Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Jun 16.
Published in final edited form as: J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2007 Mar 12;30(1):53–62. doi: 10.1080/13803390601186684

Elements of attention in HIV-infected adults: Evaluation of an existing model

Andrew J Levine 1, David J Hardy 2,3, Terry R Barclay 3, Matthew J Reinhard 3, Michael M Cole 3, Charles H Hinkin 3,4
PMCID: PMC2886732  NIHMSID: NIHMS197496  PMID: 17852595

Abstract

Because of the multifactorial nature of neuropsychological tests, attention remains poorly defined from a neuropsychological perspective, and conclusions made regarding attention across studies may be limited due to the different nature of the measures used. Thus, a more definitive schema for this neurocognitive domain is needed. We assessed the applicability of Mirsky and Duncan's (2001) neuropsychological model of attention to a cohort of 104 HIV+ adults. Our analysis resulted in a five-factor structure similar to that of previous studies, which explained 74.5% of the variance. However, based on the psychometric characteristics of the measures comprising each factor, we offer an alternative interpretation of the factors. Findings also indicate that one factor, which is generally not assessed in clinical neuropsychology settings, may be more predictive of real-world behaviors (such as medication adherence) than those composed of traditional measures. Suggestions for further research in this important area are discussed.


Deficits in attention are a common consequence of HIV infection (Hardy & Hinkin, 2002; Heaton et al., 1995). The central importance of attentional deficits in the assessment of HIV-related cognitive changes was recognized early on by the NIMH Workshop on Neuropsychological Assessment Approaches, whose members recommended special emphasis on assessing divided and sustained attention as deficits in these abilities may be among the earliest signs of cognitive decline among those with HIV (Butters et al., 1990). Because such deficits may be early indicators of progressing neuropathology, neuropsychological assessment can alert clinicians to begin or alter treatment plans for their patients. Further, deficits in attention have functional significance for the individual, as they can affect capacity to maintain adequate adherence to antiretroviral medication (Hinkin et al., 2002; Levine et al., 2005), to drive (Marcotte et al., 2004), and to perform other activities of daily living (Heaton et al., 2004). Thus, a thorough assessment of attention is an essential component of the neuropsychological evaluation of those with HIV.

Relative to other neuropsychological constructs, such as memory and language, the nature and neuroanatomical substrates of attention are poorly understood. As a consequence, the definition, behavioral markers, and methods used to assess attention have not been well defined. Within the “neuroAIDS” field, two general approaches have been used to investigate attentional functioning. The first draws from cognitive psychology and utilizes information-processing measures to assess specific aspects of attention. In this conceptualization, attention, like memory, is not a single entity but a concept that includes a variety of distinct but categorically related processes (Parasuraman & Davies, 1984). Through this approach, deficits in HIV-infected adults have been shown in several attentional processes, such as divided attention (Hinkin, Castellon, & Hardy, 2000), visuospatial orienting (Martin, Sorensen, Robertson, Edelstein, & Chirurgi, 1992; Maruff et al., 1995), inhibiting a prepotent response (Martin et al., 1992), and preparatory processing (Law et al., 1995) (see Hardy & Hinkin, 2002, for review). The second, with its roots in clinical neuropsychology, considers attention as one of a number of distinguishable cognitive “domains.” In this approach, various neuropsychological measures are grouped based upon a priori theoretical considerations. In the context of the current study, the greatest limitation of this approach has been the lack of consistency in the definition of attention and the measures used to assess it. This is largely due to an inherent limitation of most clinical neuropsychological tests—that is, their multifaceted nature. Unlike most measures used in the cognitive psychology approach, neuropsychological measures usually require multiple cognitive abilities for successful performance. This was demonstrated in the often-cited study by Heaton et al. (1995), in which the investigators used a number of such multifaceted, traditional neuropsychological measures in creating their “Attention Domain,” including Digit Span, Arithmetic, Seashore Rhythm, Speech Sounds, Digit Vigilance Test (time and errors), and Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT). In that study, the authors conducted a principal components analysis (PCA) on the dataset in order to determine the underlying factors common among their various measures. Their analysis revealed two factors believed to be related to attention: “Attention/Speed of Processing” (with highest factor loadings from Digit Symbol, Digit Vigilance time, Trails Making Test Parts A & B) and “Attention/Working Memory” (with highest factor loadings from Digit Span). That study revealed an important limitation with the domain approach—namely, that attentional factors do not necessarily overlap well with predefined domains. Despite this, researchers using traditional neuropsychological measures continue to use the domain approach. The variety of tests comprising attention domains across a sampling of neuropsychological studies is summarized in Table 1. As shown in that table, there is a wide range of measures used to assess attention in the neuroAIDS field. The obvious drawback of this is that conclusions made regarding attention may be limited to the measures used or confounded by various other cognitive processes required by each of the measures. The ultimate consequence is that the effects of HIV upon attention remain unclear.

TABLE 1.

Variations in measures used to assess attention

Study Domain name Measures
Butters et al., 1990 Attention Digit Span (WMS-R)
Visual Span (WMS-R)
Levin, Berger, Didona, & Duncan, 1992 Attention Digit Span (WAIS-R)
Corsi Cube Test (visual span)
Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure, Immediate Recall
Marsh & McCall, 1994 Attention Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test
Trail Making Test (Parts A & B)
Heaton et al., 1995 Attention Digit Span (WAIS–R)
Arithmetic (WAIS–R)
Seashore Rhythm Test
Speech Sounds Perception Test
Digit Vigilance (Errors & Time)
Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test
Brouwers et al., 1997 Attention Digit Span (WAIS–R)
Digit Symbol (WAIS–R)
Trail Making Test (Parts A & B)
Manly et al., 1997 Attention/speed of information processing Digit Span (WAIS–R)
Arithmetic (WAIS–R)
Digit Vigilance (time and no. errors)
Seashore Rhythm Test
Speech Sounds Perception Test
Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test
Nielsen-Bohlman, Boyle, Biggins, Ezekiel, & Fein, 1997 Attention Microcoga (MC)
    Numbers forward
    Numbers reversed
    Alphabet
    Word List
Stern et al., 1998 Attention Adaptive Rate Continuous Performance Testb
Performance Assessment Batteryc
    Letter-matching
    Stroop
    Time-wall
Honn & Bornstein, 2002 Attention Spatial Span (WMS-III)
Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test
Richardson, Morgan, Vielhauer, Buondonno, & Keane, 2003 Working memory Digit Span (WAIS-III)
Spatial Span (WMS-III)
Baldewicz et al., 2004 Attention 2 and 7 Testd
Heaton et al., 2004 Attention/Working memory Digit Span (WAIS-R)
Arithmetic (WAIS-R)
Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test
Hinkin et al., 2004 Attention/Working memory Letter/Number Sequencing (WAIS-III)
Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test
Digit Span (WAIS-III)
Marcotte et al., 2004 Attention/Working memory Letter/Number Sequencing (WAIS-III)
Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test

Note. WMS-R=Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised. WAIS-R=Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised. WMS-III=Wechsler Memory Scale–Third Edition. WAIS-III=Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Third Edition.

In an effort to identify discrete attentional elements, Mirsky, Anthony, Duncan, Ahearn, and Kellam (1991) used exploratory factor analysis to uncover underlying similarities among a variety of commonly used neuropsychological measures, resulting in a multicomponent model of attention. In the original study, four factors were found that explained approximately 80% of the variance across tests. The factors and their functions were described as follows: (a) focus/execute, which is the capacity to selectively attend to a stimuli and execute responses required by the task; (b) encode, the ability to briefly maintain information in memory; (c) shift, or the capacity to shift focus from one stimulus to another; and (d) sustain, the capacity to maintain focus for appreciable length of time. Since that initial study, other researchers have found similar factor structures based on a comparable battery of tests. This has been the case across healthy individuals as well as patients with a variety of neurologic and psychiatric illnesses (Kelly, 2000; Kremen, Seidman, Faraone, Pepple, & Tsuang, 1992; Mirsky & Duncan, 2001; Pogge, Stokes, & Harvey, 1994), although some have found somewhat disparate results when alternative analytic methods were used (Strauss, Thompson, Adams, Redline, & Burant, 2000). More recently, Mirsky and Duncan (2001) reported a five-factor model of attention, with the addition of a stabilize factor consisting of variables from a continuous performance task (CPT) that are related to consistency of responding over time. Recently, our group reported findings of CPT performance among a cohort of HIV+ adults, many of whom were stimulant abusers (Levine et al., 2006). It was found that only certain variables from the CPT, similar to those that comprised Mirsky's stabilize element, were able to discriminate drug users from nonusers. Specifically, the stimulant users had higher rates of omissions and reaction time variability, but these rates did not diverge from those of the nonusers until two or more minutes into the task. Therefore, shorter tasks, such as many traditional clinical neuropsychological measures used to assess attention, would have missed these differences. Further, the groups did not differ on other measures of attention, suggesting that the CPT captures a component of attention (i.e., sustained attention) often not assessed by traditional measures used in current batteries. Finally, data from our laboratory indicate that an attentional factor with high loading from CPT variables is more strongly associated with outcome measures such as medication adherence than are factors of attention composed of other neuropsychological measures. Thus, it behoves clinicians and researchers who work with HIV+ individuals to understand what attentional measures are assessing and to what degree they relate to real-world outcomes.

In the current study, we assessed the validity of Mirsky's model of attention among a cohort of HIV+ adults, many of whom were stimulant (cocaine and methamphetamine) users. The growing prevalence of HIV among stimulant users calls for examining the attentional deficits in this population as such deficits are apparently exacerbated by concomitant drug abuse (Levine et al., 2006; Rippeth et al., 2004). Mirsky's methodology has not been applied to a similar population to the best of our knowledge. Thus, we examined (a) whether similar factors to those in previous studies would emerge in our HIV-infected cohort and (b) the association of such factors with demographic characteristics, virologic factors, and functional measures (e.g., adherence and verbal IQ).

METHOD

Participants

The sample consisted of 104 HIV-infected adults recruited from the Los Angeles area. Participants were enrolled in a longitudinal study examining factors associated with medication adherence among HIV-infected adults. All participants were taking antiretroviral medication, with adherence monitored via the Medication Event Monitoring System, or MEMS cap (Aprex, Union City, CA). Overall adherence rate was expressed as a percentage of prescribed doses taken, according to MEMS cap data. A total of 66 participants (64%) met the Centers for Disease Control (CDC) diagnostic criteria for AIDS (CDC, 1994). Average age in years at study onset was 40.9 (SD=7.4), and average education was 13.1 (SD=1.9) years. A total of 17 (16.3%) participants were female. The sample was ethnically diverse, with 56% African American, 22% Caucasians, 15% Hispanics, 4% Asian or Native American, and 3% multiracial. Viral load was obtained via blood samples. Of the 104 participants, 23 were diagnosed with current stimulant use disorders (cocaine or amphetamine), and 50 with past only, according to DSM-IV diagnoses (Spitzer, Williams, Gibbon, & First, 1992). A total of 31 participants had neither past nor current stimulant use disorders. In addition, self-reported CD4 was obtained from participants. Demographic, behavioral, and virologic characteristics of the sample are shown in Table 2. History of the following medical conditions precluded participation in the study: significant head trauma with loss of consciousness greater than 1 hour, brain-related opportunistic infection (e.g., cryptococcal meningitis, progressive multifocal leukoencephalopathy, and toxoplasmosis), and psychosis. Participants were not excluded if they were current drug users; however, participants were asked to return another time if they were intoxicated at the time of testing as determined via self-report and clinical observation.

TABLE 2.

Characteristics of the study sample

Variable Mean SD % of participants
Agea 40.9 7.4
Educationa 13.1 1.8
Viral Loadb 7,848 47,713
CD4 (self-report)c 459 282
Global Neurocognitive Functioning T-score 44.3 5.7
Verbal IQd 105.4 9.3
BDI-IIe 12.7 8.9
AIDS Diagnosis 63.5
Current Stimulant Use Disorderf 22.0
Female 16.3
Ethnicity
    Caucasian 22.0
    African American 56.0
    Hispanic 15.0
    Asian/Native American 04.0
    Mixed 03.0
a

In years.

b

Median=undetectable.

c

Median=377.

d

Estimated Verbal IQ based on the American New Adult Reading Test (AMNART; Grober & Sliwinski, 1991).

e

Beck Depression Inventory, Second Edition (Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996).

Measures

As part of the study protocol, all participants underwent a psychodiagnostic interview and neuropsychological testing. The former consisted of the Structured Clinical Interview based on the fourth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, or SCID (Spitzer et al., 1992). Mood, psychotic disorders, and drug use modules were administered. Mood was also assessed with the Beck Depression Inventory, Second Edition (BDI-II; Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996). Neuropsychological testing consisted of a comprehensive battery of standardized instruments described elsewhere (Hinkin et al., 2002). For the purposes of the current study, we list in Table 3 those measures from our battery that were used in the statistical factor analysis. In addition, in order to characterize our sample's cognitive ability relative to the general population, a Global Neuropsychological Score for each participant was obtained by averaging T-scores across all tests. Demographically corrected T-scores for all measures were obtained through published normative data. In addition, all participants were administered the Conners’ Continuous Performance Test, Second Edition (CPT-II; Conners, 2000), a computerized vigilance test in which the examinee must press a button whenever they see a letter appear on a computer screen, with the exception of the target letter (X). Specific procedures for this measure were described in a previous paper (Levine et al., 2006). For the purposes of this study, we were interested in the following variables: reaction time, variations in performance over time, rates of omission errors (i.e., false negatives), and rates of commission errors (i.e., false positives). T-scores for these variables were generated by the CPT software based on a normative sample. Finally, a verbal IQ estimate was obtained with the American New Adult Reading Test, or AMNART (Grober & Sliwinski, 1991).

TABLE 3.

Comparison of the Mirsky and Duncan (2001) battery with that of the current study

Proposed factor Mirsky and Duncan (2001) measure Levine et al. measure
Encode Digit Spana Digit Spana
Arithmetica PASATb (Trial 1) Letter/Number Sequencinga
Focus/Execute Digit Symbola Digit Symbola
Stroopc (all trials) Stroop Interferencec
Trail Making Test A & Bd Trail Making Test A & Bd
Letter Cancellationg Symbol Searcha
Shift WCSTe or Reciprocal Motor Programs Test WCSTe (total errors, total conceptual level responses)
Sustain Visual CPTh (Accuracy, RT) Conners’ CPTf (Hit RT, Commissions)
Auditory CPTh (Accuracy, RT)
Stabilize Visual CPT (Variability of RT) Conners’ CPT (Variability, Omissions)
Auditory CPT (Variability of RT)

Note. PASAT=Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test. WCST=Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. CPT=continuous performance task.

Statistical analysis

A principal components analysis with promax rotation was used to explore the factor structure of our attention measures. These measures are listed in Table 3 and overlap considerably with those of Mirsky and Duncan (2001). An eigenvalue cutoff of 1 was the criterion for inclusion as a factor in the final model. The resulting factors were then saved as variables for additional analyses. Pearson's bivariate correlations were determined between the factors and a number of demographic, behavioral, and cognitive variables in order to determine the contribution of the obtained attention elements.

RESULTS

Five factors with eigenvalues greater than 1 were found to explain a cumulative total of 74.5% of variance. The factors structure is presented in Table 4. The factor structure derived from our sample is very similar to that of Mirsky and Duncan (2001). That is, similar measures loaded on similar factors when compared to their model. Factor 1, interpreted by Mirsky as the focus/execute element, included high loadings from both forms of the Trail Making Test, Symbol Search, Digit Symbol, and Stroop Interference and explained a total of 28.4% of the variance. Factor 2, Mirsky's encode element, included Digit Span, Letter–Number Sequencing, and the PASAT, explaining a total of 13.7% of the variance. Factor 3, considered the switch element by Mirsky, included the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) variables and explained 10.8% of the variance. Factor 4, Mirsky's stabilize element, included the CPT variables of total omissions and overall variability, explaining 11.8% of the variance. Finally, Factor 5, or sustain in Mirsky's schema, included CPT variables of total commissions and hit reaction time, explaining 9.8% of the variance. Total number of commission errors was negatively correlated with Factor 5, indicating that faster reaction time was associated with greater rate of false positive responses.

TABLE 4.

Correlations of individual measures with PCA factors

Factor
Measure 1 Focus/execute 2 Encode 3 Switch 4 Stabilize 5 Sustain
Symbol Search .793 .372 .277 .380 .049
Digit Symbol .768 .328 .242 .394 .049
Stroop Interference .641 .177 .099 .404 .238
Trails A .676 –.062 .112 –.106 .109
Trails B .777 .394 .179 .085 .077
Letter/Number Seq .360 .873 .167 .211 .055
Digit Span .177 .903 .138 .059 –.034
PASAT .586 .635 .333 –.052 .056
WCST Errors .228 .169 .954 –.005 .002
WCST Concept Resp .228 .190 .950 .045 .098
CPT Variability .238 .170 .104 .841 .194
CPT Omissions .157 .031 –.064 .848 –.173
CPT Commissions .047 .027 –.004 .304 –.863
CPT Hit Reaction Time .293 .049 .096 .405 .854

Note. PASAT=Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test. WCST=Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. CPT=continuous performance task. Large factor loadings are in bold italic typeface.

Pearson's bivariate correlations revealed a number of modest, positive correlations (Table 5). Factor 4 (sustain) was significantly associated with medication adherence (R=.26, p=.01). Factors 1 and 2 (focus/execute and encode) were associated with education (R=.21, p=.04, R=.27, p=.01, respectively), while age was negatively associated with Factor 3 (switching; R=.20, p=.04). Estimated verbal IQ had its strongest relationship with Factor 2 (R=.58, p < .001) and was also correlated with Factors 1 (R=.26, p=.01) and 4 (R=.26, p=.01). Finally, the BDI was correlated with Factor 5 (R=.21, p=.04).

TABLE 5.

Correlations among attention factors and other variables

Factor
1 Focus/execute 2 Encode 3 Switch 4 Stabilize 5 Sustain
% adherence .037 –.016 .006 .210* .048
Education .212* .274** –.132 .191 .010
Age .135 .067 –.203* –.118 –.142
Verbal IQ .255* .577** .118 .263** –.136
CD4 .007 –.135 –.174 .005 .053
BDI–II –.016 –.089 –.028 .002 .207*

BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory, Second Edition.

*

significant at .05.

**

significant at .001.

Large factor loadings are in bold italic typeface.

DISCUSSION

In the current study, we examined the factor structure of a battery of neuropsychological tests commonly conceptualized as attention measures. Our aims were to assess the applicability of an existing multielement model of attention in a sample of HIV-infected adults, many of whom were drug users, and to determine whether the individual derived factors were associated with a variety of cognitive, behavioral, and virologic variables. This was the first time the model described by Mirsky and colleagues (1991) has been applied to an HIV+ population.

The factor structure based on our data is very similar to that described by Mirsky and Duncan (2001) and others (Kelly, 2000; Kremen et al., 1992; Mirsky & Duncan, 2001; Pogge et al., 1994), in that there were similar loadings of the measures across factors. Five factors resulted from our PCA. The first, similar to the focus/execute element described by Mirsky, consisted primarily of measures requiring speeded visual processing and some type of cognitive operation, such as form discrimination (Symbol Search), visuomotor integration (Digit Symbol), and response inhibition (Stroop Interference). Whether or not focus and execute are accurate descriptors for the underlying similarity among these measures is open to debate. In this case, the interpretation difficulty of such a factor is not due to the multifactorial nature of attention, as tasks with specific processing demands can assess specific aspects of attention. Rather, as mentioned before, the difficulty lies in the multifactorial nature of these tests. Without good (or any) control conditions, it can be tricky to ascertain what links a group of test measures. Perhaps visual processing speed would be a more accurate characterization for this factor, as it was only timed visual measures that comprised this factor. Thus, all measures on this factor shared in common a visual stimulus and administration under the pressure of time. However, CPT reaction time, which also presumably reflects visual processing speed, did not load on this factor. This could be due to the difference in cognitive demands between the CPT and the other measures.

Factor 2, called encode by Mirsky and others, consisted exclusively of verbal tests. These tests required working-memory ability, as well as maintenance of information in a temporary buffer. Such buffers have been called a phonological loop in the past (Baddeley, 2001). Therefore, it is conceivable that this factor could be considered one of working memory and/or basic auditory attention. Indeed, as shown in Table 1, there is a trend among researchers in recent years to include some of the tests that comprised this measure in an “attention/working memory” domain. Thus, according to our results, this domain label may be accurate.

It is important to point out that the inherent characteristics of the tests that comprised Factors 1 and 2 (i.e., visual vs. auditory) may be the primary reason that they appear as different factors. In other words, it may be that the sensory modalities through which these tests are successfully completed are the underlying “factors.” It will be necessary to include cross-modality tests in order to investigate this problem. For example, including a spatial span test, considered to be one of basic attention and working memory, will help determine the nature of these factors.

Factor 3, Mirsky's switch element, was composed solely of the WCST variables, consistent with the previous studies cited above. Switching is synonymous with alternating attention and requires the ability to disengage from one stimulus or mental set and to reengage in another. However, this is an ability that is arguably necessary for successful performance on measures such as Part B of the Trail Making Test. This measure did not load at all on Factor 3. Therefore, it is possible that this factor is reflecting some other ability inherent to the WCST. The WCST itself is a highly complex test with a number of underlying factors according to a recent study (Greve, Stickle, Love, Bianchini, & Stanford, 2005). Thus, it will be necessary to include additional, yet more simple, measures of set shifting in future analyses to confirm the validity of this as the switch factor.

Factor 4, equivalent to Mirsky's stabilize, was comprised of variability and omission variables of the CPT. Based upon the definition provided by Parasuraman and Davies (1984), this appears consistent with sustained attention, or vigilance. This element is of special interest, because it is not commonly assessed psychometrically in neuropsychological evaluations per their definition. Furthermore, common everyday tasks (e.g., driving) require vigilance in addition to other aspects of attention. We have recently shown the importance of the CPT in providing additional information regarding attentional functioning in those with HIV (Levine et al., 2006). Looking specifically at sustained attention among HIV+ individuals, stimulant users were found to have a greater numbers of omission errors and variability in reaction time than had non-drug-users, indicative of impaired sustained attention. No difference was found on a general global neuropsychological ability rating or, importantly, on other tests comprising the attention domain between the groups. That finding underscores the importance of a multifaceted assessment of attention.

Finally, Factor 5 was composed of reaction time and commission errors from the CPT. Not surprisingly, the faster the reaction time in our sample, the greater number of commission errors. This factor was termed sustain by Mirsky and Duncan (2001), and was described as the capacity to maintain a “vigilant attitude” over time. This is differentiated from CPT variables that comprise the stabilize factor, which indicates “consistency or stability with which a person can respond to a designated target stimulus.” Arguably, these are actually two aspects of sustained attention, or vigilance, as defined by Parasuraman and Davies (1984). According to their definition, sustained attention is the ability to maintain a certain level of performance, especially in the ability to detect the occurrence of infrequent or unpredictable events over extended periods of time. Further, demonstration of a vigilance problem requires an interaction among task conditions, such as an incremental decline in response speed or accuracy over time. Therefore, both the stabilize and sustain elements from Mirsky's model (Factors 4 and 5 in our analysis, respectively) may be considered aspects of sustained attention, as both are important for determining a vigilance problem. Alternatively, Factor 5 may also be a reflection of impulsivity, response style (d′), or simply reaction time.

The factors had modest correlations with demographic and behavioral measures. Verbal IQ, as estimated via a reading task, had the strongest correlation with Factor 2. That verbal IQ would be related to Factor 2 is not unexpected considering that it was composed exclusively of verbal tests (Digit Span, PASAT, and Letter–Number Sequencing). Estimated verbal IQ was also mildly associated with Factors 1 and 4. One functional outcome variable, medication adherence, was associated with Factor 4, considered by the authors to reflect sustained attention. Individuals with greater adherence, expressed as a high percentage of prescribed doses that were taken over the course of a 6-month study according to the MEMS cap data, performed with less variability and fewer omission errors on the CPT. Thus there appears to be a parallel between this laboratory measure of inconsistency and omissions and a real-world measure. Finally, depression was correlated with our Factor 5. Our interpretation of this factor as one of impulsivity may be accurate if one considers a common underlying substrate for impulsivity and some aspects of depression. This has in fact been reported by others from our laboratory. Specifically, it was shown that specific items on the BDI cluster together and covary with frontal/executive cognitive abilities (Castellon et al., 2006). In addition, Castellon, Hinkin, and Myers (2000) showed that apathy, a common symptom of depression, is associated with performance on a response inhibition task. Thus, it is conceivable that as depression increased in our sample so did a risky response style, or impulsivity in responding, on the CPT.

There are several limitations to our study that need to be mentioned. First, as in previous studies, two or more variables from a single measure were all that represented a particular factor in some instances. For example, number of errors, number of categories, and number correct from the WCST were the sole variables constituting Factor 3, our equivalent of Mirsky's shift element. Because variables derived from the same measure tend to be highly correlated, this results in multicollinearity of variables and therefore an artificially inflated correlation between them. In future studies, it would be useful to have at least one additional measure to assess the switch, sustain, and stabilize categories. Second, as with the majority of the replication studies described earlier, we employed PCA, which is generally theoretically sound when used as an exploratory method for elucidating patterns of correlations among a set of variables (Tabachnik & Fidell, 1996). However, the degree to which the derived factors represent latent underlying variables or true constructs is uncertain with PCA alone. Further, because we sought to assess the validity of an existing model, it can be argued that a confirmatory approach would have been warranted. PCA is not recommended for use as a confirmatory tool, as there are strict requirements for the data, including very large sample size, use of “marker” variables, and adequate spread in scores on the variables of interest (Tabachnik & Fidell, 1996). Taking a different tact, Strauss et al. (2000) used structural equation modeling in order to as a confirmatory factor analytic approach in their attempt to validate Mirsky's model. Using structural equation modeling and a Mirsky's original four-factor solution, they failed to replicate the earlier findings. However, structural equation modeling also has significant theoretical limitations, and our decision to employ PCA was based on our goal of replicating previous investigations of Mirsky's model. Clearly, continued analysis using a variety of methods and neuropsychological measures are necessary to assess the validity of our interpretation of Mirsky's factors. While data reduction strategies such as PCA are useful as an initial step in uncovering behavioral constructs from performance across a myriad of tests, additional strategies are required to determine whether those factors represent actual endophenotypes with specific anatomical or neural systems. Both Mirsky et al. (1991) and Posner and Dehaene (1994) have suggested that different neural substrates underlie the various elements of their theoretical models. Some support has been established for Posner's model, included functional imaging and genetic association studies (Fan & Posner, 2004; Fan, Wu, Fossella, & Posner, 2001; Fossella et al., 2002). However, no such data are available for models such as Mirsky's. This may be due to the psychometric (multifactorial) nature of Mirsky's model. The research that has grown from Posner's model has generally relied upon a simple visual attention paradigm called the Attention Network Test (Fan et al., 2001), perhaps making physiological and genetic associations more feasible. Therefore, additional effort is required to empirically verify the elements found in this and previous studies of attentional elements derived from traditional neuropsychological tests.

Acknowledgments

The data for this paper were obtained via a grant awarded to Charles Hinkin by NIDA (RO1) DA13799. Dr. Levine is supported by the National Neurological AIDS Bank Grant NS-38841.

REFERENCES

  1. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Author; Washington, DC: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  2. Army Individual Test Battery . Manual of directions and scoring. War Department, Adjutant General's Office; Washington, DC: 1944. [Google Scholar]
  3. Baddeley AD. Is working memory still working? American Psychologist. 2001;56:851–846. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.56.11.851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baldewicz TT, Leserman J, Silva SG, Petitto JM, Golden RN, Perkins DO, et al. Changes in neuropsychological functioning with progression of HIV-1 infection: Results of an 8-year longitudinal investigation. AIDS and Behavior. 2004;8:345–355. doi: 10.1023/B:AIBE.0000044081.42034.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Beck A, Steer R, Brown G. Beck Depression Inventory–Second Edition manual. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1996. [Google Scholar]
  6. Brouwers P, Hendricks M, Lietzau JA, Pluda JM, Mitsuya H, Broder S, et al. Effect of combination therapy with zidovudine and didanosine on neuropsychological functioning in patients with symptomatic HIV disease: A comparison of simultaneous and altering regimens. AIDS. 1997;11:59–66. doi: 10.1097/00002030-199701000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Butters N, Grant I, Haxby J, Judd LL, Martin A, McClelland J, et al. Assessment of AIDS-related cognitive changes: Recommendations of the NIMH Workshop on Neuropsychological Assessment Approaches. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1990;12:963–978. doi: 10.1080/01688639008401035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Castellon SA, Hardy DJ, Hinkin CH, Satz P, Stenquist PK, van Gorp WG, et al. Components of depression in HIV-1 infection: Their differential relationship to neurocognitive performance. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2006;28:420–437. doi: 10.1080/13803390590935444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Castellon SA, Hinkin CH, Myers HF. Neuropsychiatric disturbance is associated with executive dysfunction in HIV-1 infection. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2000;6:373–379. doi: 10.1017/s1355617700633088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention . HIV Counseling, Testing, and Referral: Standards and Guidelines. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; Atlanta, GA: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  11. Cohen RA, Fisher M. Amantadine treatment of fatigue associated with multiple sclerosis. Archives of Neurology. 1989;46:676–680. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1989.00520420096030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Conners C. Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II. Technical guide and software manual. Multi-Health Systems, Inc.; Toronto, Canada: 2000. [Google Scholar]
  13. Fan J, Posner M. Human attention networks. Psychiatrische Praxis. 2004;31(Suppl. 2):S210–S214. doi: 10.1055/s-2004-828484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Fan J, Wu Y, Fossella JA, Posner MI. Assessing the heritability of attentional networks. BMC Neuroscience. 2001;2:14. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-2-14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fossella J, Sommer T, Fan J, Wu Y, Swanson JM, Pfaff DW, et al. Assessing the molecular genetics of attention networks. BMC Neuroscience. 2002;3:14. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-3-14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Greve KW, Stickle TR, Love JM, Bianchini KJ, Stanford MS. Latent structure of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: A confirmatory factor analytic study. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2005;20:355–364. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2004.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Grober E, Sliwinski M. Development and validation of a model for estimating premorbid verbal intelligence in the elderly. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1991;13:933–949. doi: 10.1080/01688639108405109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gronwall D. The psychological effects of concussion. University Press/Oxford University Press; Auckland, New Zealand: 1974. [Google Scholar]
  19. Hardy DJ, Hinkin CH. Reaction time performance in adults with HIV/AIDS. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2002;24:912–929. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.7.912.8391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Heaton RK. The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Psychological Assessment Resources; Odessa, FL: 1981. [Google Scholar]
  21. Heaton RK, Grant I, Butters N, White DA, Kirson D, Atkinson JH, et al. The HNRC 500: Neuropsychology of HIV infection at different disease stages. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 1995;1:231–251. doi: 10.1017/s1355617700000230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Heaton RK, Grant I, Butters N, White DA, Kirson D, Atkinson JH, et al. The impact of HIV-associated neuropsychological impairment on everyday functioning. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society. 2004;10:317–331. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704102130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hinkin C, Castellon S, Durvasula R, Hardy D, Lam M, Mason KI, et al. Medication adherence among HIV+ adults. Neurology. 2002;59:1944–1950. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000038347.48137.67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hinkin CH, Castellon SA, Hardy DJ. Dual task performance in HIV-1 infection. Journal of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology. 2000;22:16–24. doi: 10.1076/1380-3395(200002)22:1;1-8;FT016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hinkin CH, Hardy DJ, Mason KI, Castellon SA, Durvasula RS, Lam MN, et al. Medication adherence in HIV-infected adults: Effect of patient age, cognitive status, and substance abuse. AIDS. 2004;18(Suppl. 1):S19–S25. doi: 10.1097/00002030-200418001-00004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Honn VJ, Bornstein RA. Social support, neuropsychological performance, and depression in HIV infection. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society. 2002;8:436–447. doi: 10.1017/s1355617702813236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kelly TP. The clinical neuropsychology of attention in school-aged children. Child Neuropsychology. 2000;6:24–36. doi: 10.1076/0929-7049(200003)6:1;1-B;FT024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kremen WS, Seidman LJ, Faraone SV, Pepple JR, Tsuang MT. Attention/information-processing factors in psychotic disorders: Replication and extension of recent neuropsychological findings. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1992;180:89–93. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199202000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Law WA, Martin A, Mapou RL, Roller TL, Salazar AM, Temoshok LR, Rundell JR. Working memory in individuals with HIV infection. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1994;16:173–182. doi: 10.1080/01688639408402628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Levin BE, Berger JR, Didona T, Duncan R. Cognitive function in asyptomatic HIV-1 infection: The effects of age, education, ethnicity, and depression. Neuropsychology. 1992;6:303–313. [Google Scholar]
  31. Levine A, Hardy D, Miller E, Castellon S, Longshore D, Hinkin C. The effect of recent stimulant use on sustained attention in HIV-infected adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2006;28:29–42. doi: 10.1080/13803390490918066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Levine A, Hinkin C, Castellon S, Mason K, Lam M, Perkins A, et al. Variations in patterns of highly active antiretroviral therapy (HAART) adherence. AIDS & Behavior. 2005;9:355–362. doi: 10.1007/s10461-005-9009-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Manly JJ, Patterson TL, Heaton RK, Semple SJ, White DA, Velin RA, et al. The relationship between neuropsychological functioning and coping activity among HIV-positive men. AIDS and Behavior. 1997;1:81–91. [Google Scholar]
  34. Marcotte TD, Wolfson T, Rosenthal TJ, Heaton RK, Gonzalez R, Ellis RJ, et al. A multimodal assessment of driving performance in HIV infection. Neurology. 2004;63:1417–1422. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000141920.33580.5d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Marsh VN, McCall DW. Early neuropsychological change in HIV infection. Neuropsychology. 1994;8:44–48. [Google Scholar]
  36. Martin EM, Sorensen DJ, Robertson LC, Edelstein HE, Chirurgi VA. Spatial attention in HIV-1 infection: A preliminary report. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 1992;4:288–293. doi: 10.1176/jnp.4.3.288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Maruff P, Malone V, McArthur-Jackson C, Mulhall B, Benson E, Currie J. Abnormalities of visual spatial attention in HIV infection and the HIV-associated dementia complex. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 1995;7:325–333. doi: 10.1176/jnp.7.3.325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Mirsky A, Anthony B, Duncan C, Ahearn M, Kellam S. Analysis of the elements of attention: A neuropsychological approach. Neuropsychology Review. 1991;2:109–145. doi: 10.1007/BF01109051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Mirsky A, Duncan C. A nosology of disorders of attention. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2001;931:17–32. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05771.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Nielsen-Bohlman L, Boyle D, Biggins C, Ezekiel F, Fein G. Semantic priming impairment in HIV. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 1997;3:348–358. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Parasuraman R, Davies R. Varieties of attention. Academic Press; New York: 1984. [Google Scholar]
  42. Pogge DL, Stokes JM, Harvey PD. Empirical evaluation of the factorial structure of attention in adolescent psychiatric patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1994;16:344–353. doi: 10.1080/01688639408402645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Posner MI, Dehaene S. Attentional networks. Trends in Neuroscience. 1994;17:75–79. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(94)90078-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Powell DH, Kaplan EF, Whitla D, Weinstraub S, Catlin R, Funkenstein HH. MicroCog assessment of cognitive functioning. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1993. [Google Scholar]
  45. Richardson MA, Morgan EE, Vielhauer CA, Buondonno LM, Keane TM. Utility of the HIV dementia scale in assessing risk for significant HIV-related cognitive motor deficits in a high-risk urban adult sample. AIDS Care. 2003;17:1013–1021. doi: 10.1080/09540120500100858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Rippeth J, Heaton R, Carey C, Marcotte T, Moore D, Gonzalez R, et al. Methamphetamine dependence increases risk of neuropsychological impairment in HIV infected persons. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2004;10:1–14. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704101021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Rosvold HE, Mirsky AF, Sarason I, Bransome ED, Beck LH. A continuous performance test of brain damage. Journal of Consulting Psychology. 1956;20:343–350. doi: 10.1037/h0043220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Ruff RM, Evans RW, Light RH. Automatic detection and controlled search: A paper and pencil approach. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1986;62:407–416. doi: 10.2466/pms.1986.62.2.407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Spitzer R, Williams J, Gibbon M, First M. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R (SCID): I. History, rationale, and description. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1992;49:624–629. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820080032005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Stern RA, Arruda JE, Somerville JA, Cohen RA, Boland JR, Stein MD, et al. Neurobehavioral functioning in asymptomatic HIV-1 infected women. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 1998;4:172–178. doi: 10.1017/s1355617798001726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Strauss ME, Thompson P, Adams NL, Redline S, Burant C. Evaluation of a model of attention with confirmatory factor analysis. Neuropsychology. 2000;14:201–208. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.14.2.201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reaction. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1935;8:643–662. [Google Scholar]
  53. Tabachnik BG, Fidell LS. Using multivariate statistics. 3rd ed. HarperCollins College Publishers; New York: 1996. [Google Scholar]
  54. Talland GA. Deranged memory. Academic Press; New York: 1965. [Google Scholar]
  55. Thorne DR, Genser SG, Sing HC, Hegge RW. Walter Reed performance assessment battery. Neurobehavior Toxicology and Teratology. 1985;17:415–418. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–III. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1997. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES