hypothesized to result in differential patterns of impairment on a verbal serial list-learning test. Using a visual rating scale, 144 dementia patients with ischemic scores <4 were re-categorized as having mild (n = 73), moderate (n = 44), or severe LA (n = 27). Mild LA was predicted to be associated with an amnestic list-learning profile, while severe LA was predicted to be associated with a dysexecutive profile. List-learning performances were standardized to a group of healthy older adults (n = 24). Analyses were conducted on a set of four factors derived from the list-learning paradigm, as well as error scores. Data indicate that LA severity is an important marker for understanding list learning in dementia.

Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease, Vascular dementia, Binswanger’s disease, Subcortical dementia, Episodic memory, The Philadelphia (repeatable) Verbal Learning Test (PrVLT)

INTRODUCTION

It is well known that on verbal serial list learning tests, patients with pathology in the medial temporal lobes often produce a very shallow and flat learning curve on immediate recall test trials, suffer a rapid rate of forgetting, and perform poorly on delayed recognition testing. They also produce a large number of extra-list intrusion errors on category cued recall test trials (Canolle et al., 2008; Delis et al., 1991). We label this an amnesic profile. In contrast, patients with subcortical diseases such as Parkinson’s disease (PD) exhibit less forgetting, make fewer intrusion errors, and score relatively better on delayed recognition test conditions (Kramer et al., 1988; Massman, Delis, Butters, Levin, & Salmon, 1990). Elements of episodic memory impairment exhibited by these patients may be due, in part, to frontal system dysfunction or executive control deficits, and reflect a source recall problem (Dobbins, Simons, & Schacter, 2004). We label this a dysexecutive profile.

The current research investigated the direct effect of MRI-leukoaraiosis (LA; Hachinski, Potter, & Merskey, 1987) on these verbal serial list-learning performances in patients with mild to moderate dementia. Briefly, LA is not a disease but defines areas of hyperintense pixels within the white matter on CT/MRI studies. LA occurs in healthy older adults and in dementia such as Alzheimer’s disease and subcortical vascular dementia (sVD; also known as ischemic vascular dementia or small vessel vascular disease). Within these populations, LA is often believed to be a marker of ischemia (see Chui, 2007, for review), although some argue that LA may signal the presence of other pathological processes such as Wallerian degeneration (Leys et al., 1991), amyloid-induced oligodendrocyte toxicity (Xu et al., 2001), and endothelial dysfunction due to genetic variations (Szolnoki, 2007). Clinically, large amounts of LA have been associated with subcortical profiles with slow processing speed and impaired working memory. This suggests that LA may signify frontostriatal pathway disruption (for rationale review, see Libon, Price, Garrett, & Giovannetti, 2004; for review of pathways, see Alexander, DeLong, & Strick, 1986).

Among older adults with dementia, research suggests that neuropsychological profiles differ by LA severity. Patients with little to no LA demonstrate more memory than executive function impairment; patients with moderate LA (estimated to involve approximately one fourth of the white matter) present with equal disruption on memory and executive tests; patients with severe LA (estimated to involve approximately at least half of the white matter) are distinguished by a striking dysexecutive syndrome (Libon et al., 2008; Price, Jefferson, Merino, Heilman, & Libon, 2005). These three LA groups show comparable overall dementia severity (i.e., equal scores on tests such as the Mini Mental State Examination; Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975). This suggests that differential patterns of impairment on neuropsychological tests reflect discrete dementia syndromes with distinct gray and white matter etiologies rather than a single progressive LA syndrome.

In this study we sought to better understand how LA severity marks verbal serial list-learning performance, for successful list-learning is influenced by both memory and executive systems (Tremont, Halpert, Javorsky, & Stern, 2000). Based on earlier findings, we hypothesized that individuals with mild LA would demonstrate an amnestic pattern. In contrast, severe LA would demonstrate a dysexecutive profile. Moderate LA would demonstrate a mixed profile.

To investigate this general hypothesis we quantified LA in patients with mild to moderate dementia who have a Hachinski ischemic stroke score of less than 4 (Hachinski, Lassen, & Marshall, 1974). We used LA severity as the grouping variable, while ignoring dementia diagnosis (i.e., AD, sVD). This was strategic for two basic reasons. First, the clinical diagnosis of dementia is now even more complex due to accumulating literature demonstrating that, upon autopsy, many brains contain hallmark features of both AD and small vessel vascular pathology. There are increasing reports of mixed pathology in individuals meeting clinical criteria for AD (Barker et al., 2002) with fewer numbers of so-called “pure” AD patients (White et al., 2005). Comparatively, sVD patients have been shown to have significant cortical and hippocampal atrophy without AD pathology (Fein et al., 2000; Laakso et al., 1996). From a pathological viewpoint, therefore, the line between the two diagnoses has become blurred. Second, despite a number of diagnostic schemes proposed to distinguish sVD from AD, the sensitivity and specificity of these diagnostic schemes is questionable; patients meeting sVD using one diagnostic scheme do not necessarily qualify for sVD diagnosis using a different set of criteria (i.e., the ADDTC and the NINDS-AIREN criteria for the diagnosis of sVD; Cosentino et al., 2004a; Pohjasvaara, Mantyla, Ylikoski, Kaste, & Erkinjuntti, 2000; see Jellinger, 2007 for review). Cosentino et al. (2004a) also found that some patients who met NINCDS-ADRDA criteria for AD also met diagnostic criteria for sVD. Dementia seen in community-dwelling patients may therefore be driven by interactions between gray and white matter changes.

For these reasons our research group has chosen the neuroimaging variable of LA as the independent variable. This has methodological advantages as well. The radiological classifications are performed by examiners who are blind to the patients’ clinical presentation or neuropsychological data. Using LA severity, rather than diagnosis, to group patients also circumvents the circularity problem that exists when neuropsychological constructs (which are often core criteria for diagnostic classification) are studied as the dependent variables. Previous research shows that examining dementia by LA severity reveals differential patterns of impairment in grammatical comprehension (Giovannetti et al., 2008), working memory (e.g., Lamar et al., 2007), and visuoconstruction (Cosentino et al., 2006).

For the present study we specifically hypothesized distinct serial list learning patterns and error presentation. We expected LA group differences on immediate, delay, recognition index comparisons and error production (i.e., intrusion characteristics, perseverations). We hypothesized that individuals with mild amounts of LA would produce an amnesic pattern (i.e., pronounced forgetting after a delay and failure to benefit from recognition testing). Error analysis would identify this group to produce many extra-list intrusion errors on category cued recall testing, with these more frequent in the English language suggestive of reduced lexical and semantic integrity. We also expect this group to over-endorse recognition foil types, symbolizing failure to benefit from recognition testing. In contrast, the severe LA would produce a dysexecutive pattern on a nine-word list-learning test design for dementia (i.e., despite poor performance on free recall testing, these patients would benefit from delayed recognition testing). Additionally, individuals with severe LA would produce minimal intrusions throughout testing, but more perseverations than the other two groups. The severe LA group would also demonstrate susceptibility to recognition test foils that pulled items from the interference test condition. We expected that the moderate LA group will fall in between the mild and severe LA groups and present with features of the amnesic as well as the dysexecutive profiles described above.

METHOD

Participants

A cohort of 144 mild to moderate dementia outpatients was drawn from an outpatient, university-affiliated memory clinic. This group includes an original set of 115 patients reported upon by Libon et al. (2008) and 29 new prospectively recruited patients. A team consisting of a neurologist, neuropsychologist, and social worker diagnosed the presence of dementia based on (1) a decline in activities of daily living (ADLs) and/or instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs; Lawton & Brody, 1969), (2) medical and neurological evaluations including an MRI study of the brain and laboratory studies to assess for reversible causes of dementia, (3) a comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation that included the P(r)VLT, and (4) a social work assessment. A brain MRI was obtained on all dementia patients within 2 weeks of the neuropsychological assessment. Using the criteria of McKhann et al. (1984) and Chui et al. (1992), our sample consisted of 83 patients who met criteria for probable AD (McKhann et al., 1984) and 61 patients who met criteria for the diagnosis of probable/possible ischemic vascular dementia (also known as subcortical vessel vascular dementia, sVD; Chui et al., 1992). As mentioned earlier, however, due to growing evidence of pathology overlap and validity concerns for several sVD diagnostic criteria, we will not base our analyses on these dementia diagnoses. No dementia outpatient presented with an ischemic score < 4 (Hachinski et al., 1974), no patient presented with either a sudden onset of cognitive decline or a stepwise course with respect to their dementing illness, and findings on the neurological examination were non-focal for all patients.

Also included is a new set of community-dwelling non-demented (ND) older adult controls to provide a reference PrVLT for z-score calculation. The ND control participants were recruited from independent retirement centers. Neuropsychologists (CP, DL, DP) or supervised trainees screened for physical, mental health, and cognitive difficulties. Cognitive inclusion criteria required a Mini-Mental State Examination ≥26 (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975) and intact daily living (IADLs; Lawton & Brody, 1969). Additionally, each control had a Boston Naming Test age and education corrected T-score ≥40 (Heaton, Miller, Taylor, Grant, 2004; Kaplan, Goodglass, & Weintraub, 1983; group raw mean = 53.13 ± 6.30), and performed normally on a test of Clock Drawing (Libon, Malamut, Swenson, Sands, & Cloud, 1996a; e.g., group command executive error mean = .21 ± .41).

Dementia and ND participants were excluded if there was endorsement of current depression via interview and subjective measurement (Geriatric Depression Scale, GDS; Yesavage et al., 1983), history of stroke (excluding evidence of incidental small vessel lacunes on dementia MRIs, which were coded as either present or absent), major medical/CNS disease, seizure disorder, thyroid disease, closed head injury, substance abuse, major depression, or other serious psychiatric disorders. All participants were ambulatory and relatively medically well and stable. Informed written consent was obtained according to Institutional Review Board guidelines and the Declaration of Helsinki.

Brain MRI protocol

A 1.5 Tesla Siemens magnetic scanner was used to obtain brain MRI data on the outpatient dementia participants. Both T1 (TR/TE = 500ms/15ms) and T2 (TR/TE = 4000 ms/90ms) weighted studies were obtained in order to better discriminate areas with LA from perivascular spaces and lacunes. The severity of white matter alterations was quantified using a visual rating, 40-point scale, Junque Leukoaraiosis Scale (Junque et al., 1990). This scale divides each hemisphere into five areas: frontal centrum semiovale, parietal centrum semiovale, white matter around the frontal horns, white matter around the body of the lateral ventricles, and white matter around the atrium and the occipital horns. The severity of LA in each area was graded from 0 (no visible LA) to 4 (severe LA) and then all area scores were summed (WMA score range = 0–40; 40 = no intact white matter present). LA scores were calculated by a board-certified neuroradiologist who was blind to all clinical information and had established excellent inter-rater reliability for a previous investigation (inter-rater reliability, r = .98, p < .001; Libon et al., 1998). The Junque LA scale’s validity has been based on relations between severe LA scores and poor performance on a wide variety of tests that measure executive control, working memory, and information-processing speed (Lamar et al., 2007; Libon et al., 2004, 2008; Price et al., 2005).

LA group classification (mild, moderate, severe)

LA group division was based on cut-points reported in our prior work demonstrating that specific levels of LA contribute to different neuropsychological profiles in patients with dementia (see Libon et al., 2008; Price et al., 2005). In these earlier studies we calculated the Junque LA scale frequency distribution and subdivided the Junque LA scores into three equal portions (i.e., mild, Junque score = 0–8; moderate Junque = 9–11; severe Junque = 18 and higher). Similar to previous publications (Libon et al., 2008; Price et al., 2005), these cut-points resulted in the following: a mild LA group with a Junque LA mean suggesting 10.03% abnormality in the white matter (n = 73, Junque mean ± SD = 4.01 ± 2.78), a moderate LA group with a Junque LA mean indicating 30.05% abnormality (n = 44, mean ± SD = 12.02 ± 2.26); a severe LA group with a Junque LA mean indicating 54% abnormality (n = 27, mean ± SD = 22.30 ± 4.35).

There were no significant differences in age, education, MMSE, or GDS between the three LA groups (all p values ≥.06) or between education and GDS relative to the PrVLT reference control group (all p values >.11). The reference control group, used for standardizing the PrVLT factors, was similar in all demographic variables to the LA groups, with the exception of age for the moderate LA group (p = .003, control group younger). The reference control group also had higher MMSE scores than the LA groups (all p values < .001). There were more females than males in each participant group (p < .01). See Table 1 for participant details.

Table 1.

Group means, standard deviations, and frequencies for leukoaraiosis (LA) and demographics

Mild (n = 73) Moderate (n = 44) Severe (n = 27) NC (n = 24) p value
LA Junque Score 4.01 ± 2.78 12.02 ± 2.26 22.30 ± 4.35 <.001
Age 78.52 ± 5.69 80.95 ± 4.98 79.44 ± 4.43 75.63 ± 9.39 *
Education 12.56 ± 2.82 12.16 ± 2.76 11.93 ± 2.13 13.73 ± 2.67 ns
MMSE 22.58 ± 3.01 22.36 ± 3.60 21.74 ± 3.13 28.21 ± 1.25 <.001
GDS 3.56 ± 2.77 3.68 ± 3.20 4.67 ± 3.09 2.50 ± 3.55 ns
M:F 1:5 1:2 1:4 1:2 ns
*

Age: Tukey HSD shows Moderate LA < NC, p = .003, all other comparisons ns. LA Junque Score = Junque Leukoaraiosis Scale Score (score range = 0–40); education = years of education; MMSE = Mini Mental-State Examination; GDS = Geriatric Depression Scale (score of 10 = cut-off for mild depression); M:F = male/female.

Philadelphia (repeatable) Verbal Learning Test (PrVLT)

The PrVLT is a nine-word serial list-learning test with three test versions specifically designed for dementia populations. The PrVLT has been used in our earlier work (Libon et al., 2008; Price et al., 2004), but is described here in detail since it is the primary measure from which our hypotheses are assessed. The PrVLT is similar in design to the nine-word mental status version of the CVLT (Delis, Kramer, Kapla, & Ober, 1999) with the exceptions that (1) the test exemplars were constructed using a corpus of words elicited for a healthy elderly control group whose demographics match the dementia patients under consideration, and (2) the test contains all nine interference list words in the delayed recognition test (for rationale, see Davis, Price, Kaplan, Libon, 2002). The corpus of older-adult category exemplars (e.g., vegetable, fruit, etc.) were generated from 69 cognitively intact community-dwelling elderly volunteers (age = 77.90 ± 9.66). All participants were English speakers and performed normally on basic screening (MMSE < 27 & GDS ≤ 10).1 There are three versions of the PrVLT. For the current study, each P(r)VLT version was administered randomly and equivalently among participant groups (see Table 2).

Table 2.

Group raw unstandardized Philadelphia (repeatable) Verbal Learning Test (P(r)VLT) index mean and standard deviation Scores

LA group

Mild Moderate Severe ND
P(r)VLT Indices
Immediate free Recall
List A, Trials 1–5 20.64 (6.41) 21.51 (5.50) 22.30 (1.51) 32.46 (5.20)
 Trial 1 3.10 (1.29) 3.23 (1.31) 3.04 (1.22) 4.15 (1.48)
 Trial 2 3.99 (1.56) 4.18 (1.53) 4.11 (1.85) 6.00 (1.38)
 Trial 3 4.28 (1.46) 4.45 (1.23) 5.00 (1.62) 7.08 (1.21)
 Trial 4 4.56 (1.50) 4.70 (1.34) 5.00 (1.90) 7.25 (1.11)
 Trial 5 4.71 (1.59) 4.82 (1.30) 5.04 (1.76) 7.79 (1.18)
List B, Interference 2.89 (1.35) 3.16 (1.25) 3.07 (1.90) 4.63 (1.10)
Delay Recall
Short Delay Free Recall 1.23 (1.35) 1.33 (1.60) 2.48 (2.14) 6.21 (1.72)
Short Delay Cued Recall 2.93 (1.77) 3.07 (1.70) 3.48 (2.03) 6.79 (1.61)
Long Delay Free Recall 0.97 (1.48) 1.21 (1.70) 2.81 (2.59) 6.29 (1.97)
Long Delay Cued Recall 2.25 (1.69) 2.51 (1.76) 3.22 (2.03) 6.83 (1.58)
Intrusion Errors and Perseverations
Free Recall Intrusion Errors 3.05 (2.88) 1.95 (2.28) 3.07 (3.30) 1.00 (1.47)
Cued Recall Intrusion Errors 5.79 (3.77) 3.51 (3.51) 3.63 (3.21) 0.75 (1.37)
Perseverations 0.65 (1.01) 1.39 (2.56) 1.44 (1.91) 1.29 (1.37)
Recognition Test Performance
Recognition Hits 7.12 (1.91) 7.07 (1.88) 7.00 (1.94) 8.38 (.82)
Recognition False Positive 10.18 (4.75) 7.23 (4.25) 4.41 (3.04) 0.88 (1.92)
Recognition Error Type
 Semantic 4.43 (2.45) 2.66 (2.26) 1.48 (1.37) 0.33 (.70)
 Interference 4.18 (2.24) 3.70 (2.32) 2.63 (2.48) 0.21 (.41)
 Unrelated 1.56 (1.80) 0.84 (1.40) 0.26 (.45) 0.00 (.00)

LA = Leukoaraiosis, ND = Non-demented comparison group.

The administration and construction of the P(r)VLT is identical to the original 16-word CVLT and the nine-word experimental version of the CVLT (Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & Ober, 1987; Libon, Mattson, Glosser, & Kaplan, 1996b); i.e., an orally presented nine-word “shopping list” (list A), composed of three items from three categories, is administered five times, followed by an interference (list B) test condition with three words from a category in list A. The interference condition is followed by short delay free and category cued recall test conditions. After a 20-minute filled delay, free recall and category recall are reassessed, followed by a delay recognition test for list A. The recognition test contains 36 words: 9 words from list A (hits), 9 words from list B, 9 prototypic semantic foils, and 9 unrelated words (n = 27 foils). On the recognition test, participants are asked to indicate if each orally presented word was from list A. Perseverations and Intrusion errors are scored as originally described for the 16-word CVLT and the nine-word experimental version of the CVLT (Delis et al., 1987; Libon et al., 1996a).

PrVLT Outcome Measures included four composite indices and a select set of error scores (intrusion, word frequency of cued recall errors, perseveration, and recognition foils). The four factor-based composite indices (see Table 3) include a Delay Free Recall factor, Immediate Free Recall factor, Intrusion Error factor, and Recognition factor. The test indices comprising each factor were converted to composite z-scores referenced to the 24 control participants. P(r) VLT Free, Cued Recall Intrusions, Perseverations were scored as originally described for the 16-word CVLT and the nine-word experimental version of the CVLT (Delis et al., 1987; Libon et al., 1996). PrVLT Word Frequency of Cued Recall Intrusion Errors was assessed with the Francis and Kucera (1982) corpus. This corpus allowed us to calculated the average word frequency of short and long cued recall intrusions in the English language (a high score indicates greater frequency in the English language). We acknowledge that the original purpose of the Francis and Kucera (1982) corpus was to provide a measure of how frequently words appear in the English language, and that the frequency of a word may not necessarily be the same as its category word frequency. Our decision to use the Francis and Kucera (1982) corpus instead of other lists (e.g., Battig & Montague, 1969), was based on the fact that the 11 semantic categories used in the construction of the three forms of the P(r)VLT were only represented by Francis and Kucera (1982). P(r) VLT Recognition Errors by Foil Type (Semantically related, list b-interference, or unrelated) were recorded to examine our hypothesis that LA groups would differ by foil endorsement.

Table 3.

Factor loadings of P(r)VLT indices using varimax rotation: Eigenvalues and percentage of variancea

Factor loading

1 2 3 4
Delay Free Intrusions Recognition Communality
Long Delay Cued Recall .86 .19 −.09 .03 .74
Long Delay Free Recall .85 .12 −.14 −.05 .70
Short Delay Free Recall .85 .06 −.05 −.03 .77
Short Delay Cued Recall .81 .22 −.05 .05 .78
Interference Free Recall .10 .91 −.16 .01 .71
Total 1–5 Free Recall .45 .73 .13 −.03 .73
Free Intrusions Total .02 −.16 .82 −.16 .86
Cued Intrusions Total −.22 .10 .77 .24 .77
Recognition Hits .31 .06 −.16 .81 .81
Recognition False Positives −.37 .08 .25 .77 .76
 Eigenvalues 3.86 1.44 1.23 1.09
 % of variance 38.62 14.43 12.26 10.93
a

Principal component analysis using varimax rotation (five iterations) with exclusion of eigenvalues < 1.0 assessed the underlying structure of the P(r)VLT indices in the dementia sample. These factors were converted to composite z-scores based on the ND sample. The four composites have adequate normative properties for analysis purposes (i.e., Delay Free Recall skewness = .99, kurtosis = .91; Immediate Free Recall skewness = −.11, kurtosis = −.17; Intrusions skewness = −1.11, kurtosis = 2.08; Recognition skewness = .27, kurtosis = −.19).

Executive functioning

We compared executive functioning between the three LA groups in order to confirm greater executive difficulty in the severe LA group. The measures used in the composite are: the non-automatized index of the Boston Revision of the Wechsler Memory Scale – Mental Control subtest (WMS-MC; Lamar, Price, Davis, Kaplan, & Libon, 2002; Wechsler, 1945) and test of letter fluency (Benton & Hamsher, 1989). Raw test scores were converted to z-scores based on published norms (Lamar et al., 2002; Tombaugh, Kozak, & Rees, 1999, respectively). Standardized scores were averaged to reflect an overall “executive” composite score. Details regarding the composition and scoring of the non-automatized index of the Boston Revision of the WMS-MC subtest can be found in Lamar et al. (2002). Prior research has shown that performance on these tests is very sensitive to LA severity in patients with mild dementia (Libon et al., 2008; Price et al., 2005). On the letter fluency test patients were given 60 seconds to generate as many words as possible that begin with the given letter of the alphabet, excluding numbers, proper nouns, and the same words with a different suffix. Total correct for each letter were summed for the dependent variable. Imaging studies have shown that letter fluency tests activate the left dorsolateral prefrontal region in younger (Phelps, Hyder, Blamire, & Shulman, 1997) and older adults (Gourovitch et al., 2000).

Statistical analyses

PrVLT factors were converted to z-scores based on the ND group scores. A MANOVA assessed LA group differences on the four PrVLT list-learning factors with follow-up via Tukey post-hocs. Paired t-tests assessed hypothesized within-group factor differences (PrVLT Immediate versus Delay; Delay versus Recognition) and a one-way ANOVA confirmed LA group differences in false positive errors. For non-normalized data, non-parametric analyses assessed hypothesized LA between- and within-group error differences (intrusion, cued recall intrusion frequency, perseveration, and recognition foil types). One-way ANOVAs confirmed greater executive dysfunction in the severe LA group. PrVLT analyses report eta, Cohen’s d (Cohen, 1988). Significance was set at p ≤ .01.

RESULTS2

LA group differences on P(r)VLT z-score composite indices

A MANOVA identified a main LA group effect on a linear combination of the four P(r)VLT z-score composite factors, Wilk’s λ = .777, F(131, 214) = 4.61, p < .001, η = .35. Resulting follow-up univariate ANOVAs were significant for Delay Free Recall, Recognition, and the Intrusion composite factors: Delay Free Recall = F(2, 140) = 5.51, p = .005, η = .27; Intrusions = F(2, 140) = 6.95, p = .001, η = .30; Recognition = F(2, 140) = 12.20, p < .001, η = .38. For the Delay Free Recall factor, the mild and moderate LA groups scored lower than the severe group (Delay Free Recall z-score mean ± SD: mild LA = −2.72 ± 0.78; moderate LA = −2.62 ± 0.84; severe LA = −2.07 ± 1.17, ps ≤ .01; mild to severe d = 0.65; moderate to severe d = .54). For the Intrusion factor, the mild LA group scored lower (producing greater numbers of intrusion errors) than the moderate LA group (Intrusion mean ± SD: mild LA = −2.63 ± 1.91; moderate LA = −1.34 ± 1.72; severe LA = −1.79 ± 1.91, p < .001; mild to moderate d = 0.71). For the Recognition factor, the mild LA group scored lower than the other two LA groups (p < .001; mild to moderate d = 0.52; mild to severe d = 1.13). There was a trend for the moderate group to score lower than the severe group (p = .03; Recognition z-score mean ± SD: mild LA = −3.18 ± 1.24; moderate LA = −2.47 ± 1.49; severe LA = −1.75 ± 1.28; moderate to severe d = 0.52). As predicted, the severe LA group performed best on delayed recognition testing, with the Mild LA group producing many intrusion errors and the worst group performance on recognition.

Within-group factor composite comparisons: Immediate Free Recall vs Delay Free Recall; Delay Free Recall vs Recognition Composite Comparison

Only the mild and moderate LA groups scored lower on the Delay Free Recall compared to the Immediate Free Recall (both p < .001; mild d = 0.83; moderate d = 1.05) suggesting poor retention. The mild LA group also scored lower on the Recognition index relative to Delay Free Recall (p = .002; d = 0.44) suggesting a failure to benefit from recognition testing. Thus, in addition to distinct group differences, these findings identify meaningful performance patterns within each LA group.

Group and intrusion frequency (free vs cued)

LA groups only differed in total number of cued recall intrusions, cued recall: Kruskal Wallace: χ2(2) = 16.60, p < .001; free recall, p>.05. As predicted, the mild LA group produced more cued recall intrusions than the moderate and severe group (both p < .001; mild to moderate d = 0.63; mild to severe d = 0.62).

Word frequency of cued recall intrusions (short and long delay combined, see Method)

LA groups differed in cued recall intrusion word frequency as measured with the Frances and Kucera corpus, Kruskal Wallace, χ2(2) = 13.88, p < .001. The mild LA group produced intrusions that were more frequent in the English language compared to the other LA groups (all p values ≤.002; mild to moderate d = 0.54; mild to severe d = 0.64).

Perseverations

Contrary to our prediction, LA groups did not differ on total number of perseverations, Kruskal Wallace, χ2(2) = 2.37, ns.

Recognition false positives and foil endorsements

Analyses assessed for group differences in error and type of foil endorsement. Response bias to list B recognition foils may suggest a source recall problem signifying executive difficulties, while equal endorsement of all types may indicate a profound amnestic impairment.

One-way ANOVA confirmed a between-group difference in false positive foil endorsement, F(2, 140) = 20.24, p < .001, η = .47. The mild LA group produced many more false positives than the other two LA groups (both p < .001; mild to moderate d = 0.65; mild to severe d = 1.45), and the moderate more than the severe LA group (p = .001; d = 0.76).

Between-group analyses of foil type were significant (Kruskal Wallace, all p < .006); the mild LA group significantly endorsed more semantic and unrelated foils than the moderate LA group (U test, p < .001, p = .01, respectively; semantic d = 0.75; unrelated d = 0.45), and more recognition foils of all types (semantic, list B interference, unrelated) than the severe LA group (U test, all p ≤ .002; semantic d = 1.49; interference d = 0.66; unrelated d = 0.99). Within-group analyses of foil type shows that the moderate LA group significantly endorsed more list B interference relative to semantic foils, Wilcoxon sign test, p = .01; d = 0.45, and that all groups endorsed many more semantic and list B foils relative to unrelated foils (p < .001, all comparisons, all Cohen’s d < 1.0). These findings suggest that the mild LA group’s poor recognition index performance differed from the other LA groups by an over-endorsement of all foil types. A significant predilection for list B foils was only observed for the moderate LA group.

LA group and “;executive function” impairment

One-way ANOVA yielded a main effect of LA group, F(2, 139) = 13.83, p < .001, η = .41. Tukey tests show the severe LA more impaired than mild or moderate LA on the composite of executive function (mean ± SD: severe LA: −2.52 ± 1.29; moderate LA: −1.61 ± 1.37; mild: −1.15 ± 0.96, p values ≤.009).

DISCUSSION

This study indicates that a group of dementia patients with a relatively cohesive clinical picture involving insidious onset with no evidence of stepwise decline, ADL/IADL difficulties, memory difficulties (at least from a superficial perspective involving family report), and impairment in at least one other cognitive domain, can exhibit differing verbal serial list-learning profiles based solely on leukoaraiosis (LA) severity groupings. Individuals with very little to no LA exhibited the most striking list-learning impairment with characteristics of a classic amnesic profile. Relative to this group, individuals with severe amounts of LA performed substantially better on the delay and recognition indices and error analyses. These findings are most synonymous with a dysexecutive profile, although hypothesized increases in perseverations and increased recognition list B interference foils were not observed. Individuals with moderate LA produced elements of both amnesic and dysexecutive profiles. These distinct list-learning profiles show that LA is not representative of a single, continuous disease state but likely represent distinct dementia syndromes.

The mild LA group demonstrated the clearest amnesic profile on composite comparisons and error analyses. Despite a comparable level of impairment among the three LA groups on the immediate free recall in list A test trials, individuals with mild LA (where LA is estimated to be in 10% or less of the white matter) demonstrated significant forgetting such that performance declined after a delay. Recall was not aided by recognition testing. These patients also produced numerous intrusion errors, particularly on cued recall testing. These types of intrusions have been functionally associated with the left (Desgranges, et al., 2002) and bilateral (Lekeu et al., 2003) parahippocampal regions using PET methodology. Moreover, the mild LA group produced more generic intrusions such as “;dog” or “apple”. This suggests greater disruption of associative semantic processes mediated by either lateral or medial temporal lobe dysfunction (Hodges, Patterson, Oxbury, & Funnell, 1992; Warrington, 1975). A recent French study by Canolle et al. (2008) also reports that category intrusions were more prototypic among individuals with Alzheimer’s disease.

Additionally, because our list-learning paradigm contained equal numbers of recognition foil types we were also able to directly examine recognition error endorsement. In addition to failing to benefit from recognition testing, the mild LA group produced numerous false positives with no significant response bias to recognition semantic or list B interference foils. They also endorsed many unrelated foils compared to individuals with moderate or severe LA. This liberal “yes” response suggests memory impairment beyond that of proactive interference (Davis et al., 2002). Overall, the mild LA group’s pattern reveals compromised learning, loss of information over a delay, failure to benefit from recognition testing, and evidence of semantic/lexical disturbance. This pattern is most suggestive of pathology involving the medial temporal lobe, such as would be seen in pathologically pure Alzheimer’s disease.

The severe LA group, by contrast, produced the most dysexecutive profile. Despite similar immediate memory performance to those with lesser LA, this group outperformed the other two LA groups on delay and recognition testing. They also produced few cued recall intrusions and recognition foil distractors. This performance pattern, coupled with their striking impairment on executive tests (range of one to two standard deviations below the other two groups) underscores the hypothesis that memory difficulties associated with severe LA is dysexecutive in nature. However, the severe LA group did not produce significantly more perseverations or recognition interference foils as hypothesized. Future studies should compare severe LA dementia patients to classically defined subcortical diseases (e.g., PD) on list-learning paradigms in order to determine the actual extent of this dysexecutive profile. We also encourage future study of location of LA in the deep versus more surface (u-fiber) regions, and which health risk factors (e.g., vascular) predict severity of LA in this group.

The moderate LA group demonstrated a mixed memory profile with both amnesic and dysexecutive traits. They scored lower on the delay index relative to the immediate recall index, yet produced few intrusion errors. They also endorsed more semantic and list B foils than the severe LA group, and a comparison of their own within group foil response bias suggests that they were overly pulled to the list B interference foils. Moderate LA appears to mark mixed cortical and subcortical pathologies.

We believe that our choice to ignore traditional dementia diagnosis (AD, sVD) and focus on LA severity groupings was experimentally and theoretically beneficial. First, it allowed us to examine the differential role of LA on list-learning profiles without prejudice of dementia diagnosis based partially on neuropsychological and neuroradiological data. Second, we have preliminary clinical evidence that LA is not necessarily a progressive disease state. At least in individuals with similar dementia severity, LA can manifest in different amounts. Third, when combined with neuropsychological testing, LA severity may foretell underlying pathology and have therapeutic implications. Recent research suggests that individuals with greater amounts of LA and a more dysexecutive profile may benefit most from acetylcholinesterase inhibitors such as donepezil (e.g., Thomas, Libon, Ledakis, 2005). Bypassing diagnostic criteria in favor of combined neuropsychologic and neuroradiologic profiles appears relevant given growing awareness that vascular dementia includes a heterogeneous group of cognitive disorders (Jellinger & Attems, 2007) and the dominance of mixed dementias in medical settings.

Indeed, there is growing evidence that LA can have distinct radiological and pathological patterns. LA presenting as confluent on MRI (widespread throughout the deep white matter) has been shown to be progressive, while punctuate LA (isolated regions on MRI) is not (e.g., Gouw, et al., 2008; Sachdev, Wen, Chen, & Brodaty, 2007). Sjobeck, Haglund, and Englund (2006) found that neuropathologically pure AD brains with LA showed primary bifrontal loss of oligodendrocytes despite no other vascular components. They speculate that this bifrontal pattern may clinically associate with executive dysfunction and incorrectly lead clinicians to a diagnosis of vascular dementia. There are also reports that LA is associated with reduced vascular integrity both in and beyond the LA lesion in the white matter (e.g., Brown, Moody, Thore, Challa, & Anstrom, 2007; Young, Halliday, & Kril, 2008). Within these regions LA has variable vascular densities, which may affect the brain globally with capillary loss while not necessarily leading to infarction of tissue (Brown et al., 2007). In some research, overall severity of LA associates with the degree of cortical (Capizzano et al., 2004) and medial temporal atrophy (van der Flier et al., 2005; but note contrary evidence reported by Brun & Englund, 1986; Sjobeck & Englund, 2003).

Questions about topographical location of LA and regional association in cortical changes have been raised (Sjobeck et al., 2006), yet the issue still needs clinical investigation. White matter fibers have many multi-modal and limbic regions (Filley, 2001); LA near white matter u-fibers may have distinct clinical and pathological characteristics from that in the deep white matter. While there are a growing number of sophisticated pathology studies examining regional LA in periventricular and deep regions by lobe (e.g., Young et al., 2008), there remain few studies integrating neuropsychological patterns with MRI LA location severity or pathology. We are expanding our studies to examine these issues, and encourage other investigators to do so as well.

We acknowledge limitations in our current study. The 40-point Junque LA scale, as well as any other MRI visual rating scale, is best viewed as only a proxy of true white matter involvement. The means by which we operationally measured LA, therefore, is semi-quantitative and provides only an approximate estimate of LA involvement. Another limitation was that we had limited availability to medical information that could have potentially shed light on the underlying etiology of MRI-LA (i.e., hypertension, diabetes). This study also did not assess dissociations between LA and other imaging variables such as hippocampal volume and cortical thickness. While we identified that the presence or absence of lacunae did not alter the overall results of the study (see footnote 2), region of lacunae was not accounted for in the current analyses and may provide important information. Finally, we recognize that our control group for the PrVLT is small and thus serves only as a preliminary normative reference set for dementia. A larger control group that has imaging data as well will be beneficial for future studies. Despite these caveats we believe that the current research provides convincing evidence that LA severity can be associated with distinct patterns of impairment on episodic memory tests.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We wish to thank the editor (JS) and reviewers for their insightful and constructive comments/suggestions. This research was supported in part by the following NIH grants: K23NS060660_01 (CCP) and F32-AG021362 (CCP); R03-AG026610 (ALJ), R03-AG027480 (ALJ), K23-AG030962 (Paul B. Beeson Career Development Award in Aging; ALJ), and P30-AG013846 (Boston University Alzheimer’s Disease Core Center; ALJ). Portions of this research were presented at the 3rd Biannual Meeting of the International Society for Vascular Behavioural and Cognitive Disorders, San Antonio, TX.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf

This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

1

Please contact the authors for more information on P(r)VLT test construction.

2

Some readers may question whether lacune presence altered the PrVLT factor structure or results. We re-examined these data after excluding patients with lacunes (new sample: mild LA n = 57, moderate LA n = 26, severe LA n = 2). The PrVLT factor structure remained the same with the exception of a slightly lower percent variance (new cumulative percentage = 74.13). Result findings were unchanged.

No authors have conflicts of interest to report.

REFERENCES

  1. Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick PL. Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1986;9:357–381. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.09.030186.002041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Barker WW, Luis CA, Kashuba A, Luis M, Harwood DG, Loewenstein D, et al. Relative frequencies of Alzheimer disease, lewy body, vascular and frontotemporal dementia, and hippocampal sclerosis in the State of Florida Brain Bank. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders. 2002;16(4):203–212. doi: 10.1097/00002093-200210000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Battig WF, Montague WE. Category norms for verbal items in 56 categories: A replication of the Connecticut Category Norms. Journal of Experimental Psychology Monograph. 1969;80:1–46. [Google Scholar]
  4. Benton AL, Hamsher K, de S. Multilingual aphasia examination. Iowa City, IA: AJA Associates; 1989. [Google Scholar]
  5. Brown WR, Moody DM, Thore CR, Challa VR, Anstrom JA. Vascular dementia in leukoaraiosis may be a consequence of capillary loss not only in lesions, but also in normal-appearing white matter and cortex as well. Journal of the Neurological Sciences. 2007;257(1–2):62–66. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2007.01.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brun A, Englund E. A white matter disorder in dementia of the Alzheimer type: A pathoanatomical study. Annals of Neurology. 1986;19(3):253–262. doi: 10.1002/ana.410190306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Canolle M, Messaoudi M, Ayoub B, Descours I, Bocquet P, Gely-Nargeot MC, et al. Prototypic value of semantic intrusion errors in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychologie & Neuropsychiatrie du Vieillissement. 2008;6(1):67–79. doi: 10.1684/pnv.2008.0115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Capizzano AA, Acion L, Bekinschtein T, Furman M, Gomila H, Martinez A, et al. White matter hyperintensities are significantly associated with cortical atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 2004;75(6):822–821. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2003.019273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  10. Cosentino SA, Jefferson AL, Carey M, Price CC, Davis-Garrett KL, Swenson R, et al. The clinical diagnosis of cerebrovascular dementia: A comparison between four classification systems. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 2004a;18(1):6–21. doi: 10.1080/13854040490507118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cosentino SA, Jefferson A, Chute DL, Kaplan E, Libon DJ. Clock drawing errors in dementia: Neuropsychological and neuroanatomical considerations. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology. 2004b;17(2):14–84. doi: 10.1097/01.wnn.0000119564.08162.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chui HC. Subcortical ischemic vascular dementia. Neurologic Clinics. 2007;25:777–740. doi: 10.1016/j.ncl.2007.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chui HC, Victoroff JI, Margolin D, Jagust W, Shankle R, Katzman R. Criteria for the diagnosis of ischemic vascular dementia proposed by the State of California Alzheimer’s Disease Diagnostic and Treatment Centers. Neurology. 1992;42(3):413–480. doi: 10.1212/wnl.42.3.473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Davis KL, Price CC, Kaplan E, Libon DJ. Error analysis of the nine-word California Verbal Learning Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 2002;16(1):81–89. doi: 10.1076/clin.16.1.81.8330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Delis D, Kramer J, Kaplan E, Ober B. California Verbal Learning Test - Second Edition (CVLT-II) Austin, TX: Pearson PsychCorp.; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  16. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober B. California Verbal Learning Test: Adult Version Manual. Austin, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  17. Delis DC, Massman PJ, Butters N, Salmon DP, Cermak LS, Kramer JH. Profiles of demented and amnesic patients on the California Verbal Learning Test: Implications for the assessment of memory disorders. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1991;3(1):19–26. [Google Scholar]
  18. Desgranges B, Baron JC, Giffard B, Chételat G, Lalevée C, Viader F, et al. The neural basis of intrusions in free recall and cued recall: A PET study in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroimage. 2002;17(3):1658–1664. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dobbins IG, Simons JS, Schacter DL. fMRI evidence for separable and lateralized prefrontal memory monitoring processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2004;16:908–920. doi: 10.1162/0898929041502751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fein G, Di Sclafani V, Tanabe J, Cardenas V, Weiner M, Jagust W, et al. Hippocampal and cortical atrophy predict dementia in subcortical ischemic vascular disease. Neurology. 2000;55:1626–1635. doi: 10.1212/wnl.55.11.1626. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Filley CM. The behavioral neurology of white matter. New York: Oxford University Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  22. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1975;12(3):189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Francis WN, Kucera H. The frequency analysis of English usage. Boston, MA: Houghton-Mifflin Co.; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  24. Giovannetti T, Hopkins MW, Fried J, Bettcher BM, Schmidt KS, Libon DJ. Syntactic comprehension deficits are associated with MRI white matter alterations in dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2008;14(4):542–551. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708080715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gourovitch ML, Kirkby BS, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR, Gold JM, Esposito G, et al. A comparison of rCBF patterns during letter and semantic fluency. Neuropsychology. 2000;14(3):353–360. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.14.3.353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gouw AA, van der Flier WM, Fazekas F, van Straaten EC, Pantoni L, Poggesi A, et al. Progression of white matter hyperintensities and incidence of new lacunes over a 3-year period: The Leukoaraiosis and Disability Study. Stroke. 2008;39(5):1414–1420. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.107.498535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hachinski VC, Lassen NA, Marshall J. Multi-infarct dementia: A cause of mental deterioration in the elderly. The Lancet. 1974;2:207–210. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(74)91496-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hachinski VC, Potter P, Merskey H. Leuko-araiosis. Archives of Neurology. 1987;44(1):21–23. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1987.00520130013009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Heaton RK, Miller SW, Taylor MJ, Grant I. Revised comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead-Reitan battery (norms, manual and computer program) Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  30. Hodges JR, Patterson K, Oxbury S, Funnell E. Semantic dementia: Progressive fluent aphasia with temporal lobe atrophy. Brain. 1992;115:1783–1806. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.6.1783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Jellinger KA. The enigma of vascular cognitive disorder and vascular dementia. Acta Neuropathologica. 2007;113:349–388. doi: 10.1007/s00401-006-0185-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Jellinger KA, Attems J. Neuropathological evaluation of mixed dementia. Journal of the Neurological Sciences. 2007;257(1–2):80–87. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2007.01.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Junque C, Pujol J, Vendrell P, Bruna O, Jodar M, Ribas JC, et al. Leukoaraiosis on magnetic resonance imaging and speed of mental processing. Archives of Neurology. 1990;47(2):151–156. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1990.00530020047013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kaplan E, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. Boston naming test: Revised edition. Philadelphia, PA: Lea & Febiger; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  35. Kramer JH, Delis DC, Blusewicz MJ, Brandt J, Ober BA, Strauss M. Verbal memory errors in Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s dementias. Developmental Neuropsychology. 1988;4:1–15. [Google Scholar]
  36. Laakso MP, Partanean K, Riekkinen P, Riekkinen P, Lehtovirta M, Helkala EL, et al. Hippocampal volumes in Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia, and in vascular dementia: An MRI study. Neurology. 1996;46(3):678–681. doi: 10.1212/wnl.46.3.678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Lamar M, Price CC, Davis KL, Kaplan E, Libon DJ. Capacity to maintain mental set in dementia. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40(4):435–445. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00125-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lamar M, Price CC, Libon DJ, Penney DL, Kaplan E, Grossman M, et al. Alterations in working memory as a function of leukoaraiosis in dementia. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(2):245–254. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.07.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: Self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist. 1969;9(3):179–186. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lekeu F, Van der Linden M, Chicherio C, Collette F, Degueldre C, Fanck G, et al. Brain correlates of performance in a free/cued recall task with semantic encoding in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders. 2003;17(1):35–45. doi: 10.1097/00002093-200301000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Leys D, Pruvo JP, Parent M, Vermersch P, Soetaert G, Steinling M, et al. Could Wallerian degeneration contribute to “leuko-araiosis” in subjects free of any vascular disorder? Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 1991;54(1):46–50. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.54.1.46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Libon DJ, Bogdanoff B, Cloud BS, Skalina S, Giovannetti T, Gitlin HL, et al. Declarative and procedural learning, quantitative measures of the hippocampus, and subcortical white alterations in Alzheimer’s disease and ischemic vascular dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1998;20(1):30–41. doi: 10.1076/jcen.20.1.30.1490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Libon DJ, Malamut BL, Swenson R, Sands LP, Cloud BS. Further analyses of clock drawings among demented and nondemented older adults. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 1996a;11(3):193–205. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Libon DJ, Mattson RE, Glosser G, Kaplan E. A nine-word dementia version of the California Verbal Learning Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 1996b;10:237–244. [Google Scholar]
  45. Libon DJ, Price CC, Garrett KD, Giovannetti T. From Binswanger’s disease to leuokoaraiosis: What we have learned about subcortical vascular dementia. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 2004;18(1):83–100. doi: 10.1080/13854040490507181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Libon DJ, Price CC, Giovannetti T, Swenson R, Bettcher BM, Heilman KM, et al. Linking MRI subcortical vascular disease with patterns of neuropsycho-logical impairment: Evidence for a threshold effect. Stroke. 2008;39(3):806–813. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.107.489997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Massman PJ, Delis DC, Butters N, Levin BE, Salmon DP. Are all subcortical dementias alike? Verbal learning and memory in Parkinson’s and Huntington’s disease patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1990;12(5):129–144. doi: 10.1080/01688639008401015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology. 1984;34(1):939–944. doi: 10.1212/wnl.34.7.939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Phelps EA, Hyder F, Blamire AM, Shulman RG. FMRI of the prefrontal cortex during overt verbal fluency. Neuroreport. 1997;8(20):561–565. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199701200-00036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Price CC, Garrett KD, Libon DJ, Swenson R, Penney D, Jefferson A, et al. Verbal serial list learning among dementia patients with and without white matter changes: Factor solutions [abstract] Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2004;10:S1–S8. [Google Scholar]
  51. Price CC, Jefferson AL, Merino JG, Heilman KM, Libon DJ. Subcortical vascular dementia: Integrating neuropsychological and neuroradiologic data. Neurology. 2005;65(3):316–382. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000168877.06011.15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Pohjasvaara T, Mantyla R, Ylikoski R, Kaste M, Erkinjuntti T. Comparison of different clinical criteria (DSM-III, ADDTC, ICD-10, NINDS-AIREN, DSM-IV) for the diagnosis of vascular dementia. National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke-Association Internationale pour la Recherche et l’Enseignement en Neurosciences. Stroke. 2000;31:2952–2957. doi: 10.1161/01.str.31.12.2952. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Sachdev P, Wen W, Chen X, Brodaty H. Progression of white matter hyperintensities in elderly individuals over 3 years. Neurology. 2007;68(3):214–222. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000251302.55202.73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Sjobeck M, Englund E. Glial levels determine severity of white matter disease in Alzheimer’s disease: A neuropathological study of glial changes. Neuropathology and Applied Neurobiology. 2003;29(2):159–169. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2990.2003.00456.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Sjobeck M, Haglund M, Englund E. White matter mapping in Alzheimer’s disease: A neuropathological study. Neurobiology of Aging. 2006;27(5):673–680. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Szolnoki Z. Pathomechanism of leukoaraiosis: A molecular bridge between the genetic, biochemical, and clinical processes (a mitochondrial hypothesis) Neuromolecular Medicine. 2007;9(1):21–34. doi: 10.1385/nmm:9:1:21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Thomas DA, Libon DJ, Ledakis GE. Treating dementia patients with vascular lesions with donepezil: A preliminary analysis. Applied Neuropsychology. 2005;12(1):12–18. doi: 10.1207/s15324826an1201_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Tombaugh TN, Kozak J, Rees L. Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 1999;14(2):161–177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Tremont G, Halpert S, Javorsky DJ, Stern RA. Differential impact of executive function on verbal list learning and story recall. Clinical Neuropsychology. 2000;14(3):295–302. doi: 10.1076/1385-4046(200008)14:3;1-P;FT295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. van der Flier WM, van Straaten EC, Barkhof F, Ferro JM, Pantoni L, Basile AM, et al. Medial temporal lobe atrophy and white matter hyperintensities are associated with mild cognitive deficits in non-disabled elderly people: The LADIS study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 2005;76(11):1497–1500. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2005.064998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Warrington EK. The selective impairment of semantic memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1975;27:635–657. doi: 10.1080/14640747508400525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Wechsler D. A standardized memory scale for clinical use. Journal of Psychology. 1945;19:87–95. [Google Scholar]
  63. White L, Small BJ, Petrovitch H, Ross GW, Masaki K, Abbott RD, et al. Recent clinical-pathologic research on the causes of dementia in late life: Update from the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology. 2005;18(4):224–227. doi: 10.1177/0891988705281872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, Lum O, Huang V, Adey M, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression severity scale: A preliminary report. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1983;17(1):37–49. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Young VG, Halliday GM, Kril JJ. Neuropathologic correlates of white matter hyperintensities. Neurology. 2008;71(11):804–811. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000319691.50117.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Xu J, Chen S, Ahmed SH, Chen H, Ku G, Goldberg MP, et al. Amyloidbeta peptides are cytotoxic to oligodendrocytes. Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21(1):RC118. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-01-j0001.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES