Skip to main content
American Journal of Physiology - Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology logoLink to American Journal of Physiology - Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology
. 2010 Jan 27;298(5):R1428–R1434. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00486.2009

Baroreflexes of the rat. VI. Sleep and responses to aortic nerve stimulation in the dmNTS

Xiaorui Tang 1,, Barry R Dworkin 1
PMCID: PMC2867524  PMID: 20106994

Abstract

The sensitivity of the baroreflex determines its stability and effectiveness in controlling blood pressure (BP). Sleep and arousal are reported to affect baroreflex sensitivity, but the findings are not consistent across studies. After statistically correcting the effect of sleep on the baselines in chronically neuromuscular-blocked (NMB) rats, we found that sleep affects BP and heart period (HP) baroreflex gain similarly. This finding is consistent with baroreflex modulation of HP and BP before divergence of the sympathetic and parasympathetic pathways. Therefore, we hypothesized that the gain modulation occurs in the dorsal medial nucleus of the solitary tract (dmNTS). The present study used long-term dmNTS recordings in NMB rats and single-pulse aortic depressor nerve stimulation. Under these conditions, the magnitude of A-fiber evoked responses (ERs), recorded from second- or higher-order dmNTS baroreflex neurons, was reliably augmented during high-amplitude low-frequency EEG activity (slow-wave sleep) and reduced during low-amplitude high-frequency EEG activity (arousal; ΔER = 11%, t = 9.49, P < 0.001, degrees of freedom = 1,016). This result has methodological implications for techniques that use changes in HP to estimate baroreflex BP gain and general implications for understanding the relationship between sleep and cardiovascular control.

Keywords: baroreflex gain, baroreflex sensitivity, nucleus of the solitary tract, arousal, aortic depressor nerve


the negative-feedback baroreflexes have a central role in regulating blood pressure (BP), and baroreflex sensitivity or gain determines the stability and effectiveness of BP control. Sleep and arousal are reported to affect baroreflex sensitivity, but the findings are not consistent across studies that differ in experimental methodology and/or species. With the Oxford method, a vasoconstrictive drug elevates BP, and the cardiac baroreflex gain is defined as the heart rate (HR) change (ΔHR) divided by the BP change (ΔBP). Pickering and others (12, 13, 16) reported clear evidence of greater cardiac baroreflex gain during stage II and IV slow-wave sleep than during wakefulness in humans. Using a noninvasive correlational method that deduces the baroreflex gain from spontaneous BP and HR sequences, Parati et al. (11) also found that gain increased during sleep, but in chronic animal studies that used a similar sequence method, but with more sophisticated criteria for sleep, Zoccoli et al. (21) found that, for rats, gain was substantially independent of states of wakefulness, active sleep, and quiet sleep.

The Oxford and sequence methods measure BP effects on HR, i.e., the baroreflex cardiac gain, but they do not directly measure the vasomotor baroreflex gain or the total loop gain. This is because measurement of the former requires implanted flowmeters and measurement of the latter requires direct access to the baroreceptors. Thus, in practice, to make inferences to total loop gain, which is the variable of interest, the cardiac reflex is taken as a surrogate for the overall reflex, and it is assumed that changes in cardiac gain proportionally represent changes in loop gain; however, the literature does not consistently support this assumption. For example, in chronic dog studies, implanted carotid sinus nerve stimulators elicit 12–22% greater bradycardia during sleep, but sleep produces no change in the net BP depressor effect (20); in baboons, during sleep, compared with the awake resting state, although the carotid occlusion (baroreceptor inhibition) effect on heart period (HP) is increased, lower abdominal conductance is unaffected, and the renal conductance response to occlusion is attenuated (1). In an earlier study (4), using chronically neuromuscular-blocked (NMB) rats, which have distinct diurnal and sleep-wakefulness cycles, we compared the effects of EEG arousal on the open-loop HP and BP baroreflex gains. Using the power of the δ-band of the EEG (EEGδ) as an index of sleep and arousal and aortic depressor nerve (ADN) electrical stimulation as a baroreflex input, we observed that although the HP baroreflex gain increased with sleep, the open-loop BP baroreflex gain (the decrease in BP in response to the ADN test stimulus) did not. However, on further analysis, we discovered that the raw correlational result was misleading. A partial-correlations analysis, which also took into account the effects of sleep on BP, showed that because BP was directly related to arousal and BP baroreflex gain was directly related to BP, the effect of sleep on BP gain had been “masked.” However, HP gain was different: although, as with BP, HP depended on arousal, HP baroreflex gain did not depend on HP. By mathematically “partialing out” the BP effects on the baroreflex BP gain, the underlying relationship between EEGδ and BP baroreflex gain was shown to be approximately the same as that for HP baroreflex gain. Because the effects of sleep on HP and BP gain were almost exactly the same, we hypothesized that the modulation occurred at a common anatomic locus, before divergence of the parasympathetic and sympathetic pathways. Testing that hypothesis led to the present study.

The parasympathetic (cardiac chronotropic) and sympathetic (vascular tone; cardiac inotropic) baroreflex pathways diverge at the dorsal medial nucleus of the solitary tract (dmNTS). For the parasympathetic pathway, the baroreflex dmNTS neurons project to the nucleus ambiguus, and the dorsal motor nucleus of vagus, and then to the heart. For the sympathetic pathway, the dmNTS neurons project to the caudal ventrolateral medulla, the rostral ventrolateral medulla, the sympathetic preganglionic neurons at the intermediolateral column of the spinal cord, and then to the peripheral vascular, renal, and cardiac sympathetic nerves. The post-dmNTS divergence provides ample opportunity for modulating influences from sleep and arousal to have separate effects on the baroreceptor parasympathetic and sympathetic reflexes; however, our partial-correlations analysis indicated that the effects were, in fact, quite similar (see discussion in Ref. 4 for details and references). In the present study, we extend the NMB rat preparation to enable direct measurement of the magnitude of single-pulse ADN stimulation-elicited evoked responses (ERs) from second- or higher-order baroreflex neurons in the dmNTS. Sleep and arousal are defined as epochs of high-amplitude low-frequency and low-amplitude high-frequency EEG, respectively. Using these definitions, we found clear evidence that the ER magnitude was reliably augmented during sleep.

METHODS

Twelve female Sprague-Dawley rats (230–270 g body wt) were studied one at a time, monitored continuously, and attended around the clock. These same rats were used in our previous studies (17, 18). All acute surgery or any possible irritating manipulation, such as replacement of the bladder cannula or adjustment of the tracheal cannula, was done with accurately controlled and carefully monitored >1.5% deep isoflurane anesthesia. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Care and Use Committee of the Pennsylvania State University College of Medicine and comply with the American Physiological Society's “Guiding Principles for Research Involving Animals and Human Beings” and National Institutes of Health and US Department of Agriculture regulations.

General information about the preparation.

The surgical details are described elsewhere (17); briefly, NMB was induced on the first day by injection of α-cobrotoxin (100 μg iv) and maintained by continuous infusion (250 μg/day iv). Mechanical ventilation was at 72 breaths/min at inspiratory-to-expiratory ratio of 1:2, positive end-expiratory pressure of 1–5 cmH2O, minute volume of 180–220 ml, and hyperinflation (antiatelectasis) of 18 cmH2O (2 s/10 min). The EEG electrodes were two 0-80 screws inserted 2.0 mm from the midline in the left parietal bone (1.0 mm anterior to bregma and to lambda). A transurethral bladder cannula, subcutaneous ECG electrodes (no. 30 silver wire), and Teflon femoral artery and silicone vein cannulas were implanted. Core (vaginal) temperature was servoregulated at 37°C. The left ADN was dissected, set on an anodized Ta-Ta2O5 capacitance electrode (17), and imbedded in a silicone compound (Kwik-Sil, WPI, Sarasota, FL). This electrode arrangement enables thousands of stimulations without detectable damage to the ADN. The baroreflex area in the dmNTS was located using atlas coordinates, as well as the somatotopic features of the gracile nucleus, which at the search coordinates is 50–150 μm dorsal to the baroreflex cells of the dmNTS (17). For the multiunit dmNTS recording, 1- to 2-MΩ glass-insulated tungsten microelectrodes (Alpha-Omega, Alpharetta, GA) were mounted in a hydraulic probe drive (model 50-16-1, FHC), which was mounted in a digital stereotaxic carrier. Signals were amplified (gain = 20,000, bandpass = 0.3–3 kHz; XCELL-3X4, 40-#40-8B, FHC) and digitized at 10 kHz. A typical preparation was maintained for an average of 14 days, and the data were acquired on days 4–9. After healing of the surgical incisions and stabilization, which typically required 48–72 h, the isoflurane concentration was set at ≤0.5%, which is an analgesic level consistent with patterned sleep.

EEG sleep/arousal identification.

From the bandpass-filtered (0.1–100 Hz) EEG, two kinds of sleep-arousal cycles were identified (Fig. 1): from sleep to wakefulness (S-W) and from wakefulness to sleep (W-S). The S-W cycles were identified as approximately symmetrical periods of high-amplitude low-frequency EEG activity followed by low-amplitude high-frequency activity; correspondingly, the W-S cycles were symmetrical periods of low-amplitude high-frequency EEG activity followed by high-amplitude low-frequency activity. Because of the NMB, EMG or electrooculogram criteria could not be used to distinguish arousal from rapid eye movement (REM) sleep; thus, to ensure accurate identification of sleep-arousal cycles, we imposed several additional constraints. 1) Cycle start points were set ≥2 min after an EEG transition was complete, and cycle stop points were set ≥2 min before the next transition. To eliminate the need to interpret ambiguous midcycle EEG transitions (dashed oval in Fig. 1), we used ERs from only the first 25% of the cycle after the cycle start point and the last 25% before the cycle stop point (i.e., Q1 and Q4 in Fig. A1). 2) For a cycle to be valid, it must not have had any abrupt (>25%) change in BP or HP within the cycle, except at the midcycle transition. 3) Complete cycles had durations of >10 and <40 min (see details in the appendix). Figure 1 shows a typical recording of EEG, HP, arterial BP, and venous BP from an individual rat. Six sleep-wakefulness sequences, three S-W and three W-S, are identified in this example record.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

A typical recording from an individual rat of EEG, heart period (HP), femoral arterial blood pressure (ABP), and venous blood pressure (VBP). Three sleep-to-wakefulness (S-W) and wakefulness-to-sleep (W-S) cycles are identified as approximately symmetrical periods of high-amplitude low-frequency EEG activity followed by low-amplitude high-frequency activity (S-W) and, correspondingly, symmetrical periods of low-amplitude high-frequency EEG activity followed by high-amplitude low-frequency activity (W-S). Start and end points of each cycle are defined at 120 s after and before the visually apparent start and end points of the cycle. Dashed oval designates a typical midcycle EEG transition (cf. Fig. A1).

Average EEG power.

Average EEG power is the root mean square (RMS) of the bandpass-filtered (0.1–100 Hz) EEG signal.

Magnitude of the A-fiber ERs.

Figure 2 shows a stimulus-triggered ensemble-averaged A-fiber ER trace from a dmNTS recording site. Using electrophysiology, pharmacology, and histology, we verified that the recordings were from second- or higher-order dmNTS baroreflex neurons, and not from ADN fibers in the solitary tract (17, 18). The A-fiber ER is the activity in the poststimulus latency range of 4–20 ms. Because the Pythagorean distance between the dmNTS recording and ADN stimulation sites is >11.5 mm and only A-fibers conduct at >2 m/s (5), it is virtually certain that any activity arriving (at the dmNTS) within 20 ms is propagated in A-fibers (17).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Typical example of a stimulus-triggered ensemble-averaged (n = 179) A-fiber evoked response (ER) trace from a dorsal medial nucleus of the solitary tract (dmNTS) recording site. Aortic depressor nerve (ADN) was stimulated with single pulses (300 μs, interpulse interval = 6 ± 1 s) at 10 μA. Pythagorean distance between dmNTS recording and ADN stimulation sites is >11.5 mm; because only A-fibers conduct at >2 m/s (5), it is virtually certain that any activity at the dmNTS electrode <20 ms poststimulus was propagated in A-fibers. For additional details see Ref. 17.

The magnitude of A-fiber ERs was determined [from a stimulus-triggered, at ts (ts = 0 in Fig. 2), ensemble-averaged ER trace] as follows. After removing the signal mean and rectifying, we integrated the signal in the interval of (ts + 4) to (ts + 20) ms and in the corresponding interval symmetrically reflected around the trigger [(ts − 20) to (ts − 4) ms, i.e., the baseline]. The A-fiber ER magnitude (MagER) on a trial was defined as the difference between the ER area and the corresponding baseline area

MagER=ts+4ts+20|Signal|ts20ts4|Signal|

Experimental protocol.

Using a transduction curve analysis (17), we characterized the relationship of the ADN stimulus amplitude to the magnitude and latency of the ER complex and chose a test amplitude near the center of the linear range of the amplitude-magnitude curve. To evaluate the effects of sleep/arousal on the magnitude of A-fiber ERs, we stimulated the ADN with single-current pulses (300 μs, pseudorandom interpulse interval = 6 ± 1 s) at the test amplitude and recorded the A-fiber ERs at a constant location in the dmNTS.

Data acquisition and analysis.

Data were acquired using Spike2 software and Power 1401 hardware (Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK). For EEG and ER results, to pool results across sleep-arousal cycles and rats, the data are represented as percentages of the cycle mean activity. Each cycle was divided into four quarters, the magnitudes of ensemble-averaged A-fiber ERs and RMS powers of the EEG were calculated for each quarter, the mean of the four quarters in the cycle was determined, and the ratios of the measurement from each quarter to the mean for the four quarters were calculated for each variable and multiplied by 100 (see appendix). The average value of each cardiovascular variable, i.e., systolic blood pressure (sBP), diastolic blood pressure (dBP), and HP, was determined for each quarter cycle. Data were pooled across all rats, and means and SE were calculated for each variable.

Statistical analysis.

Two-tailed Student's t-tests were used for statistical analysis (SAS, version 9.13).

RESULTS

Twelve rats were analyzed; one rat was excluded because of insufficient data; 1,017 sleep cycles (505 S-W and 512 W-S) were identified in the 11 rats. The average number of cycles (mean ± SE) per rat was 46 ± 5 for S-W and 47 ± 6 for W-S, and average duration was 27 ± 1.73 and 24 ± 1.23 min for S-W and W-S cycles, respectively. We found no effect of cycle sequence order, i.e., S-W vs. W-S, on the sleep/arousal effects on any cardiovascular variables or on the magnitude of the A-fiber ERs (Table 1); thus we combined the S-W and W-S data. Over the 1,017 cycles, the EEG power was higher [ΔEEG = 31%, t = 93.66, P < 0.001, degrees of freedom (df) = 1,016], the magnitude of single-pulse ADN stimulation-elicited A-fiber dmNTS ERs was larger (ΔER = 11%, t = 9.47, P < 0.001, df = 1,016), sBP and dBP were lower [ΔsBP = −14 mmHg (t = −33.67, P < 0.001, df = 1,016) and ΔdBP = −10 mmHg (t = −28.1, P < 0.001, df = 1,016)], and HP was longer [ΔHP = 9 ms (t = 14.4, P < 0.0001, df = 1,013); Fig. 3] during sleep (high-amplitude low-frequency EEG) than during arousal (low-amplitude high-frequency EEG).

Table 1.

The cycle sequences of s-w and w-s have no influence on sleep/arousal effects on the magnitude of dmNTS A-fiber ERs and other measured cardiovascular variables

High-Amplitude Low-Frequency EEG Periods (Sleep)
Low-Amplitude High-Frequency EEG Periods (Arousal)
ERs, % sBP, mmHg dBP, mmHg HP, ms ERs, % sBP, mmHg dBP, mmHg HP, ms
S-W 106 ± 0.93 131 ± 0.74 80 ± 0.7 164 ± 1 95 ± 0.94 143 ± 0.8 89 ± 0.83 155 ± 1
W-S 105 ± 1.02 129 ± 0.89 78 ± 0.85 162 ± 1 93 ± 1.04 144 ± 0.78 90 ± 0.76 154 ± 1
t-test (S-W vs. W-S)
    t 0.87 1.63 1.2 1.02 1.73 1.09 0.73 0.54
    df 1,015 1,015 1,015 1,001 1,015 1,015 1,015 1,010
    P NS NS NS NS NS NS NS NS

Values are means ± SE (n = 11 rats). ERs, (ADN A-fiber dmNTS) evoked responses; sBP, systolic blood pressure; dBP, diastolic blood pressure; HP, heart period; df, degrees of freedom; NS, not significant. The magnitude of A-fiber ERs during a quarter period of sleep or arousal is percentage of the mean magnitude of A-fiber ERs for the 4 quarters in that sleep-wakefulness (S-W) or wakefulness-sleep (W-S) cycle (see detailed calculation in appendix).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

Summary of sleep-wakefulness differences in EEG activity and A-fiber dmNTS ERs (see appendix for procedure used to calculate percentages; A), systolic and diastolic blood pressure (sBP and dBP; B), and HP (C) for the 11 rats. Magnitude of EEG activity and ERs were higher, sBP and dBP were lower, and HP was longer during sleep than during wakefulness. Values are means ± SE. *P ≤ 0.0001.

Table 2 summarizes the sleep/arousal effects on sBP, dBP, HP, and A-fiber dmNTS ERs for all 11 rats shown in Fig. 3. In nine of the rats (i.e., rats 1–9 in Table 2), taken individually, the magnitude of A-fiber dmNTS ERs was reliably larger during the high-amplitude low-frequency EEG (sleep) periods than during the low-amplitude high-frequency EEG (arousal) periods; in two rats (i.e., rats 10 and 11 in Table 2), the A-fiber ERs did not change significantly.

Table 2.

Sleep/arousal effects on A-fiber ERs in the dmNTS

EEG, %
sBP, mmHg
dBP, mmHg
HP, ms
ER
Rat No. Cycles Cycle Length, min Sleep Wake Sleep Wake Sleep Wake Sleep Wake Sleep, % Wake, % P (S vs. W)
1 79 24 114 85 135 147 75 85 167 161 106 94 <0.0005
2 74 26 113 87 110 126 63 74 182 167 106 91 <0.0001
3 55 20 119 82 136 145 91 97 150 144 110 93 <0.005
4 137 25 125 77 127 139 82 92 148 142 104 96 <0.005
5 93 35 120 80 136 156 80 95 159 148 108 93 <0.001
6 68 26 118 85 140 158 80 97 143 138 114 89 <0.0005
7 96 20 117 82 127 150 75 95 154 144 104 94 <0.001
8 84 30 121 81 134 135 76 77 178 170 108 89 <0.0001
9 161 31 121 80 122 134 77 85 173 163 106 94 <0.0005
10 54 24 115 85 134 144 84 92 153 146 99 102 NS
11 116 25 118 79 137 153 85 94 181 170 99 102 NS
Mean 92 26 118 82* 131 144* 79 89* 163 154* 106 94*
SE 10.15 1.36 1.04 0.93 2.62 3.01 2.17 2.42 4.28 3.71 1.33 1.32
S vs. W
    t 19.13 −7.63 −6.39 9.52 4.42
    P <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 0.0001

Individually, compared with arousal, the magnitude of A-fiber dmNTS ERs was significantly increased during sleep in rats 1–9 and decreased (not significantly) in rats 10 and 11. Over all 11 rats, sleep and arousal had significant effects on every variable.

*

P ≤ 0.0001.

Figure 4 depicts the effects of sleep/arousal on baroreflex gain for HP, baroreflex gain for BP, and magnitude of A-fiber dmNTS ERs. In Fig. 4, A and B, each point represents the RMS amplitude of 1 min of EEGδ activity and the relative ADN-to-HP and ADN-to-BP open-loop gain ratios during the same 1-min period. The open-loop gain ratio was calculated as the sBP or HP change from baseline during the ADN stimulus divided by the mean change for all responses to that kind and amplitude of stimulus, e.g., 35 impulses/s at 20 μA. Variables were converted to “z-scores” by subtracting the individual rat's mean from each measure and dividing by the corresponding standard deviation (4). Each point in Fig. 4C represents the EEG activity of each quarter period and the magnitude of A-fiber dmNTS ERs of that same quarter period, and all sleep and arousal periods, as well as the midcycle transitional quarter periods, are included. Figure 4 shows that HP baroreflex gain increased with sleep (A; r = 0.315, P < 0.0001, df = 424), the uncorrected BP baroreflex gain was independent of sleep/arousal (B; r = 0.001, P = not significant, df = 424), and, as predicted in an earlier study (4), the magnitude of A-fiber dmNTS ERs was increased with sleep (C; r = 0.2, P < 0.0001, df = 4,067).

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

Effect of sleep/wakefulness on relative baroreflex gain for HP (A), “uncorrected” relative baroreflex gain for blood pressure (B; see Ref. 4), and single-pulse ERs in the dmNTS (C). Each point in A and B represents root-mean-squared amplitude of 1-min δ-band of the EEG (EEGδ) and the averaged evoked cardiovascular responses to a standard stimulus test train during the same 1-min period: in A, r = 0.315, degrees of freedom (df) = 424, P < 0.0001; in B, gray dashed line shows slope of the partial correlations-corrected relationship between EEGδ and blood pressure gain (r = 0.26, df = 424, P < 0.0005), and black solid line shows slope of uncorrected relationship between EEGδ and blood pressure gain (r = 0.001, df = 424, P = not significant). Each point in C represents magnitude of EEG activity for each quarter period of a sleep-arousal cycle and ER magnitude of the same quarter period in the cycle (r = 0.2, df = 3,387, P < 0.0001). A is from Fig. 7 of Dworkin and Dworkin (4). B is modified (with gray dashed line to indicate difference in relationship between EEGδ and blood pressure gain before and after partial-correlations correction) from Fig. 7 of Dworkin and Dworkin (4): after partial-correlations correction for blood pressure baseline, the relationship between blood pressure gain and “EEG slow-wave activity” was 0.26, which is comparable to the correlation of 0.20 between EEG and the nucleus of the solitary tract neural ER in the present study.

DISCUSSION

Studies of modulation by sleep and arousal of baroreflex sensitivity across different species and/or experimental methodologies (10, 19, 21) do not agree. To understand why, it is necessary to know where and how the modulation occurs. In the present study, we have, for the first time, directly measured the effects of naturally occurring cycles of sleep and arousal on the open-loop baroreflex sensitivity at the dmNTS and found clear evidence that sensitivity increases during slow-wave sleep.

Slow-wave sleep is distinguishable from wakefulness by EEG criteria; however, distinguishing wakefulness from desynchronized REM sleep is more problematic. Conventionally, EMG and/or electrooculogram signals are used to help distinguish the states, but paralysis by NMB precluded those criteria; thus, to minimize possible misidentification, we restricted sampling to only stable, nontransitional periods of sleep and wakefulness. It is known that, in the cat, the end of a desynchronized episode in one cycle through the end of a desynchronized episode in the subsequent sleep cycle includes a regular progression of component states: arousal, slow-wave sleep, transition state, and desynchronized sleep (8). A similar pattern, including slow-wave sleep, REM sleep, and arousal states, is seen in freely moving rats (15). Similarly, over days or weeks, NMB rats have stable regular alternating sleep-arousal patterns, within which we were able to identify clear S-W and W-S sequences. To control for possible sequence order effects, we first analyzed the S-W and W-S sequences separately and, finding no reliable difference, pooled the S-W and W-S data. Irrespective of absolute cycle length, each sequence was divided into quarters: the first and last quarters were the S or W phases used in the analyses, and the two midcycle quarters were designated transitional, and, except for Fig. 4C, the transitional quarters were excluded from the analysis. Although our criteria possibly might be seen as too stringent, they offered, in exchange for a loss of half of the data, a high degree of confidence that the identification of the states was as objective as possible.

Because the baroreflex is a closed loop, the overall loop gain, which is the decrease in BP per unit increase in receptor pressure (mmHg/mmHg), cannot be directly measured without opening the loop and isolating the receptors. An open loop requires surgical interventions, which are technically difficult and, in humans, ethically unacceptable. Instead, most studies have estimated relative baroreflex gain from HR, either by manipulating receptor pressure and measuring corresponding changes in rate (12, 13) or by statistically analyzing the sequential relationship of natural pressure and rate fluctuations (11). Both of these methods assume that the cardiac rate response is representative of, and affected in parallel with, the overall baroreflex depressor response; however, some studies suggest that this assumption may not be correct (20).

Previously (4), using NMB rats, with ADN electrodes, but without dmNTS brain stem recordings, we found that the HP gain was greater during periods of greater EEGδ (Fig. 4A) but that the BP gain was unaffected by the EEGδ (Fig. 4B); this result appeared to show differential effects of sleep on the two efferent baroreflex mechanisms. A true differential effect would imply that HP gain was not a good surrogate for BP gain. Although the raw variables gave different results, sleep also lowers the HR and BP baseline, and a partial-correlations analysis showed that the apparent difference could be explained by a dependence of the BP gain, but not the HP gain, on the corresponding baseline level (4). The difference between the HP and BP gains was an artifact of measurement, and not a physiologically relevant effect: the lowered baseline BP is closer to the hydrostatic “floor,” and responses at lowered baseline pressure have a higher probability of being “clipped” or limited by the floor. In a previous study (4), after partial-correlations correction for BP baseline, the relationship between BP gain and “EEG slow-wave activity” was 0.26 (Fig. 7 in Ref. 4), which is comparable to the correlation between EEG and the nucleus of the solitary tract (NTS) neural ER of 0.20 in the present study. The partial-correlations analysis thus showed that the HP and BP gains were affected approximately in parallel.

If the underlying effect of sleep on HP and BP gain is similar, this would be consistent with a common site of modulation, and, indeed, other physiological and neurophysiological evidence suggests that the dmNTS is the common site. Hypothalamic defense area stimuli inhibit carotid- or ADN-elicited baroreflex bradycardia and depressor responses (2, 6, 7), and dmNTS cells with excitatory inputs from the baroreceptor sinus nerve and inhibitory inputs from the hypothalamic defense area are hyperpolarized by hypothalamic defense area stimulation in a way that shunts sinus nerve-evoked excitatory postsynaptic potentials (9). These data led us to hypothesize that arousal attenuates baroafferent transmission through the dmNTS (4).

To examine that hypothesis, we added dmNTS recordings to the cardiovascular and EEG measures and found that natural sleep modulates transmission of baroreceptor activity in the dmNTS. As predicted, the magnitude of A-fiber ERs from the second- or higher-order dmNTS baroreflex neurons, elicited by single-pulse ADN A-fiber stimulation, was reliably augmented during high-amplitude low-frequency EEG activity and reduced during low-amplitude high-frequency EEG activity (Fig. 4C). We have thus confirmed the conjecture of the partial-correlations analysis (4) by direct physiological measurement.

Perspectives

Doba and Reis (3) found that lesions in the NTS produced “fulminating hypertension,” which was “abolished by midcollicular decerebration.” Reis (14) initially thought that he had found an important locus of integration of excitatory and inhibitory influences on BP, but as evidence of a more extensive and anatomically distributed baroreflex began to emerge, his supposition appeared to be wrong. With time, the NTS came to be regarded by most physiologists as a “relay,” rather than an important integrative center. However, in concordance with studies in acute and slice preparations, our recent report in NMB rats of long-term potentiation of the baroreflex input and, now, of sleep/wakefulness dependence of ADN-elicited ERs in the NTS affirms that there is functionally important modulation of the baroreceptor input at or before the NTS.

GRANTS

The studies were supported by National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute Division of Heart and Vascular Diseases Grant HL-40837 (to B. R. Dworkin).

APPENDIX

Identification and analysis of sleep cycles.

Because we did not find a reliable automated procedure for paralyzed rats, individual sleep cycles were identified by manual scanning of the EEG record. Our specific goal was limited to comparison of sleep and wakefulness; thus, by rigorously defining a procedure for parsing the cycles into sleep and wakefulness periods, we sought to eliminate systematic bias that could influence the results. When a putative EEG sleep cycle was identified, to minimize ambiguity at the transitions, the start and end points, designated as t0 and tend, were set at 120 s later and earlier than the visually apparent start and end points of the cycle; i.e., the first and last 2 min of data were discarded (Fig. A1). Sleep cycles have variable durations (Fig. 1), and the cycle duration was defined as (tendt0). Somewhere in the midsection of a cycle, there is a transition from sleep to wakefulness or wakefulness to sleep, but the precise point of transition is often not distinct; thus, to eliminate the need for a judgment, we divided the cycle into quarters (i.e., Q1–Q4), discarded the data from Q2 and Q3, and took the data for analysis only from Q1 and Q4. Thus, given that the duration of a quarter cycle was (tendt0)/4, the data were taken from the intervals [t0] to [t0 + (tendt0)/4] and [tend − (tendt0)/4] to [tend]. The corresponding EEG activity was calculated as follows

graphic file with name zh600510-7153-m02.jpg

and

graphic file with name zh600510-7153-m03.jpg

where VEEG is the instantaneous EEG amplitude.

Fig. A1.

Fig. A1.

Identification of cycles and isolation of sleep and wakefulness stages for analysis. S-W cycles are defined by approximately symmetrical periods of high-amplitude low-frequency EEG activity followed by low-amplitude high-frequency activity; W-S cycles are symmetrical low-amplitude high-frequency EEG activity followed by high-amplitude low-frequency activity. Typical S-W cycle shows defined components of sleep and wakefulness stage. Transitions are often ambiguous; thus, to minimize errors of judgment, the start and end points, designated t0 and tend, were truncated at 120 s after and 120 s before the visually apparent start and end points of a cycle (dashed vertical lines). Truncated cycle was divided into 4 equal time intervals (Q1–Q4). Visual identification of a cycle depends on observing a transition. Thus, somewhere in Q2 or Q3, a transition occurs from sleep to wakefulness or from wakefulness to sleep; however, it is sometimes difficult to designate exactly where this transition occurs. Thus, to eliminate errors of judgment and make the data selection as objective as possible, data from Q2 and Q3 were discarded and only data from Q1 and Q4 were used for analysis (Fig. 4 was an exception: because identification of the midcycle transition was unnecessary, all 4 quarters were used). In the example, Q1 is a sleep stage (S), and Q4 is wakefulness (W). Qn represents the nth quarter. S-W is the visually identified sleep-wakefulness cycle.

To enable pooling of EEG data across cycles and rats, the EEG activity for each quarter was converted to a percentage using the mean of the four quarters as the base

EEGQn%=(EEGQni=14EEGQi4)×100%(n=1, 2, 3, and 4)

Thus, EEGQn% is a measure of the relative magnitude of EEG activity during Qn compared with the average EEG magnitude of the four quarters and is a random variable that is >100% when the magnitude of EEG activity during Qn is greater than the average EEG magnitude of the four quarters and <100% when the magnitude of EEG activity during Qn is less than the average EEG magnitude of the four quarters.

Analysis of ERs.

Similar to the EEG analysis, to pool ER data across cycles and rats, we converted the magnitude of ER for each quarter to a percentage using the mean of the 4 quarters as the base

MagERQn%=(MagERQni=14MagERQi4)×100%(n=1, 2, 3, and 4)

Thus MagERQn% is a measure of the relative magnitude of ER activity during Qn compared with the average ER magnitude of the four quarters and is >100% when the magnitude of ER during Qn is greater than the average ER magnitude of the four quarters and <100% when the magnitude of ER during Qn is less than the average ER magnitude of the four quarters.

The only exception to restricting the analysis to Q1 and Q4 was for the correlational analysis in Fig. 4, which compares the neural ERs of the present study with the cardiovascular ERs in an earlier study (4). Because in the earlier study we did not classify the sleep state, using all four quarters for Fig. 4 better aligned the ER with the cardiovascular data.

REFERENCES

  • 1. Combs CA, Smith OA, Astley CA, Feigl EO. Differential effect of behavior on cardiac and vasomotor baroreflex responses. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 251: R126–R136, 1986 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Coote JH, Hilton SM, Perez-Gonzalez JF. Inhibition of the baroreceptor reflex on stimulation in the brain stem defence centre. J Physiol 288: 549–560, 1979 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Doba N, Reis DJ. Acute fulminating neurogenic hypertension produced by brainstem lesions in the rat. Circ Res 32: 584–593, 1973 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Dworkin BR, Dworkin S. Baroreflexes of the rat. III. Open-loop gain and electroencephalographic arousal. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 286: R597–R605, 2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Fan W, Andresen MC. Differential frequency-dependent reflex integration of myelinated and nonmyelinated rat aortic baroreceptors. Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol 275: H632–H640, 1998 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Hilton SM. Inhibition of baroreceptor reflexes on hypothalamic stimulation. J Physiol 165: 56P–57P, 1962 [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Hilton SM. A search for brain stem cell groups integrating the defence reaction in the rat. J Auton Nerv Syst 19: 127–136, 1987 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Lydic R, McCarley RW, Hobson JA. Forced activity alters sleep cycle periodicity and dorsal raphe discharge rhythm. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 247: R135–R145, 1984 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Mifflin SW, Spyer KM, Withingtonwray DJ. Baroreceptor inputs to the nucleus tractus solitarius in the cat: modulation by the hypothalamus. J Physiol 399: 369–387, 1988 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Oosting J, Struijker-Boudier HA, Janssen BJ. Validation of a continuous baroreceptor reflex sensitivity index calculated from spontaneous fluctuations of blood pressure and pulse interval in rats. J Hypertens 15: 391–399, 1997 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Parati G, Di Rienzo M, Bertinieri G, Pomidossi G, Casadei R, Groppelli A, Pedotti A, Zanchetti A, Mancia G. Evaluation of the baroreceptor-heart rate reflex by 24-hour intra-arterial blood pressure monitoring in humans. Hypertension 12: 214–222, 1988 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Pickering GW, Sleight P, Smyth HS. The reflex regulation of arterial pressure during sleep in man. J Physiol 194: 46P–48P, 1968 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Pickering GW, Sleight P, Smyth HS. The relation of arterial pressure to sleep and arousal in man. J Physiol 191: 76P–78P, 1967 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Reis D. Conversation About the Role of NTS in Blood Pressure Regulation With B, Dworkin B. New York: Cornell University College of Medicine, 1974 [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Roldan E, Weiss T. Comparative study of sleep cycles in rodents. Experientia 20: 280–281, 1964 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Smyth HS, Sleight P, Pickering GW. Reflex regulation of arterial pressure during sleep in man. A quantitative method of assessing baroreflex sensitivity. Circ Res 24: 109–121, 1969 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Tang X, Dworkin BR. Baroreflexes of the rat. IV. ADN-evoked responses at the NTS. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 293: R2243–R2253, 2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Tang X, Dworkin BR. Baroreflexes of the rat. V. Tetanus-induced potentiation of ADN A-fiber responses at the NTS. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 293: R2254–R2259, 2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Vandeborne P, Nguyen H, Biston P, Linkowski P, Degaute JP. Effects of wake and sleep stages on the 24-h autonomic control of blood pressure and heart rate in recumbent men. Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol 266: H548–H554, 1994 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Vatner SF, Franklin D, Braunwald E. Effects of anesthesia and sleep on circulatory response to carotid sinus nerve stimulation. Am J Physiol 220: 1249–1255, 1971 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Zoccoli G, Andreoli E, Bojic T, Cianci T, Franzini C, Predieri S, Lenzi P. Central and baroreflex control of heart rate during the wake-sleep cycle in rat. Sleep 24: 753–758, 2001 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from American Journal of Physiology - Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology are provided here courtesy of American Physiological Society

RESOURCES