Abstract
Preschool-aged children (n = 58) were randomly assigned to receive small group instruction in letter names and/or sounds or numbers (treated control). Alphabet instruction followed one of two approaches currently utilized in early childhood classrooms: combined letter name and sound instruction or letter sound only instruction. Thirty-four 15 minute lessons were provided, with children pre- and post-tested on alphabet, phonological awareness, letter-word identification, emergent reading, and developmental spelling measures. Results suggest benefits of combined letter name and sound instruction in promoting children’s letter sound acquisition. Benefits did not generalize to other emergent literacy skills.
Keywords: Emergent literacy, alphabet knowledge, letter-sound correspondence, preschool intervention
The acquisition of alphabet knowledge, or knowledge of letter names and corresponding sounds, is an important accomplishment in children’s early literacy development (Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998) and recognized as the strongest predictor of later reading ability (Hammill, 2004; National Research Council, 1998; Scarborough, 1998; Schatschneider, Fletcher, Francis, Carlson, & Foorman, 2004; Share, Jorm, Maclean, & Matthews, 1984). Young children with high levels of letter name knowledge tend to develop better reading skills, and children who demonstrate extremely low letter name tend to experience difficulties in reading acquisition (National Research Council, 1998; O’Connor & Jenkins, 1999; Snowling, Gallagher, & Frith, 2003; Torppa, Poikkeus, Laakso, Eklund, & Lyytinen, 2006).
Letter sound knowledge is essential in learning to read alphabetic languages like English (Adams, 1990; Ehri, 1987, 1997, 1998). Consequently, alphabet knowledge is an important component of early literacy instruction (Justice, Pence, Bowles, & Wiggins, 2006), and its development is incorporated into a number of recent federal educational initiatives (e.g., Head Start, Administration for Children and Families, 2003; Reading First, U. S. Department of Education, 2002; Early Reading First, U. S. Department of Education, 2003) and state curriculum frameworks (e.g., Florida Department of Education, n. d.; Massachusetts Department of Education, 2001).
Unfortunately, little is known regarding how alphabet knowledge is best imparted. Early studies have been criticized based on research design and ecological validity (see Ehri, 1983) and more recent studies have generally combined alphabet training with other types of instruction such as phonological awareness (e.g., Roberts, 2003). The National Early Literacy Panel (2008) identified only three studies that investigated the impact of alphabetic instruction by itself. Curricula designed to facilitate early literacy skills vary considerably in how alphabet knowledge is taught (Justice et al., 2006), especially with respect to whether instruction targets letter names and sounds or only letter sounds.
Letter Name and Sound Instruction
Although knowledge of letter sounds would seem to be more important for learning to read and write than knowledge of letter sounds, strong predictive relations with later literacy skills have been found for knowledge of letter names as well as letter sounds (e.g., Scarborough, 1998; Schatschneider et al., 2004). Letter naming abilities have been found to uniquely predict emergent and later reading skills once letter sound knowledge was controlled (e.g., Burgess & Lonigan, 1998; Lonigan, Burgess, Anthony, & Barker, 1998; Schatschneider et al., 2004). Many early childhood curricula, in fact, include a letter naming component (e.g., Open Court, Bereiter et al., 2003; Creative Curriculum, Heroman & Jones, 2004; Land of the Letter People, 1996). Letter name knowledge also may help acquire letter sound knowledge (Evans, Bell, Shaw, Moretti, & Page, 2006; Justice et al., 2006; McBride-Chang, 1999; Piasta, 2006; Share, 2004; Treiman, Berch, & Weatherston, 1993; Treiman et al., 1994; Treiman, Tincoff, Rodriguez, Mouzaki, & Francis, 1998; Treiman, Weatherston, & Berch, 1994).
Support for the idea that letter name knowledge is used to acquire letter sound knowledge is provided by the letter name structure effect (e.g., Evans et al., 2006; Justice et al., 2006; McBride-Chang, 1999; Piasta, 2006; Treiman et al., 1998). Some letters like B and F include cues to letter sounds (i.e., /b/and/f/) in their names. Other letters have names that do not provide cues to their sounds, like W and H. When children’s patterns of alphabet acquisition are examined, a letter name structure effect is apparent: letter names and sounds are most likely to be known for letters that include cues to their sounds as opposed to letters with unrelated names and sounds (Justice et al., 2006; McBride-Chang, 1999; Piasta, 2006; Treiman et al., 1998). Additionally, in studies of acquiring knowledge of the Hebrew alphabet, Share (2004) found that children were more likely to learn the sounds of pseudoletters when they had been previously taught a pseudoletter name linked to its corresponding sound, and Levin and colleagues (Levin, Shatil-Carmon, & Asif-Rave, 2006) found that children who were taught letter names attempted to extrapolate letter sounds from this knowledge.
Letter Sound Only Instruction
Despite the above evidence, others have postulated that letter name learning holds no benefit for letter sound acquisition (for discussion, see Adams, 1990, pp. 350–355; Groff, 1984; McGuinness, 2004). Proponents of this view have suggested that letter name knowledge is merely correlated with letter sound knowledge and literacy outcomes (Foulin, 2005; Groff, 1984). Studies that compare countries in which letter names are taught (e.g., Israel, United States) to countries that only teach letter sounds (e.g., United Kingdom) do not find differences in literacy outcomes by age 7 or 8 (Caravolas, Hulme, & Snowling, 2001; Ellefson, Treiman, & Kessler, in press; Levin et al., 2006). Others have argued not only that letter name knowledge does not facilitate letter sound acquisition and early literacy, but that knowledge of letter names actually interferes with the development of these skills (Groff, 1984; McGuinness, 2004). Consequently, some early childhood curricula refer to letters only by sounds (e.g., Jolly Phonics, Jolly Learning Ltd., n.d.; Montessori; Montessori & Gutek, 2004), and some educators within the deaf community object to letter name instruction for children with hearing impairments (e.g., the program Children’s Early Intervention for Speech-Language-Hearing [CEI], Tade & Vitali, 1994; see also Easterbrooks & Estes, 2007; Steinberg & Harper, 1983).
The purpose of the present study was to directly compare the effectiveness of combined letter name and sound instruction with letter sound only instruction for teaching alphabet knowledge and other emergent literacy skills. Additionally, we compared both types of alphabet instruction to a treated control in which number instruction was provided. Based on the extent literature, we hypothesized that combined letter name and sound instruction would prove the most beneficial for children’s learning. Benefits for both types of alphabet instruction over the treated control were also expected.
Method
Participants
English-speaking, preschool-aged children were recruited from classrooms serving 3- and 4-year olds at four private childcare centers in a mid-sized Southeastern city.. Children for whom parental consent was granted (N = 113) were screened to determine their uppercase letter naming abilities (task described in Measures section). Children producing fewer than eight letter names were considered eligible for study participation. The cutoff of eight letter names was determined through examination of a large database containing information on over 1,000 3- and 4-year old children living in the same mid-sized Southeastern city. Knowledge of eight or fewer letter names represented below-average letter knowledge for children of this age (C. Lonigan, personal communication, June 13, 2007).
Sixty-three children met the screening criteria and were invited to further participate in the study. Five eligible children did not complete the study. Two children moved, two children refused to complete study assessments, and one child displayed serious behavior problems that disallowed participation in the small groups. Sample characteristics for the 58 children completing the intervention are presented in Table 1. Forty-eight percent of the sample was female. The majority of children in the sample (72.41%) were Caucasian, with 13.79% African-American and 13.79% of other ethnicities (Indian, Hispanic, or Asian). Fifty percent of the children had parents who had graduated from college, and 8.62% of parents had additional postgraduate training. Parents of three children failed to provide their education information.
Table 1.
Full sample (n = 58) |
LNLS condition (n = 20) |
LS condition (n = 20) |
Number condition (n = 18) |
||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
α | M | SD | Range | M | SD | Range | M | SD | Range | M | SD | Range | |
Screening | |||||||||||||
LN productiona | .65 | 3.53 | 2.70 | 0 – 8 | 3.65 | 2.80 | 0 – 8 | 3.40 | 2.50 | 0 – 8 | 3.56 | 2.96 | 0 – 8 |
LN recognitiona | .72 | 5.21 | 3.50 | 0 – 14 | 5.05 | 3.82 | 0 – 14 | 5.35 | 3.54 | 1 – 12 | 5.22 | 3.28 | 0 – 12 |
Pretest | |||||||||||||
Parent education | 5.53 | 1.12 | 1.00 – 7.00 | 5.73 | 0.88 | 4.00 – 7.00 | 5.14 | 1.54 | 1.00 – 7.00 | 5.71 | 0.71 | 4.50 – 7.00 | |
Age (years)b | 3.77 | 0.62 | 2.77 – 5.00 | 3.70 | 0.65 | 2.77 – 5.00 | 3.78 | 0.56 | 2.79 – 4.86 | 3.83 | 0.66 | 2.97 – 4.95 | |
LS productiona | .78 | 0.95 | 1.88 | 0 – 9 | 0.90 | 2.02 | 0 – 7 | 1.05 | 2.06 | 0 – 9 | 0.89 | 1.57 | 0 – 5 |
LS recognitiona | .79 | 2.17 | 2.85 | 0 – 15 | 2.30 | 3.73 | 0 – 15 | 2.05 | 2.67 | 0 – 12 | 2.17 | 1.95 | 0 – 8 |
LWid W score | .58 | 310.19 | 17.23 | 276 – 340 | 310.10 | 13.68 | 293 – 336 | 308.95 | 16.32 | 276 – 340 | 311.67 | 22.01 | 276 – 340 |
LWid SS | 96.98 | 9.61 | 72 – 114 | 97.65 | 9.54 | 76 – 110 | 96.53 | 8.34 | 78 – 114 | 96.72 | 11.32 | 72 – 111 | |
Voc W score | 42.36 | 10.40 | 13 – 67 | 43.20 | 9.13 | 27 – 60 | 40.30 | 10.48 | 13 – 55 | 43.72 | 11.80 | 24 – 67 | |
Voc SS | 98.29 | 11.36 | 64 – 125 | 100.40 | 9.80 | 85 – 119 | 95.50 | 10.80 | 64 – 108 | 99.06 | 13.42 | 77 – 125 | |
PA raw score | .86 | 10.95 | 5.34 | 0 – 23 | 11.20 | 5.15 | 1 – 22 | 10.60 | 5.88 | 0 – 23 | 11.06 | 5.20 | 2 – 21 |
PA SS | 92.72 | 12.89 | 69 – 124 | 94.35 | 10.97 | 72 – 110 | 91.40 | 13.83 | 69 – 120 | 92.39 | 14.26 | 72 – 124 | |
Number ID | .85 | 2.84 | 3.10 | 0 – 10 | 2.75 | 3.19 | 0 – 9 | 2.50 | 2.78 | 0 – 8 | 3.33 | 3.43 | 0 – 10 |
Posttest | |||||||||||||
LN productiona | .91 | 8.84 | 6.52 | 0 – 25 | 10.40 | 8.22 | 0 – 25 | 7.35 | 5.27 | 0 – 16 | 8.78 | 5.51 | 0 – 20 |
LN recognitiona | .91 | 11.03 | 6.98 | 0 – 25 | 12.00 | 8.42 | 1 – 25 | 9.90 | 6.34 | 0 – 23 | 11.22 | 6.05 | 2 – 24 |
LS productiona | .90 | 3.03 | 4.42 | 0 – 20 | 4.35 | 5.98 | 0 – 20 | 2.60 | 3.44 | 0 – 15 | 2.06 | 3.00 | 0 – 9 |
LS recognitiona | .89 | 5.10 | 5.35 | 0 – 23 | 6.35 | 7.24 | 0 – 23 | 4.45 | 4.31 | 0 – 19 | 4.44 | 3.73 | 0 – 12 |
LWid W score | .71 | 321.10 | 18.49 | 264 – 359 | 321.60 | 21.36 | 276 – 359 | 319.80 | 13.43 | 293 – 336 | 322.00 | 20.79 | 264 – 345 |
LWid SS | 99.88 | 10.34 | 73 – 115 | 100.70 | 12.95 | 73 – 115 | 99.20 | 7.12 | 88 – 113 | 99.72 | 10.63 | 75 – 115 | |
PA raw score | .82 | 13.28 | 4.43 | 3 – 27 | 13.90 | 4.23 | 7 – 21 | 12.80 | 4.46 | 3 – 21 | 13.11 | 4.79 | 7 – 27 |
PA SS | 97.48 | 11.46 | 72 – 126 | 101.25 | 9.09 | 85 – 115 | 95.15 | 12.48 | 72 – 123 | 95.89 | 12.18 | 79 – 126 | |
Emergent Readingc | .00 | 6.26 | 1.94 | 2 – 10 | 6.50 | 1.79 | 3 – 10 | 6.00 | 1.75 | 2 – 9 | 6.28 | 2.35 | 2 – 10 |
DSTd | .35 | 0.33 | 0.78 | 0 – 3 | 0.45 | 1.00 | 0 – 3 | 0.30 | 0.80 | 0 – 3 | 0.22 | 0.43 | 0 – 1 |
Number ID | .88 | 5.19 | 4.11 | 0 – 16 | 4.85 | 3.54 | 1 – 11 | 3.75 | 3.40 | 0 – 11 | 7.17 | 4.81 | 0 – 16 |
Gain | |||||||||||||
LN productiona | 5.31 | 4.98 | −2 – 17 | 6.75 | 6.32 | −2 – 17 | 3.95 | 4.35 | −2 – 12 | 5.22 | 3.59 | −1 – 12 | |
LN recognitiona | 5.82 | 5.47 | −3 – 17 | 6.95 | 6.19 | −1 – 17 | 4.55 | 5.76 | −3 – 16 | 6.00 | 4.12 | −1 – 15 | |
LS productiona | 2.09 | 4.01 | −5 – 20 | 3.45 | 5.40 | 0 – 20 | 1.55 | 2.35 | −3 – 6 | 1.17 | 3.43 | −5 – 8 | |
LS recognitiona | 2.93 | 4.21 | −4 – 21 | 4.05 | 5.74 | 0 – 21 | 2.40 | 2.66 | −2 – 7 | 2.28 | 3.92 | −4 – 12 | |
LWid W score | 10.91 | 18.05 | −41 – 54 | 11.50 | 19.55 | −29 – 54 | 10.85 | 16.40 | −12 – 44 | 10.33 | 19.03 | −41 – 38 | |
PA raw score | 2.33 | 4.48 | −9 – 12 | 2.70 | 4.65 | −9 – 11 | 2.20 | 4.30 | −6 – 11 | 2.06 | 4.72 | −8 – 12 | |
Number IDe | 2.34 | 2.21 | −1 – 10 | 2.10 | 1.37 | −1 – 5 | 1.25 | 1.45 | −1 – 4 | 3.83 | 2.85 | −1 – 10 |
Notes. LNLS = letter name and letter sound; LS = letter sound; LN = letter name; LWid = Woodcock-Johnson Letter-Word
Identification subtest; SS = standard score (M = 100, SD = 15); Voc = Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test; PA = Test of Preschool Emergent Literacy Phonological Awareness subtest; Number ID = number identification subtest; DST = Developmental Spelling Test.
Maximum score = 26.
Years of parent education scored according to the Hollingshead (1975) index.
Maximum score = 17.
Maximum score = 30.
Maximum score = 16.
To better ensure equivalency of groups on initial alphabet knowledge at the start of the study, children were blocked within-classroom with respect to their letter knowledge before being randomly assigned to one of the three intervention conditions (Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002): combined letter name and sound instruction (LNLS), letter sound only instruction (LS), and number instruction. Each condition thus included children of all ability levels and from each participating classroom. Within conditions, children were also randomly assigned to the small groups through which instruction was delivered. In this manner, neither condition nor small groups were confounded with initial alphabet knowledge levels, Centers, teachers, or classes.
Instruction
Curricula consisting of 34 individual lessons delivered to participating children over a 9-week period (8 weeks of instruction plus a week of mid-testing). All instruction was implemented by the researcher and two graduate students as a pull-out program to small groups of 3–5 children. Each implementer provided instruction in all three conditions to avoid confounding intervention condition with implementer.
Lessons were scripted and fidelity of implementation was monitored throughout the course of the study. Audio recordings of 30% of all lessons were reviewed by the researcher, with implementation issues discussed as they arose. Fidelity was assessed on 15% of all lessons by coding audio recordings for the presence or absence of key lesson components. Research assistants blind to the study’s hypotheses individually coded audio recorded lessons. Double-coding of 20% of the fidelity recordings indicated continued high interrater reliability (κ = .93).
LNLS and LS conditions
Lessons followed a similar format in both alphabet instruction conditions. In the LNLS condition, the letter was consistently referred to by its name and linked to its corresponding sound (e.g., the letter C that makes the sound/k/). In the LS only condition, the letter was designated only through reference with its corresponding sound (e.g., the letter that makes the /k/ sound). Both conditions taught the single most common sound associated with individual consonants and short vowel sounds. Thirty-four alphabet lessons were developed: 26 individual letter lessons plus 8 weekly review lessons. All lessons lasted approximately 10–15 minutes. The individual letter lessons focused on a single uppercase letter and sound. To avoid bias due to typical alphabet order (see, e.g., McBride-Chang, 1999), letters were taught following a fixed random sequence (T, J, S, N, M, G, A, V, H, W, P, Z, O, C, Q, E, B, Y, R, K, U, I, F, X, D, L). Sample lessons are available from the first author.
Review lessons were included at the end of each week. These lessons began with a brief review of the letters learned during the week, followed by a shared alphabet book reading. The alphabet book was read with children’s attention specifically drawn to the printed letter forms and their corresponding names and/or sounds, depending on the instructional condition
Control/Number condition
Children in the control condition received number identification instruction. Similar to the alphabet instruction conditions, a curriculum consisting of 34 10–15 minute lessons was provided. Sixteen lessons were devoted to instruction on a particular number, 0–15, with one to three individual number lessons provided each week. To equate on number of lessons and total instructional time, children in the Number condition received 18 review lessons. Review lessons were conducted for every set of two (e.g., 0–1), four (e.g, 0–3), and eight (e.g., 0–7) numbers learned. The last four lessons in the instructional sequence were full reviews of all numbers taught (i.e., 0–15). Number lessons were designed to be as similar as possible to alphabet lessons in format, activities, intensity, and duration.
Measures
Background/demographics
Parent/guardian surveys were distributed with consent forms and used to collect demographic and background information on participating children.
Vocabulary
Vocabulary was measured at the beginning of the study using the Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (ROWPVT; Brownell, 2000). Children are shown color pictures of a number of objects and asked to point to the object named by the assessor
Alphabet knowledge
Letter name and sound production were assessed by asking children to give the name and sound of each uppercase letter as presented on randomly ordered flashcards (Cronbach’s α = .97 and .96 respectively; Wagner et al., 1994; reliabilities for all measures as administered to the present sample are presented in Table 1). Children were given a second opportunity to demonstrate knowledge of the letters they were unable to identify in a recognition task. A sheet containing all 26 uppercase letters in random order was placed in front of the child, and he or she was asked to point to the correct letter as the examiner gave its name or sound.1 Responses for both tasks were scored as correct or incorrect, with only sounds taught in the course of the study considered correct. While composite scores (summed number of correct responses, ranging from 0 to 26) for the production tasks simply represent the number of letters or sounds correctly produced by the child, composite scores for the recognition task represent a more general assessment of the number of letters or sounds produced or recognized by the child (i.e., letters or sounds produced or recognized on either of the two tasks).2 Letters were presented in a different random order for each child, and the order in which the letter name and sound tasks were administered was counter-balanced across children at midtest and posttest. Four composite scores were used in analyses: letter name production, letter name recognition, letter sound production, and letter sound recognition.
Phonological processing skills
Children’s phonological awareness/processing were assessed using the Phonological Awareness subtest from the Test of Preschool Emergent Literacy (TOPEL; Lonigan, Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 2007; α = .86). The Phonological Awareness subtest involves two tasks, phonological elision and phonological blending.
Emergent reading and spelling abilities
Children’s emergent reading and spelling were assessed with two tasks. The first was adapted from the forced-choice word reading task used with preschoolers by Byrne and Fielding-Barnsley (1991, 1993, 1995; Hindson et al., 2005). This task involves presenting children with three printed words (e.g., mat, sat, hat) and asking the child to identify the printed form of a spoken word (e.g., Which one says sat?). Responses to each item were scored as correct (1) or incorrect (0) and summed to create the composite score used in analyses. The original measure correlated .39 to .43 with later kindergarten and grade 1 reading abilities (B. Byrne, personal communication, April 11, 2007) and had an internal consistency of α = .57 (Hindson et al., 2005). Analysis of internal consistency with the present sample indicated problems with reliability (α = 0), due to an average inter-item covariance of zero. Explanations for the low reliability estimate include the forced-choice nature of the task and ability of children to guess correct answers, as well as potential violations of independence of items (Cronbach & Shavelson, 2004) such as fatigue effects anecdotally noted in this sample of young children. Caution is warranted when interpreting results using this measure.
Emergent spelling was assessed through the Developmental Spelling Test (DST; Ball & Blachman, 1991; Blachman, 1994; Blachman, Tangel, Ball, Black, & McGraw, 1999; Tangel & Blachman, 1992). Children are asked to spell five dictated words (lap, sick, train, elephant, pretty). Each word is scored on a scale of 0 (random letter string) to 6 (conventional spelling). Intermediary scores are given for inventive spellings capturing phonologically-relevant information (e.g., L for elephant). A 10-item version of the DST correlated .85 with a standardized spelling measure (Wide-Range Achievement Test - Revised Spelling subtest; Jastak & Wilkinson, 1984) when administered in first grade, and the 5-item DST as administered in kindergarten showed predictive validity with first grade reading skills (r = .61 to .63; Tangel, 1991). The internal consistency for the DST as administered to the present sample was rather low (α = .35), owing to the small number of items and generally poor performance of children on this measure (see descriptive statistics in Table 1). Scores for each of the five words were summed to create the single composite score used in analyses.
Letter-word identification
The Letter-Word Identification subtest of the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement-III (Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001) was used to measure of letter knowledge and word recognition ability.
Number identification
Children’s cardinal number knowledge was assessed through a number identification task, as adapted from Malofeeva et al. (Malofeeva, Day, Saco, Young, & Ciancio, 2004). Children were asked to name each number from 0–15, as presented on flashcards (α = .90; Malofeeva et al., 2004).
Procedures
All assessments were administered by trained research assistants blind to the conditions to which the children were assigned. Assessment batteries were often administered in multiple sessions, depending on the needs of individual children. In August, all children were screened via a letter name production/recognition task. Children meeting screening criteria were pretested in late August/early September, at which time their verbal abilities, letter sound knowledge, phonological processing, and number identification were assessed, and a standardized letter and word identification task was also administered. For progress monitoring purposes, letter name and sound knowledge was reassessed at the mid-point of the instructional intervention; these results are not reported. Posttesting took place during the three weeks following the conclusion of the instructional intervention.
Results
Preliminary Analyses
Descriptive statistics for children’s pretest, posttest, and gain scores in each of the intervention conditions are reported in Table 1. Raw scores were used in all analyses. Preliminary analyses indicated that children in the three conditions did not differ on gender, χ2 (2, N = 58) = 2.156, p = .340, ethnicity (comparing Caucasian, African-American, and Other; χ2 [4, N = 58] = 2.234, p = .693), age, F(2, 55) = 0.217, p = .806, or average parent education, Welch’s test, F(2, 32.64) = 1.092, p = .347. Nor did the screening/pretest scores of children differ across the three conditions (letter name production, F[2, 55] = 0.042, p = .959, letter name recognition, F[2, 55] = 0.036, p = .965, letter sound production, F[2, 55] = 0.043, p = .957, letter sound recognition, F[2, 55] = 0.037, p = .964, number identification, F[2, 55] = 0.349, p = .707, letter-word identification, F[2, 55] = 0.115, p = .892, vocabulary, F[2, 55] = 0.603, p = .551, phonological awareness, F[2, 55] = 0.066, p = .936).
Correlations among measures are provided in Table 2. Scores from pretest to posttest were relatively stable. Alphabet, number identification, and letter-word identification scores were generally positively correlated within assessment points. Vocabulary, phonological awareness, emergent reading, and developmental spelling tended to be unrelated to alphabet measures and number identification, although vocabulary, phonological awareness, and developmental spelling appeared interrelated. Age also tended to correlate with assessment scores and was found to suppress unwanted variance in outcomes in initial analyses. Thus, participant age was centered and used as a covariate in all analyses.
Table 2.
2. | 3. | 4. | 5. | 6. | 7. | 8. | 9. | 10. | 11. | 12. | 13. | 14. | 15. | 16. | 17. | 18. | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Age (years) | .360** | .310* | −.065 | .126 | .514*** | .287* | .543*** | .475*** | .258** | .235** | .266* | .190 | .245** | .416** | .283* | .412** | .436*** |
2. T1 LN production | -- | .916*** | .486*** | .500*** | .248** | .608*** | .211 | .588*** | .707*** | .704*** | .511*** | .485*** | .573*** | .289* | .083 | .240** | .540*** |
3. T1 LN recognition | -- | .575*** | .626*** | .192 | .593*** | .231** | .554*** | .618*** | .636*** | .470*** | .492*** | .590*** | .193 | .103 | .212 | .479*** | |
4. T1 LS production | -- | .886*** | .032 | .240** | .140 | .107 | .346** | .455*** | .417** | .479*** | .338** | .084 | −.169 | −.072 | .072 | ||
5. T1 LS recognition | -- | .165 | .319* | .216 | .320* | .444*** | .532*** | .523** | .596*** | .487*** | .053 | −.030 | −.018 | .289* | |||
6. T1 Voc W score | -- | .323* | .495*** | .261* | .207 | .196 | .227** | .141 | .205 | .469** | .093 | .277* | .289* | ||||
7. T1 LWid W score | -- | .172 | .491*** | .501*** | .528*** | .433*** | .363** | .492*** | .255** | .159 | .097 | .420** | |||||
8. T1 PA raw score | -- | .165 | .063 | .033 | .220** | .111 | .222** | .593** | .159 | .210 | .032 | ||||||
9. T1 Number ID | -- | .583 | .587*** | .405** | .407** | .444*** | .165 | .167 | .311* | .848*** | |||||||
10. T2 LN production | -- | .939*** | .616*** | .611*** | .712*** | .100 | .251** | .065 | .544*** | ||||||||
11. T2 LN recognition | -- | .669*** | .703*** | .701*** | .137 | .171 | .088 | .580*** | |||||||||
12. T2 LS production | -- | .925*** | .438*** | .164 | .030 | .037 | .327* | ||||||||||
13. T2 LS recognition | -- | .462*** | .090 | -.009 | .034 | .371** | |||||||||||
14. T2 LWid W score | -- | .126 | .200 | −.034 | .414** | ||||||||||||
15. T2 PA raw score | -- | .077 | .272* | .145 | |||||||||||||
16. T2 Emergent reading | -- | .059 | .136 | ||||||||||||||
17. T2 DST | -- | .286* | |||||||||||||||
18. T2 Number ID | -- |
Note. T1 = screening or pretest; LN = letter name; LS = letter sound; Voc = Woodcock-Johnson Vocabulary subtest; LWid = Woodcock-Johnson Letter-Word Identification subtest; PA = Test of Preschool Emergent Literacy Phonological Awareness subtest; Number ID = number identification subtest; T2 = posttest; DST = Developmental Spelling Test.
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
Fidelity
On average, implementers delivered the instructional lessons with 97.71% fidelity (SD = 5.87). Letter names were erroneously given in less than 3% of all LS only lessons, with the letter name used once during three lessons and three times during one review lesson. Planned pairwise comparisons using Dunnett’s T3 procedure (Maxwell & Delaney, 2004) indicated no significant differences in fidelity ratings among the intervention conditions (t[40.237] = 0.516, p = .938 for LNLS versus LS conditions, t[40.237] = −1.684, p = .272 for LNLS versus Number conditions, t[40.237] = −2.506, p = .051 for LS versus Number conditions). Although fidelity ratings were high across all three implementers (100.00%, 98.72%, and 93.75%), the implementer with the lowest rating demonstrated significantly lower fidelity than the other two implementers, t(24.267) = 3.678, p = .003 and t(24.267) = −2.676, p = .033, who did not differ from one another, t(24.267) = 1.709, p = .282. The same implementer also erred more frequency in providing letter names during LS only lessons (4) than the other implementers (1 each). Given the differences in fidelity ratings, dummy codes representing implementer were used as fixed effect covariates in all analyses.
Nested nature of the data
Potential impacts of children’s classrooms, teachers, and schools were not confounded with the three conditions in the study, through random assignment at the child level and the block randomization technique. Instruction, however, was delivered within the context of small groups. Hierarchical linear models (HLM; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002), nesting children within groups, indicated minimal shared variance among groups for all outcomes except letter sound recognition (p = .012). Between-group variance ranged from less than 1% to 19.17%. Adding dummy codes representing instructional implementer reduced all shared group variance to non-significant levels (4.95% or less for all outcomes except emergent reading [10.65%]). Given this finding and use of random assignment in forming the groups themselves, all analyses were conducted at the child level, with implementer treated as a fixed effect, to preserve power.
Impact of Alphabet Instruction
A series of 3 (condition) × 2 (time) repeated measures ANOVAs were used to compare pretest to posttest gains on alphabet knowledge, phonological processing, and letter-word identification outcomes (see Table 1 for gain scores).3 Planned interaction contrasts were used to compare pretest to posttest gains among the three instructional conditions. Posttest emergent reading and spelling scores were compared using one-way ANOVAs. Again, planned contrasts were used for individual comparisons among conditions. Given the directional nature of hypotheses regarding the impact of instruction on literacy-related outcomes (i.e., LNLS > LS > Number), effects of condition were tested using one-tailed significance values (Knotternerus & Bouter, 2001; Pillemer, 1991). Furthermore, given recent attention to the value of effect sizes (Wilkinson & APA Task Force on Statistical Inference, 1999) together with the limited power of the small-scale study, we also examined effect size magnitude for all contrasts of interest.4
Alphabet knowledge
Main effects of time indicated that children made significant gains from pretest to posttest for all measures of alphabet knowledge, F(1, 52) = 37.881, 49.382, 20.717, 30.967 for letter name production, letter name recognition, letter sound production, and letter sound recognition, respectively, all ps < .001. Interaction contrasts indicated differential gains in letter name production between children in the LNLS condition and those in the LS condition, F(1, 52) = 3.129, p = .041, favoring the LNLS condition (d = 0.53). No differences in letter name production gains emerged when the two alphabet instruction conditions were compared to the Number condition, (F[1, 52] = 0.973, p =.164, d = 0.19 for LNLS versus Number conditions, and F[1, 52] = 0.489, p = .756, d = −0.30 for LS versus Number conditions), nor did any of the conditions differ in comparison of letter name recognition gains (F[1, 52] = 1.696, p = .099, d = 0.41 for LNLS versus LS conditions, F[1, 52] = 0.462, p =.250, d = 0.11 for LNLS versus Number conditions, and F[1, 52] = 0.315, p = .711, d = −.029 for LS versus Number conditions).
Significant differences among conditions were found for the measures of letter sound knowledge. Specifically, children in the LNLS condition learned more letter sounds than children in the Number condition (F[1, 52] = 5.997, p = .009, d = 0.47 for letter sound production; F[1, 52] = 3.097, p = .042, d = 0.29 for letter sound recognition); a similar trend favoring the LNLS condition over the LS condition for letter sound production was also apparent, F(1, 52) = 2.767, p = . 051, d = 0.51. Letter sound recognition gains of children in the LS condition did not significantly differ from those of children in either the LNLS or Number conditions (F[1, 52] = 1.349, p = .125, d = 0.34 for LNLS versus LS and F[1, 52] = 0.421, p = .260, d = 0.04 for LS versus Number), nor did letter sound production gains significantly differ from gains of children in the Number condition, F(1, 52) = 0.736, p = .197, d = 0.14. Note that only the letter sound production contrast between the LNLS and Number conditions remained significant after application of a linear step-up procedure to control the Type I error rate (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995; Kesselman, Cribbie, & Holland, 1999).
Emergent literacy skills
Children demonstrated significant pretest to posttest gains on the Letter-Word Identification subtest, F(1, 52) = 3.490, p = .034, but their learning did not differ among instructional conditions, F(1, 52) = 0.024, p = .439, d = 0.04 for LNLS versus LS conditions, F(1, 52) = 0.017, p = .448, d = 0.02 for LNLS versus Number conditions, F(1, 52) = 0.077, p = .391, d = 0.03 for LS versus Number conditions. Children’s phonological abilities increased from pretest to posttest, F(1, 52) = 5.746, p = .010, but, again, these gains were not differential across conditions, F(1, 52) = 0.041, p = .420, d = 0.10 for LNLS versus LS conditions, F(1, 52) = 0.000, p = .500, d = 0.05 for LNLS versus Number conditions, F(1, 52) = 0.032, p = .429, d = 0.03 for LS versus Number conditions. Contrasts on the emergent reading and developmental spelling tasks showed no significant differences among conditions (for emergent reading, F[1, 52] = 0.961, p = .166, d = 0.28 for LNLS versus LS conditions, F[1, 52] = 0.072, p =.395, d = 0.11 for LNLS versus Number conditions, and F[1, 52] = 0.443, p = .254, d = −0.14 for LS versus Number conditions; for developmental spelling, F[1, 52] = 0.728, p = .199, d = 0.17 for LNLS versus LS conditions, F[1, 52] = 1.065, p =.153, d = 0.29 for LNLS versus Number conditions, and F[1, 52] = 0.047, p = .291, d = 0.12 for LS versus Number conditions).
Number identification
Children made significant pretest to posttest gains in number identification, F(1, 52) = 49.157, p < .001. Interaction contrasts indicated that children in the Number condition learned significantly more numbers than children in either the LNLS or LS conditions, F(1, 52) = 7.682, p = .004, d = 0.22 and F(1, 52) = 16.123, p < .001, d = 0.71, respectively, and these differences remained significant after application of the linear step up procedure to control the error rate. Number learning did not differ between the two alphabet instruction groups, F(1, 52) = 1.740, p = .193, d = 0.26 (two-tailed).
Discussion
In employing two methods of imparting alphabet knowledge to preschool-aged children, this small-scale training study examined the short-term impact of alphabet instruction on letter learning and emergent literacy skill development. The results, although largely exploratory in nature, provide preliminary evidence concerning benefits of combined letter name and sound instruction for alphabet knowledge acquisition.
Impact on Alphabet Outcomes
As supported by the extent literature, we hypothesized that children receiving letter name and sound instruction would show the greatest advantages in alphabet learning. Effect size estimates supported such an advantage over children in either of the other two study conditions. Statistical results showed that these children did, in fact, learn to produce more letter sounds than children in the treated control, along with a trend towards a similar benefit over those in the letter sound only condition. Letter name outcomes also favored the combined letter name and sound condition over the letter sound only condition. Children receiving letter sound instruction and those in the treated control generally did not differ on alphabet outcomes.
Children receiving letter name and sound instruction learned an average of two more letter sounds than those provided with letter sound or number instruction. Although a gain of two sounds may be somewhat modest, the effect sizes associated with these results were moderate in size and similar to those found in multi-componential studies of alphabet learning (see National Early Literacy Panel, 2008; Piasta & Wagner, 2008 for review). Such effects would most likely lead to greater gains in studies of greater duration or intensity (e.g., more than a single 15 minute lesson devoted to each letter).
Importantly, the advantage of combined letter name and sound instruction over letter sound only instruction cannot be attributed simply to greater print exposure, instructional time, or instructional intensity, given the design of the study. Rather, the advantage must be attributed to the fact that both letter names and sounds were taught.
Such results are consistent with letter name-to-sound facilitation, expanding the work of Share (2004) and Levin et al. (2006) using a more ecologically-valid paradigm. Nevertheless, we must note that the mechanism for the combined letter name and sound advantage is not entirely clear. First, we recognize that the majority of children across conditions were exposed to letter names at home as well as in their preschool classrooms. This fact does not negate our causal attribution regarding the advantage of combined letter name and sound instruction, given the true experimental design (Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002). However, we cannot rule out alternative mechanisms for the effect including interactions between our supplemental alphabet instruction and the home and classroom literacy practices already in place.
Additionally, we acknowledge that referring to letters only by sounds may have been an “unnatural” form of labeling for these U.S. children. The fact that children receiving letter sound only instruction performed worse on letter name measures than children in the treated control may support this interpretation. Both groups of children presumably experienced equivalent amounts of incidental letter name exposure at home and school, suggesting that the “new” letter sound labels may have led to confusion or interference in alphabet learning. As argued by Groff (1984) and McGuinness (2004), letter sound only instruction may yet represent the most effective means of instruction when working with children for whom letter sounds are a common, natural label.
Finally, the lack of advantage in letter name learning between children in the combined letter name and sound condition and treated control was unanticipated. As we expected that the supplemental letter name instruction would lead to greater letter name learning and thus provide the mechanism for letter name-to-sound facilitation, the fact that the additional instruction did not benefit children beyond the letter name experiences they were already encountering is somewhat perplexing. In sum, although combined letter name and sound instruction may be most beneficial for U.S. preschoolers alphabet learning, more research is necessary to fully understand the mechanisms
Transfer to Other Emergent Literacy Skills
Despite correlational research suggesting causal relations among alphabetic and other emergent literacy skills (e.g., Lonigan, Burgess, & Anthony, 2000; Scarborough, 1998; Schatschneider et al., 2004; Wagner et al., 1994), the present study found no immediate evidence of transfer from alphabet instruction to measures of letter-word identification, phonological awareness, emergent reading, or developmental spelling. Difficulty in generalization to new tasks and measures has often been described in the literature on literacy development (e.g., Brady, Fowler, Stone, & Winbury, 1994; Byrne & Fielding-Barnsley, 1989; Fuchs, Fuchs, & Karns, 2001; Stahl & Fairbanks, 1986; Torgesen et al., 1999), particularly for children with reading difficulties (e.g, Al Otaiba & Fuchs, 2002; Gersten, Fuchs, Williams, & Baker, 2001; Torgesen & Hudson, 2006; Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte, 1997).
A number of factors may have contributed to the lack of transfer. The provided instruction was, by design, limited to the specific domain of letter name and sound knowledge and anticipated to causally impact phonological processing, reading, and spelling development through impacts on letter name and sound knowledge. Tests of causal relations were thus predicated on the success of the alphabet instruction provided. Despite trends favoring the alphabet training conditions, the current study did not produce large, robust gains for children relative to children in the control condition. Such small changes in alphabet knowledge may not be enough to affect changes in phonological processing, reading, or spelling development. Moreover, two of the emergent literacy measures (emergent reading and developmental spelling) proved rather difficult for the current sample of preschoolers, resulting in low reliabilities and warranting caution in their interpretation. Hence, although alphabet knowledge and reading/spelling development may be causal related, the current study and its emergent literacy measures may not have been powerful enough to detect these relations.
Limitations and Future Directions
Several limitations in the present study deserve attention and may be addressed in future work. First, the intensity and duration of the alphabet instruction provided in the current study may have been too modest to produce large gains in children’s emergent literacy skills. Although similar “letter of the day” methods are common in early childhood classrooms (e.g., Dickson, 1998; McGill-Franzen, Lanford, & Adams, 1997; Pressley, Rankin, & Yokoi, 1996; Slavin, 1999; SRA/McGraw-Hill, 2003), the current approach may have been less intense than other early literacy interventions (e.g., Blachman, Ball, Black, & Tangel, 1994; Blachman et al., 1999; Brady et al., 1994; Brennan & Ireson, 1997; Iversen & Tunmer, 1993; Stuart, 1999; Torgesen et al., 1999) and perhaps could have been strengthened through incorporation of additional instructional techniques such as mnemonic devices (Ehri, Deffner, & Wilce, 1984) or letter writing (Aram & Biron, 2004). Future studies might also consider integrating supplemental and classroom instruction, with letter names and/or sounds taught in the explicit daily lesson reinforced throughout the day.
Issues of statistical power must also be noted and attended to in future studies. Although our sample size was based on a priori power analyses and effect sizes in the extent literature (e.g., Levin et al., 2006; McBride-Change, 1999; Share, 2004), actual effects were smaller than anticipated. Correspondingly, observed power was substantially lower than the intended 80% (ranging from .085 to .671 for alphabet outcomes and from .050 to .173 for emergent literacy outcomes). Additional studies producing larger effects or including larger numbers of participants are necessary.
The lack of transfer to emergent literacy skills also deserves further attention. As the ultimate intention of providing effective early alphabet instruction is to impact reading and spelling abilities during the elementary years and beyond, demonstration of such transfer is largely desirable. Although the present study tested only the immediate effects of supplementary alphabet instruction, increased instructional and statistical power in future projects would allow effects beyond preschool to be studied. Continued assessment beyond the early preschool years would permit use of more valid and reliable measures of decoding and spelling, rather than the experimental measures currently utilized which proved problematic for participating children.
Footnotes
Alphabet production and recognition abilities are typically assumed to represent two separate constructs of letter knowledge, as discussed by Dodd & Carr (2003) and reflected in a number of studies of emergent literacy (e.g., Lafferty, Gray, & Wilcox, 2005; Solity, 1996; Stuart, 1999; Sumbler, 1999; Vandervelden & Siegel, 1997; Williams, 1980).
Tasks were administered in this manner to minimize both assessment time and children’s frustration.
Significant positive skew was noted for the letter sound production, letter sound recognition, and developmental spelling tasks (skewness statistic ranging from 1.906 to 2.826). Transformations (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996) with this variable did not significantly improve its distribution; reported analyses were conducted on the untransformed variable and ought to be interpreted cautiously. Given the relative robustness of ANOVA to violations of non-normality (Maxwell & Delaney, 2004), isolation of skew to the LNLS condition, and meaningful metric of these measures (i.e., number of letter sounds known), no further transformations were conducted on the letter sound outcomes, and analyses were conducted on both the original and outlier-adjusted variables. These analyses yielded similar results; thus, only the analyses for the unadjusted variables are reported. Additional assumptions for repeated measures and one-way ANOVAs were satisfied (Maxwell & Delaney, 2004).
With the exception of the emergent reading and developmental spelling measures, on which only posttest data was available, effect sizes were computed to align with interaction contrast results. Thus, effect sizes reflect relative differences among conditions in learning gains as opposed to simple posttest comparisons.
Contributor Information
Shayne B. Piasta, The Ohio State University
David J. Purpura, Florida State University and the Florida Center for Reading Research
Richard K. Wagner, Florida State University and the Florida Center for Reading Research
References
- Adams MJ. Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Administration for Children and Families, Department of Health and Human Services. The Head Start Path to Positive Child Outcomes. 2003 Summer; Retrieved November 12, 2006, from http://www.hsnrc.org/CDI/outcontent.cfm.
- Al Otaiba S, Fuchs D. Characteristics of children who are unresponsive to early literacy intervention: A review of the literature. Remedial and Special Education. 2002;23:300–316. [Google Scholar]
- Aram D, Biron S. Joint storybook reading and joint writing interventions among low ses preschoolers: Differential contributions to early literacy. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2004;19:588–610. [Google Scholar]
- Ball EW, Blachman BA. Does phoneme awareness training in kindergarten make a difference in early word recognition and developmental spelling? Reading Research Quarterly. 1991;26:49–66. [Google Scholar]
- Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B. 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
- Bereiter C, Campione J, Carruthers I, Hirshber J, McKeough A, Pressley M, et al. Open Court Reading Pre-K. 2003 Retrieved June 8, 2008, from https://www.sraonline.com/products.html?PHPSESSID=5f4933c7b29f0ddaf8f04a56eef7f143&tid=16&sid=147.
- Berko-Gleason J. The development of language. Boston: Allyn & Bacon; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Blachman BA. What we have learned from longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading, and some unanswered questions: A response to Torgesen, Wagner, and Rashotte. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 1994;27:287–291. doi: 10.1177/002221949402700503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blachman BA, Ball EW, Black RS, Tangel DM. Kindergarten Teachers Develop Phoneme Awareness in Low-Income, Inner-City Classrooms: Does It Make a Difference? Reading and Writing. 1994;6:1–18. [Google Scholar]
- Blachman BA, Tangel DM, Ball EW, Black R, McGraw CK. Developing phonological awareness and word recognition skills: A two-year intervention with low-income, inner-city children. Reading and Writing. 1999;11:239–273. [Google Scholar]
- Brady S, Fowler A, Stone B, Winbury N. Training phonological awareness: A study with inner-city kindergarten children. Annals of Dyslexia. 1994;44:26–59. doi: 10.1007/BF02648154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brennan F, Ireson J. Training phonological awareness: A study to evaluate the effects of program of metalinguistic games in kindergarten. Reading and Writing. 1997;9:241–263. [Google Scholar]
- Brownell R. Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Burgess SR, Lonigan CJ. Bidirectional relations of phonological sensitivity and prereading abilities: Evidence from a preschool sample. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1998;70:117–141. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1998.2450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byrne B, Fielding-Barnsley R. Phonemic awareness and letter knowledge in the child’s acquisition of the alphabetic principle. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1989;81:313–321. [Google Scholar]
- Byrne B, Fielding-Barnsley R. Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young children. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1991;83:451–455. [Google Scholar]
- Byrne B, Fielding-Barnsley R. Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young children: A 1-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1993;85:104–111. [Google Scholar]
- Byrne B, Fielding-Barnsley R. Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young children: A 2- and 3-year follow-up and a new preschool trial. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1995;87:488–503. [Google Scholar]
- Caravolas Mt, Hulme C, Snowling MJ. The foundations of spelling ability: Evidence from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language. 2001;45:751–774. [Google Scholar]
- Cronbach LJ, Shavelson RJ. My current thoughts on coefficient alpha and successor procedures. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 2004;64:391–418. [Google Scholar]
- Dickson S. Sing, Spell, Read, & Write: PreK Readiness Program. Parsipanny, NJ: Modern Curriculum Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Dodd B, Carr A. Young children’s letter-sound knowledge. Language, Speech, & Hearing Services in Schools. 2003;34:128–137. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2003/011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunn LM, Dunn LM. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. 3. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Easterbrooks SR, Estes EL. Helping deaf and hard of hearing students to use spoken language: A guide for educators and families. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press; 2007. Developing literacy skills in children with hearing losses; pp. 105–129. [Google Scholar]
- Ehri LC. A critique of five studies related to letter-name knowledge and learning to read. In: Gentile LM, Kamil ML, Blanchard JS, editors. Reading research revisited. Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill; 1983. pp. 131–153. [Google Scholar]
- Ehri LC. Learning to read and spell words. Journal of Reading Behavior. 1987;19:5–31. [Google Scholar]
- Ehri LC. Sight word learning in normal readers and dyslexics. In: Blachman BA, editor. Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia: Implications for early intervention. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1997. pp. 163–189. [Google Scholar]
- Ehri LC. Grapheme-phoneme knowledge is essential to learning to read words in English. In: Metsala JL, Ehri LC, editors. Word recognition in beginning literacy. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1998. pp. 3–40. [Google Scholar]
- Ehri LC, Deffner ND, Wilce LS. Pictorial mnemonics for phonics. Journal of educational psychology. 1984;76:880–893. [Google Scholar]
- Eidlitz-Neufeld MR. Early letter form errors as a predictor of later literacy outcomes and the short- and long-term benefits of early instruction in proper letter formation. ProQuest Information & Learning; US: 2003. Unpublished Dissertation/Thesis. [Google Scholar]
- Ellefson M, Treiman R, Kessler B. Learning to label letters by names or sounds: A comparison of England and the United States. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.05.008. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans MA, Bell M, Shaw D, Moretti S, Page J. Letter names, letter sounds, and phonological awareness: An examination of kindergarten children across letters and of letters across children. Reading & Writing. 2006;19:959–989. [Google Scholar]
- Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavioral Research Methods. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146. (in press) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Florida Department of Education. (n. d.) Grade Level Expectations for the Sunshine State Standards: Language Arts, Grades K-2. Retrieved November 12, 2006, from http://www.firn.edu/doe/curric/prek12/pdf/langglek.pdf.
- Foulin JN. Why is letter-name knowledge such a good predictor of learning to read? Reading &Writing. 2005;18:129–155. [Google Scholar]
- Fuchs LS, Fuchs D, Karns K. Enhancing kindergartners’ mathematical development: effects of peer-assisted learning strategies. Elementary School Journal. 2001;101:495–510. [Google Scholar]
- Gersten R, Fuchs LS, Williams JP, Baker S. Teaching reading comprehension strategies to students with learning disabilities: A review of research. Review of Educational Research. 2001;71:279–320. [Google Scholar]
- Groff PJ. Resolving the letter name controversy. Reading Teacher. 1984;37:384–388. [Google Scholar]
- Hammill DD. What We Know About Correlates of Reading. Exceptional Children. 2004;70:453–468. [Google Scholar]
- Heroman C, Jones C. Literacy: The Creative Curriculum Approach. Washington, D.C: Teaching Strategies, Inc; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Hindson B, Byrne B, Fielding-Barnsley R, Newman C, Hine DW, Shankweiler D. Assessment and early instruction of preschool children at risk for reading disability. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2005;97:687–704. [Google Scholar]
- Iversen S, Tunmer WE. Phonological processing skills and the Reading Recovery Program. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1993;85:112–126. [Google Scholar]
- Jastak S, Wilkinson GS. The Wide Range Achievement Test--Revised. Wilmington, DE: Jastak Associates; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Jolly Learning Ltd. (n.d.) Jolly Phonics Parent/Teacher Guide. Retrieved June 8, 2008, from http://www.jollylearning.co.uk/2008%20UK%20%20Guide.pdf.
- Justice LM, Pence K, Bowles RB, Wiggins A. An investigation of four hypotheses concerning the order by which 4-year-old children learn the alphabet letters. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2006;21:374–389. [Google Scholar]
- Kesselman HJ, Cribbie R, Holland B. The pairwise multiple comparison multiplicity problem: An alternative approach to familywise and comparisonwise Type I error control. Psychological Methods. 1999;4:58–69. [Google Scholar]
- Knotternerus JA, Bouter LM. The ethics of sample size: Two-sided testing and one-sided thinking. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2001;54:109–110. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(00)00276-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lafferty AE, Gray S, Wilcox MJ. Teaching alphabetic knowledge to pre-school children with developmental language delay and with typical language development. Child Language Teaching & Therapy. 2005;21:263. [Google Scholar]
- Land of the Letter People. Waterbury, CT: Abrams & Company; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Levin I, Shatil-Carmon S, Asif-Rave O. Learning of letter names and sounds and their contribution to word recognition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2006;93:139–165. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lonigan CJ, Burgess SR, Anthony JL. Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool children: Evidence from a latent-variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology. 2000;36:596–613. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.36.5.596. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lonigan CJ, Burgess SR, Anthony JL, Barker TA. Development of phonological sensitivity in 2- to 5-year-old children. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1998;90:294–311. [Google Scholar]
- Lonigan CJ, Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA. Test of Preschool Emergent Literacy. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Malofeeva E, Day J, Saco X, Young L, Ciancio D. Construction and Evaluation of a Number Sense Test with Head Start Children. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2004;96:648–659. [Google Scholar]
- Mann VA, Foy JG. Phonological Awareness, Speech Development, and Letter Knowledge in Preschool Children. Annals of Dyslexia. 2003;53:149–173. doi: 10.1007/s11881-007-0002-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Massachusetts Department of Education. Massachusetts English Language Arts Curriculum Framework. 2001 June; Retrieved November 12, 2006, from http://www.doe.mass.edu/frameworks/ela/0601.pdf.
- Maxwell SE, Delaney HD. Designing Experiments and Analyzing Data: A Model Comparison Perspective. 2. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- McBride-Chang C. The ABCs of the ABCs: The development of letter-name and letter-sound knowledge. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 1999;45:285–308. [Google Scholar]
- McGill-Franzen A, Lanford C, Adams E. Learning to be literate: A comparison of five urban early childhood programs. Albany, NY: National Research Center on English Learning & Achievement; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- McGuinness D. Early reading instruction: What science really tells us about how to teach reading. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Montessori M, Gutek GL. The Montessori Method: The Origins of an Educational Innovation. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- National Early Literacy Panel. Developing early literacy. Washington, D. C: National Institute for Literacy; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- National Research Council. Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, D. C: National Academy Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- O’Connor RE, Jenkins JR. Prediction of reading disabilities in kindergarten and first grade. Scientific Studies of Reading. 1999;3:159–197. [Google Scholar]
- Piasta SB. Acquisition of alphabetic knowledge: Examining letter- and child-level factors in a single, comprehensive model. Florida State University; Tallahassee, FL: 2006. Unpublished thesis. [Google Scholar]
- Piasta SB, Wagner RK. Developing emergent literacy skills: A meta-analysis of alphabet learning and instruction. 2008 doi: 10.1598/RRQ.45.1.2. Manuscript submitted for publication. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pillemer DB. One- versus two-tailed hypothesis tests in contemporary educational research. Educuational Researcher. 1991;20:13–17. [Google Scholar]
- Pressley M, Rankin J, Yokoi L. A survey of instructional practices of primary teachers nominated as effective in promoting literacy. The Elementary School Journal. 1996;96:363–384. [Google Scholar]
- Raudenbush SW, Bryk AS. Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and Data Analysis Methods. 3. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts TA. Effects of alphabet-letter instruction on young children’s word recognition. Journal of educational psychology. 2003;95:41. [Google Scholar]
- Scarborough HS. Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities. In: Shapiro BK, Accardo PJ, Capute AJ, editors. Specific reading disability: A view of the spectrum. Timonium, MD: York Press; 1998. pp. 75–120. [Google Scholar]
- Schatschneider C, Fletcher JM, Francis DJ, Carlson CD, Foorman BR. Kindergarten Prediction of Reading Skills: A Longitudinal Comparative Analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2004;96:265–282. [Google Scholar]
- Shadish WR, Cook TD, Campbell DT. Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Generalized Causal Inference. New York: Houghton Mifflin; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Share DL. Knowing letter names and learning letter sounds: A causal connection. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2004;88:213–233. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Share DL, Jorm AF, Maclean R, Matthews R. Sources of individual differences in reading acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1984;76:1309–1324. [Google Scholar]
- Slavin RE. Comprehensive approaches to cooperative learning. Theory into Practice. 1999;38:74–79. [Google Scholar]
- Snowling MJ, Gallagher A, Frith U. Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: Individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development. 2003;74:358–373. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.7402003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solity J. Phonological awareness: Learning disabilities revisited? Educational and Child Psychology. 1996;13:103–113. [Google Scholar]
- SRA/McGraw-Hill. Open Court Reading Pre-K. Colubmus, OH: Author; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Stahl SA, Fairbanks MM. The effects of vocabulary instruction: A mode-based meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research. 1986;56:72–110. [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg DD, Harper H. Teaching written language as a first language to a deaf boy. In: Coulmas F, Ehlich K, editors. Writing in Focus. New York: Mounton; 1983. pp. 327–354. [Google Scholar]
- Stuart M. Getting ready for reading: Early phoneme awareness and phonics teaching improves reading and spelling in inner-city second language learners. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 1999;69:587. doi: 10.1348/000709904322848806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sumbler K. Phonological awareness combined with explicit alphabetic coding instruction in kindergarten: Classroom observations and evaluation. University of Toronto; Ontario, Canada: 1999. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. [Google Scholar]
- Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using Multivariate Statistics. 3. New York: HarperCollins; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Tade WJ, Vitali G. Children’s early intervention for speech-language-reading (CEI) East Aurora, NY: Slosson Educational Publications; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Tangel DM. Effect of phoneme awareness training on kindergarten and first-grade children’s invented spelling. Syracuse University; Syracuse, NY: 1991. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. [Google Scholar]
- Tangel DM, Blachman BA. Effect of phoneme awareness instruction on kindergarten children’s invented spelling. Journal of Reading Behavior. 1992;24:233–261. [Google Scholar]
- Torgesen JK, Hudson RF. Reading Fluency: Critical Issues for Struggling Readers. In: Samuels SJ, Farstrup AE, editors. What research has to say about fluency instruction. Newark, DE, US: International Reading Association; 2006. pp. 130–158. [Google Scholar]
- Torgesen JK, Wagner RK, Rashotte CA. Prevention and remediation of severe reading disabilities: Keeping the end in mind. Scientific Studies of Reading. 1997;1:217–234. [Google Scholar]
- Torgesen JK, Wagner RK, Rashotte CA, Rose E, Lindamood P, Conway T, et al. Preventing reading failure in young children with phonological processing disabilities: Group and individual responses to instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1999;91:579–593. [Google Scholar]
- Torppa M, Poikkeus AM, Laakso ML, Eklund K, Lyytinen H. Predicting Delayed Letter Knowledge Development and Its Relation to Grade 1 Reading Achievement Among Children With and Without Familial Risk for Dyslexia. Developmental Psychology. 2006;42:1128–1142. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treiman R, Berch D, Weatherston S. Children’s use of phoneme-grapheme correspondences in spelling: Roles of position and stress. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1993;85:466–477. [Google Scholar]
- Treiman R, Tincoff R, Rodriguez K, Mouzaki A, Francis DJ. The foundations of literacy: Learning the sounds of letters. Child Development. 1998;69:1524–1540. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treiman R, Weatherston S, Berch D. The role of letter names in children’s learning of phoneme-grapheme relations. Applied Psycholinguistics. 1994;15:97–122. [Google Scholar]
- U. S. Department of Education. Guidance for the Reading First Program. Washington, D. C: Author; 2002. Apr, [Google Scholar]
- U. S. Department of Education. Guidance for the Early Reading First Program. Washington, D. C: Author; 2003. Mar, [Google Scholar]
- Vandervelden MC, Siegel LS. Teaching phonological processing skills in early literacy: A developmental approach. Learning Disability Quarterly. 1997;20:63–81. [Google Scholar]
- Venezsky RL. The Curious Role of Letter Names in Reading Instruction. Visible Language. 1975;9:7–23. [Google Scholar]
- Venezsky RL. Harmony and Cacophony from a Theory-Practice Relationship. In: Resnick LB, Weaver PA, editors. Theory and Practice of Early Reading. Vol. 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA. Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology. 1994;30:73–87. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 3. New York: The Psychological Corporation; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Whitehurst GJ, Lonigan CJ. Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development. 1998;69:848–872. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilkinson L APA Task Force on Statistical Inference. Statistical methods in psychology journals: Guidelines and explanations. American Psychologist. 1999;54:594–604. [Google Scholar]
- Williams JP. Teaching decoding with an emphasis on phoneme analysis and phoneme blending. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1980;72:1–15. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woodcock R, McGrew KS, Mather N. Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement. 3. Itasca, IL: Riverside; 2001. [Google Scholar]