Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Biol Psychiatry. 2010 Feb 25;67(11):1026–1032. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.01.002

Maternal Prenatal Cortisol and Infant Cognitive Development: Moderation by Infant-Mother Attachment

Kristin Bergman 1, Pampa Sarkar 1, Vivette Glover 1, Thomas G O'Connor 2
PMCID: PMC2872196  NIHMSID: NIHMS171871  PMID: 20188350

Abstract

Background:

Experimental animal studies suggest that early glucocorticoid exposure may have lasting effects on the neurodevelopment of the offspring; animal studies also suggest that this effect may be eliminated by positive postnatal rearing. The relevance of these findings to humans is not known.

Methods:

We prospectively followed 125 mothers and their normally developing children from pregnancy through 17 months postnatal. Amniotic fluid was obtained at, on average, 17.2 weeks gestation; infants were assessed at an average age of 17 months with the Bayley Scales of Infant Development and ratings of infant-mother attachment classification were made from the standard Ainsworth Strange Situation assessment.

Results:

Prenatal cortisol exposure, indexed by amniotic fluid levels, negatively predicted cognitive ability in the infant, independent of prenatal, obstetric, and socioeconomic factors. This association was moderated by child–mother attachment: in children with an insecure attachment the correlation was (r(54) = −.47, p <.001); in contrast, the association was nonexistent in children who had a secure attachment (r(70) = −.05, ns).

Conclusions:

These findings mimic experimental animal findings and provide the first direct human evidence that increased cortisol in utero is associated with impaired cognitive development, and that its impact is dependent on the quality of the mother-infant relationship.

Keywords: cortisol, attachment, cognitive development, amniotic fluid, programming, prenatal stress


The fetal programming hypothesis proposes that the environment in utero can alter the development of the fetus, with a permanent effect on the phenotype. This concept has been particularly developed by Barker and his colleagues who have shown that the prenatal environment can have lasting effects on cardiovascular and metabolic functioning, with effects persisting into adulthood(1-2). Recent work has begun to examine the extent to which a programming model may also apply to behavioral, psychiatric, and neurodevelopmental outcomes in humans(3-5). The current study builds on this research by directly examining one proposed causal mechanism of a programming model, prenatal cortisol exposure, as a predictor of infant cognitive development; and, it seeks to translate animal findings in this area by investigating the moderating impact of early child rearing.

The hypothesis that there may be developmental programming effects for human bio-behavioral development derives from several sets of findings. The first is the accumulating evidence that prenatal anxiety or stress predicts cognitive, behavioral and psychiatric outcomes in the child, independent of postnatal stress or anxiety(6-12). Child outcomes that are most consistently increased by prenatal stress or prenatal anxiety (both concepts have been used) are signs of distress/anxiety, symptoms of ADHD, and reduced cognitive ability. These findings are strikingly consistent with experimental animal work, which uses the prenatal stress model as a leading paradigm for showing programming effects on offspring outcomes(13-17). A further feature of the animal data is that they provide good evidence that exposure to glucocorticoids such as cortisol is one mechanism for these effects. For example, in non-human primates, prenatal stress effects can be mimicked by injecting adrenocorticotropic hormone, which stimulates the production of cortisol, to the pregnant mother(17). Animal studies also demonstrate long-term effects of prenatal administration of synthetic glucocorticoids, such as dexamethasone, on offspring brain development and behavior(18-19).

Whether or not a comparable glucocorticoid mechanism accounts for prenatal anxiety or stress effects on the child is not yet clear. There is, for example, human evidence that synthetic glucocorticoids that cross the placenta, including dexamethasone, can affect infant neurodevelopment when given in pregnancies threatened with preterm delivery(20). More broadly, the potentially widespread role for exposure to increased cortisol in human fetal brain development is indicated by a study showing, by microarray analysis, that increased cortisol exposure affects the expression of over a thousand genes in fetal brain cells(21). However, human studies have not yet shown that fetal cortisol exposure is directly associated with neurodevelopment in the child and is a mechanism accounting for the prenatal stress effect.

A separate line of investigation that provides indirect support for a human programming model involving early cortisol exposure is that which connects early stress with human psychological and psychiatric outcomes and HPA axis functioning(22-24). Although the impact of early cortisol exposure per se is unclear in these studies, the results suggest that the early rearing environment may alter HPA axis functioning, and also predict behavior and neurodevelopment. Other evidence(25-26) that links cortisol exposure and impaired cognitive functioning in adults underscores the need for further investigation of early glucocorticoid exposure on neurodevelopment.

The current study was designed to extend research on prenatal stress/anxiety and fetal programming in humans in two new directions. First, we test the hypothesis that prenatal cortisol exposure, as indexed by amniotic fluid cortisol, predicts infant cognitive development. Collecting amniotic fluid provided us with particular leverage for assessing if prenatal cortisol exposure was directly linked with child outcome and mediated the association between prenatal maternal anxiety/stress and children's cognitive development. The second novel feature is the inclusion of the leading index of infant-parent relationship quality as a potential moderator of prenatal cortisol exposure. Animal data consistently show that the early rearing environment can reverse the adverse effects of prenatal stress(27-28) and there is a growing evidence in the animal literature showing that early rearing can alter biological risk, whether the risk derived from experimentally induced prenatal stress or from genetic risk(29). The application of these animal findings to humans is not known. Here we investigate, for the first time, the association between amniotic fluid cortisol, infant cognitive development and any moderating influence of caregiving quality. We focus on child cognitive development, which has been reliably predicted from prenatal anxiety/stress(6-7); the availability of amniotic fluid cortisol allowed us to examine further a previously reported association between prenatal stress and cognitive development(6).

Methods

Participants

Mothers and babies were recruited as part of a prospective study on fetal hormone exposure and child development(30). Women were recruited sequentially from an amniocentesis clinic for kayotyping in a large urban maternity hospital between December, 2001 and January, 2005. Written informed consent was obtained from mothers; the study was approved by the institutional research ethics committee at Imperial College London.

Of the 365 women who were recruited at amniocentesis, 109 were excluded because of clinical findings, prematurity, non-routine amniocentesis, or unknown birth outcome. The remaining 256 English-speaking mothers with full-term (≥37 weeks), healthy and singleton infants for whom prenatal biological samples were obtained were invited to return to the pediatric clinic when the child was between 14 and 19 months old. Of these, we were unable to locate 71 and a further 60 did not wish to participate or could not attend the clinic (because of moving away from the area), resulting in 125 children who were eligible and agreed to participate. The sample on which we obtained longitudinal follow-up data (n=125) did not differ from those who were not followed up (n=131) on key parameters listed in Table 1; however, mothers on whom we had follow-up data were slightly older than those on whom we did not (Mean=36.6[SD=4.1] years compared with Mean=35.2[SD=5.4] years, respectively, p=.02).

Table 1.

Characteristics of the study sample (n=125)

Mean (SD)/range or number (%)
Maternal age (years) 36.62 (4.12)/25-45
Gestational age at amniocentesis (weeks) 17.30 (3.24)/15 – 37
Time of amniotic fluid collection (hours) 10.56 (1.09)/9.05 – 16.10
Maternal education
 No qualifications 4 (3.2%)
 GCSE or equivalent 14 (11.2%)
 A levels or equivalent 19 (15.2%)
 Diploma or equivalent 24 (19.2%)
 University degree 41 (32.8%)
 Postgraduate degree 23 (18.4%)
Parity
 Nulliparous 51 (40.8%)
 1 previous child 45 (36%)
 2 or more previous children 29 (23.2%)
Racial background
 White/Caucasian 103 (82.4%)
 Asian-Indian subcontinent 7 (5.6%)
 Afro-Caribbean 10 (8.0%)
 Middle-eastern 3 (2.4%)
 Far-eastern 2 (1.6%)
Smoking during pregnancy .32 (1.86)
 0 cigarettes/day 111 (88.8%)
 1-2 cigarettes/day 12 (9.6%)
 >2 cigarettes/day 2 (1.6%)
Alcohol during pregnancy .53 (1.53)
 0 units/week 87 (69.6%)
 1-2 units/week 33 (26.4%)
 >2 units/week 5 (4.0%)
State anxiety: prenatal 49.58 (13.47)/21 - 77
Gestational age at birth (weeks) 39.48 (1.17)/37-42
Birth weight (g) 3489 (475)/2338-6000
Female sex 65 (52%)
Child age at postnatal visit (months) 16.73 (1.41)/14.4-20.0)
BSID Mental Development Index 97.33 (10.20)/70-122
BSID Physical developmental index 97.23 (10.24)/70 - 121
Amniotic fluid cortisol (nmol/l) 18.12 (8.98)/.66 – 50.25
Secure attachment classification 70 (57%)

Note: GCSE is the approximate equivalent to high school diploma; A level indicates that the individual passed college entrance exams; diploma indicates a degree less than a university degree.

Mean gestational age at the time of amniotic fluid sampling was 17.2 weeks (median was 16 weeks; the range was 15-32, with 91% between 15-20 weeks). Gestational age was assessed to the nearest day by ultrasound-determined fetal biometry. Crown-rump-length(CRL) was used at and before 13 weeks and biparietal diameter(BPD) after 13 weeks to establish gestation using Hadlock's charts installed in the reporting software(31).

Amniotic fluid cortisol samples

During amniocentesis an aliquot of up to 4ml of amniotic fluid surplus to clinical requirement was drawn for the study and stored at −80°C until assay. Time of collection, to the nearest 15 minutes, was recorded. Total cortisol in amniotic fluid was assayed by radio-immunoassay (Coat-A-Count, DPC, Los Angeles,CA), cortisol having been extracted by dichloromethane and reconstituted prior to assay. Intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation for the amniotic fluid cortisol assay were 4.4% and 6.5% respectively. There was some variation in the time of day of assessment and gestational age of amniotic fluid collection; these factors are considered as covariates.

Maternal plasma cortisol

Maternal plasma was sampled at the same prenatal assessment; it is included for exploratory purposes. Maternal blood samples were collected immediately before the amniocentesis procedure, centrifuged, and supernatant plasma was stored at −80°C until batch assay. Total cortisol was assayed by radio-immunoassay using Coat-a-Count (DPC,Los Angeles,CA). Intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation for the cortisol assay were 5.4% and 4.1%, respectively.

Infant-mother attachment

Ainsworth's Strange Situation(32) is an extensively researched 7-episode laboratory assessment that capitalizes on the mild stress of the separation-reunion paradigm to assess the extent to which the child uses the parent as a secure base for exploration. Coding was made using established procedures for classifying dyads as Insecure-Avoidant, Secure, Insecure-Ambivalent/Resistant, and Insecure-disorganized(32-33). The Strange Situation is arguably the most extensively studied and valid index of parent-child relationship quality in infancy. Securely attached children experience significantly more sensitive and response parenting than those rated as having an Insecure pattern(34-35), and Secure attachment predicts optimal behavioral and social development. Ratings from the Strange Situation index current relationship quality, but the relative stability in attachment (in)Security(36) in infancy implies that it indexes the predominant pattern of caregiving quality in the child's early months of life. The primary coder received standard training at the Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota, and achieved > 80% agreement on a reliability test of 35 cases. Strange Situation data could not be obtained on one child, i.e., n=124.

Infant cognitive and physical development

A developmental researcher who was blind to antenatal data administered the Bayley Scales of Infant Development–Second Edition (BSID-II)(37), a widely-used standardized assessment of infant mental development that includes language and cognitive items (Mental Developmental Index, MDI) and physical development (Physical Developmental Index, PDI). The researcher underwent extensive training in administration and achieved an inter-rater reliability with an expert trainer of 90% for MDI and 97% for PDI. We report standardized scores which have a mean of 100 and SD of 15.

Psychosocial and obstetric covariates

Information on maternal age, parity, ethnicity (categorized according to UK National Health Service ethnic codes), smoking (cigarettes per day; because of a restricted range, this was re-coded to no smoking versus any smoking), alcohol (number of units per week) and prescription drug use during pregnancy (prescription drug categories: Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors; anti-hypertensive; anti-asthmatic; anti-epileptic; steroids; and other) was collected at recruitment. Information regarding birth outcomes and child sex was collected from the child's hospital notes. Standard deviation score of birth weight adjusted for gestational age and sex was calculated using software based upon 1990 British Growth Reference data.

Maternal anxiety and depressive symptoms and stress

Maternal mood and stress in the prenatal period were included as adjuncts to the prenatal cortisol exposure data. The Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)(38) was completed at the prenatal and postnatal visits. The STAI is a widely used index of anxiety symptoms and has considerable validity, reliability, and clinical utility; we report state anxiety because of our interest in change from the pre- to post-natal periods. The Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS)(39), a widely used index with considerable validity, was used to measure maternal depressive symptoms at the postnatal visit. Mothers also completed a 26-item Stressful Life Events Questionnaire (SLEQ), adapted from Barnett et al.(40), at the postnatal visit, and reported if the event occurred and, if it did, whether the event “affected me a little” or “affected me a lot.” Mothers reported if the event occurred antenatally or postnatally or both. We focus on the number of events because it is a more objective assessment of stress exposure (the findings are substantially identical for frequency and perceived impact of events).

Data analysis

We first present sample characteristics and background correlations between amniotic fluid cortisol, attachment, maternal mood/stress and Bayley scale scores. Second, we report bivariate associations between amniotic fluid cortisol and infant cognitive development, followed by a multivariate analysis in which we accounted for several sets of possible confounds. Prior analyses(30) indicated a small-moderate effect of gestational age and time of collection on amniotic fluid cortisol; these variables are therefore included in multivariate analyses. We also include, on an a priori basis, common obstetric and psychosocial covariates. Pre- and postnatal maternal mood and stress are also considered. Third, regression analyses are used to examine if amniotic fluid cortisol mediated the previously reported association between prenatal stress and cognitive development and whether the link between amniotic fluid cortisol and cognitive development is moderated by attachment security; we distinguish between Secure and Insecure attachment, but then also explore moderation in each of the sub-classifications of Insecure attachment. We followed the convention of transforming cortisol data using a ln transformation; this transformed variable is used for parametric analyses (in practice, the findings are substantively identical with transformed and non-transformed data). Also, although our primary focus is on cognitive development, we also report findings for physical development.

Results

Demographic data show that, as might be expected from a sample undergoing karyotyping, the sample included a sizable set of older pregnant women, but ranged widely; there is also a comparatively high representation of individuals with a university degree. Both maternal age and education are included as covariates. In several other key areas, the sample was diverse and within normal range in terms of socio-demographic indicators, attachment classification, and child cognitive developmental scaled scores (Table 1).

Preliminary analyses indicated that amniotic fluid cortisol was not significantly correlated with maternal self-report measures of pre- or postnatal anxiety and depressive symptoms or life stress (all r's < .10); also, there was no association between amniotic fluid cortisol and quality of attachment, indexed by the Secure (n=70; 2.83[.57]) versus Insecure designation (n=54; 2.79[.59]; F(1,122) =.14; ln values of amniotic fluid cortisol shown). Attachment security was, however, associated with cognitive ability according to an analysis of variance (ANOVA): children with Secure attachments scored higher (n=70; 99.31[9.28]) than children with an Insecure attachment (n=54; 94.94[10.87]), F(1,123)=5.82, p<.05. ANOVA also indicated that Securely attached children also had mothers who reported lower levels of postnatal state anxiety compared with children with an Insecure attachment (n=69, 29.99 [7.35] and n=53, 33.42[11.39] for Secure and Insecure groups, respectively; F(1,120)=4.20, p<.05). No other significant associations with attachment secured were found. Finally, the mental development index of the Bayley scales was significantly associated with postnatal state anxiety (r(123)= −.23, p<.05); postnatal state anxiety was therefore retained for regression analyses.

Prenatal cortisol exposure predicts infant cognitive development

Correlation analysis indicated a significant inverse association between amniotic fluid cortisol (ln transformed values) and standard scores from the Bayley cognitive development scale (r(125)= −0.25, p<0.01). We then undertook a multivariate regression analysis that included maternal education, maternal age, time of prenatal sample collection, gestational age at amniocentesis, child sex, alcohol and smoking in pregnancy, birth weight for gestational age (Model 1, Table 2); maternal anxiety and stress were also included. Results indicated that amniotic fluid cortisol remained an independent predictor of infant cognitive development; maternal education and prenatal stress were also significant predictors.

Table 2.

Prediction of Cognitive Development from Prenatal, Obstetric, and Postnatal data.

Model 1 Model 2
Predictor B (SE) β B (SE) β
Intercept 111.62(18.28) 125.12 (18.03)
Maternal age .02 (.22) .01 −.14 (.22) −.06
Maternal education 1.69 (.58) .23** 1.87 (.57) .26**
Child age at postnatal visit −.70 (.62) −.10 −.68 (.59) −.09
Child sex (1=F,2=M) −2.69 (1.63) −.13 −2.71 (1.56) −.13
Collection time of amniotic 1.11 (.73) .13 1.00 (.70) .12
fluid
Gestational age at amniotic .18 (.27) .06 .08 (.26) .02
clinic visit
Birthweight/gestational age −1.50 (.78) −.16 −1.52 (.75) −.17*
(ratio)
Alcohol in pregnancy −.33 (.57) −.05 −.17 (.55) −.03
Smoking in pregnancy 1.04 (1.51) .06 1.51 (1.46) .08
Amniotic fluid cortisol (ln) −5.84 (1.55) −.33*** −9.45 (2.15) −.53***
State anxiety: prenatal .00 (.06) .01 .01 (.06) −.02
State anxiety: postnatal −.08 (.10) −.07 −.04 (.09) −.04
SLE: prenatal −2.88 (.57)*** −.44 −2.60 (.55) −.40***
SLE: postnatal .48 (.43) .10 .55 (.41) .12
Secure attachment −15.26 (8.11) −.75
Amniotic fluid cortisol X 6.90 (2.85) .99*
Secure attachment

Note: SLE = number of stressful life events.

*

p < .05,

**

p <.01,

***

p < .001.

Further analyses indicated that prenatal stress and amniotic fluid cortisol were essentially separate predictors of child cognitive development. For example, the association between amniotic fluid cortisol and infant cognitive development was substantively unchanged when prenatal and postnatal maternal anxiety and stress measures were included (in a regression model the coefficient for amniotic fluid cortisol was B= −6.21[1.74] and B= −5.84[1.55], without and with anxiety and stress measures, respectively). That, along with the lack of association between amniotic fluid cortisol and prenatal stress noted above, indicates that there was no evidence that the prenatal stress effect on cognitive development was mediated by amniotic fluid cortisol.

Amniotic fluid cortisol was significantly correlated with the physical development index from the Bayley Scales (r(124)= −.25, p<.01), but regression analyses showed that it did not significantly predict physical development independent of obstetric and psychosocial covariates.

Prenatal cortisol prediction is moderated by infant-parent attachment

The moderating role of infant-parent attachment quality on the association between prenatal cortisol exposure and cognitive development is demonstrated in the regression (Model 2 in Table 2) and illustrated in Figure 1. The Figure shows that prenatal cortisol exposure, indexed by amniotic fluid concentration, strongly predicted cognitive development in children with an Insecure attachment history (r(54)= −.47, p<.001); in contrast, the association among children with a Secure attachment history was essentially zero (r(70)= −.05, ns). Model 2 in Table 2 demonstrates that the significant prenatal cortisol exposure X infant-parent attachment interaction is independent of obstetric and psychosocial covariates as well as pre- and postnatal anxiety and stress. The association between prenatal cortisol and infant cognitive development was comparable (i.e., not significantly different) for the different subtypes of Insecure attachment (Avoidant [r= −.27], Ambivalent [r=−.50], Disorganized [r= −.43]). We did not detect a significant amniotic fluid X attachment security interaction for the physical development index.

Figure 1. Association between Amniotic Fluid Cortisol and Mental Development According to Early Caregiving Quality.

Figure 1

The association between amniotic fluid cortisol (LN) and Mental Development Index (MDI) of the Bayley Scale of Infant Development is significant and moderate in those children with Insecure attachments (r(54) = −.47, p <.001), but negligible in children with Secure attachments (r(70) = −.05, ns).

Supplementary analyses

We conducted several sets of supplementary analyses to confirm the robust nature of the associations reported above and explore them further. First, although distribution of the amniotic fluid data did not suggest statistical outliers, we nonetheless examined if the interaction findings were unduly influenced by comparatively extreme scores. We re-analyzed the data excluding those individuals with comparatively low scores on amniotic fluid cortisol (i.e., the 4 values < 1 in the figure). With these scores excluded we still obtained a significant interaction between amniotic fluid cortisol and attachment security in predicting score on mental development index (p<.05), demonstrating the robust nature of the interaction. Second, the availability of maternal plasma cortisol allowed us to examine its role in the hypothesized cascade from prenatal anxiety/stress to child cognitive outcome. A key finding in this regard is that, although maternal prenatal plasma cortisol was associated with amniotic fluid cortisol (in the subsample with child outcome data, r(107)= .25, p<.01), it was not significantly associated with infant cognitive development (r(107)= −.13, ns). Third, the exploratory hypothesis that there may be developmental timing effects was tested in a regression analysis in which we added the interaction between gestational age at amniotic fluid assessment X amniotic fluid cortisol in predicting cognitive development after other main effects (Model 1 in Table 2). The regression coefficient (B 1.08, SE .57) has a significance value of p=.06, and may indicate that the association between amniotic fluid cortisol and child cognitive development was more negative earlier in gestation. Importantly, when the gestational age X amniotic fluid interaction was retained in the model, the amniotic fluid cortisol X infant-parent attachment interaction remained significant at p<.05.

Discussion

A sizable literature of experimental animal work on the programming effects of prenatal stress on the offspring is now being translated to human development. Among the findings so far reported, the association between prenatal anxiety or stress and child cognitive development is one of the most reliable and widely reported(6,41) An underlying hypothesis, based on experimental animal work, is that the effect of prenatal stress or anxiety on infant outcomes such as cognitive development derives from an HPA-mediated pathway. Specifically, elevated maternal prenatal stress or anxiety is thought to be related to an elevation in maternal prenatal cortisol, which crosses the placental barrier (nothwithstanding the role of 11 beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase 2 [11BHSD-2] in metabolizing maternal cortisol), to influence fetal brain development(42-43). There is limited support for this model in humans(44), but extensive animal work shows that prenatal exposure to raised glucocorticoids in rodents can damage the brain(45) and, in monkeys, administration of dexamethasone in late gestation caused a reduction in hippocampal volume(46).

The present study provides the first direct human evidence that cortisol level in utero predicts infant cognitive development, and that this effect is eliminated by a sensitive early rearing environment. However, although the prediction of infant cognitive development from prenatal cortisol exposure was significant and robust, it was essentially independent of multiple measures of maternal prenatal stress and anxiety. Accordingly, this research does not support cortisol as the mediating mechanism for the effects of prenatal stress on child cognitive development. Limitations of the study that may account for these non-findings (see below), but these results should encourage further investigation of alternative mediating mechanisms (e.g., cardiovascular system and catecholamines) to account for the link between prenatal stress and anxiety and child development.

The prediction of cognitive development from amniotic fluid cortisol may derive from several mechanisms. Understanding the mechanisms involved requires, in the first instance, a careful analysis of the causes of the variation in cortisol level in the amniotic fluid. Cortisol in the fetal circulation is a combination of that produced endogenously by the fetus and that derived from the mother and placenta. Cortisol in amniotic fluid reflects that excreted by the fetus – which may be genetically determined and a result of fetal stress – and includes transfer across fetal skin in early gestation, and fetal urine and lung liquid production from mid-gestation. We were unable to distinguish whether amniotic fluid cortisol reflects fetal exposure (e.g., cortisol of maternal or placental origin predicts lower cognitive ability, as in the animal models) or production (e.g., fetuses producing higher levels of cortisol have lower cognitive ability). Human research is limited in differentiating between these alternatives because experimental leverage is limited for ethical reasons. Nonetheless, our novel observation that the amniotic fluid cortisol effect was moderated by quality of caregiving is more straightforward in its interpretation and in its implications for further research.

The prediction from amniotic fluid cortisol was stronger than that from maternal plasma cortisol, despite the moderate degree of overlap(30). It is well-known that the barrier enzyme 11BHSD2 metablizes cortisol to protect fetal development, although it is equally clear that there are individual differences in that process. Previous research on this sample showed, for example, that the association between maternal cortisol and amniotic fluid cortisol was increased in more anxious mothers(47). An important methodological implication is that amniotic fluid cortisol is a more direct index of fetal exposure than is cortisol (or CRH or ACTH) measured from the pregnant mother – although it is clearly more of a challenge to obtain.

The hypothesis that a sensitive or optimal early rearing environment has causal influence on biological processes in the young offspring is well-established in animal work(29,48-49). Collectively, these and similar findings provide a basis for proposing that early life experiences – and caregiving is perhaps the most important type – have a lasting impact on the organism, and may constitute a model for non-genetic intergenerational transmission(50). These models and the underlying mechanisms may apply to humans, but little evidence has been reported, and much available data derives from atypical samples and settings(51). Our finding in a normal risk sample that sensitive postnatal rearing, as indicated by Secure infant-mother attachment, eliminates the effect of a biological risk exposure, indexed by amniotic fluid cortisol, is strikingly comparable to the animal work on prenatal stress and the broader animal literature on positive caregiving environment compensating for neurobiological vulnerability. These results provide a major translation and extension of the prenatal work in particular, and of the research into the biological impact of early rearing more generally. Equally important, they raise a fundamental conceptual-methodological point that research into biological mechanisms of risk and disorder for neuropsychiatric outcomes needs to attend to how early caregiving experiences may alter the unfolding of these processes. The effects of caregiving in moderating prenatal influence may vary with the nature of the prenatal risk. We previously demonstrated, in the case of fearfulness, that prenatal stress was exacerbated by an insecure/resistant attachment(52).

Limitations

One limitation is that amniotic fluid cortisol was assessed on a single occasion. That is an insurmountable limitation because repeat assessments of amniotic fluid for research purposes are not possible; children of mothers who undergo multiple samplings are at very high risk for poor obstetric outcome. Although there was some diurnal influence on amniotic fluid cortisol(30) the effect is modest and, in any event, including time of data collection had no effect on the results; gestational age was also adjusted for. The negative association between amniotic fluid cortisol and child cognitive development may not be causal. Additionally, we were not able to conduct postnatal visits on all eligible mothers on whom we had originally collected amniotic fluid data. It is also important to note that, of course, glucocorticoids have adaptive significance and should not be seen merely as an index of risk exposure. Further work is needed to identify the point at which cortisol levels are clinically elevated and may require clinical attention. Lastly, we note that our ability to test a component of the model may have been impaired because we included total maternal cortisol (from plasma) rather than free cortisol (we did not have an index of cortisol binding globulin).

Clinical application

These findings encourage further attention to the application of a programming model for public health and prevention of neuropsychiatric outcomes. Specifically, the findings suggest that early postnatal interventions may confer benefit to the child on both behavior and biology, and that some prenatal effects may be modifiable by infant-parent attachment in the postnatal period. Early interventions using attachment models are now wide-spread, and could be integrated with research on biological risk mechanisms. On the other hand, it may be too preliminary to judge the potential value of gathering amniotic fluid to infer potential risk for cognitive development; moreover, amniotic fluid alone would not be a reliable predictor of later cognitive development without a solid index of early caregiving quality.

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank Diana Adams for help with patient recruitment and the scoring of the questionnaires. The study was supported by a grant from the March of Dimes. Support for the research was also provided by NIH grant MH073019 and MH073842. TO'C and VG had full access to all of the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

The authors reported no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

References

  • 1.Barker DJ. In utero programming of chronic disease. Clin Sci (Lond) 1998;95:115–128. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Gluckman PD, Hanson M. The fetal Matrix. Cambridge University Press; New York, NY: 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Brown AS, Susser ES. Prenatal nutritional deficiency and risk of adult schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:1054–1063. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Rutter M, O'Connor TG. Are there biological programming effects for psychological development? Findings from a study of Romanian adoptees. Dev.Psychol. 2004;40:81–94. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.1.81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Khashan AS, et al. Higher risk of offspring schizophrenia following antenatal maternal exposure to severe adverse life events. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:146–152. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bergman K, Sarkar P, O'Connor TG, Modi N, Glover V. Maternal stress during pregnancy predicts cognitive ability and fearfulness in infancy. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;46:1454–1463. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e31814a62f6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Laplante DP, et al. Stress during pregnancy affects general intellectual and language functioning in human toddlers. Pediatr.Res. 2004;56:400–410. doi: 10.1203/01.PDR.0000136281.34035.44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Davis EP, et al. Prenatal exposure to maternal depression and cortisol influences infant temperament. J.Am.Acad.Child Adolesc.Psychiatry. 2007;46:737–746. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e318047b775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Huizink AC, Robles de Medina PG, Mulder EJ, Visser GH, Buitelaar JK. Stress during pregnancy is associated with developmental outcome in infancy. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44:810–818. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Van den Bergh BR, Marcoen A. High antenatal maternal anxiety is related to ADHD symptoms, externalizing problems, and anxiety in 8- and 9-year-olds. Child Dev. 2004;75:1085–1097. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00727.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.O'Connor TG, Heron J, Golding J, Glover V. Maternal antenatal anxiety and behavioural/emotional problems in children: a test of a programming hypothesis. J.Child Psychol.Psychiatry. 2003;44:1025–1036. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Talge NM, Neal C, Glover V. Antenatal maternal stress and long-term effects on child neurodevelopment: how and why? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48:245–261. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01714.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Coe CL, Lubach GR, Karaszewski JW, Ershler WB. Prenatal endocrine activation alters postnatal cellular immunity in infant monkeys. Brain Behav.Immun. 1996;10:221–234. doi: 10.1006/brbi.1996.0020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Coe CL, et al. Prenatal stress diminishes neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of juvenile rhesus monkeys. Biol.Psychiatry. 2003;54:1025–1034. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00698-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Maccari S, et al. Prenatal stress and long-term consequences: implications of glucocorticoid hormones. Neurosci.Biobehav.Rev. 2003;27:119–127. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(03)00014-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Weinstock M. Alterations induced by gestational stress in brain morphology and behaviour of the offspring. Prog.Neurobiol. 2001;65:427–451. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(01)00018-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Schneider ML, Coe CL, Lubach GR. Endocrine activation mimics the adverse effects of prenatal stress on the neuromotor development of the infant primate. Dev.Psychobiol. 1992;25:427–439. doi: 10.1002/dev.420250604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Setiawan E, Jackson MF, MacDonald JF, Matthews SG. Effects of repeated prenatal glucocorticoid exposure on long-term potentiation in the juvenile guinea-pig hippocampus. J Physiol. 2007;581:1033–1042. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.127381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Coe CL, Lubach GR. Developmental consequences of antenatal dexamethasone treatment in nonhuman primates. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29:227–235. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Sizonenko SV, et al. Impact of intrauterine growth restriction and glucocorticoids on brain development: insights using advanced magnetic resonance imaging. Mol Cell Endocrinol. 2006;254-255:163–171. doi: 10.1016/j.mce.2006.04.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Salaria S, et al. Microarray analysis of cultured human brain aggregates following cortisol exposure: implications for cellular functions relevant to mood disorders. Neurobiol Dis. 2006;23:630–636. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2006.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Heim C, et al. Childhood trauma and risk for chronic fatigue syndrome: association with neuroendocrine dysfunction. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:72–80. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Gunnar MR, Quevedo KM. Early care experiences and HPA axis regulation in children: a mechanism for later trauma vulnerability. Prog Brain Res. 2008;167:137–149. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)67010-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Tyrka AR, et al. Childhood parental loss and adult hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal function. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:1147–1154. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.01.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Newcomer JW, et al. Decreased memory performance in healthy humans induced by stress-level cortisol treatment. Arch.Gen.Psychiatry. 1999;56:527–533. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.6.527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Sapolsky RM. Why stress is bad for your brain. Science. 1996;273:749–750. doi: 10.1126/science.273.5276.749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Maccari S, et al. Adoption reverses the long-term impairment in glucocorticoid feedback induced by prenatal stress. J.Neurosci. 1995;15:110–116. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-01-00110.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Wakshlak A, Weinstock M. Neonatal handling reverses behavioral abnormalities induced in rats by prenatal stress. Physiol Behav. 1990;48:289–292. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(90)90315-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Suomi SJ. Risk, resilience, and gene x environment interactions in rhesus monkeys. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006;1094:52–62. doi: 10.1196/annals.1376.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Sarkar P, Bergman K, Fisk NM, O'Connor TG, Glover V. Ontogeny of foetal exposure to maternal cortisol using midtrimester amniotic fluid as a biomarker. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf) 2007;66:636–640. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2265.2007.02785.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Hadlock FP, et al. An evaluation of two methods for measuring fetal head and body circumferences. J Ultrasound Med. 1982;1:359–360. doi: 10.7863/jum.1982.1.9.359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ainsworth MDS, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ: 1978. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Main M, Solomon J. In: Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Strange Situation. Greenberg M, Cicchetti D, Cummings EM, editors. University of Chicago Press; Chicago: 1990. 1990. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.van Ijzendoorn MH, Schuengel C, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Disorganized attachment in early childhood: meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae. Dev.Psychopathol. 1999;11:225–249. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499002035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Belsky J, Rovine M, Taylor DG. The Pennsylvania Infant and Family Development Project, III: The origins of individual differences in infant-mother attachment: maternal and infant contributions. Child Dev. 1984;55:718–728. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Waters E. The reliability and stability of individual differences in infant-mother attachment. Child Development. 1978;49:483–494. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development. 2nd edition The Psychological Corporation; New York, NY: 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Spielberger CDGI, Lushene RE. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press; Palo Alto, CA: 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R. Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. Br.J Psychiatry. 1987;150:782–786. doi: 10.1192/bjp.150.6.782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Barnett BE, Hanna B, Parker G. Life event scales for obstetric groups. J Psychosom Res. 1983;27:313–320. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(83)90054-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Buitelaar JK, Huizink AC, Mulder EJ, de Medina PG, Visser GH. Prenatal stress and cognitive development and temperament in infants. Neurobiol.Aging. 2003;24(Suppl 1):S53–S60. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(03)00050-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Matthews SG. Antenatal glucocorticoids and programming of the developing CNS. Pediatr Res. 2000;47:291–300. doi: 10.1203/00006450-200003000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Challis JR, et al. The fetal placental hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, parturition and post natal health. Mol.Cell Endocrinol. 2001;185:135–144. doi: 10.1016/s0303-7207(01)00624-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Lewinn KZ, et al. Elevated maternal cortisol levels during pregnancy are associated with reduced childhood IQ. Int J Epidemiol. 2009 doi: 10.1093/ije/dyp200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Mitra R, Sapolsky RM. Acute corticosterone treatment is sufficient to induce anxiety and amygdaloid dendritic hypertrophy. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:5573–5578. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705615105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Uno H, et al. Neurotoxicity of glucocorticoids in the primate brain. Horm Behav. 1994;28:336–348. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1994.1030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Glover V, Bergman K, Sarkar P, O'Connor TG. Association between maternal and amniotic fluid cortisol is moderated by maternal anxiety. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:430–435. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Hofer MA. Hidden regulators in attachment, separation, and loss. Monogr Soc.Res.Child Dev. 1994;59:192–207. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Weaver IC, et al. Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nat.Neurosci. 2004;7:847–854. doi: 10.1038/nn1276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Francis D, Diorio J, Liu D, Meaney MJ. Nongenomic transmission across generations of maternal behavior and stress responses in the rat. Science. 1999;286:1155–1158. doi: 10.1126/science.286.5442.1155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Nelson CA, 3rd, et al. Cognitive recovery in socially deprived young children: the Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Science. 2007;318:1937–1940. doi: 10.1126/science.1143921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Bergman K, Sarkar P, Glover V, O'Connor TG. Quality of child-parent attachment moderates the impact of antenatal stress on child fearfulness. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49:1089–1098. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01987.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES