Abstract
We examined the presence of maximum information preservation, which may be a fundamental principle of information transmission in all sensory modalities, in the Drosophila antennal lobe using an experimentally grounded network model and physiological data. Recent studies have shown a nonlinear firing rate transformation between olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) and second-order projection neurons (PNs). As a result, PNs can use their dynamic range more uniformly than ORNs in response to a diverse set of odors. Although this firing rate transformation is thought to assist the decoder in discriminating between odors, there are no comprehensive, quantitatively supported studies examining this notion. Therefore, we quantitatively investigated the efficiency of this firing rate transformation from the viewpoint of information preservation by computing the mutual information between odor stimuli and PN responses in our network model. In the Drosophila olfactory system, all ORNs and PNs are divided into unique functional processing units called glomeruli. The nonlinear transformation between ORNs and PNs is formed by intraglomerular transformation and interglomerular interaction through local neurons (LNs). By exploring possible nonlinear transformations produced by these two factors in our network model, we found that mutual information is maximized when a weak ORN input is preferentially amplified within a glomerulus and the net LN input to each glomerulus is inhibitory. It is noteworthy that this is the very combination observed experimentally. Furthermore, the shape of the resultant nonlinear transformation is similar to that observed experimentally. These results imply that information related to odor stimuli is almost maximally preserved in the Drosophila olfactory circuit. We also discuss how intraglomerular transformation and interglomerular inhibition combine to maximize mutual information.
Introduction
How is sensory information received by sensory receptor cells transferred to higher brain regions? The data processing inequality of information theory states that any kind of information processing can only reduce the amount of information [1]. Sensory information is therefore gradually lost as it is passed to the next processing stage. However, for sensory information to be conveyed accurately to higher brain regions, as much information as possible should be preserved. Thus, it is conceivable that a principle common to all sensory modalities is ‘to maximally preserve the information’ [2]. Here, we investigated the presence and mechanisms of maximum information preservation in the olfactory system using a network model and physiological data of neural responses [3], [4].
We chose the Drosophila antennal lobe as a model circuit because it has many advantages for investigating information transformation within the circuit. First, it is organized into discrete compartments termed glomeruli as in the vertebrate olfactory bulb (Fig. 1 (A)) [5], [6]. All olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) expressing the same odorant receptor gene send their axons to the same glomerulus, where they synapse onto second-order projection neurons (PNs) [7], [8]. The dendrite of each PN is confined within a single glomerulus [9]–[11]. Local neurons (LNs) interconnect glomeruli and mediate both excitation and inhibition [6], [12]–[22]. This glomerular architecture simplifies physiological investigations of the circuit's connectivity. Second, there are only approximately 50 glomeruli in Drosophila [5] compared with approximately 1800 in mice. In each glomerulus, about 40 ORNs converge onto an average of three PNs [23]–[26]. Third, the responses of ORNs and PNs to various odors have been extensively analyzed [3], [4], [23], [24], [27]. These advantages enabled us to study information processing in the olfactory system on the basis of an olfactory network model that takes account of (1) the actual connectivity, (2) almost all neurons engaged in the olfactory processing, and (3) the response properties of ORNs and PNs to real odorants.
Importantly, because odor information in both the ORN and PN layers is represented by population activities of various types of ORNs and PNs [3], [4], [27], [28], the investigation of information processing in the olfactory system requires consideration of as many of the neurons that contribute to information processing as possible. However, quantitative assessment of information processing in large neuronal populations is difficult and few studies have examined large neural populations engaged in sensory information processing [29]. Here, we utilized the above three advantages to construct a network model that includes approximately half of all the neurons engaged in olfactory information processing and computed the amount of information contained in the entire neural population.
Recent investigations have shown that PNs are broadly tuned to odors, whereas ORNs are narrowly tuned [3], [30]. In ORNs, most odor responses cluster in the weak end of their dynamic range. In PNs, however, odor responses are distributed more uniformly throughout their dynamic range. This is a result of nonlinear transformation between ORN and PN responses. The nonlinearity amplifies weak ORN inputs greatly, but does not amplify strong ones as much. As PNs use their dynamic range more efficiently than ORNs, this transformation is thought to assist the decoder in discriminating between different odors. However, it is also expected that the neural variability of PN responses will increase when weak ORN inputs are amplified strongly. Confirmation that nonlinear transformation does increase odor discriminability requires quantitative verification that considers not only the separation of mean neural responses, but also the variability of responses. In this study, we quantitatively determined whether the nonlinear firing rate transformation was optimum in terms of maximum information preservation by computing the mutual information between odorant stimuli and PN responses in our network model. Mutual information quantifies odor discriminability taking into account not only the separation of mean neural responses but also the variability of responses without any assumption of specific decoders.
In the Drosophila antennal lobe, two main processes contribute to transform neural representations in ORNs into those in PNs, namely intraglomerular transformation and interglomerular interaction through LNs. The shape of the nonlinear transformation between ORN and PN firing rates is therefore formed by these two factors [3], [12]–[16], [31]. We simply parameterized the form of intraglomerular transformation as one variable and the strength of LN input to each glomerulus as another variable. By systematically varying these two variables, we found that mutual information between odor stimuli and PN responses was maximized when the intraglomerular transformation preferentially amplified a weak ORN input and the net LN input was inhibitory. This is the very combination observed experimentally [13], [31]. Furthermore, the shape of the resultant nonlinear transformation was similar to that obtained experimentally [3]. These results suggest that ORN activity is transformed into PN activity in a near-optimal manner so as to preserve the maximum information. We also discuss how the intraglomerular transformation and interglomerular interaction contribute to increase mutual information.
Methods
Network model of the Drosophila antennal lobe
In this section, we describe the construction of a network model of the Drosophila antennal lobe (Fig. 1 (B)). There are three types of neurons in the Drosophila antennal lobe: ORNs, PNs, and LNs. We assume that these neurons fire according to a Poisson process with a time-independent firing rate for ORNs and a time-dependent firing rate for PNs and LNs. Our network model has a two-layer feed-forward architecture consisting of an ORN layer and a PN layer. The antennal lobe is subdivided into characteristic structures called glomeruli that constitute discrete processing channels. All the ORNs expressing a particular receptor converge onto the same glomerulus and connect to PNs [7], [8], with the dendritic arbors of individual PNs confined within a single glomerulus [9]–[11]. Each PN therefore receives direct input from just one ORN type (Figs. 1 (A) and (B)). Our model incorporates all these characteristics of the antennal lobe circuit.
First, for the model of ORNs, we assumed that ORNs show only excitatory responses to odors and that these responses are time-independent. We determined the ORN firing rates for a given odor by using Hallem and Carlson's [4] comprehensive study, which measured responses of 24 types of ORNs to over 100 odors. The value of the mean ORN firing rate in response to odor is denoted by , where superscript indicates the glomerular identity. The values of are shown in Fig. 2 (A) (Fig. 1 in ref. [4]). Figure 2 (B) shows a histogram of the ORN firing rate. Most ORN odor responses are clustered at the weak end of the dynamic range of the ORNs, with this being a characteristic feature of their responses.
Second, for the LN model, we assumed that (1) LNs receive synaptic input from all ORNs and (2) LNs innervate all PNs. These assumptions were made in order to reflect recent experimental observations that the strength of an inhibitory lateral input was positively correlated with the total ORN activity evoked by each odor [13] and that all the PNs examined received interglomerular excitation [12]. The former observation would imply that the odor tuning of the lateral input is similar across glomeruli. For simplicity, we assumed that the synaptic strengths between ORNs and LNs and between LNs and PNs are homogeneous. The total inputs from ORNs to LNs are modeled by
(1) |
where is the number of ORNs within a single glomerulus, is the total number of glomeruli in the network, is the time when the th ORN in the th glomerulus fires, and is the synaptic strength between ORNs and LNs. is set to 10. Synaptic inputs from ORNs are modeled by an exponential with a time constant . We set to 2 ms. The results of our study were insensitive to the absolute value of (data not shown). Synaptic events before time are all summed up linearly in Eq. 1. We assumed that the LN firing rate increases linearly with the strength of input , i.e.,
(2) |
Third, for the PN model, we chose a configuration in which each PN receives direct input from ORNs in a single glomerulus, , and lateral input from LNs, . Therefore, the total inputs received by a PN at time are modeled by
(3) |
(4) |
(5) |
where is the number of LNs and is the time when the th LN fires. In Eq. 4, is the synaptic strength between ORNs and PNs. We chose a configuration where ORNs are connected to PNs in an all-to-all manner and the synaptic strength between ORNs and PNs is homogeneous, reflecting the experimental findings [31], [32]. In Eq. 5, is a parameter controlling the strength of lateral input from LNs. Lateral input is excitatory when and inhibitory when . Although the net LN input is inhibitory, as observed experimentally [13], excitatory LNs are also present within the antennal lobe [12], [14], [15]. We examined the effects of both excitatory () and inhibitory () lateral inputs on odor discriminability on the basis of PN responses. The PN firing rate at time is determined by the strength of input as
(6) |
where is an input threshold value below which the PN firing rate is 0 and is the value above which the PN firing rate is saturated at the maximum value . The relationship between ORN and PN firing rates for different values when , , , and (no lateral input) is shown in Fig. 3. Here, controls the shape of the transformation between ORN and PN firing rates within a glomerulus. The functional form of Eq. 6 suitably describes the actual relationship between ORN and PN firing rates [3], [13]. When , the intraglomerular transformation preferentially amplifies weak ORN inputs and when , it rather suppresses weak ORN inputs. We call the firing rate transformation concave when and convex when .
From Eqs. 3–6, we can see that the strength of feed-forward connections between ORNs and PNs is just a scaling parameter, i.e., free parameters are only the ratio of strength of feed-forward and lateral connections, , and . For simplicity, we set to 1 without loss of generality. Parameters and determine the relationship between ORN and PN firing rates. We investigated the optimum firing rate transformation between ORNs and PNs from the viewpoint of maximum mutual information by systematically changing and .
The parameters and were fixed as follows. First, we determined so that the PN firing rate saturates when the firing rate of a presynaptic ORN is nearly 250 Hz (Fig. 3). Specifically, was set to 0.4. Second, we determined based on the experimentally observed relationship between ORN and PN firing rates without lateral input [13]. This experiment showed that the slope of the ORN-to-PN firing rate transformation close to the origin was very steep in the absence of lateral input. This indicates that is very small. The relationship between ORN and PN firing rates for different values of is shown in Fig. 4. When is 0.04 (dot-dashed line in Fig. 4), the slope at the origin is nearly 0. This is inconsistent with the experimental data [13]. For simplicity, we set to 0 (see ‘Effect of static firing threshold on mutual information’ for cases of different ). When we determine and as described above, our model emulates the experimentally observed firing rate transformation in [13] for a certain value of .
Mutual information
We computed the mutual information between population activities of PNs and odors . A component of vector is the number of spikes emitted by a PN within a time bin . We set to 10 ms. Since we defined the maximum PN firing rate as 200 Hz, two spikes are emitted on average by PNs with the highest firing rate. To reduce the amount of computation, we set a threshold value for , denoted by , and reset the number of spikes as whenever a PN spikes more than this value. We set to 5 considering that the probability of there being more than five spikes within a bin is less than .
The mutual information is given by
(7) |
(8) |
(9) |
where is the entropy and is the noise entropy and is uniform for all odors; that is, , where is the number of odors. We estimated the conditional probability distribution by simulating the network model 400 times. represents the summation over all possible PN activity patterns. The number of all possible PN activity patterns is , where is the number of glomeruli and is the number of PNs within each glomerulus. The computational costs grow exponentially with the number of neurons, so the mutual information calculation is limited by the size of the neural population. When we computed the mutual information using Eqs. 7–9, we set to 1 and to 8.
When we considered a larger number of neurons (, ), we estimated the mutual information by using the decoding approach [33]. In this approach, we trained support vector machine (SVM) classifiers and evaluated their performance. Decoding performance is usually quantified by the correct classification rate, which is the average of the diagonal elements in the confusion matrix. Whereas the classification rate deals with only the most likely stimulus predicted by the decoders given a particular neural response, the mutual information quantifies the overall knowledge about the presented stimulus, such as which stimulus is unlikely given a particular neural response. To link the information theoretic and decoding approaches, we must take into account the off-diagonal elements of the confusion matrix. We can estimate mutual information from the confusion matrix after decoding using the following equation [33], [34].
(10) |
where denotes the stimulus prediction of SVM classifiers for stimulus when the PN responses are given. Note that always holds from the data processing theorem [1]. Although the decoding approach underestimates the amount of information that neural responses carry, it can deal with much larger neural populations than methods that calculate the exact amount of mutual information. We used the information theoretic and decoding approaches in a complimentary manner to evaluate odor discriminability from neural responses.
A Library for Support Vector Machines (LIBSVM) was used to implement the SVM classifiers [35]. We used the one-against-one method for multiclass SVMs [36]. For classes, this method constructs different 2-class SVM classifiers for all possible pairs of classes. Test points are then classified according to a majority vote of these SVM classifiers as to which class is more likely. We chose a linear kernel because it gave the best classification performance and the closest estimate to the exact mutual information.
Results
Information theoretic approach
First, we computed the mutual information between odor stimuli and PN responses while systematically varying the intraglomerular transformation parameter and LN input strength (see Eqs. 5 and 6). Although the actual average numbers of ORNs, , and PNs, , within a single glomerulus in the Drosophila antennal lobe are said to be 40 and 3, respectively [23]–[26], we set to 40 and to 1 considering the cost of the mutual information computation. For the same reason, we reduced the number of glomeruli to 8 although data on ORN responses are available for 24 glomeruli (Fig. 2 (A)). We divided this data set into three non-overlapping groups consisting of eight glomeruli each and then computed the mutual information in these three groups. The number of LNs was set to 10. Later, we estimate the mutual information without reducing the number of neurons and using the entire data set at once (see ‘Decoding approach’).
Contour plots of the mutual information in a two-dimensional parameter space where the vertical axis is and the horizontal axis is are shown in Figs. 5 (A)(B)(C). Although the plots are for computations on different sets of glomeruli, the results are qualitatively similar. Therefore, we focus on the results shown in Fig. 5 (A), where two peaks are prominent in this graphical representation of the mutual information. At the lower left peak (denoted peak i), , , and , while at the upper right peak (denoted peak e), , , and . At peak i, the intraglomerular transformation is concave (dot-dashed line in Fig. 3) and the LN input is inhibitory. This combination of and is consistent with previous experimental results for the Drosophila olfactory system [13], [31]. In contrast, at peak e, the intraglomerular transformation is convex (dashed line in Fig. 3) and the LN input is excitatory. There is less mutual information at peak e than at peak i, so the mutual information is maximized at peak i.
The solid line in Fig. 6 shows the relationship between ORN and PN responses at peak i, and the dashed line shows the same relationship with the LN input removed. The nonlinear transformation shapes represented by the solid and dashed lines in Fig. 6 are similar to those observed in previous experiments [3], [13]. Olsen and Wilson [13] demonstrated the relationship between ORN and PN responses before and after removal of the lateral input, and these responses correspond to the solid line (before) and dashed line (after) in Fig. 6. This similarity in the nonlinear transformation suggests that from the viewpoint of information preservation, ORN activity is transformed in an almost optimal manner into PN activity in the Drosophila antennal lobe.
How the LN input affects the PN responses can be visualized by comparing the PN response histogram at peak i with that at point n, where mutual information is maximized under the condition of no LN input () (Fig. 5 (A)). PN response histograms at peak i and point n are shown in Figs. 7 (A) and (B), respectively. As a consequence of the intraglomerular transformation, these histograms are flatter than the ORN response histogram (Fig. 2 (B)). However, by comparing these histograms, we can see that PN odor responses are slightly clustered around the weak end of the PNs' dynamic range at peak i. This is because only the intraglomerular transformation has an effect at point n, while the LN input has an additional effect at peak i. The PN response histogram shown in a previous experiment has similar characteristics to the histogram at peak i [3]. When mutual information at peak i is compared with that at point n, the value at peak i is larger than that at point n, (). These results suggest that not only the intraglomerular transformation but also the LN input contribute to increase mutual information in the olfactory system as it did in our network model.
Mechanisms underlying the enhancement of mutual information
Next, we examined how the intraglomerular transformation and the interglomerular interaction contribute to increase mutual information. Mutual information is the difference between entropy and noise entropy (Eq. 7). Entropy measures the variability of neural responses to different odors and is related to the degree of flatness in the histogram of the neural response magnitudes [3], [37]. Noise entropy measures the average variability of neural responses to a particular odor. For a large amount of mutual information to be obtained, entropy should be large and noise entropy should be small.
We examined how mutual information, entropy, and noise entropy changed when or was changed around peak i in Fig. 5 (A). We found that both entropy and noise entropy increased as the intraglomerular transformation shape was changed from linear () to concave () (Fig. 8 (A)). Mutual information increased because entropy increased more rapidly than noise entropy. This result indicates that the concave intraglomerular transformation increases mutual information by increasing the variability of neural responses to different odors. In contrast, both entropy and noise entropy decreased as the strength of inhibitory LN input increased (Fig. 8 (B)). Mutual information increased because noise entropy decreased more than entropy. This result indicates that the inhibitory LN input increases mutual information by decreasing the noise of neural responses.
When or was changed around peak e, the behavior of the entropy and noise entropy was opposite to that around peak i. The convex intraglomerular transformation increased mutual information by decreasing noise entropy, and the excitatory LN input increased mutual information by increasing the entropy (Fig. 8 (C)(D)).
Decoding approach
In the previous section, we used the subdivided data sets obtained from the data set in Fig. 2 (A). In this section, we describe the use of the whole data set containing ORN responses to 110 odors in 24 glomeruli. We also set the numbers of ORNs and PNs within a single glomerulus to 40 and 3, respectively, to match the actual average numbers of neurons in the Drosophila antennal lobe. The number of LNs was set to 10, as in the previous section. To assess a large number of neurons, we estimated mutual information using the decoding approach [33] rather than computing it exactly. To estimate mutual information in Eq. 10, we ran simulations of the olfactory network described in the previous section. We then trained linear SVM classifiers by using the simulation data set and tested their performance. Finally, we estimated mutual information from the performance of the linear SVM classifiers (see ‘Methods’ for details).
First, we examined how well the mutual information estimated from the decoding approach matched the actual mutual information. We performed this comparison using the subdivided data set presented in Fig. 5 (A). Figures 9 (A) and (B) show the exact and estimated mutual information when was changed around peaks i and e, respectively. Although the estimated mutual information converged to a level that underestimates the real mutual information, we were able to estimate with relatively high accuracy the positions of both peaks (Fig. 9). Therefore, with regard to the positions of and relationship between the peaks, the mutual information estimated from the SVM classifiers provides a reliable answer. We subsequently set the number of trainings and test data to 200 each. With this approach, we next estimated mutual information using the entire data set.
A contour plot of the estimated mutual information is shown in Fig. 10 (A). As in Fig. 5 (A) there are two peaks. At peak i, , , and ; at peak e, , , and . We increased the number of glomeruli, so these mutual information estimates are larger than those obtained in the previous section. There was significantly more mutual information at peak i than at peak e. Figures 10 (B) and (C) show the relationship between ORN and PN responses and the histogram of PN response magnitudes at peak i, respectively. The results in these figures qualitatively match the results obtained in previous physiological experiments [3], [13], further suggesting that the principle of maximum information preservation is used in the Drosophila antennal lobe.
To compare the coding efficiency in PNs with that in ORNs, we compared the mutual information of ORNs with that of PNs when the mutual information was maximized (at peak i). The estimated mutual information contained in all ORNs and in all PNs were and ; therefore, was larger than , which is consistent with the data processing theorem [1]. When we computed the mutual information using the same numbers of ORNs and PNs, however, the estimated ORN mutual information became , which is markedly smaller than . This demonstrates that PNs encode odor information more efficiently than ORNs at peak i. This is consistent with the experimental results of Bhandawat et al. [3].
Adaptive gain control
As described in the ‘Methods’ section, the strength of the inhibitory lateral input is positively correlated with the total ORN activity evoked by each odor [13]. This lateral inhibition is considered to mediate gain control in the olfactory circuit. In this section, we discuss how the adaptive gain control promotes a more efficient neural code for odors by considering the discrimination of pairs of odors.
Table 1 shows how the inhibitory LN input changed the performance of binary SVM classifiers, the distance of mean responses, and the mean variance of responses for all possible pairs of odors. and are values at peak i. The distance of the mean responses to two odors is the distance between two vectors of the mean number of spikes emitted by PNs within ms. The mean variance of PN responses to an odor is the mean of the distance between the trial-averaged PN response vector and the individual PN response vectors in all the trials. The number of trials was 1000. To enable the responses to different odors to be separated, the distance of mean responses should be large and the mean variance of responses should be low. As can be seen in Table 1, inhibition basically decreases the neural variability for all PN responses.
Table 1. Effects of inhibitory LN input on odor pair discrimination.
Ratio(%) | Averaged firing rate without inhibition (Hz) | Averaged firing rate with inhibition (Hz) | Difference of averaged firing rate (Hz) | Difference of correct rate (%) | Difference of variance (Hz) | Difference of distance (Hz) | ||
Correct rate increase | Distance increases and variance decreases | 57 | 146 | 89 | 5.00 | 67 | ||
Distance decreases and variance decreases | 18 | 127 | 79 | 0.82 | ||||
Correct rate decreases | Distance increases and variance decreases | 2 | 130 | 80 | 15 | |||
Distance decreases and variance decreases | 21 | 121 | 77 | |||||
Correct rate does not change | 2 | 123 | 78 | 0 |
We found that the correct classification rate was increased for 75% of pairs of odors by inhibitory LN input (Table 1). In 76% of cases within this category, the distance of mean responses was increased while the mean variance of responses was decreased, which are both beneficial for odor discrimination. These odor pairs evoked strong responses in ORNs. Since the inhibitory inputs were strong when the total ORN activity was high, these responses were strongly inhibiting. We visualized how the strong inhibitory LN input separated PN responses to odor pairs of this type by using principal component analysis. In Fig. 11 (A), where there is no inhibition (), two clusters corresponding to the PN responses to two odor stimuli are concentrated near the point (large circle) where the firing rates of all PNs are maximum. This shows that many PNs received a strong input from ORNs when these two odors were presented. In this case, the distance between mean responses to two odors was small because of the saturation of responses caused by the concave intraglomerular transformation. When inhibition was induced, the two clusters separated and moved toward a point (large cross) where all PNs were silent (Fig. 11 (B)).
In the other pairs for which the correct classification rate was increased, the distance between mean responses was decreased. However, the correct classification rate was increased since the variability of neural responses was also decreased. The PN responses of pairs of these types are shown in Figs. 11 (C)(D). In these pairs, PN firing rates were relatively low, which means that the inhibition was not strong. In Fig. 11 (D), the distance between the center points of clusters is decreased as well as variability of neural responses compared with Fig. 11 (C). However, the amount of the increase in the correct classification rate is relatively small.
In 21% of pairs, the correct classification rate was decreased due to the decrease in the distance between mean responses. For these pairs, the inhibitory input was small because PN firing rates were relatively low. The amount of the decrease in the correct classification was also relatively small. In 2% of the odors, the correct classification rate did not change. In these pairs, the correct classification was 100% with or without inhibition.
Taken together, these results indicate that an inhibitory LN input enhances odor discriminability mainly by separating the responses of PNs that receive a strong ORN input. Without lateral input, these PN responses saturated because of the concave intraglomerular transformation. For odors where the total ORN activity was relatively small, inhibitory LN input did not affect odor separability much because the amount of inhibition was not high. In this case, the separability of odors was increased for some of pairs (18%) and decreased for some of pairs (21%). On the whole, adaptively changing the inhibitory LN input helps odor discrimination.
Effect of static firing threshold on mutual information
In the previous sections, we assumed that the PN firing threshold was fixed at 0, reflecting the experimental observation [13] that the slope of a firing rate transformation curve was very steep even when the ORN firing rate was close to 0. In this section, we report on varying and investigating the effects of raising the firing threshold. First, we examined how increasing affected the mutual information when was fixed. The mutual information was estimated by using the decoding approach, as in the previous section. The estimated mutual information when is shown in Fig. 12. The mutual information was maximized when . Thus, raising the PN firing threshold can increase the mutual information like increasing the strength of adaptive inhibitory inputs can.
The contour plot of the estimated mutual information when is shown in Fig. 13(C). In this case, the mutual information was maximized when was nearly 0, and the beneficial effect of LN input on the mutual information was significantly diminished. This is because the PN firing rates were already fairly suppressed by the firing threshold. When was smaller than the optimized value (), the mutual information was maximized in a region where inhibitory gain control worked. For instance, when (Fig. 13(B)), we can see an i peak, as in the case of (Fig. 13(A)).
The firing transformation between ORNs and PNs when and is shown in Fig. 4. There, the slope at the origin is nearly 0, which is inconsistent with the experimental data [13]. We therefore could conclude that the PN firing threshold in the actual olfactory system is smaller than this optimized value and that an adaptive inhibitory input can promote efficient neural coding of odors (see ‘Discussion’).
Discussion
Maximum information preservation in the Drosophila antennal lobe
In this study, we investigated whether information related to odor stimuli is maximally preserved in the Drosophila antennal lobe. Taking account of approximately half of all the neurons engaged in olfactory processing (24 out of a total of approx. 50 glomeruli) and ORN responses to 110 odorants, we computed the mutual information between odor stimuli and PN responses in an antennal lobe model. Our network model is simple but incorporates the essential architecture and connectivity of the antennal lobe. We found that mutual information was maximized when the intraglomerular transformation was concave (Fig. 3) and the LN input was inhibitory, which is consistent with previous experimental results [13], [31]. Furthermore, the shape of the resultant nonlinear transformation between ORN and PN responses is similar to that observed experimentally [3], [13]. This indicates that the principle of maximum information preservation is used in the Drosophila primary olfactory center.
Neural mechanisms underlying maximum information preservation
We also examined how the intraglomerular transformation and inhibitory LN input contribute to increase the mutual information. In ORNs, odor responses are clustered at the weak end of their dynamic range. The concave intraglomerular transformation increases mutual information by equally distributing PN response magnitudes in their dynamic range. In terms of entropy and noise entropy, the concave intraglomerular transformation increases mutual information by increasing entropy more than noise entropy (Fig. 8 (A)).
Inhibitory LN input has two beneficial effects. The first is to decrease the neural variability of PN responses evoked by a given odor, as shown in Table 1. The second is to separate saturated PN responses by inhibiting them (Figs. 11 (A)(B)). Importantly, the inhibitory LN input is adaptive, i.e., the inhibitory input strength depends on the overall ORN activity [13]. This adaptive gain control mechanism enables the actual olfactory system to deal with odors with a wide range of magnitudes. Raising the PN firing threshold, which can be considered as static inhibition, can increase the mutual information like adaptive inhibitory LN input can (Fig. 12). However, raising the firing thresholds has the disadvantage that it equally inhibits PN responses regardless of the magnitude of ORN responses whereas adaptive inhibition does not inhibit weak PN responses much when the total ORN activity is low. This will prevent the brain from recognizing low-concentration odors. Because the olfactory system has to deal with a wide variety of odors, we infer that the firing threshold of a real PN is low and that adaptive gain control mechanisms, rather than a static threshold, are used. In fact, we found that the firing rate transformation between ORNs and PNs when the PN firing threshold was high did not resemble the actually observed one (Fig. 4).
Two possible mechanisms promoting odor discrimination
We computed the mutual information between stimuli and PN responses by systematically changing the parameters of intraglomerular transformation and LN input strength. We found two peaks in the graphical representation of the mutual information (Fig. 5 (A)). At one of them (peak i), the intraglomerular transformation is concave (dot-dashed line in Fig. 3) and LN input is inhibitory, which is consistent with the experimental results. At the other (peak e), the intraglomerular transformation is convex (dashed line in Fig. 3) and the LN input is excitatory. Although both of these neural mechanisms promote odor discrimination, the combination at peak i is used in the Drosophila olfactory circuit. One reason for the use of this combination is demonstrated by our finding that the peak value of mutual information at peak i is higher than that at peak e. Another reason is that excitatory LNs cannot perform adaptive gain control. If the net LN input is excitatory, the olfactory system cannot discriminate between odors over a wide range of concentrations or odor mixtures. For these reasons, the combination of concave intraglomerular transformation and inhibitory LN input can be considered the most appropriate in the olfactory circuit.
Robustness against change in nonlinear firing rate transformation shape
In Fig. 8 (A), which shows the dependence of mutual information on the intraglomerular transformation shape, we can see two significant features. One is that the mutual information decreases rapidly as parameter increases and approaches the region where the transformation function is convex (). The other is that the mutual information changes little in the wide region where the transformation function is concave (). These features indicate that the Drosophila olfactory system is robust against changes in the shape of the intraglomerular firing transformation for odor discrimination provided that the transformation is concave.
Bhandawat et al. [3] examined the shapes of the nonlinear transformation between ORN and PN firing rates in seven different glomeruli and observed two features similar to those observed in our network model. First, the shape was concave in every glomerulus. Second, these shapes showed some degree of variation. From the viewpoint of odor discrimination, our results provide explanations as to why the shape of the nonlinear transformation between ORN and PN responses should be concave in every glomerulus and why the nonlinear transformation shapes could differ from glomerulus to glomerulus as long as they are concave.
Approaches for understanding neural mechanisms
We demonstrated that the optimum nonlinear firing rate transformation between ORNs and PNs obtained by maximizing mutual information is similar to that observed in previous experiments (Figs. 6 and 10 (B)). Similarly, in many previous studies, it has been reported that optimum neural representations of sensory stimuli, which are predicted theoretically, resemble the actual response properties of early sensory neurons [38]–[42]. In this study, however, we investigated not only the optimum information transmission from the viewpoint of information maximization, but also the mechanisms of information maximization in the neural circuit, which had not previously been rigorously theoretically investigated.
We studied them by taking a different approach from previous studies to obtain optimum information transmission. First, we used actual physiological data as input stimuli. Second, we constructed an experimentally grounded network model of the Drosophila olfactory circuit and computed the mutual information between stimuli and PN responses in that network model. Third, by systematically changing the network's parameters, we searched for the neural mechanism that maximized the mutual information. This approach was possible owing to the characteristic advantages in the Drosophila olfactory circuit, namely a simple glomerular structure, a relatively small number of neurons engaged in sensory processing, and well studied response properties and connectivity of those neurons. By using this approach, we showed that the neural mechanisms underlying information maximization are consistent with previous experimental results. That is, when mutual information is maximized in the network model, the shape of the intraglomerular function is concave and the net LN input is inhibitory.
For the sake of simplicity, we used a simple neuron model and did not implement realistic LN inputs [12]–[16] or synaptic depression and refractory periods, which are thought to be the main origins of the concave firing rate transformation within glomeruli [3], [31]. In the future, realistic implementation of synaptic depression and LN interactions should give us a more detailed understanding of the nature of maximum information preservation in actual biological systems. This should also enable us to compare theoretical and experimental results in a more quantitative manner. It will be interesting to further investigate how maximum information preservation is implemented in the olfactory circuit in light of the basic findings obtained from this study.
Footnotes
Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Funding: This work was supported by Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research (18079003, 20240020, 20650019) from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology, Japan (to M. Okada). M. Oizumi was supported by a Grant-in-Aid for JSPS Fellows (08J08950). H.K. was supported by a postdoctoral fellowship from the National Institutes of Health (NIH) (F32DC009538). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Cover T, Thomas J. Elements of Information Theory 1991 [Google Scholar]
- 2.Linsker R. Self-organization in a perceptual network. Computer. 1988:105–117. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Bhandawat V, Olsen SR, Schlief ML, Gouwens NW, Wilson RI. Sensory processing in the drosophila antennal lobe increases reliability and separability of ensemble odor representations. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:1474–1482. doi: 10.1038/nn1976. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hallem EA, Carlson JR. Coding of odors by a receptor repertoire. Cell. 2006;125:143–160. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2006.01.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Laissue PP, Reiter C, Hiesinger P, Halter S, Fischbach K, et al. Three-dimensional reconstruction of the antennal lobe in drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Neurol. 1999;405:543–552. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Stocker RF, Lienhard MC, Borst A, Fischbach KF. Neuronal architecture of the antennal lobe in drosophila melanogaster. Cell Tissue Res. 1990;262:9–34. doi: 10.1007/BF00327741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Vosshall LB, Wong AM, Axel R. An olfactory sensory map in the fly brain. Cell. 2000;102:147–159. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)00021-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Gao Q, Yuan B, Chess A. Convergent projection of drosophila olfactory neurons to specific glomeruli in the antennal lobe. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:780–785. doi: 10.1038/77680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Jefferis G, Marin EC, Stocker RF, Luo L. Target neuron prespecification in the olfactory map of drosophila. Nature. 2001;414:204–208. doi: 10.1038/35102574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wong AM, Wang JW, Axel R. Spatial representation of the glomerular map in the drosophila protocerebrum. Cell. 2002;109:229–241. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(02)00707-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Marin EC, Jefferis G, Komiyama T, Zhu H, Luo L. Representation of the glomerular olfactory map in the drosophila brain. Cell. 2002;109:243–255. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(02)00700-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Olsen SR, Bhandawat V, Wilson RI. Excitatory interactions between olfactory processing channels in the drosophila antennal lobe. Neuron. 2007;54:89–103. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.03.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Olsen SR, Wilson RI. Lateral presynaptic inhibition mediates gain control in an olfactory circuit. Nature. 2008;452:956–960. doi: 10.1038/nature06864. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Shang Y, Claridge-Chang A, Sjulson L, Pypaert M, Miesenbock G. Excitatory local circuits and their implications for olfactory processing in the fly antennal lobe. Cell. 2007;128:601–612. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2006.12.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Root CM, Semmelhack JL, Wong AM, Flores J, Wang JW. Propagation of olfactory information in drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104:11826–11831. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704523104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Root CM, Masuyama K, Green DS, Enell LE, Nässel DR, et al. A presynaptic gain control mechanism fine-tunes olfactory behavior. Neuron. 2008;59:311–321. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Wilson RI, Laurent G. Role of gabaergic inhibition in shaping odor-evoked spatiotemporal patterns in the drosophila antennal lobe. J Neurosci. 2005;25:9069–9079. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2070-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ng M, Roorda RD, Lima SQ, Zemelman BV, Morcillo P, et al. Transmission of olfactory information between three populations of neurons in the antennal lobe of the fly. Neuron. 2002;36:463–474. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00975-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Lai SL, Awasaki T, Ito K, Lee T. Clonal analysis of drosophila antennal lobe neurons: diverse neuronal architectures in the lateral neuroblast lineage. Development. 2008;135:2883–2893. doi: 10.1242/dev.024380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Das A, Sen S, Lichtneckert R, Okada R, Ito K, et al. Drosophila olfactory local interneurons and projection neurons derive from a common neuroblast lineage specified by the empty spiracles gene. Neural Dev. 2008;3:33. doi: 10.1186/1749-8104-3-33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Okada R, Awasaki T, Ito K. Gamma-aminobutyric acid (gaba)-mediated neural connections in the drosophila antennal lobe. J Comp Neurol. 2009;514:74–91. doi: 10.1002/cne.21971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Chou YH, Spletter ML, Yaksi E, Leong JCS, Wilson RI, et al. Diversity and wiring variability of olfactory local interneurons in the drosophila antennal lobe. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13:439–449. doi: 10.1038/nn.2489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.de Bruyne M, Clyne PJ, Carlson JR. Odor coding in a model olfactory organ: the drosophila maxillary palp. J Neurosci. 1999;19:4520–4532. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-11-04520.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.de Bruyne M, Foster K, Carlson JR. Odor coding in the drosophila antenna. Neuron. 2001;30:537–552. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00289-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Stocker RF. The organization of the chemosensory system in drosophila melanogaster: a review. Cell Tissue Res. 1994;275:3–26. doi: 10.1007/BF00305372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Vosshall LB, Amrein H, Morozov PS, Rzhetsky A, Axel R. A spatial map of olfactory receptor expression in the drosophila antenna. Cell. 1999;96:725–736. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)80582-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hallem EA, Ho MG, Carlson JR. The molecular basis of odor coding in the drosophila antenna. Cell. 2004;117:965–979. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2004.05.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Wang JW, Wong AM, Flores J, Vosshall LB, Axel R. Two-photon calcium imaging reveals an odor-evoked map of activity in the fly brain. Cell. 2003;112:271–282. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(03)00004-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Pillow JW, Shlens J, Paninski L, Sher A, Litke AM, et al. Spatio-temporal correlations and visual signalling in a complete neuronal population. Nature. 2008;454:995–999. doi: 10.1038/nature07140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Wilson RI, Turner GC, Laurent G. Transformation of olfactory representations in the drosophila antennal lobe. Science. 2004;303:366–370. doi: 10.1126/science.1090782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kazama H, Wilson RI. Homeostatic matching and nonlinear amplification at identified central synapses. Neuron. 2008;58:401–413. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Kazama H, Wilson RI. Origins of correlated activity in an olfactory circuit. Nature Neurosci. 2009;12:1136–1144. doi: 10.1038/nn.2376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Quian Quiroga R, Panzeri S. Extracting information from neuronal populations: information theory and decoding approaches. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:173–185. doi: 10.1038/nrn2578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Robertson RG, Rolls ET, Georges-Francois P, Panzeri S. Head direction cells in the primate subiculum. Hippocampus. 1999;9:206–219. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1999)9:3<206::AID-HIPO2>3.0.CO;2-H. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Chang C, Lin CJ. LIBSVM: a library for support vector machines. 2001. Software available at http://www.csie.ntu.edu.tw/cjlin/libsvm.
- 36.Hsu CW, Lin CJ. A comparison of methods for multi-class support vector machines. IEEE Trans Neural Networks. 2002;13:415–425. doi: 10.1109/72.991427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Laughlin S. A simple coding procedure enhances a neuron's information capacity. Z Naturforsch [C] 1981;36:910–912. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Atick JJ, Redlich AN. What does the retina know about natural scenes? Neural Computat. 1992;4:196–210. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Olshausen BA, Field DJ. Emergence of simple cell receptive field properties by learning a sparse code for natural images. Nature. 1996;381:607–609. doi: 10.1038/381607a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Lewicki MS. Efficient coding of natural sounds. Nature Neurosci. 2002;5:356–363. doi: 10.1038/nn831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Smith EC, Lewicki MS. Efficient auditory coding. Nature. 2006;439:978–982. doi: 10.1038/nature04485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Dan Y, Atick JJ, Reid RC. Efficient coding of natural scenes in the lateral geniculate nucleus: experimental test of a computational theory. J Neurosci. 1996;16:3351–3362. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-10-03351.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]