Abstract
Reflectometric techniques estimate the directionality of the retinal cones, by measuring the distribution of light at the pupil plane of light reflected off the bleached retina. The waveguide-scattering model [Marcos et al. (1998), J. Opt. Soc. Am A] predicts that the shape of this intensity distribution is determined by both the waveguide properties of the cone photoreceptors and by the topography of the cone mosaic (cone spacing). We have performed two types of cone directionality measurements. In the first type, cone directionality estimates are obtained by measuring the spatial distribution of light returning from the retina with a single entry pupil position (single-entry measurements). In the second type, estimates are obtained by measuring the total amount of light guided back through the pupil as a function of entry pupil position (multiple-entry measurements). As predicted by the model, single-entry measurements provide narrower distributions than the multiple-entry measurements, since the former are affected by both waveguides and scattering and the latter primarily by waveguides. Measurements at different retinal eccentricities and at two different wavelengths are consistent with the model. We show that the broader multiple-entry measurements are not accounted for by cone disarray. Results of multiple-entry measurements are closer to results from measurements of the psychophysical Stiles-Crawford effect (although still narrower), and the variation with retinal eccentricity and wavelength is similar. By combining single- and multiple-entry measurements we can estimate cone spacing. The estimates at 0 and 2 deg retinal eccentricities are in good agreement with published anatomical data.
Introduction
It is widely accepted that the cone-photoreceptors in the human retina exhibit directional sensitivity1. If the cones are pointing toward the center of the pupil, light entering through the center of the pupil (or equivalently, entering the cones along their axis) is perceived as brighter than the same light coming from the edge of the pupil (i.e. at a larger angle). The Stiles-Crawford effect of the first kind (SCE), as this effect is known, is typically measured using psychophysical techniques2,3. Since normal photoreceptor directionality requires a normal cone morphology and relation to the extracellular space, photoreceptor directionality has been of clinical interest4,5,6,7,8. Unfortunately, the long experimental sessions and high degree of cooperation required for the psychophysical measures have restricted the availability of these measurements.
Recently developed reflectometric techniques allow a much more rapid estimation of cone directionality9,10,11,12. The common principle of these techniques is that, when the photopigment is bleached, part of the light entering the cone inner segments is guided along the outer segment, and then scattered back toward the region of the pupil corresponding to the axis of the cones. The relative luminous efficiency (in psychophysical measurements) or the distribution of the directed or guided reflectance (in the reflectometric measurements) are typically fit at the plane of the pupil by a gaussian function Imax10−ρ[(x−x0)2+(y−y0)2], where x0 and y0 represent the coordinates of the peak location for which either the luminous efficiency or the guided reflectance are highest (i.e. the location at the pupil plane where the cones are aimed), and ρ is a measure of directionality (the higher ρ, the more narrowly tuned the function). It has been reported that reflectometric techniques provide the same estimate for the location at the pupil plane to which the photoreceptors are oriented, although directionality is, in general higher (higher rho values) in reflectometric measurements than in psychophysical measurements9,13,14.
Predictions of the waveguide-scattering model
We have proposed a more complete model15 of the intensity distributions measured at the plane of the pupil obtained in Burns et al.'s reflectometric technique11. In this technique, the distribution at the pupil plane of light reflected off the retina is imaged on a CCD camera, while a small patch of the bleached retina is illuminated in maxwellian view. In our model, we propose that the light distribution at the pupil plane is affected by both the waveguide properties of the photoreceptors and the interference of light re-emitted from the cones. Our assumption is that since the photoreceptors have slightly different lengths, light emerges from each cone with a different phase and interferes at the plane of the pupil. For the dimensions of the eye and cone mosaic, the effect is similar to that produced at the far-field plane by light scattered from a rough surface16. The scattering alone produces a gaussian distribution at the pupil plane and the rho value (ρscatt) depends on the spatial distribution of the cones15: ρscatt = k(fλ)−2 s2, where f is the axial length of the eye, λ is the wavelength, k is a constant and s is the row-to-row cone spacing.
As a result, the overall intensity distribution at the plane of the pupil is the product of two gaussian distributions15: the angular distribution of light emitted by the cones (ρwg) and the gaussian distribution caused by scattering. The rho value for the final distribution is then the sum of the rho values from the two components: ρ=ρscatt+ρwg
The waveguide-scattering model15 predicts that techniques measuring the waveguide properties of the cones alone should produce lower rho values (ρwg) than the rho value in Burns et al.'s reflectometric technique11. In addition, according to the model, if the waveguide properties can be measured independently, both ρwg and ρscatt can be determined, and the cone spacing of the retinal mosaic can be calculated. In the current paper, we present a technique to separate these components and show that the resulting cone spacing agrees with anatomical estimates in normal subjects, supporting our model. In addition, we show that different reflectometric techniques are differentially sensitive to ρwg and ρscatt. This differential sensitivity can explain some of the differences in measurements of cone directionality between laboratories.
Single-entry and multiple-entry photoreceptor directionality measurements
In Burns et al.'s technique11, typically a single image is measured for the entry pupil position that produces the highest directional intensity. This distribution is used to determine rho. If the entry location is moved away from this optimal entry pupil, less light is coupled into the photoreceptors, and consequently the amount of guided light reflected back from the photoreceptors decreases. According to our model15, while the shape of the light distribution depends on both waveguides and scattering, the total amount of guided light at each entry pupil location depends only on the angular tuning of the cones. As a result, the total amount of light guided as a function of entry pupil position represents a measure of the waveguide properties of the cones, and should be more similar to psychophysical estimates of cone directionality than to the estimates from the conventional reflectometric technique. We will refer to the measurements based on a single intensity distribution at the pupil as single-entry reflectometric measurements. The measurements based on the estimation of the total guided intensity as a function of entry pupil will be referred to as multiple-entry reflectometric measurements, since they are derived from multiple images.
In this paper, we present single- and multiple-entry measurements of photoreceptor directionality for a group of subjects. As predicted by the model15, multiple-entry measurements (that depend on the waveguide properties alone) provide consistently lower estimates of rho (broader functions) than the single-entry measurements. In the Discussion, we study the potential role of cone disarray17,18,20 on the measurements and we compare these results to previous measurements of the Stiles-Crawford effect in the same subjects14. We also present cone spacing estimates obtained by applying the model to directionality measurements. We finally reinterpret the results from other reflectometric measurements9,10,12 by applying the waveguide-scattering model.
Methods
Apparatus
We used an imaging reflectometer to measure the spatial distribution of the guided light reflected off the retina (single-entry measurements), as well as to measure the total amount of guided light reflected off the retina as a function of entry pupil position (multiple-entry measurements). The apparatus has been described previously11,18,15. Briefly, a 1-deg area of the retina is illuminated in Maxwelliam view by projecting a 0.18 μm laser spot at the pupil plane. Green or red illumination light is provided by two He-Ne lasers respectively (543 nm and 632 nm). A diode-pumped laser (532 nm) provides a wide bleaching field (∼6%105 Td). The position of both the entrance pupil and the retinal fixation location is under computer control. Light returning from the retina through a 2 deg- retinal field stop is collected at the plane of the pupil by means of a high resolution, scientific grade, cooled CCD camera (Princeton Instruments), located in a pupil conjugate plane. A separate channel allows infrared viewing of the pupil, and is used for centration and alignment of the subject to the instrument throughout the session. Subjects' head positions are stabilized by means of a bite bar and forehead rests.
Single-entry reflectometric measurements are based only on the spatial distribution of light guided towards the pupil within a single image. However, the new multiple-entry measurements require accurate measurements of relative intensity for all images within a session. To ensure that fluctuations in either the light source or the detector did not cause artifacts, the tip of a fiber optic collecting part of the output of the laser source was placed at a plane conjugate to the pupil and imaged together with light coming from the retina. Fluctuations were not detected within any single session, so we present unnormalized data.
Subjects and conditions
Single- and multiple-entry reflectometric measurements were collected on three normal subjects (SM, JH, and SB), ages 27, 38 and 48, one female and two males. JH had deuteranomalous color vision. Subjects were dilated with 0.5% Mydriacil after informed consent was obtained.
Two retinal locations (0 and 2 deg temporal) were tested in all subjects. Measurements were made typically using green light (543 nm); for subject JH data were also collected using red light (632 nm).
Experimental Procedure
Previous measurements13,18,15 on each subject provided an estimate of the entry location that produced the highest directionality. This location was taken as the optimal entry pupil for each subject: (0, -1) for subject SM, (-1,1) for subject JH and (0, -2) mm for subject SB. Positive coordinates stand for temporal and superior coordinates at the pupil plane, respectively, and negative coordinates for nasal and inferior coordinates at the pupil plane.
Series of images were obtained while moving the entry pupil in 0.5-mm steps along the horizontal and vertical axis. Five consecutive images were obtained at each entry pupil position and 6 to 11 entry positions were tested per axis. For a given wavelength, the intensity of the illuminating beam was kept constant throughout the experiment. For each condition, the entire series was repeated at least once on a different day.
Data analysis
Figure 1 sketches the basic idea of both single-entry and multiple-entry measurements and how to extract cone directionality information from the two types of measurements. The top panel represents a series of images obtained with the imaging reflectometer as the illumination beam moves nasally or temporally across the pupil.
For the single-entry estimates of cone directionality (see Fig. 1, left panel), images at the optimal entry pupil are processed as described elsewhere11,18. First, the corneal reflexes corresponding to the first and 4th Purkinje images are eliminated. The intensity distribution at the pupil plane is fit with the following equation: B + Imax10−ρs[(x−x0)2+(y−y0)2], where B is a constant that accounts for light diffusely reflected, forming a constant background that fills the pupil) and the second term is a 2-dimensional gaussian function that represents the light directly guided from the photoreceptors; Imax is the intensity at the peak, and it is highest for images obtained when illuminating through the optimal entry pupil; ρs is the directionality factor. For each session and each condition (retinal eccentricity and wavelength), we select the ρs corresponding to the best fit (the lowest rms between measurement and fit). Final ρs's are averages across sessions.
For the multiple-entry estimates of cone directionality (see Fig. 1, right panel) we did not use the same fitting procedure, since the quality of the fit is poor when Imax is small. Instead, we obtained estimates of the total guided intensity by analyzing the images directly. As before, corneal reflexes are eliminated. We select a small region of the pupil far from the distribution of guided light. The average intensity over that region is taken as an estimate of the diffuse background. The background is then subtracted from the image. The remaining total intensity in the image is used as an estimate of the total guided intensity. The total guided intensity (average over 5 measurements) as a function of entry pupil position is fit to a gaussian: tgImax10−ρmx(x−x0)2 and tgImax10−ρmy(y−y0)2, for the horizontal and vertical axes respectively. If the spatial distribution of the guided light at the pupil plane fits a gaussian whose rho value does not change with entry pupil position, then fitting the total guided intensity or the maximum guided intensity should be equivalent. We confirmed this by analyzing sample data both ways. We decided to use the total guided intensity instead of the maximum guided intensity because the first one is based on the entire image, and is therefore a more robust estimate.
For each session and each condition, ρm is obtained as the average of the rho values obtained by fitting the total guided intensity estimates across the horizontal and vertical axis (ρm=(ρmx+ρmy)/2). Except for one case (SB, 0 deg) we did not find significant asymmetry between the estimates for the horizontal axis and vertical axis respectively. Final ρm's are averages across sessions. The peak locations (x0 and y0) obtained from the fit to the multiple-entry measurements [(-0.15, -1.08) for SM, (-1.09, 0.79) for JH and (-0.54,-2.24) mm for SB, on average] are similar to those obtained from single-entry measurements: (-0.28, -1.29) for SM, (-1.01, 0.77) for JH, and (-0.07,-2.24) mm for SB, on average. These peak locations are very close to the horizontal and vertical coordinates chosen for the multiple-entry measurements. The reader should note that even if the sample transverse does not pass through the true peak location, the values of ρ and coordinates of the maximum would be constant, because of the nature of the gaussian function we are using in our analysis19.
Results
Single-entry and multiple-entry directionality measurements
Figure 1 (top panel) shows a typical example of the intensity distribution at the pupil plane for a series of entry pupil positions (subject SM, 0 deg, horizontal axis). Single-entry rhos (ρs) are extracted from images obtained at the optimal entry pupil.
Figure 2 shows various representative examples of multiple-entry reflectometric functions for different subjects and conditions, obtained in separate sessions. Filled symbols represent measurements across the horizontal axis of the pupil, and open symbols measurements across the vertical axis. Each symbol is the average of five measurements, obtained consecutively. Reproducibility within a session is high (in most cases the error bars, !1 standard error of the mean, are smaller than the size of the symbols). The measurements were well fit by gaussian functions (represented by dashed and dotted lines for the fits to measurements across the horizontal and vertical axes respectively). Variability is highest for red illumination, probably because the background component is generally higher than the guided component, and slight errors in the determination of the background have a higher impact on the estimate of the total guided component than for green illumination.
Variation of rho value as a function of retinal eccentricity and wavelength
As our model predicts15, multiple-entry reflectometric functions are systematically broader (lower rho) than single-entry reflectometric distributions (higher rho). The difference is particularly clear at 2 deg, although at the center of the fovea, it is significant in 2 of the 3 subjects. Also, the increase of rho with retinal eccentricity is steeper for single-entry measurements than for multiple-entry measurements. Figure 3 (a) shows rho derived from the two types of measurements as a function of retinal eccentricity for the three subjects, using green (543 nm) light. Filled symbols represent single-entry measurements (ρs) and open symbols represent multiple-entry measurements (ρm). Each symbol is the average of estimates of rho values from at least two sessions. For the sake of clarity, error bars are not shown in the figure, but standard errors never exceeded 0.0166 mm-2 for ρs and 0.0108 mm-2 for ρm. Table 1 shows ρs and ρm values for 0 and 2 deg for the three subjects, as well as the standard deviations of the measurements.
Table 1. ρdir and ρind for 0 and 2 deg retinal eccentricity (3 subjects and average) for the experiments in the present study.
ρdir (±1 SD) |
ρind (±1 SD) |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
0 deg | 2 deg | 0 deg | 2 deg | |
JH | 0.136±0.004 | 0.197±0.004 | 0.111±0.012 | 0.102±0.015 |
SM | 0.110±0.014 | 0.182±0.023 | 0.089±0.010 | 0.106±0.017 |
SB | 0.083±0.002 | 0.189±0.020 | 0.082±0.015 | 0.093±0.007 |
average | 0.110 | 0.189 | 0.094 | 0.100 |
Single-entry measurements as a function of wavelength (in JH, 0 and 2 deg) are markedly broader for red light than for green light, as shown elsewhere15. However, multiple-entry measurements in the same subject are not significantly different for the two wavelengths. Figure 3 (b) shows rho value as a function of retinal eccentricity for the two wavelengths for subject JH. Circles represent single-entry measurements, and squares multiple-entry measurements. Filled symbols stand for 543 nm and open symbols stand for 632 nm. Whereas for single-entry measurements, rho (ρs) decreases as the wavelength increases, for multiple-entry measurements rho (ρm) is very similar for the two wavelengths.
Discussion
Our model predicts that the spatial distribution of light guided back through the pupil is controlled by two factors: waveguide properties and interference effects arising from the retinal cone mosaic15. We have separated these two factors by measuring cone directionality using two different approaches (measuring the spatial intensity distribution at the plane of the pupil for the optimal entry position, and computing the total amount of light guided as a function of entry pupil position). As expected, multiple-entry measurements produce lower estimates of rho values than single-entry measurements, since the former should depend only on the waveguide properties, whereas the latter incorporates the additional contribution of scattering.
In the next section we discuss the potential effects of photoreceptor disarray and show that they are not responsible for the differences that we find between the two measurements.
Effect of cone-photoreceptor disarray
Photoreceptor disarray can potentially broaden the multiple-entry measurements with respect to single-entry measurements: if there are cones at an angle away from the group mean, those cones will return relatively more light at an angle of illumination along their own axis, corresponding to an entry pupil away from the optimal location. As a consequence the multiple-entry measurements will broaden. Previous measurements show that cone disarray is small in the human fovea and parafovea17,18. McLeod17 calculated that the acceptance angle of an individual cone is only two percent less than the global tuning of a group of photoreceptors when a realistic amount of disarray is considered.
To calculate possible influence of disarray on our measurements, we simulated the differences between single-entry and multiple-entry measurements in the presence of cone disarray (assuming the extreme case of no scattering being involved). Figure 4 shows the rational that we followed in the computer simulation. We assume the cones with a given distribution of orientations20 or distribution of pupil intercepts, using the same terminology used by McLeod17: Gdis. In our calculations, Gdis is a gaussian function, but for simplicity in Fig. 4 we represent it as three delta functions. The emission angle of a single cone is also represented as a gaussian function at the pupil plane, Gwg. For convenience, we suppose that both Gdis and Gwg are concentric with the geometrical center of the pupil: 10−ρdis(x2+y2) and 10−ρwg(x2+y2). We then compute the intensity distribution at the pupil plane for different entry pupils across the horizontal axis, by performing the following convolution: (Gwg(x − xi) × Gdis)⊗ Gwg, where xi represents the entry pupil position of the illuminating beam. Gdis is multiplied by Gwg since the amount of light captured by cones not oriented along the direction of illumination is proportional to both the number of cones oriented toward a specific location at the pupil and the intensity of the ‘tail’ of the angular tuning at that particular pupil location. Significant disarray predicts two findings18 (Fig. 4): First, the location of the peak of the intensity distribution should move toward the direction of the entry pupil location; second, the total guided intensity vs entry pupil position should be broader than the spatial intensity distribution from a single image. Both functions are affected by cone disarray, but single-entry measurements to a lesser extent than multiple-entry measurements. From our simulations, and given values of Gdis and Gwg, we computed the displacement of the peak position, and rho values for single- and multi-entry measurements (ρs and ρm respectively).
The actual measured variation in the location of the maximum in light exiting the eye was smaller than 0.5 mm when the entry location varied as much as 2.5 mm from the optimal entry pupil. We found similar peak displacements for both 0 and 2 deg retinal eccentricities, and for both horizontal and vertical axes.
The simulations did not find a combination of waveguide properties and disarray that are consistent with both the peak displacements and broadening. We simulated single- and multiple-entry functions for different cases. For example, if we choose ρwg=0.22 mm-2 and ρdis=1.3 mm-2, we can compute single-entry intensity distributions (ρs=0.19 mm-2) to match our experimental estimates (ρs=0.189 mm-2 on average) at 2 deg; these conditions generate also peak displacements close to our experimental estimates (∼0.5 mm). However, for these conditions the computed values for multiple-entry measurements (ρm=0.18 mm-2) do not match the experimental multiple-entry estimates (ρm=0.106 mm-2). In the example just shown, disarray would only be responsible for ∼10% of the broadening that we found experimentally. If we choose ρwg=0.12 mm-2 and ρdis=0.8 mm-2, we can compute multiple-entry functions (ρm =0.10 mm-2) that match our experimental estimates for ρm and peak displacements. However, the predicted single-entry rho value (ρs =0. 11 mm-2) is much lower than the experimental value. We conclude that another factor (scattering) must be involved in the differences between single- and multiple-entry measurements.
Reflectometric measurements and the Stiles-Crawford effect
Various studies show that estimates of the point in the pupil toward which the photoreceptors are optimally aligned agree well with measurements of the psychophysical Stiles-Crawford effect10,12,13,14. However, the directionality factor rho is consistently higher for reflectometric measurements than for psychophysical measurements. He et al.14 compared in the same subjects the directionality factors obtained using Burns et al.'s reflectometric technique11 (single-entry measurements, using the terminology coined in the current paper) with psychophysical Stiles-Crawford measurements (using a criterion of flicker thresholds, and bleaching adaptation fields to avoid self-screening and to isolate as much as possible the waveguide properties). On average rho for reflectometric measurements was ∼2% rho for the psychophysical measurements at the center of the fovea, and ∼4% at -2 deg retinal eccentricity. Several causes are pointed to in that study to explain the narrowing of the single-entry reflectometric measurements14. Gorrand and Delori21 proposed a model that explained the differences between psychophysical and reflectometric measurements, that suggested that some modes guided within the photoreceptors are poorly excited backwards, giving rise to a narrowing of the reflected distribution. While in some animals it is possible to image waveguides in vivo22, in humans it has not been possible; so although a very plausible explanation, this hypothesis is not proven. Our model also predicted that single-entry reflectometric measurements should be broader than the psychophysical SCE, since according to the model, the former are affected by both waveguide properties and scattering, and the latter are affected primarily by the waveguide component15. According to our reasoning, multiple-entry measurements are also not affected by scattering from the cones, and depend only on the waveguide properties of the photoreceptors. A question then arises: do the Stiles-Crawford effect and the multiple-entry reflectometric measurements reflect the same waveguide properties of the photoreceptors?
Results from the three types of measurements for the three subjects are displayed in Figure 5. Fig. 5 (a), (b) and (c) show rho value as a function of retinal eccentricity for the single- and multiple-entry reflectometric measurements from the present study, and SCE measurements from He et al.14, for subjects JH, SM and SB (who participated in both studies). ρSCE values are more similar to ρm than to ρs values. The slight increase of SCE directionality with retinal eccentricity is consistent with previous data in the literature23,24 and with predictions from waveguide models25,15. For all subjects both SCE and multiple-entry measurements vary with eccentricity more slowly than do the single-entry measurements. Fig. 5 (d) compares rho values as a function of wavelength for the three types of measurements, for subject JH. Whereas single-entry measurements decrease markedly with wavelength, both SCE measurements and multiple-entry measurements barely change. We had shown in a previous paper that the decrease of ρs with λ supports the presence of scattering15,16: the scattering component should decrease with wavelength since it is inversely proportional to λ2. In addition, our finding that both ρSCE and and ρm change minimally with wavelength is consistent with the waveguide properties of the photoreceptors. Waveguide models predict a non-systematic variation of rho as a function of wavelength, very dependent upon the specific cone dimensions and indices of refraction25,26. In the bleached state27 the SCE change little with wavelength28. Also, recent computer simulations of light propagation within the vertebrate retinal rod have shown a flat dependence of directionality with wavelength29.
Despite the parallel behavior of SCE and multiple-entry measurements as a function of retinal eccentricity and wavelength, we found that SCE functions are still broader than multiple-entry functions (ρSCE is shifted toward lower values). Such a discrepancy cannot be explained by our model. Chen and Makous30 suggested cross-talk (light escaping one cone and being absorbed in adjacent cones) as a potential reason for the broadening of the SCE with respect to the acceptance angle of a single cone. However, as it has been pointed out14, 30, cross-talk is more likely at the foveal center where the cones are more tightly packed than at 2 deg, where the cone coverage is smaller. From Gorrand and Delori's model21 differences could arise also from differences between the angular dependence of the absorption and emission of light by the photoreceptors. Finally, as demonstrated in the previous subsection, cone-photoreceptor disarray seems a negligible factor. Also, it should contribute to a broadening of both SCE and multiple-entry measurements.
Estimates of cone spacing
Our model predicts that ρs =ρwg + ρscatt, where ρwg is the angular tuning of the cone (assuming no disarray) and ρscatt is given by scattering theory16. Since, assuming no disarray, ρm =ρwg., the scattering component can be derived combining the two techniques: ρscatt =ρs-ρm. As we mentioned above, the scattering component (ρscatt) and row-to-row cone spacing (s) are linked by the following expression15: , where k= 1.20753, assuming cone apertures equal to 80% of the cone spacing, f is the axial length of the eye and λ is the wavelength used in the experiments (0.543 μm and 0.632 μm). Figure 6 shows the calculated row-to-row cone spacing as a function of retinal eccentricity for the three subjects. The axial length of the eye was measured for each of the three subjects using A-scan ultrasonography (f= 25.30 mm for SB, f=25.52 mm for SM and f=23.60 mm for JH). Filled symbols represent cone spacing estimates for the three subjects with green light. Open symbols are independent measurements using red light, for JH. The solid line represents an average across our subjects and the dashed line is an average from Curcio et al.'s data31, very close to our average data. Note that, as from previous anatomical31 and in vivo32 cone spacing estimates, intersubject variability is higher at the foveal center than at 2 deg eccentricity.
In its present form, obtaining cone spacing is slower and more indirect than alternative imaging techniques32,33,34. However, the fact that we obtain consistent estimates of cone spacing provides further support for the waveguide-scattering model, suggesting that more information can be extracted from reflectometric directionality measurements than had been assumed.
Interpretation of other cone directionality techniques using the waveguide-scattering model
While reflectometric techniques used to measure cone directionality are all based on the measurement of light reflected back from the cones with bleached photopigment, the particular design of each approach yields somewhat different rho directionality factors. By taking into account the particular optical configuration and experimental conditions, our model allows us to improve the comparability of the different techniques.
Figure 7 compares the pupil configurations for Gorrand and Delori's10, de Lint et al.'s12, van Blockland's9 and the single- and multiple-entry techniques presented in the current paper. The sampled retinal area is 1 deg in our measurements as well as in Gorrand and Delori's; 1.5 deg in van Blokland's and 2 deg in de Lint et al.'s35. Wavelengths are also comparable: 543 nm in Gorrands and Delori's and ours; 514 nm in Lint et al.'s and 568 nm in van Blokland's.
Gorrand and Delori10 scanned the exit and entry circular pupils across the eye's pupil. De Lint et al.12 also used a double scanning configuration, with the exit pupil being a half aperture of bigger radius than Gorrand and Delori's (2 mm instead of 1 mm); however they only scanned across the horizontal axis and measure the intensity distribution at a plane conjugate to the retina instead of in the pupil plane. These two techniques should yield similar rho values (as in fact they do in different sets of subjects). Apart from the difference in the exit pupil sizes and distance between entry and exit pupils, the main difference must arise from the size of the sampled retina: de Lint's et al. averaged over 2 deg, providing higher estimates of rho value35. Compared to our measurements, both approaches represent intermediate conditions between single- and multiple-entry measurements (ignoring the effects of instrumental anisotropy, and design details such as finite size of the sampling exit pupil aperture) the directionality is given by ρs +ρm. That is, according to our model these two techniques are affected twice by the waveguide properties (owing to the double scanning) as well as by cone spacing. Increasing the exit pupil aperture should decrease rho –with the limiting case being a sampling aperture which fills the dilated pupil at all locations, as we have in our multiple-entry measurements. Van Blokland9 scanned the exit pupil with a 1.2 mm pupil aperture while the illumination beam entered the eye through a fixed location. This configuration is equivalent to our single-entry measurements. In our case, we image the pupil all at once, whereas van Blokland obtained sequential measurements along one axis.
Table 2(a) shows simulated rho values derived from the measurements on our subjects (using the rho values of Table 1) for other group's configurations: Gorrand and Delori's10, de Lint et al12, and van Blokland9. Table 2(b) shows the corresponding rho values reported from these groups9,10,12. We have used our fitted rhos and translated the maximum intensities to the center of the pupil. In addition, we simulated Gorrand and Delori's technique10 only for a scan along the horizontal axis instead of scanning the entire pupil. The predictions from the simulations of the results from techniques (Table 2a) agree well with the experimental findings (Table 2b). As expected, the simulations of Gorrand and Delori's measurements10 are slightly narrower than the simulations for deLint et al.'s measurements12 (unlike the experimental measurements, which are most likely narrowed by the increase in retinal area) and both sets of data are narrower than our single- and multiple-entry measurements, as well as van Blockland's9.
Table 2. (a) Simulated ρGorrand-Delori, ρdeLint et al and ρvanBlokland (0 and 2 deg) based on results from our experiments (3 subjects and average).
simulated ρGorrand-Delori | simulated ρdeLint et al | simulated ρvanBlokland | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
0 deg | 2 deg | 0 deg | 2 deg | 0 deg | 2 deg | |
JH | 0.250 | 0.298 | 0.229 | 0.261 | 0.134 | 0.189 |
SM | 0.202 | 0.288 | 0.187 | 0.256 | 0.198 | 0.176 |
SB | 0.175 | 0.281 | 0.165 | 0.246 | 0.083 | 0.182 |
average | 0.209 | 0.289 | 0.194 | 0.254 | 0.108 | 0.182 |
Table 2 (b) Experimental ρ values reported from Gorrand and Delori (1995), deLint et al. (1997) and van Blokland (1986) | ||||||
experimental ρGorrand-Delori | experimental ρdeLint et al | experimental ρvan Blokland | ||||
0 deg | 2 deg | ∼0 deg35 | 2 deg | 0 deg | 2 deg | |
average | 0.204 | 0.279 | 0.226 | not reported | 0.103 | not measured |
Conclusions
From the single- and multiple-entry reflectometric measurements of cone directionality we can conclude the following:
Single-entry reflectometric measurements (based on a single image) depend on waveguide properties and scattering from the photoreceptors.
Multiple-entry reflectometric measurements (based on a series of images with different pupil entry locations) depend primarily on the waveguide properties of the cones, and thus are fit by broader functions than the single-entry measurements.
The differences between single- and multiple-entry measurements cannot be accounted for by photoreceptor disarray.
The estimates of cone spacing obtained by applying the model to the two sets of measurements agree well with anatomical data.
Multiple-entry reflectometric measurements are more similar to Stiles-Crawford measurements than single-entry reflectometric measurements. They follow similar dependencies with retinal eccentricity and wavelength. However, the Stiles-Crawford functions are still broader than the multiple-entry functions.
The waveguide-model can be used to explain differences between estimates of cone directionality obtained with different reflectometric techniques.
Acknowledgments
We thank Ji Chang He for his help as an experimenter and as a subject, and Dennis Donovan for performing the ultrasonography. This research was supported by National Institutes of Health, grant EY-04395, DOE-DE-FG-02-91ER61229, and by support of the Massachussets Lions Research Foundation. S. Marcos was supported by a Human Frontier Science Program Postdoctoral Fellowship, LT542/97.
References and Notes
- 1.Stiles WS, Crawford BH. The luminous efficiency of rays entering the eye pupil at different points. Proc R Soc London B. 1933;112:428–450. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Enoch JM, Bedell HE. The Stiles-Crawford effects. In: Enoch JM, Tobey FL, editors. Vertebrate Photoreceptor Optics. Springer Series in Optical Sciences; Heidelberg: 1981. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Applegate RA, Lakshminarayanan V. Parametric representation of Siles-Crawford functions: normal variation of peak location and directionality. J Opt Soc Am A. 1993;10:1611–1623. doi: 10.1364/josaa.10.001611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Vos JJ, Huigen A. A clinical Stiles-Crawford apparatus. Am J Optom Arch Am Acad Optom. 1962;39:68–76. doi: 10.1097/00006324-196202000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Fankhauser F, Enoch JM, Cibis P. Receptor orientation in retinal pathology. Am J Optom Physiol Opt. 1978;55:807–812. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Smith VC, Pokorny J, Diddie KR. Color matching and Stiles-Crawford effect in central serous detachment repair. Mod Probl Opthalmol. 1978;19:284–295. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Pokorny J, Smith VC, Johnston PB. Photoreceptor misalignment accompanying a fibrous scar. Arch Opthalmol. 1979;97:867–869. doi: 10.1001/archopht.1979.01020010425003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Fitzgerald CR, Birch DG, Enoch JM. Functional analysis of vision in patients following retinal detachment repair. Arch Opthalmol. 1980;98:1237–1244. doi: 10.1001/archopht.1980.01020040089012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Van Blockland GJ. Directionality and aligment of the foveal photoreceptors assessed with light scattered from the human fundus in vivo. Vision Res. 1986;26:495–500. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(86)90192-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Gorrand JM, Delori FC. A reflectometric technique for assessing photoreceptor alignment. Vision Res. 1995;35:999–1010. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)00203-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Burns SA, Wu S, Delori FC, Elsner AE. Direct measurement of human cone-photoreceptor alignment. J Opt Soc Am A. 1996;12:2329–2338. doi: 10.1364/josaa.12.002329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Delint PJ, Berendschot TTJM, van Norren D. Local photoreceptor alignment measured with a scanning laser opthalmoscope. Vision Res. 1997;37:243–248. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(96)00118-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Burns SA, Elsner AE, Gorrand JM, Kreitz MR, Delori FC. Noninvasive Assessment of the Visual System Vol 1 of 1992 OSA Technical Digest Series. Optical Society of America; Washington D.C.: 1992. Comparison of reflectometric and psychophysical measures of cone orientation; pp. 160–163. [Google Scholar]
- 14.He JC, Marcos S, Burns SA. Comparison of cone directionality measured using psychophysical and reflectometric techniques. submitted to J Opt Soc Am A. doi: 10.1364/josaa.16.002363. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Marcos S, Burns SA, He JC. A model for cone directionality reflectometric measurements based on scattering. J Opt Soc Am A. 1998;15:2012–2022. doi: 10.1364/josaa.15.002012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Beckmann P, Spizzino A. The Scattering of Electromagnetic Waves from Rough Surfaces. Pergamon; New York: 1963. [Google Scholar]
- 17.MacLeod DIA. Directionally selective light adaptation: a visual consequence of cone disarray? Vision Res. 1974;14:369–378. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(74)90235-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Burns SA, Wu S, He JC, Elsner AE. Variations in photoreceptor directionality across the central retina. J Opt Soc Am A. 1997;14:2033–2040. doi: 10.1364/josaa.14.002033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Lakshminarayanan V, Enoch JM. Shape of the Stiles-Crawford function for traverses of the entrance pupil not passing through the peak of sensitivity. Am J Optom Physiol Opt. 1985;62:127–128. doi: 10.1097/00006324-198502000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Safir A, Hyams LJ. Distribution of cone orientations as an explanation of the Stiles-Crawford effect. J Opt Soc Am. 1969;59:757–765. doi: 10.1364/josa.59.000757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gorrand JM, Delori FC. A model for assessment of cone directionality. J Mod Opt. 1997;44:473–491. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Li G, Zwick H, Elliott R, Akers A, Stuck BE. Mode structure alterations in normal and laser exposed vertebrate photoreceptors in the small high numerical aperture of the snake. Technical Digest Optical Society of America Annual Meeting; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Westheimer G. Dependence of the magnitude of the Stiles-Crawford effect on retinal location. J Physiology. 1967;192:309–315. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1967.sp008301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Enoch JM, Hope GM. Directional sensitivity of the foveal and parafoveal retina. Invest Ophthalmol Visual Sci. 1973;12:497–503. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Snyder AW, Pask CL. The Stiles-Crawford effect -explanation and consequences. Vision Res. 1973;13:1115–1137. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(73)90148-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Enoch JM. Optical properties of the retinal receptors. J Opt Soc Am. 1963;53:71–85. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Miller ND. The changes in the Stiles-Crawford effect with high luminance adapting fields. Am J Optom Arch Am Acad Optom. 1964;41:599–608. doi: 10.1097/00006324-196410000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Starr SJ. Ph D dissertation. University of Chicago; Chicago, Illinois: 1977. Effect of luminance and wavelength on the Stiles-Crawford effect in dichromats. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Piket-May MJ, Taflove A, Troy JB. Electrodynamics of visible light interactions with the vertebrate retinal rod. Opt Lett. 1993;18:568–570. doi: 10.1364/ol.18.000568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Chen B, Makous W. Light capture by human cones. J Physiol (London) 1989;190:583–593. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1989.sp017678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Curcio CA, Sloan KR, Kalina RE, Hendrickson AE. Human photoreceptor topography. J Comp Neurol. 1992;292:497–523. doi: 10.1002/cne.902920402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Marcos S, Navarro R, Artal P. Coherent imaging of the cone mosaic in the living human eye. J Opt Soc Am A. 1996;13:897–905. doi: 10.1364/josaa.13.000897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Miller DT, Williams DR, Morris GM, Liang J. Images of the cone photoreceptors in the living human eye. Vis Res. 1996;36:1067–1079. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00225-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Wade AR, Fitzke FW. High-resolution imaging of the human cone photoreceptor mosaic using a laser scanning ophthalmoscope. Invest Opthl Vis Sci (suppl) 1998;39:204. [Google Scholar]
- 35.In de Lint et al's instrument (Ref. 12), the sampled retinal area is in fact given by the angular pixel size in the SLO images. However, their processing includes pixel smoothing (10 % 10), and final rho values are given after subsequent spatial average across the 2 deg central region.