Skip to main content
Brain logoLink to Brain
. 2010 Apr 22;133(6):1747–1754. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq079

Olfactory dysfunction, central cholinergic integrity and cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease

Nicolaas I Bohnen 1,2,3,, Martijn L T M Müller 1, Vikas Kotagal 2, Robert A Koeppe 1, Michael A Kilbourn 1, Roger L Albin 2,3, Kirk A Frey 1,2
PMCID: PMC2877903  PMID: 20413575

Abstract

Olfactory dysfunction is common in subjects with Parkinson’s disease. The pathophysiology of such dysfunction, however, remains poorly understood. Neurodegeneration within central regions involved in odour perception may contribute to olfactory dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease. Central cholinergic deficits occur in Parkinson’s disease and cholinergic neurons innervate regions, such as the limbic archicortex, involved in odour perception. We investigated the relationship between performance on an odour identification task and forebrain cholinergic denervation in Parkinson’s disease subjects without dementia. Fifty-eight patients with Parkinson’s disease (mean Hoehn and Yahr stage 2.5 ± 0.5) without dementia (mean Mini-Mental State Examination, 29.0 ± 1.4) underwent a clinical assessment, [11C]methyl-4-piperidinyl propionate acetylcholinesterase brain positron emission tomography and olfactory testing with the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test. The diagnosis of Parkinson’s disease was confirmed by [11C]dihydrotetrabenazine vesicular monoamine transporter type 2 positron emission tomography. We found that odour identification test scores correlated positively with acetylcholinesterase activity in the hippocampal formation (r = 0.56, P < 0.0001), amygdala (r = 0.50, P < 0.0001) and neocortex (r = 0.46, P = 0.0003). Striatal monoaminergic activity correlated positively with odour identification scores (r = 0.30, P < 0.05). Multiple regression analysis including limbic (hippocampal and amygdala) and neocortical acetylcholinesterase activity as well as striatal monoaminergic activity, using odour identification scores as the dependent variable, demonstrated a significant regressor effect for limbic acetylcholinesterase activity (F = 10.1, P < 0.0001), borderline for striatal monoaminergic activity (F = 1.6, P = 0.13), but not significant for cortical acetylcholinesterase activity (F = 0.3, P = 0.75). Odour identification scores correlated positively with scores on cognitive measures of episodic verbal learning (r = 0.30, P < 0.05). These findings indicate that cholinergic denervation of the limbic archicortex is a more robust determinant of hyposmia than nigrostriatal dopaminergic denervation in subjects with moderately severe Parkinson's disease. Greater deficits in odour identification may identify patients with Parkinson's disease at risk for clinically significant cognitive impairment.

Keywords: acetylcholinesterase, cognitive impairment, Parkinson’s disease, positron emission tomography, smell

Introduction

The pathophysiology of hyposmia in Parkinson’s disease is poorly understood. Neuronal degeneration with deposition of α-synuclein within the olfactory bulb and anterior olfactory nucleus occurs early in Parkinson’s disease (Braak et al., 2002; Hawkes et al., 2009). There is evidence also of α-synuclein pathology within the limbic rhinencephalon (Silveira-Moriyama et al., 2009). Deficits in olfactory function in Parkinson’s disease are described in odour identification, odour discrimination, threshold detection and odour recognition memory (Mesholam et al., 1998). While there is some evidence that odour identification and discrimination may be impaired independently of olfactory detection threshold sensitivity in Parkinson’s disease (e.g. Boesveldt et al., 2009), effect sizes are large in Parkinson’s disease for all three of these nominally distinct domains (Mesholam et al., 1998), and tests of odour identification, detection and discrimination typically share considerable variance (Doty et al., 1994). Odour discrimination tasks may preferentially recruit the hippocampus, possibly reflecting its role in the working memory element of such tasks (Kareken et al., 2003). We reported previously that impaired odour identification in early Parkinson’s disease is correlated with hippocampal more than amygdala or striatal dopaminergic denervation (Bohnen et al., 2008). These findings suggest that hippocampal dopaminergic denervation and/or dysfunction may contribute to olfactory dysfunction in early Parkinson’s disease. Olfactory impairments, however, are not affected by dopaminergic medications, and correlate poorly, if at all, with disease stage or duration of motor features (Doty et al., 1988; Tissingh et al., 2001; Herting et al., 2008), suggesting a ‘floor’ phenomenon in hyposmia with respect to dopaminergic degeneration in advancing Parkinson’s disease. Diminished olfactory performance in non-demented subjects with Parkinson’s disease, however, may indicate increased risk of visual hallucinations, implying higher risk for developing dementia 2–6 years later, features that cannot be attributed easily to dopaminergic neuron degeneration (Stephenson et al., 2008).

Hyposmia occurs also in Alzheimer’s disease (Doty et al., 1987) and increases with severity of dementia (Murphy et al., 1990; Serby et al., 1991; Wilson et al., 2009). Higher density of entorhinal cortex and hippocampal neurofibrillary tangles correlate with greater deficits of odour identification, suggesting a role for hippocampal dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease hyposmia (Wilson et al., 2007). Loss of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons is an important feature of Alzheimer’s disease (Yan and Feng, 2004), with early involvement of septohippocampal projections (Lehericy et al., 1993). Cholinergic system degeneration is also an early feature of Parkinson’s disease and worsens with the appearance of dementia (Ruberg et al., 1986; Shimada et al., 2009). It is possible that limbic cholinergic denervation may also be a contributory factor to Parkinson’s disease hyposmia. If so, hyposmia would be most marked in those subjects with evidence of cognitive dysfunction. The goal of this study was to test the hypothesis that deficits in odour identification would correlate with central cholinergic denervation, especially of the hippocampus, and that odour identification scores correlate with cognitive performance in Parkinson’s disease.

Materials and methods

Subjects and clinical test battery

This cross-sectional study involved 58 subjects with Parkinson’s disease (49 males and 9 females), mean age 69.0 ± 7.6 (range 51–83) years and 26 control non-Parkinson’s disease subjects (17 males and 9 females), mean age 67.2 ± 10.5 (range 50–84) years. Patients with Parkinson’s disease met the UK Parkinson’s Disease Society Brain Bank Research Centre clinical diagnostic criteria (Hughes et al., 1992). The diagnosis of Parkinson’s disease was also confirmed by the presence of nigrostriatal dopaminergic denervation on (+)-[11C]dihydrotetrabenazine vesicular monoamine type 2 (VMAT2) positron emission tomography (PET). Dihydrotetrabenazine binding is a good marker of nigrostriatal dopaminergic denervation, as over 95% of the striatal signal is attributable to dopaminergic vesicular binding and shows excellent correlation with tyrosine hydroxylase-positive neuron density in the substantia nigra pars compacta (Vander Borght et al., 1995). Thirty-one Parkinson’s disease subjects were taking a combination of dopamine agonist and carbidopa-levodopa medications, 18 were using carbidopa-levodopa alone, 7 were taking dopamine agonists alone and 2 were not on dopaminergic drugs. No subjects were on anti-cholinergic or cholinesterase inhibitor drugs. Most subjects had moderate severity of disease: 1 patient in Stage 1, 2 in Stage 1.5, 11 in Stage 2, 24 in Stage 2.5, 19 in Stage 3 and 1 in Stage 4 of the modified Hoehn and Yahr classification (Hoehn and Yahr, 1967). Mean duration of disease was 7.0 ± 3.8 (standard deviation) years (range 0.5–17). Subjects with a Mini-Mental State Examination score of 24 or less were not eligible for the study (Folstein et al., 1975).

The Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale was performed (Fahn and Elton, 1987). Subjects on dopaminergic drugs were examined and imaged in the morning after withholding dopaminergic drugs overnight. Patient and control subjects underwent olfactory testing using the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test (UPSIT, Sensonics, Inc. Haddon Heights, NJ, USA; (Doty et al., 1984), administered by a trained examiner. The UPSIT incorporates microencapsulation technology and consists of encapsulated odours, one per page (Doty et al., 1984). There are 40 different odours and a forced choice is made from four possible answers. Each subject underwent a neuropsychological examination. Neuropsychological tests evaluating different cognitive domains were used for analysis following an approach as previously reported for cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease (Aarsland et al., 2009). These tests included: measures of verbal and non-verbal memory—California Verbal Learning Test (Delis et al., 2000) and Benton Visual Retention Test (Benton, 1974); and executive/reasoning functions—Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III Picture Arrangement test (Wechsler, 1997) and Stroop Colour Word Interference test (Stroop, 1935), together with a switching version of the Stroop 3 test in which subjects name the ink, unless the word is surrounded by a box, in which case, they read the word itself (Stroop 4). This measure makes an additional demand on cognitive flexibility (Bohnen et al., 1992). Stroop Colour Word Interference Test scores were calculated as the time difference for completion of the interference measures minus the non-interference tasks; attention/psychomotor speed as absolute times on the Stroop 1 and 2 subtests (Stroop, 1935). Visuospatial function was also measured, with the Benton Judgment of Line Orientation test (Benton et al., 1975). Composite z-scores were calculated for these different cognitive domains (memory, executive, attention and visuospatial functions) based on normative data from the control subjects.

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the University of Michigan and Ann Arbor Veterans Affairs Medical Centre for studies involving human subjects. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects.

Imaging techniques

All subjects underwent brain magnetic resonance imaging and acetylcholinesterase (AChE) and VMAT2 PET. Magnetic resonance imaging was performed on a 3 Tesla Philips Achieva system (Philips, Best, The Netherlands) utilizing an eight-channel head coil and the ‘ISOVOX’ exam card protocol primarily designed to yield isotropic spatial resolution. A standard T1-weighted series of a 3D inversion recovery-prepared turbo field echo was performed in the sagittal plane using repetition time/echo time/inversion time = 9.8/4.6/1041 ms; turbo factor = 200; single average; field of view = 240 × 200 × 160 mm; acquired matrix = 240 × 200. One hundred and sixty slices were reconstructed to 1 mm isotropic resolution. This sequence maximizes contrast among grey matter, white matter and cerebrospinal fluid and provides high-resolution delineation of cortical and subcortical structures.

AChE and VMAT2 PET imaging were performed in 3D imaging mode using an ECAT HR+ tomograph (Siemens Molecular Imaging, Inc., Knoxville, TN), which acquires 63 transaxial slices (slice thickness = 2.4 mm; intrinsic in-plane resolution = 4.1 mm full width at half maximum over a 15.2 cm axial field of view). A NeuroShield (Scanwell Systems, Montreal, Canada) head-holder/shielding unit was attached to the patient bed to reduce the contribution of detected photon events originating from the body outside the scanner field of view (Thompson et al., 2001). Prior to the dihydrotetrabenazine and methyl-4-piperidinyl propionate injections, a 5 min transmission scan was acquired using rotating 68Ge rods for attenuation correction of emission data using the standard vendor-supplied segmentation and re-projection routines.

[11C]methyl-4-piperidinyl propionate was prepared in high radiochemical purity (>95%) by N-[11C]methylation of piperidin-4-yl propionate using a previously described method (Snyder et al., 1998). Dynamic PET scanning was performed for 70 min immediately following a bolus intravenous injection of 666 mega-Becquerel (18 milliCuries) of [11C]methyl-4-piperidinyl propionate. The dose contained less than 200 µg cold methyl-4-piperidinyl propionate mass. Emission data were collected in 16 sequential emission scans: four × 30 s; three × 1 min; two × 2.5 min; two × 5 min; and five × 10 min.

No-carrier-added (+)-[11C]dihydrotetrabenazine (250–1000 Ci/mmol at the time of injection) was prepared as reported previously (Jewett et al., 1997). Dynamic PET scanning was performed for 60 min immediately following a bolus injection of 55% of 666 mega-Becquerel (18 milliCuries) of (+)-[11C]dihydrotetrabenazine dose (containing less than 50 µg of cold dihydrotetrabenazine mass) over the first 15–30 s of the study, while the remaining 45% of the dose was continuously infused over the next 60 min, resulting in stable arterial tracer levels and equilibrium with brain tracer levels after 30 min (Koeppe et al., 1997). A series of 15 frame sequence of scans over 60 min were obtained as following: four × 30 s; three × 1 min; two × 2.5 min; two × 5 min; and four × 10 min. All subjects were studied supine, with eyes and ears unoccluded, resting quietly in a dimly lit room.

Analysis

All image frames were spatially co-registered within subjects with a rigid-body transformation to reduce the effects of subject motion during the imaging session (Minoshima et al., 1993). IDL image analysis software (Research systems, Inc., Boulder, CO) was used to trace volumes of interest manually on the magnetic resonance imaging scan, representing the hippocampus, amygdala and the striatum. Total cortical volumes of interest were defined using semi-automated thresholding delineation of cortical grey matter signal on the magnetic resonance imaging scan.

AChE hydrolysis rates (k3) were estimated using the striatal volume of interest (defined by manual tracing on the magnetic resonance imaging scan of the putamen and caudate nucleus) as the reference input tissue (Nagatsuka et al., 2001).

Dihydrotetrabenazine images were analysed using equilibrium modelling to estimate the non-displaceable binding potential (BPND), which is equivalent to the ratio of specific (VS) to-non-displaceable (VND) binding in each imaged voxel or target volume of interest (Koeppe et al., 1997). We estimated specific dihydrotetrabenazine binding by subtraction of the global neocortex value, a reference region very low in VMAT2 binding sites, with the assumption that the non-displaceable distribution is uniform across the brain at equilibrium (Koeppe et al., 1999).

Standard pooled-variance t or Satterthwaite’s method of approximate t-tests (tapprox) were used for group comparisons (SAS version 9.1, SAS institute, Cary, North Carolina). Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated for correlation between clinical or PET variables.

Results

Olfactory and cognitive findings

There were no significant differences in age or education between the groups (Table 1). Patients with Parkinson’s disease had significantly reduced UPSIT scores compared to controls (Table 1). Parkinson’s disease subjects had significantly worse performance on most cognitive measures compared to the control subjects (Table 1).

Table 1.

Demographic, olfactory and cognitive measures in the Parkinson’s disease and control subjects

Parkinson’s disease (n = 58) Controls (n = 26) Statistical significance
Age (year) 69.0 ± 7.6 67.2 ± 10.5 t = 0.8; P = 0.44
Education (year) 15.0 ± 3.1 16.0 ± 2.8 t = 1.4; P = 0.16
Mini-Mental State Examination 29.0 ± 1.4 29.8 ± 0.5 tapprox = 4.0; P = 0.0001
University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test 16.7 ± 9.0 30.3±8.3 t =6.6; P < 0.0001
California Verbal Learning Test – Immediate Memory 8.3 ± 2.1 10.2 ± 2.2 t = 3.8; P = 0.0003
California Verbal Learning Test – Short Term Memory 8.2 ± 3.0 11.2 ± 2.7 t = 4.4; P < 0.0001
California Verbal Learning Test – Long Term Memory 9.4 ± 3.5 11.1 ± 3.0 t = 4.6; P < 0.0001
Benton Visual Retention Test 6.1 ± 2.0 7.7 ± 1.0 tapprox = 4.6; P < 0.0001
Stroop Colour Word Test 1 (s) 62.4 ± 17.7 51.6 ± 12.6 t = 2.8; P = 0.007
Stroop Colour Word Test 2 (s) 80.2 ± 16.7 66.4 ± 19.1 t = 3.4; P = 0.001
Stroop Colour Word Test 3 (s) 155.5 ± 47.1 121.1 ± 41.1 t = 3.2; P = 0.001
Stroop Colour Word Test 4 (s) 173.9 ± 58.8 141.2 ± 43.2 t = 2.5; P = 0.013
Picture Arrangement Test 11.5 ± 5.0 13.0 ± 3.5 tapprox = 1.5; P = 0.13
Judgment of Line Orientation Test 23.8 ± 4.2 24.5 ± 3.8 t = 0.6; P = 0.52

Data are mean (±SD).

Acetylcholine PET imaging findings in Parkinson’s disease and control subjects

Analysis of the AChE PET data demonstrated reduced mean neocortical, hippocampal and amygdala AChE activity in the Parkinson’s disease group (Table 2). There were no significant left-right hemispheric differences for AChE activity in the patients with Parkinson’s disease.

Table 2.

Limbic and neocortical AChE hydrolysis rates (k3; min−1) and striatal VMAT2 binding potential (BPND) in the Parkinson’s disease and control subjects

Parkinson’s disease (n = 58) Controls (n = 26) Statistical significance
Neocortical AChE k3 0.0272 ±0.0026 0.0303 ± 0.0035 t = 4.6; P < 0.0001
Hippocampal AChE k3 0.0426 ± 0.0058 0.0472 ± 0.0091 tapprox = 2.3; P = 0.025
Amygdala AChE k3 0.0596 ± 0.0096 0.0681 ± 0.0156 tapprox = 2.5; P = 0.015
Striatal VMAT2 BPND 0.96 ± 0.32 1.98 ± 0.32 t = 13.4; P < 0.0001

Data are mean (±SD).

Relationship between olfaction, in vivo imaging and cognitive findings

UPSIT scores correlated positively with AChE activity in the hippocampus (r = 0.56, P < 0.0001; Fig. 1), amygdala (r = 0.50, P < 0.0001) and neocortex (r = 0.46, P = 0.0003). Thus, individuals with higher UPSIT scores had higher AChE activity, and those with lower UPSIT scores had lower AChE activity, in these brain regions. Striatal VMAT2 activity correlated positively with UPSIT scores (r = 0.30, P = 0.022).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Scatter plot of the relationship between hippocampal AChE k3 hydrolysis (min−1) rates and UPSIT score in the Parkinson’s disease subjects (r = 0.56, P < 0.0001).

Multiple regression analysis including limbic (hippocampal and amygdala) and neocortical AChE activity and striatal VMAT2 activity using UPSIT scores as the dependent variable demonstrated an overall significant regression model (F = 10.0, P < 0.0001) with significant regressor effect for limbic AChE activity (F = 10.1, P < 0.0001), borderline for striatal VMAT2 activity (F = 1.6, P = 0.13), but no longer significant for cortical AChE activity (F = 0.3, P = 0.75).

Multiple regression analysis was performed to control for potential confounders (age, disease duration and Unified Parkinson’s disease Rating Scale motor severity) to evaluate the main regression effect between UPSIT scores and limbic AChE activity. The overall regression model was significant (F = 8.1, P < 0.0001) with a significant regressor effect for limbic AChE activity (F = 19.2, P < 0.0001) independent of significant effects for motor severity (F = 5.0, P = 0.03). Effects for age (F = 0.2, P = 0.70) or duration of motor disease (F = 0.7, P = 0.40) were not significant in the model.

Higher UPSIT scores were associated with better scores on cognitive measures of memory (composite memory z-score, r = 0.26, P < 0.05). Comparison between verbal versus non-verbal learning demonstrates a significant effect for episodic verbal learning (r = 0.30, P = 0.023) but not for visual non-verbal memory (r = 0.18, P = 0.17). There was a borderline positive correlation between the UPSIT and the Mini-Mental State Examination scores (r = 0.25, P = 0.055). There were no significant correlations between the UPSIT and visuospatial function (r = 0.001, P = 0.99), attention (r = –0.05, P = 0.80) or executive (r = 0.1, P = 0.46) function composite z-scores.

Limbic AChE activity correlated positively with executive cognitive (r = 0.36, P = 0.006), memory (r = 0.29, P = 0.03), borderline with attention (r = –0.26, P = 0.054) but not with visuospatial function (r = −0.04, P = 0.76) composite z-scores.

Post hoc analysis

A post hoc analysis was performed to evaluate a possible nigrostriatial dopaminergic denervation statistical ‘floor’ effect with more advanced disease relative to its correlation with the olfactory measures. Analysis limited to Parkinson’s disease subjects stage Hoehn and Yahr 2 or below (n = 14) demonstrated significant correlation between striatal VMAT2 activity and UPSIT (r = 0.61, P = 0.02). In contrast, analysis limited to Parkinson’s disease subjects Hoehn and Yahr stage 2.5 and higher demonstrated a borderline trend between striatal VMAT2 activity and UPSIT scores (r = 0.25, P = 0.10). Significant correlations between limbic and neocortical AChE activity and UPSIT scores were present in both Parkinson’s disease severity subgroups. Analysis limited to Parkinson’s disease subjects in stage Hoehn and Yahr 2 or below demonstrated positive correlations between UPSIT scores and limbic (r = 0.55, P = 0.04) and cortical (r = 0.60, P = 0.024) AChE hydrolysis rates. Analysis limited to Parkinson’s disease subjects Hoehn and Yahr stage 2.5 and higher demonstrated positive correlations between UPSIT scores and limbic (r = 0.60, P < 0.0001) and cortical AChE hydrolysis rates (r = 0.42, P = 0.0045).

Discussion

We found a positive association between odour identification performance and a measure of forebrain cholinergic pathway integrity in Parkinson’s disease. Diminished cholinergic innervation of the limbic archicortex, in particular, was associated with olfactory dysfunction. This finding suggests that impaired cholinergic function may contribute to the pathophysiology of Parkinson’s disease olfactory dysfunction.

Olfactory deficits in Parkinson’s disease are described in odour identification, odour discrimination, odour threshold detection and odour recognition memory (Mesholam et al., 1998; Haehner et al., 2009), and these tests share considerable variance (Doty et al., 1994). Proper interpretation of olfactory dysfunction in these different domains requires a distinction between the cognitive processes involved in an olfactory task and the physiological olfactory element of the task. For example, odour discrimination tasks may preferentially recruit the hippocampus, possibly reflecting its role in the working memory element of such tasks (Kareken et al., 2003). Odour identification requires recognizing or naming the odour, a long-term memory function, whereas forced-choice threshold tests recruit short-term memory processes. These operational processes in olfactory tests will in part depend on the integrity of structures involved in higher order cognitive or memory processing, such as the limbic cortex (Larsson et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2005). For example, regional grey matter atrophy of the paralimbic cortex has been found to correlate with the presence of olfactory dysfunction in early Parkinson’s disease whereas atrophy of the limbic cortex correlated with olfactory deficits in patients with moderately advanced Parkinson’s disease (Wattendorf et al., 2009). We found a robust positive association between limbic AChE activity and performance on an odour identification task in a population of non-demented subjects with Parkinson’s disease (mean Mini-Mental State Examination score of 29.0). Furthermore, the association between limbic AChE activity and memory performance was only modest, implying that our findings cannot be explained on the basis of memory deficits alone.

Hippocampal and entorhinal pathology is an early feature of Alzheimer’s disease and odour identification deficits occur early in the disease course. Alzheimer disease olfactory defects reflect deficits of odour cognitive processing rather than odour detection, which is not altered until later in Alzheimer’s disease (Serby et al., 1991). We found that the UPSIT had more selective association with performance on episodic verbal learning. This is likely to reflect the specific verbal cognitive functions involved in odour identification. Episodic memory impairment is a hallmark of Alzheimer’s disease, and loss of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons that innervate the hippocampus and neocortex is an early and key feature of Alzheimer’s disease (Yan and Feng, 2004). Post-mortem studies describe marked regional variations in the extent of cholinergic losses with the temporal lobe displaying the most significant losses of cholinergic fibers (Geula and Mesulam, 1996). These findings provide evidence for selective loss of the cholinergic septohippocampal pathway and subregions within the nucleus basalis of Meynert, namely the posterior portion that innervate the temporal lobe, including the amygdala and the hippocampal formation (Emre et al., 1993; Lehericy et al., 1993; Geula and Mesulam, 1996). Our findings of more robust correlation between deficits in odour identification and limbic than neocortical AChE activity are concordant with these observations in Alzheimer’s disease.

Involvement of the limbic cholinergic system in olfactory processing is also supported by animal studies. For example, disruption of normal cholinergic function alters odour memory, including both habituation to a familiar odour and odour-based social recognition (Hunter and Murray, 1989; Paolini and McKenzie, 1993, 1996; Berger-Sweeney et al., 2000), short-term memory for odours (Ravel et al., 1992, 1993, 1994) and acquisition of a complex odour discrimination task (De Rosa et al., 2001; Mandairon et al., 2006). Systemic physostigmine enhances discrimination between extremely similar odours in rats (Doty et al., 1999). Conversely, systemic scopolamine impairs olfactory perceptual learning (Fletcher and Wilson, 2002). Local injection of scopolamine in the hippocampal formation and prelimbic cortices impairs memory for socially transmitted food preference in rats (Carballo-Marquez et al., 2009). Selective lesioning of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons is associated with anterograde and retrograde deficits in a social transmission of food preference task in rats (Vale-Martinez et al., 2002), and lesions of the horizontal limb of the diagonal band of Broca impair olfactory memory (Linster and Hasselmo, 2000). There is a single human study showing that the AChE inhibitor drug donepezil improves odour identification in patients with Alzheimer’s disease, providing preliminary clinical support for a cholinergic component of olfactory processing in neurodegeneration (Velayudhan and Lovestone, 2009).

There is increasing evidence that cholinergic denervation occurs early in Parkinson’s disease and progressive cholinergic denervation is associated with the presence of dementia in this disorder (Shimada et al., 2009). Lewy first identified the eponymous Lewy body in neurons of the nucleus basalis of Meynert (Lewy, 1913), the source of cholinergic innervation of the cerebral cortex. In the Braak et al. (2003) staging scheme of Parkinson’s disease pathology, nigral and magnocellular basal forebrain pathology occur simultaneously. A recent AChE PET study found evidence of significant cholinergic denervation in early drug naïve Parkinson’s disease subjects without cognitive symptoms (Shimada et al., 2009). We found evidence of cholinergic denervation that correlated with cognitive performance on memory and executive tasks in patients with Parkinson’s disease without dementia. In vivo imaging studies have also shown that the presence of dementia in Parkinson’s disease is associated with more severe and widespread cholinergic denervation compared to Parkinson’s disease without dementia (Kuhl et al., 1996; Shinotoh et al., 1999; Bohnen et al., 2003; Hilker et al., 2005). These imaging results are consistent with post-mortem evidence that basal forebrain cholinergic system degeneration appears early in Parkinson’s disease and worsens with the appearance of dementia (Ruberg et al., 1986). A post-mortem study found greater reductions of AChE in the frontal cortex of demented (−68%) compared with non-demented (−35%) patients with Parkinson’s disease (Ruberg et al., 1986). Furthermore, cognitive impairment has been found to correlate with cortical choline acetyltransferase levels, but not with the extent of plaque or tangle formation in Parkinson’s disease (Perry et al., 1985; Mattila et al., 2001). Similarly, Mattila et al. (2001) found that cognitive decline in Parkinson’s disease was associated with lower cortical choline acetyltransferase levels, even in the absence of Alzheimer’s disease pathology. Therefore, impairment or degeneration of the cholinergic system may play a significant role in the cognitive decline in Parkinson’s disease (Perry et al., 1985).

We previously reported significant correlations between UPSIT scores and hippocampal, amygdala and striatal dopaminergic activity using a highly specific dopamine transporter PET radioligand, [11C](−)-2-β-Carbomethoxy-3-β-(4-fluorophenyl)tropane, in (very) early Parkinson’s disease, including drug-naïve subjects (Bohnen et al., 2007, 2008). A limitation of the present study is that the VMAT2 ligand cannot be used for specific assessment of extra-striatal dopaminergic innervation as this ligand fails to discriminate between dopaminergic and serotonergic or adrenergic innervation of the hippocampus and amygdala. Furthermore, the Parkinson’s disease subjects enrolled in the present study had lower smell performance and approximately twice the duration of disease and clinical severity of motor impairment than patients with Parkinson’s disease from our previous study. As there may be a ‘floor’ effect underlying the relationship between dopaminergic denervation and odour identification, we performed a subgroup analysis stratified for severity of disease and found significant correlations between striatal VMAT2 activity and UPSIT scores in the subjects with mild disease but not in those with more advancing disease. These data provide supportive evidence for the presence of a ‘floor’ effect with respect to hyposmia and nigrostriatal dopaminergic denervation in advancing Parkinson’s disease. In addition, multiple regression analysis demonstrated significant regression for odour identification scores and limbic AChE activity while regression with striatal VMAT2 activity was only borderline significant in the present study. We did not find evidence of a cholinergic denervation ‘floor’ effect relative to odour identification in the present study. Thus, cholinergic denervation may be a more important determinant of odour identification deficits than nigrostriatal dopaminergic denervation in patients with advancing Parkinson’s disease.

Parkinson’s disease is now being recognized as a multi-system neurodegeneration syndrome (Langston, 2006). It is plausible that hyposmia in Parkinson’s disease may have multiple components including olfactory bulb and primary olfactory cortex components, and limbic dysfunction secondary to dopaminergic, cholinergic and other denervations. Olfactory impairments have been shown in an animal model with reduced monoaminergic storage capacity (Taylor et al., 2009). Dopaminergic effects may be more significant in hyposmia of early Parkinson’s disease because dopaminergic, unlike cholinergic, denervation is uniformly and severely present in all subjects with early prototypical Parkinson’s disease. In contrast, cholinergic denervation is more heterogeneous and more severe loss of smell may reflect cholinergic denervation in the subset of patients with Parkinson’s disease and incipient cognitive impairment. Our findings cannot be explained by tandem multi-system neurodegeneration as the cholinergic denervation hyposmia finding was independent from the effects of age, duration of disease, severity of motor disease or degree of nigrostriatal dopaminergic denervation.

We conclude that cholinergic denervation, especially of the limbic archicortex, is a more robust determinant of odour identification deficits than nigrostriatal dopaminergic denervation in subjects with moderately severe Parkinson’s disease. Future longitudinal research is needed to determine whether lower smell performance predicts development of dementia in Parkinson’s disease.

Funding

Department of Veterans Affairs; the Michael J. Fox Foundation; and National Institutes of Health (grant number P01 NS015655). Funding to pay the Open Access publication charges for this article was provided by University of Michigan internal investigator fund.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Christine Minderovic, Virginia Rogers, the PET technologists, cyclotron operators, and chemists, for their assistance.

Glossary

Abbreviations

AChE

acetylcholinesterase

UPSIT

University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test

VMAT2

vesicular monoamine transporter type 2

References

  1. Aarsland D, Bronnick K, Larsen JP, Tysnes OB, Alves G. Cognitive impairment in incident, untreated Parkinson disease: the Norwegian ParkWest study. Neurology. 2009;72:1121–6. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000338632.00552.cb. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Benton AL. Revised Visual Retention Test 4th. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1974. [Google Scholar]
  3. Benton AL, Varney NR, Hamsher K. Judgment of Line Orientation, Form V. Iowa City: University of Iowa Hopsitals; 1975. [Google Scholar]
  4. Berger-Sweeney J, Stearns NA, Frick KM, Beard B, Baxter MG. Cholinergic basal forebrain is critical for social transmission of food preferences. Hippocampus. 2000;10:729–38. doi: 10.1002/1098-1063(2000)10:6<729::AID-HIPO1010>3.0.CO;2-M. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Boesveldt S, de Muinck Keizer RJ, Knol DL, Wolters EC, Berendse HW. Extended testing across, not within, tasks raises diagnostic accuracy of smell testing in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2009;24:85–90. doi: 10.1002/mds.22325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bohnen N, Jolles J, Twijnstra A. Modification of the Stroop Color Word Test improves differentiation between patients with mild head injury and matched controls. The Clin Neuropsychologist. 1992;6:178–84. doi: 10.1080/13854049208401854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bohnen NI, Kaufer DI, Ivanco LS, Lopresti B, Koeppe RA, Davis JG, et al. Cortical cholinergic function is more severely affected in parkinsonian dementia than in Alzheimer disease: an in vivo positron emission tomographic study. Arch Neurol. 2003;60:1745–8. doi: 10.1001/archneur.60.12.1745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bohnen NI, Gedela S, Kuwabara H, Constantine GM, Mathis CA, Studenski SA, et al. Selective hyposmia and nigrostriatal dopaminergic denervation in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol. 2007;254:84–90. doi: 10.1007/s00415-006-0284-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bohnen NI, Gedela S, Herath P, Constantine GM, Moore RY. Selective hyposmia in Parkinson disease: association with hippocampal dopamine activity. Neurosci Lett. 2008;447:12–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.09.070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Braak H, Del Tredici K, Bratzke H, Hamm-Clement J, Sandmann-Keil D, Rub U. Staging of the intracerebral inclusion body pathology associated with idiopathic Parkinson's disease (preclinical and clinical stages) J Neurol. 2002;249(Suppl 3):III/1–5. doi: 10.1007/s00415-002-1301-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Braak H, Del Tredici K, Rub U, de Vos RA, Jansen Steur EN, Braak E. Staging of brain pathology related to sporadic Parkinson's disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2003;24:197–211. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(02)00065-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carballo-Marquez A, Vale-Martinez A, Guillazo-Blanch G, Marti-Nicolovius M. Muscarinic receptor blockade in ventral hippocampus and prelimbic cortex impairs memory for socially transmitted food preference. Hippocampus. 2009;19:446–55. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. De Rosa E, Hasselmo ME, Baxter MG. Contribution of the cholinergic basal forebrain to proactive interference from stored odor memories during associative learning in rats. Behav Neurosci. 2001;115:314–27. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. California Verbal Learning Test Manual, Adult Version. 2nd. The Psychological Corporation; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  15. Doty RL, Shaman P, Dann M. Development of the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test: a standardized microencapsulated test of olfactory function. Physiol Behav. 1984;32:489–502. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(84)90269-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Doty RL, Reyes PF, Gregor T. Presence of both odor identification and detection deficits in Alzheimer's disease. Brain Res Bull. 1987;18:597–600. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(87)90129-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Doty RL, Deems DA, Stellar S. Olfactory dysfunction in parkinsonism: a general deficit unrelated to neurologic signs, disease stage, or disease duration. Neurology. 1988;38:1237–44. doi: 10.1212/wnl.38.8.1237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Doty RL, Smith R, McKeown DA, Raj J. Tests of human olfactory function: principal components analysis suggests that most measure a common source of variance. Percept Psychophys. 1994;56:701–7. doi: 10.3758/bf03208363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Doty RL, Bagla R, Kim N. Physostigmine enhances performance on an odor mixture discrimination test. Physiol Behav. 1999;65:801–4. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(98)00238-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Emre M, Heckers S, Mash DC, Geula C, Mesulam M-M. Cholinergic innervation of the amygdaloid complex in the human brain and its alterations in old age and Alzheimer's disease. J Comp Neurol. 1993;336:117–34. doi: 10.1002/cne.903360110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fahn S, Elton R. Members of the UPDRS development committee. Unified Parkinson’s disease rating scale. In: Fahn S, Marsden C, Calne D, Goldstein M, editors. Recent developments in Parkinson’s disease. Florham Park. NJ: Macmillan Healthcare Information; 1987. pp. 153–64. [Google Scholar]
  22. Fletcher ML, Wilson DA. Experience modifies olfactory acuity: acetylcholine-dependent learning decreases behavioral generalization between similar odorants. J Neurosci. 2002;22:(RC201):1–5. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-02-j0005.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatry Res. 1975;12:189–98. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Geula C, Mesulam MM. Systematic regional variations in the loss of cortical cholinergic fibers in Alzheimer's disease. Cereb Cortex. 1996;6:165–77. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.2.165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Haehner A, Boesveldt S, Berendse HW, Mackay-Sim A, Fleischmann J, Silburn PA, et al. Prevalence of smell loss in Parkinson's disease–a multicenter study. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2009;15:490–4. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2008.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hawkes CH, Del Tredici K, Braak H. Parkinson's disease: the dual hit theory revisited. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2009;1170:615–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04365.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Herting B, Schulze S, Reichmann H, Haehner A, Hummel T. A longitudinal study of olfactory function in patients with idiopathic Parkinson's disease. J Neurol. 2008;255:367–70. doi: 10.1007/s00415-008-0665-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hilker R, Thomas AV, Klein JC, Weisenbach S, Kalbe E, Burghaus L, et al. Dementia in Parkinson disease: functional imaging of cholinergic and dopaminergic pathways. Neurology. 2005;65:1716–22. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000191154.78131.f6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hoehn M, Yahr M. Parkinsonism: onset, progression, and mortality. Neurology. 1967;17:427–42. doi: 10.1212/wnl.17.5.427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, Lees AJ. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: a clinicopathologic study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1992;55:181–4. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.55.3.181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hunter AJ, Murray TK. Cholinergic mechanisms in a simple test of olfactory learning in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1989;99:270–5. doi: 10.1007/BF00442821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Jewett DM, Kilbourn MR, Lee LC. A simple synthesis of [11C]dihydrotetrabenazine (DTBZ) Nucl Med Biol. 1997;24:197–9. doi: 10.1016/s0969-8051(96)00213-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kareken DA, Mosnik DM, Doty RL, Dzemidzic M, Hutchins GD. Functional anatomy of human odor sensation, discrimination, and identification in health and aging. Neuropsychology. 2003;17:482–95. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.17.3.482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Koeppe RA, Frey KA, Kume A, Albin R, Kilbourn MR, Kuhl DE. Equilibrium versus compartmental analysis for assessment of the vesicular monoamine transporter using (+)-alpha-[11C]dihydrotetrabenazine (DTBZ) and positron emission tomography. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 1997;17:919–31. doi: 10.1097/00004647-199709000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Koeppe RA, Frey KA, Kuhl DE, Kilbourn MR. Assessment of extrastriatal vesicular monoamine transporter binding site density using stereoisomers of [11C]dihydrotetrabenazine. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 1999;19:1376–84. doi: 10.1097/00004647-199912000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kuhl D, Minoshima S, Fessler J, Frey K, Foster N, Ficaro E, et al. In vivo mapping of cholinergic terminals in normal aging, Alzheimer's disease, and Parkinson's disease. Ann Neurol. 1996;40:399–410. doi: 10.1002/ana.410400309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Langston JW. The Parkinson's complex: parkinsonism is just the tip of the iceberg. Ann Neurol. 2006;59:591–6. doi: 10.1002/ana.20834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Larsson M, Nilsson LG, Olofsson JK, Nordin S. Demographic and cognitive predictors of cued odor identification: evidence from a population-based study. Chem Senses. 2004;29:547–54. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjh059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lehericy S, Hirsch EC, Cervera-Pierot P, Hersh LB, Bakchine S, Piette F, et al. Heterogeneity and selectivity of the degeneration of cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain of patients with Alzheimer's disease. J Comp Neurol. 1993;330:15–31. doi: 10.1002/cne.903300103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lewy FH. Zur pathologischen Anatomie der Paralysis agitans. Dtsch Ztschr Nervenheilkunde. 1913;50:50–5. [Google Scholar]
  41. Linster C, Hasselmo ME. Neural activity in the horizontal limb of the diagonal band of broca can be modulated by electrical stimulation of the olfactory bulb and cortex in rats. Neurosci Lett. 2000;282:157–60. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(00)00885-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Mandairon N, Ferretti CJ, Stack CM, Rubin DB, Cleland TA, Linster C. Cholinergic modulation in the olfactory bulb influences spontaneous olfactory discrimination in adult rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;24:3234–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05212.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Mattila PM, Roytta M, Lonnberg P, Marjamaki P, Helenius H, Rinne JO. Choline acetytransferase activity and striatal dopamine receptors in Parkinson's disease in relation to cognitive impairment. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 2001;102:160–6. doi: 10.1007/s004010100372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Mesholam RI, Moberg PJ, Mahr RN, Doty RL. Olfaction in neurodegenerative disease: a meta-analysis of olfactory functioning in Alzheimer's and Parkinson's diseases. Arch Neurol. 1998;55:84–90. doi: 10.1001/archneur.55.1.84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Minoshima S, Koeppe RA, Fessler JA, Mintun MA, Berger KL, Taylor SF, et al. Integrated and automated data analysis method for neuronal activation studying using O15 water PET. Tokyo: Excerpta Medica; 1993. pp. 409–18. [Google Scholar]
  46. Murphy C, Gilmore MM, Seery CS, Salmon DP, Lasker BR. Olfactory thresholds are associated with degree of dementia in Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. 1990;11:465–9. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(90)90014-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Nagatsuka S, Fukushi K, Shinotoh H, Namba H, Iyo M, Tanaka N, et al. Kinetic analysis of [(11)C]MP4A using a high-radioactivity brain region that represents an integrated input function for measurement of cerebral acetylcholinesterase activity without arterial blood sampling. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2001;21:1354–66. doi: 10.1097/00004647-200111000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Paolini AG, McKenzie JS. Effects of lesions in the horizontal diagonal band nucleus on olfactory habituation in the rat. Neuroscience. 1993;57:717–24. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(93)90017-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Paolini AG, McKenzie JS. Lesions in the magnocellular preoptic nucleus decrease olfactory investigation in rats. Behav Brain Res. 1996;81:223–31. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(96)89082-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Perry EK, Curtis M, Dick DJ, Candy JM, Atack JR, Bloxham CA, et al. Cholinergic correlates of cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease: comparisons with Alzheimer's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1985;48:413–21. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.48.5.413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Ravel N, Vigouroux M, Elaagouby A, Gervais R. Scopolamine impairs delayed matching in an olfactory task in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1992;109:439–43. doi: 10.1007/BF02247720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Ravel N, Elaagouby A, Gervais R. Scopolamine injection into the olfactory bulb impairs short-term olfactory memory in rats. Behav Neurosci. 1994;108:317–24. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.108.2.317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Roman FS, Simonetto I, Soumireu-Mourat B. Learning and memory of odor-reward association: selective impairment following horizontal diagonal band lesions. Behav Neurosci. 1993;107:72–81. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.107.1.72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Ruberg M, Rieger F, Villageois A, Bonnet AM, Agid Y. Acetylcholinesterase and butyrylcholinesterase in frontal cortex and cerebrospinal fluid of demented and non-demented patients with Parkinson's disease. Brain Res. 1986;362:83–91. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(86)91401-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Serby M, Larson P, Kalkstein D. The nature and course of olfactory deficits in Alzheimer's disease. Am J Psychiatry. 1991;148:357–60. doi: 10.1176/ajp.148.3.357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Shimada H, Hirano S, Shinotoh H, Aotsuka A, Sato K, Tanaka N, et al. Mapping of brain acetylcholinesterase alterations in Lewy body disease by PET. Neurology. 2009;73:273–8. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181ab2b58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Shinotoh H, Namba H, Yamaguchi M, Fukushi K, Nagatsuka S, Iyo M, et al. Positron emission tomographic measurement of acetylcholinesterase activity reveals differential loss of ascending cholinergic systems in Parkinson's disease and progressive supranuclear palsy. Ann Neurol. 1999;46:62–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Silveira-Moriyama L, Holton JL, Kingsbury A, Ayling H, Petrie A, Sterlacci W, et al. Regional differences in the severity of Lewy body pathology across the olfactory cortex. Neurosci Lett. 2009;453:77–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Snyder SE, Tluczek L, Jewett DM, Nguyen TB, Kuhl DE, Kilbourn MR. Synthesis of 1-[11C]methylpiperidin-4-yl propionate ([11C]PMP) for in vivo measurements of acetylcholinesterase activity. Nucl Med Biol. 1998;25:751–4. doi: 10.1016/s0969-8051(98)00045-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Stephenson R, Houghton D, Sundararajan SH, Doty RL, Siderowf A, Stern M. Impaired olfaction and subsequent risk of long-term complications of Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2008;23(Suppl 1):S282. [Google Scholar]
  61. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18:643–62. [Google Scholar]
  62. Taylor TN, Caudle WM, Shepherd KR, Noorian A, Jackson CR, Iuvone PM, et al. Nonmotor symptoms of Parkinson's disease revealed in an animal model with reduced monoamine storage capacity. J Neurosci. 2009;29:8103–13. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1495-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Thompson CJ, Kecani S, Boelen S. Evaluation of a neck-shield for use during neurological studies with a whole-body PET scanner IEEE Trans. Nucl Sci. 2001;48:1512–7. [Google Scholar]
  64. Tissingh G, Berendse HW, Bergmans P, DeWaard R, Drukarch B, Stoof JC, et al. Loss of olfaction in de novo and treated Parkinson's disease: possible implications for early diagnosis. Mov Dis. 2001;16:41–6. doi: 10.1002/1531-8257(200101)16:1<41::aid-mds1017>3.0.co;2-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Vale-Martinez A, Baxter MG, Eichenbaum H. Selective lesions of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons produce anterograde and retrograde deficits in a social transmission of food preference task in rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2002;16:983–98. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02153.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Vander Borght TM, Sima AA, Kilbourn MR, Desmond TJ, Kuhl DE, Frey KA. [3H]methoxytetrabenazine: a high specific activity ligand for estimating monoaminergic neuronal integrity. Neuroscience. 1995;68:955–62. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(95)00167-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Velayudhan L, Lovestone S. Smell identification test as a treatment response marker in patients with Alzheimer disease receiving donepezil. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2009;29:387–90. doi: 10.1097/JCP.0b013e3181aba5a5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Wang J, Eslinger PJ, Smith MB, Yang QX. Functional magnetic resonance imaging study of human olfaction and normal aging. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2005;60:510–4. doi: 10.1093/gerona/60.4.510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Wattendorf E, Welge-Lussen A, Fiedler K, Bilecen D, Wolfensberger M, Fuhr P, et al. Olfactory impairment predicts brain atrophy in Parkinson's disease. J Neurosci. 2009;29:15410–3. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1909-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Wechsler D. WAIS III Technical Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  71. Wilson RS, Arnold SE, Schneider JA, Tang Y, Bennett DA. The relationship between cerebral Alzheimer's disease pathology and odour identification in old age. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2007;78:30–5. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.099721. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Wilson RS, Arnold SE, Schneider JA, Boyle PA, Buchman AS, Bennett DA. Olfactory impairment in presymptomatic Alzheimer's disease. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2009;1170:730–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04013.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Yan Z, Feng J. Alzheimer's disease: interactions between cholinergic functions and beta-amyloid. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2004;1:241–8. doi: 10.2174/1567205043331992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Brain are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES