Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Mem Cognit. 2010 Jul;38(5):541–554. doi: 10.3758/MC.38.5.541

Positive and negative remember judgments and ROCs in the plurals paradigm: Evidence for alternative decision strategies

Aycan Kapucu 1, Neil A Macmillan 1, Caren M Rotello 1
PMCID: PMC2887610  NIHMSID: NIHMS168260  PMID: 20551335

Abstract

Using old-new ratings and remember-know judgments we explored the plurals paradigm, in which studied words must be distinguished from plurality-changed lures. The paradigm allowed us to investigate negative remembering, that is, the remembering of a plural-altered study item; capacity for this judgment was found to be poorer than or equivalent to the conventional positive remembering. A response-bias manipulation affected positive but not negative remembering. The ratings were used to construct ROC curves and test the prediction of the most common dual-process theory of recognition memory (Yonelinas, 2001) that the amount of recollection can be independently estimated from ROC curves and from remember judgments. By fitting the individual data with pure signal-detection (SDT) models and dual-process models that combined SDT and threshold components (HTSDT), we identified two types of subjects. For those who were better described by HTSDT, the predicted convergence of remember-know and ROC measures was observed. For those who were better described by SDT the ROC intercept could not predict the remember rate. The data are consistent with the idea that all subjects rely on the same representation but base their decisions on different partitions of a decision space.

Recognition memory is thought by many to be served by two processes: recollection, in which a test item evokes the studied event in context; and familiarity, in which the merestrength of the test item leads to the inference that it was studied. One class of models for recognition experiments, naturally referred to as dual-process, incorporates these two types of analysis; specific models within this class differ in the details of assumed representation and response strategies (Jacoby, 1991; Yonelinas, 1994; Wixted, 2007). Dual-process models correspond to some common intuitions, for example that an item (a face, say) can seem familiar even if it appears in a new context that undermines recollection. The most popular dual-process model (Yonelinas 1994, 1997) assumes that recognition on a given test trial results from either a high-threshold recollection process (in which false alarms do not occur) or a continuous, signal-detection based familiarity process. We term this model HTSDT.i

An alternative class of models imagines that a single strength variable underlies recognition judgments. To account for the dual-process intuition, some models of this type assume that the judgment dimension sums the outputs of continuous familiarity and retrieval processes on each trial (Wixted & Stretch, 2004). The most common single-process model, which we call simply SDT, assumes a Gaussian signal detection representation and response rule. This model can be viewed as a special case of HTSDT in which the high-threshold recollective process has zero sensitivity.

The threshold and SDT assumptions lead to different predictions about the ROC curves generated in a recognition experiment in which subjects provide confidence ratings rather than simple binary responses. Both models yield nonlinear, convex ROCs, but HTSDT predicts a non-zero y-intercept whereas SDT does not. An appealing aspect of the HTSDT approach is that the y-intercept (on probability coordinates) provides a quantitative estimate of the proportion of trials on which recollection occurs.

This calculated recollection rate would be most compelling if it could be shown to correspond to values of recollection accuracy obtained by some other means, and the remember-know paradigm (Tulving, 1985; Gardiner & Richardson-Klavehn, 2000) has often been used to obtain such estimates. In this paradigm, subjects report for each item identified as “old” the subjective basis for their decision: do they “remember” the item from the study trial, that is, have a specific memory of experiencing it; or do they just “know” that it was presented based on its strength. Remember-know judgments are seductive because they promise direct access to the dual-process intuition. Agreement between recollection rates estimated from ROCs and from remember-know judgments would be valuable support for HTSDT.

Yonelinas (2001) measured ROC intercepts and remember rates in separate experiments, and found good agreement between them. Few experiments have examined both measures in the same experiment, but Rotello, Macmillan, Reeder, and Wong (2005) showed that remember rates could be strongly influenced by instructions that left the ROCs unchanged. Their most conservative instructions (argued by HTSDT theorists to be the most appropriate) showed the closest agreement between the remember rates and the ROC intercept; the other conditions showed substantial discrepancies. Clearly an experimental comparison of remember-know and ratings-based estimates of recollection must take biasing instructions into account.

The plurals paradigm

In most recognition experiments with words, the lures used at test are selected randomly from a large set. We chose instead to use lures that differed from targets only in plurality: if frog and computers were studied, then frogs and computer were to be treated as new rather than old items. The appropriate response to a test item depended on the presence or absence of the final s. There are several reasons for using this paradigm to investigate the agreement between the ROCs and remember-know judgments.

First, no one has collected remember-know judgments in the plurals paradigm. When a “remember” judgment is offered in a conventional random-lure experiment a wide range of context information may contribute to the response, but in the plurals paradigm it can only be that single piece of information that distinguishes singular and plural words. This reduction in variability might be expected to produce less noisy data, but in any case a comparison with otherwise similar standard experiments allows us to compare remember-know performance across different sets of lures.

Second, the plurals paradigm allows us to study what might be called “negative remembering.” Typically, remember-know judgments are solicited only following “old” responses, but we asked for them following “new” judgments as well. A remember response to an item thought to be a lure is justified if the subject remembers that the same word with different plurality was studied. For example, if frog was studied and frogs is presented as a lure, a “new, remember” response indicates a strong memory for frog, implying that frogs must not have been studied. To our knowledge, soliciting negative remembering judgments in an item recognition experiment is relatively novel, having been used previously by Migo, Montaldi, Norman, Quamme, and Mayes (2009) and by Jones and Atchley (2006) in studying compound words. An obvious question about this design is whether positive and negative remember rates are of the same magnitude, as would be expected if subjects are equally able to use negative and positive information about plurality. Using confidence ratings and a multinomial (high-threshold) model, Rotello (2000) found that recall-to-accept processing was about twice as likely as recall-to-reject process for plurality-changed stimuli, so we expected more positive than negative remembering under HTSDT assumptions.

A final motivation for using the plurals paradigm is that it has been seen to provide an ideal testbed for model comparison. Parks and Yonelinas (2007, p. 191) argued that “…relational recognition tasks, including source, associative, and plurality-reversed recognition tests, provide a perfect arena for pitting these two models against one another because they call for the retrieval of some specific detail about a test item.” One special aspect of the paradigm is that the ROCs obtained are unusual. Rotello, Macmillan, and Van Tassel (2000) plotted the hit rate for targets vs the false-alarm rate for plural-altered lures and found linear functions; ROCs for targets vs new lures, like those from other experiments with random lures, were curvilinear and consistent with a Gaussian model (see also Rotello, 2000). Heathcote, Raymond, and Dunn (2006) reported curvilinear old-similar ROCs, a result they viewed as a failure to replicate Rotello et al., but Figure 1 shows that the discrepancy is just a matter of degree. Rotello et al.’s ROCs differ slightly but systematically from linearity; those of Heathcote et al. show more curvature, but not as much as is obtained in experiments with random lures. We call ROCs of this intermediate shape “flattened” to stress their relation to those found in standard recognition studies. To provide a theoretical account of the ROC data we used a modeling approach, to be described later.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Old-similar ROCs from Rotello et al. (2000, Figs 3b and 5b) and Heathcote et al. (2006, Fig 6).

The present experiments

We conducted two experiments that differed in their instructions. Proponents of the HTSDT model (Yonelinas, 2001) have argued that remember responses can be expected to reflect recollection only if subjects are instructed to limit them to cases in which they remember specific details about the item’s presentation. In view of Rotello et al.’s (2005) results, we used standard remember-know instructions in one experiment and strongly restrictive instructions in the other. We expected to replicate Rotello et al.’s finding that remember rates agreed more closely with the HTSDT recollection parameter in the latter case. Subjects studied lists of words, and at test had to distinguish those words from similar (plural-altered) words and from entirely new words. They used a rating scale to make this old-new judgment. On all trials, they also contributed a remember-know response; a “remember” following an “old” response meant they remembered studying the word and following a “new” response that they remembered studying the alternate-plurality version of the word.

Method

Participants

Fifty-six undergraduate students at the University of Massachusetts participated (27 in Experiment 1, 29 in Experiment 2) in exchange for a small payment or extra credit in their psychology courses. All were native English speakers.

Stimuli

Three hundred common nouns (mean frequency, 70.6 per million; Kucera & Francis, 1967) and their plurals (e.g., frog-frogs) were selected from the MRC Psycholinguistic Database (Coltheart, 1981). Nouns were 3–12 characters in length and were chosen only if their plural form could be created by adding an “s.”

Design

One word from each of 144 singular-plural pairs was randomly selected, independently for each subject, to form a set of 72 singular and 72 plural words. The selected words were evenly divided into three study lists of 48 critical words each. Four additional words from the word pool were used to fill primacy and recency buffers on each list. Therefore, each study list was composed of 52 words in total. Presentation order was randomized for each list and each participant.

For each study list, the test list was composed of three types of items: Twenty-four randomly-chosen studied words served as targets (twelve singular, twelve plural); another 24 words were presented in the altered plurality (we call these similar lures); and a final 24 words, selected from the remaining 144 singular-plural pairs, were completely unstudied and served as new lures. Test order was randomized for each subject.

In addition, a practice test list comprised six items: two old words, two similar lures, and two new lures. Two of the four primacy words were chosen to serve as old items, the unstudied altered form of the remaining two served as similar lures, and two unstudied words served as new items.

Procedure

Participants studied three lists of 52 words each, at a presentation duration of 3 s. They were told to pay attention to the plurality of each word in preparation for an unspecified memory test. Immediately following each list, participants received a recognition test composed of words that had been studied previously (targets), plural/singular altered forms of the studied words (similar lures) and completely new words (new lures). For each test item, participants gave old/new confidence ratings on a 6-point scale (1 = sure new, 2 = probably new, 3 = maybe new, 4 = maybe old, 5 = probably old, 6 = sure old). Following each old-new judgment, participants were asked to make a binary remember-know judgment for the same test item. They were informed that words were presented ineither singular or plural form, never both.

The standard remember-know instructions of Rajaram (1993) were modified to require a remember-know decision on all trials. If the test item had been judged new, a remember response indicated the recollection of specific information inconsistent with the test item. Participants were given the example that if they could recall studying computers – if they for instance remembered imagining a computer lab full of computers while studying the word computers – they could not have studied computer and should give computer a “new” response (at some confidence level) and a “remember” judgment. In Experiment 2 (but not Experiment 1) participants were in addition told to give a “remember” judgment only if they could recall specific plurality information about the test item.

Results

Before analyzing the data, we eliminated subjects whose accuracy on target and new-lure trials was below 60 percent. This restriction reduced the number of usable subjects from 27 to 20 in Experiment 1 and from 29 to 25 in Experiment 2. The ability of the remaining subjects to distinguish targets from lures (using just the old-new judgment, not the ratings) is summarized in the first three lines of Table 1. Results of the two experiments were quite similar, with an average hit rate of .66, false-alarms to .35 of similar items, and to .21 of new lures. Little effect of the instructional manipulation (which referred directly to remember-know, not old-new, judgments) is in evidence.

Table 1.

Hits, False-alarms, and Remember Rates.

Stimulus Class Experiment 1 Experiment 2 Average
Mean SE Mean SE
Probability of saying “old”
 Targets .67 .026 .65 .023 .66
 Similar lures .37 .044 .34 .030 .35
 New lures .17 .021 .24 .029 .21
Positive remember rate
 Targets .41 .047 .33 .042 .37
 Similar lures .12 .028 .09 .016 .10
 New lures .02 .007 .02 .008 .02
Negative remember rate
 Targets .03 .010 .05 .010 .04
 Similar lures .27 .053 .29 .042 .28
 New lures .12 .027 .06 .017 .09

The remember-know data are also summarized in Table 1. Positive remember rates are reliably higher than negative ones in Experiment 1 (.41 vs .27, t[19] = 2.44, p = .025) and numerically higher in Experiment 2 (.33 vs .29). Apparently subjects were able to report negative remembering, but at least some of them found the “new, remember” response hard to use or understand. The numerically lower positive remember rate in Experiment 2 indicates responsiveness to the more conservative instructions in that experiment; the lack of such an effect for negative remembers may be because using this response already required a rather conservative decision strategy. The overall remember rates are similar to those typically obtained with randomly chosen rather than plural-altered lures. In Dunn’s (2004) database of 400 remember-know studies, the average remember hit rate was .42; in our experiments, the value for positive remembering is .37. We conclude that the presence of plural-altered lures in the test list did not affect the use of positive remember-know judgments by our subjects.

Figure 2 shows ROCs based on data pooled across subjects for both experiments. The form of the curves is consistent with past work: the old-new ROCs display curvature of the sort usually found with such random lures, whereas the old-similar ROCs are flattened. Although it has been common to argue from ROC shape to an underlying model, such inferences face a number of difficulties, and we instead resort to explicit models of the sort outlined earlier. We now describe this modeling effort.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Pooled old-similar and old-new ROCs: (a) Experiment 1, (b) Experiment 2.

Model-fitting analysis

All the models we evaluated are submodels of a general dual-process model that combines categorical recollection and SDT-based familiarity. Our experiments required recognition judgments for three stimulus classes – targets, lures, and plural-altered lures – rather than two. The signal-detection process therefore comprises three distributions corresponding to these classes. Recollection is assumed to occur only for targets and similar items. The most general model, which we call HTSDT Unequal-Variance, has 11 parameters: RO and RN (the proportions of target and similar trials on which correct positive and negative recollection occurs), dTand dL(the means of the target and similar distributions), sTand sL(the standard deviations of the target and similar distributions), and 5 criterion locations. The new-lure distribution has a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1. There are 15 independent data points (5 = 6 – 1 ratings × 3 stimulus types), so the HTSDT Unequal-Variance model has 4 degrees of freedom.

We also considered 5 models nested within HTSDT Unequal-Variance, as shown in Table 2. The HTSDT Equal-Variance model sets sTand sLequal to 1 (the standard deviation of the new-lure distribution). The HTSDT Similar = Lure model is nested within the HTSDT Equal-Variance model, with the added restriction that the means of the similar and new-lure distributions are equal. The HTSDT Similar = Target model is also nested within the HTSDT Equal-Variance model, with the added restriction that the means of the similar and target distributions are equal. The final two models dispense with the recollection process, and are therefore single-process SDT. Either all three variances can be unequal (the SDT Three-Variance model) or sTand sLare set equal ( SDT Two-Variance) Equations expressing data in terms. of model parameters are provided in the Appendix.ii

Table 2.

Models Tested.

Model Parametersa Restriction on Full HTSDT #parsb dfc Nested in
Recollection Familiarity
#1. HTSDT RO, RN dT, dL, sT, sL --- 11 4 ---
Unequal-Variance
#2. HTSDT RO, RN dT, dL sT = sL = 1 9 6 #1
Equal-Variance
#3. HTSDT RO, RN dT sT = sL = 1 8 7 #2
Similar = Lure Means dL = 0
#4. HTSDT RO, RN dT, dL sT = sL = 1 8 7 #2
Similar = Target Means dT = dL
#5. SDT --- dT, dL, sT, sL RO = RN = 0 9 6 #1
Three-Variance
#6. SDT --- dT, dL, sT RO = RN = 0 8 7 #5
Two-Variance sT = sL
a

Equations relating parameters to response rates are given in the Appendix.

b

Number of parameters, including 5 criterion locations.

c

There are 15 independent data cells, so the df is obtained by subtracting the number of parameters from 15.

Quantitative models of memory experiments have almost always been evaluated against group data of the sort shown in Figure 2. The risks in this kind of averaging have long been understood in general and in the context of memory modeling (Estes & Maddox, 2005; Cohen, Sanborn, & Shiffrin, 2008). When two ROC curves are averaged, the shape of the resulting group function may not resemble that of either component. More generally, if Model A provides the best fit for two subjects, Model B may outperform it with the average data. Cohen, Rotello, and Macmillan (2008) observed that in remember-know experiments in which old-new judgments were made prior to remember-know decisions, a version of the dual-process model was often found to fit the group data best even though the individual subjects (or simulated subjects) were best described by a SDT model. Because one interpretation of our models is as distinct, optional decision strategies, we expected that not all subjects would be fit by the same model; in such a situation, analysis of data separately for each subject is essential.

Each of the submodels listed in Table 2 was fit to the data of all subjects whose accuracy on target and new-lure trials was at least 60 percent. To choose the best model of the ratings data, we used the AIC and BIC statistics (Akaike, 1973; Schwarz, 1978), which help to adjust for differing degrees of freedom when comparing non-nested models. Table 3 shows the breakdown of best-fitting models by subject. Across Experiments 1 and 2, an SDT model was best in 22 cases and the HTSDT model in 23 cases according to AIC, 26 and 19 according to BIC. The two partitions are very similar, but they are not identical; which provides the better picture of the distribution of models across subjects?

Table 3.

Number of Subjects Best Fit by Models 1–6 in Experiments 1 and 2, According to AIC and BIC.

Model Experiment 1 Experiment 2
AIC BIC AIC BIC
HTSDT 1 0 0 4 1
Unequal-Variance
2 2 0 2 0
Equal-Variance
3 1 1 1 1
Similar = Lure
4 7 9 6 7
Similar = Target
SDT 5 2 0 2 0
Three- Variance
6 8 10 10 16
Two-Variance
All HTSDT (1, 2, 3, & 4) 10 10 13 9
All SDT (5 & 6) 10 10 12 16

Note: For details of models, see Table 2.

An issue that is not dealt with by the information criteria is model complexity. More complex models can account for a wider variety of empirical outcomes, even when they have the same number of parameters as a less complex model (Pitt, Myung, & Zhang, 2002). Because AIC and BIC only penalize the models for their parameters, not their complexity, they provide imperfect measures for model selection. Cohen, Rotello, and Macmillan (2008) studiedan experimental paradigm that is closely related to the current one: subjects made old-new ratings and remember-know judgments, although the latter were omitted on trials for which the test item was called new. They concluded that some versions of the HTSDT model are more complex than the SDT models and that conclusions based on AIC in that paradigm are biased in favor of the HTSDT model. An optimal criterion is one that takes account of model complexity as well as number of parameters, and for the models studied by Cohen, Rotello, and Macmillan BIC is closer to optimal than AIC. Thus, to the extent that we can generalize from those models to the current set of similar but not identical models, we should put greater faith in the implications of BIC. To be conservative, however, we describe our results both ways. The parameter estimates themselves differ according to which criterion is used, but the overall pattern is very similar.

It is instructive to examine the ROC curves generated by individuals. Figure 3 shows old-similar and old-new ROCs for four subjects best described by HTSDT and four others best described by SDT (according to both AIC and BIC). These particular ROCs provide a by-eye confirmation of the modeling diagnosis (some of the HTSDT ROCs appear to be linear and to have non-zero y-intercepts), although for some other subjects the conclusion is not so intuitively clear. The individual curves in Figure 3 vary widely in shape, spread, and of course performance level. The best performer in either experiment, S18 in the figure, is the only subject whose accuracy exceeded 90%. A complete set of ROC data for all individual subjects is available from the authors.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Old-similar ROCs for individual subjects in Experiments 1 and 2. Left column: HTSDT provides best fit; right column: SDT provides best fit (according to both AIC and BIC). Solid lines refer to old-similar data, dashed lines to old-new. Superimposed functions merely connect the points and do not reflect any model.

The nesting structure of models (shown in Table 2) can be used to test statistical hypotheses about their parameters. Our focus on individual subjects limits the interest in such hypothesis testing, but Table 3 allows some interesting conclusions nonetheless. First, models that permitted all variances to be unequal (#1 and #5) received very little support according to both AIC and BIC. A reasonable conclusion is that variances are equal for at least the target and similar distributions. Comparison of the HTSDT models reveals that the HTSDT Similar = Target model (#4) is the most successful. This finding is compatible with an interpretation of these models in which the signal-detection process reflects familiarity, which is the same for target and plural-altered items. Within the two SDT models, it is also clearly better to force targets and similar items to have equal variance.

Determining the average parameter values for the two classes of subjects allows us to describe them in more detail. To obtain meaningful averages, we analyzed all subjects according to the best-fitting model for their group: the Similar = Target model for HTSDT subjects and the Two-Variance model for SDT subjects. As Table 3 shows, these two models were in fact best fitting for 31 of the 45 subjects by the AIC standard and 42 of the 45 according to BIC.

Average values of the model parameters for the two subgroups are given in Table 4. In Experiment 1, AIC and BIC always agree on whether a subject should be assigned to the HTSDT or SDT groups, but in Experiment 2 the classification depends on which information criterion is used in four cases. In that experiment, therefore, we consider the data separately from both points of view. As is evident in Table 4, the outcomes are similar for both experiments and both information criteria are similar.

Table 4.

Average Parameter Values of Appropriate Models.

HTSDT Subjects
Experiment & criterion N dT RO RN c3
(old vs new)
c5
(“sure old”)
M SE M SE M SE M SE M SE
1 10 0.85 0.07 .32 .05 .23 .06 0.98 0.15 2.60 0.58
2 – AIC 13 0.65 0.05 .35 .05 .36 .05 0.66 0.11 2.58 0.61
2 – BIC 9 0.72 0.07 .34 .07 .33 .08 0.68 0.15 3.04 0.84
SDT Subjects
Experiment & criterion N dT dL sT c3
(old vs new)
c5
(“sure old”)
M SE M SE M SE M SE M SE
1 10 1.99 0.40 0.09 0.49 1.66 0.20 1.09 0.13 2.01 0.14
2 – AIC 12 1.57 0.20 0.07 0.20 2.05 0.19 0.88 0.15 2.30 0.21
2 – BIC 16 1.65 0.17 -0.19 0.20 2.19 0.17 0.83 0.12 2.24 0.18

Note: For subjects better fit by HTSDT, the parameters are for the HTSDT Similar = Target model. For those better fit by SDT, the values are for the SDT Two-Variance model. In Experiment 1, AIC and BIC agree on the classification of subjects; in Experiment 2 they differ, and the results are given for both breakdowns.

HTSDT subjects

Adherents of HTSDT recollected the appropriate study item about 34 percent of the time, approximately equally for positive and negative recollection. The signal detection process was of modest accuracy for targets (dT = 0.65 to 0.85). These values can be interpreted as distances from the mean of the new-lure distribution along a decision axis, so the signal-detection process is somewhat successful in distinguishing targets and similar lures from new lures. Recall that, according to the model, it cannot distinguish targets from similar lures at all, as is to be expected for a process based on “familiarity.” The more conservative instructions in Experiment 2 numerically increased the negative but not the positive recollection rates. Table 4 reports the old-new criterion (c3) and the most conservative one (c5); the latter would be expected to be most responsive to the biasing manipulation. Neither these nor the other criteria were reliably affected. The lack of change in recollection is consistent with the threshold nature of that construct in the model, and the failure of the criteria in the signal-detection component to respond to this instruction supports the claim that remember responses are driven by the recollection component for HTSDT subjects.

SDT subjects

For subjects operating under SDT principles, the average data imply that targets are well-discriminated from both similar and new lures, which are poorly discriminated from each other. However, the individual data reveal a division among these subjects. For some [7 of 10 in Experiment 1, 7 of 12 in Experiment 2 (AIC), and 7 of 16 in Experiment 2 (BIC)], the similar lures differ from new lures in the same direction as targets, with dL averaging 0.82 and 0.46 in the two experiments. For the others [3 in Experiment 1, 5 in Experiment 2 (AIC), and 9 in Experiment 2 (BIC)], the similar lures differ from the new lures in the opposite direction as targets, with dL averaging −1.63, −0.48, and −0.69 respectively. One might interpret the decision axis for the first subset as familiarity, with similar lures having values that are lower than targets but still positive, whereas for the second group the decision axis is better understood as strength of plural-appropriateness. We consider how both of these representations might occur in the Discussion.

The middle criterion (c3 in Table 4) was lower under conservative instructions for SDT subjects, but because these instructions referred to the remember response the key criterion is the highest one (c5). The location of c5 is, as expected, slightly higher in Experiment 2 (2.27 versus 2.01), but the difference is not reliable.

Finally, the standard deviations of the target and similar-lures distributions (which are set equal) are greater than those of the new lure distribution by factors of 1.66 and 2.12 in the two experiments. This finding is consistent with the asymmetric old-new ROCs almost always found in recognition memory experiments.

Remember responses

How do the models interpret “remember” responses? For HTSDT subjects, the remember hit rate and the remember correct rejection rate are estimates of plurality-information recollection rates. They should therefore agree with the parameters obtained in HTSDT model fits.

The proportion of remember responses after an “old” judgment was computed for the highest confidence “old” response (rating 6) and the proportion of remember responses after a “new” judgment was computed for the highest confidence “new” response (rating 1).iii The remember rates were “corrected” by subtracting the appropriate remember false-alarm rate, a procedure that allows the comparison of remember hit rates when false-alarm rates differ. This adjustment was recommended by Yonelinas, Dobbins, Szymanski, Dhaliwal, and King (1996, p. 431), and is justified if the HDSDT model is correct, but only then (Rotello, Masson, & Verde, 2008).

A comparison between remember rates and estimated recollection rates for the HTSDT subjects is provided in Table 5. Both mean values and correlations are relevant, and positive and negative remember and recollection rates can be considered separately or together. No matter how the data are examined, there is good support for the HTSDT model among these (HTSDT) subjects. Averaging across experiments, the positive and negative remember rates are .28 and .25 and the recollection rates estimated by the model are very similar, .33 and .29. Thus remember rates slightly but consistently underestimate recollection rates. The correlation between the two measures is at least .93 in both experiments. Subsets of the data show the same effects. A curious aspect of the data is that the more stringent remember instructions in Experiment 2 led to an increased rate of both positive and negative remembering for this subgroup.

Table 5.

Comparison of Recollection Rates in HTSDT Model with Corrected Remember Rates for HTSDT Subjects.

Experiment and criterion N Ro Positive remember rate Correlation RN Negative remember rate Correlation
M SE M SE M SE M SE
1 10 .32 .05 .28 .05 .95 .23 .06 .21 .06 .99
2 - AIC 13 .35 .05 .30 .06 .93 .36 .05 .32 .06 .94
2 - BIC 9 .34 .07 .28 .09 .94 .33 .08 .28 .09 .94

What about the subjects who behave more in accordance with SDT? Clearly the remember rates for our SDT subjects cannot be predicted from RO and RN, which are zero for these individuals. [The covariance between two variables, one of which is a constant (in this case, zero) is zero.] We attempted a partial test by examining the fits of the full HTSDT model (Model 1) and correlating the recollection estimates from that model with the remember rates. The resulting correlation was .36 in both experiments (averaging across positive and negative remembering); if a few outliers were omitted, the values were .29 and .09 in Experiments 1 and 2. The relation between recollection and remembering for these subjects is clearly weak, certainly weaker than for HTSDT subjects; this result is, of course, to be expected if our division of subjects into groups was appropriate.

The uncorrected remember hit rates for both groups, displayed in Table 6A, are comparable: .38 vs .37 for positive remembering and .28 vs .26 for negative remembering (averaging across the two experiments). For SDT (but not HTSDT) subjects, the instruction manipulation showed a trend toward working as intended for positive remembering: the non-reliable .16 change in the positive remember rate is comparable to the .20 difference reported by Rotello et al. (2005) for a similar manipulation. The trend in the same direction for negative remembering is much smaller (.05). For completeness, Table 6B contains remember false-alarm rates for both subject groups in both experiments.

Table 6.

Positive and Negative Remember Hit and False-alarm Rates (Uncorrected) for HTSDT and SDT Subjects.

A. Remember Hit Rates.
Subject class Type of remembering Experiment 1 Experiment 2
AIC criterion BIC criterion
Proportion N Proportion N Proportion N
M SE M SE M SE
HTSDT Positive .38 .05 10 .39 .06 13 .36 .09 9
Negative .24 .06 .36 .03 .30 .09
SDT Positive .45 .08 10 .27 .05 12 .32 .04 16
Negative .29 .09 .20 .04 .28 .05
All Positive .41 .05 20 .33 .04 25 .33 .04 25
Negative .27 .05 .29 .04 .29 .04
B. Remember False-alarm Rates
Subject class Type of remembering Experiment 1 Experiment 2
AIC criterion BIC criterion
Proportion N Proportion N Proportion N
M SE M SE M SE
HTSDT Positive .09 .03 10 .09 13 .08 .02 9
Negative .03 .01 .04 .02 .01
SDT Positive .13 .05 10 .10 12 .10 .02 16
Negative .04 .01 .05 .07 .01
All Positive .11 .03 20 .09 25 .09 .02 25
Negative .04 .01 .05 .05 .01

Note: Positive remember responses are for old items, negative for new items.

The SDT models assert that the “remember” response simply indicates high strength (Donaldson, 1996). Unlike the HTSDT models that include recollection, they does not predict an equivalence between remember rates and any aspect of the rating data – for example, the remember criterion need not be the same as any of the ratings criteria (Wixted & Stretch, 2004). We cannot use the remember-know data to directly test this model without making specific assumptions (Dougal & Rotello, 2007; Kapucu, Rotello, Ready, & Seidl, 2008; Rotello & Macmillan, 2006; Rotello, Macmillan, Hicks, & Hautus, 2006).iv

Discussion

By systematic model-fitting, we have identified two classes of subjects, one relying entirely on a continuous signal-detection process and the other supplementing such a process with threshold-based recollection. This finding accounts for the difficulty people have encountered in distinguishing these classes of models and may help to explain the variety of ROC shapes obtained in this and related paradigms. For example, the support that is sometimes found for mixture models interpreted in terms of attention might actually result from a mixture of different subject strategies. Similar issues arise in associative and source memory, so the result is of broad interest.

We now address three questions: (1) are there really two subsets, or is model identification just a noisy business? (2) how might multiple models derive in different subjects from the same information? And (3) what implications do these results have for conventional analysis of data from this and related paradigms?

Subject classification or model mimicry?

Identifying the model that best describes data is always an inferential process. In the past, models in the class we have considered have usually been compared by examining specific aspects of ROC curves (most often, type and degree of curvature). Model-fitting, unlike reliance on such “signature predictions,” takes all of the data into account. The noisiness of data guarantees that conclusions will be tentative, that is, that the incorrect model may have been identified. We have taken several approaches to reassure ourselves that our model-fitting process has not seriously misled us.

Akaike weights and evidence ratios

First, we asked how convincing the statistical process is by which one model is declared the winner for an individual subject. The magnitude of the differences in AIC and BIC varies from substantial to rather small. One way to assess the solidity of our procedure is to use Akaike weights for AIC values and corresponding weights for BIC (Wagenmakers & Farrell, 2004). These weights compare the differences in information criterion value among the models. The weights can in turn be used to calculate evidence ratios, which are the odds favoring the best model over each of the others.

Table 7 shows both statistics for both experiments. The Akaike weights compare the best model for a particular subset of subjects with the other models for those same subjects. In Experiment 1 the two dominant models (SDT Two-Variance and HTSDT Similar = Target) have weights of .44, which can be interpreted as the likelihood that these models are correct for those subjects. In Experiment 2, the weights are higher (and again equal) for both sets of subjects at .53. The evidence ratios comparing the dominant model with its closest competitor (usually the other dominant model) are between 1.5 and 2.7; these odds ratios correspond to probabilities of .66 to .73.

Table 7.

AIC and BIC Weights and Evidence Ratios for Both Experiments.

AIC BIC
Model Number Subjects Weight Evidence Ratio Number Subjects Weight Evidence Ratio
Experiment 1
#2. HTSDT Equal-Variance 2 0.46 1.51
#3. HTSDT Similar = Lure 1 0.48 1.93 1 0.62 1.93
#4. HTSDT Similar = Target 7 0.44 1.82 9 0.66 3.15
#5. SDT Three-Variance 2 0.48 1.42
#6. SDT Two-Variance 8 0.44 1.54 10 0.65 5.78
Total 20 20
Experiment 2
#1. HTSDT Unequal-Variance 4 0.66 4.47 1 0.70 4.83
#2. HTSDT Equal-Variance 2 0.40 1.22
#3. HTSDT Similar = Lure 1 0.56 2.69 1 0.74 3.43
#4. HTSDT Similar = Target 6 0.53 2.12 7 0.77 8.08
#5. SDT Three-Variance 2 0.60 2.77
#6. SDT Two-Variance 10 0.53 2.25 16 0.73 7.50
Total 25 25

The Bayesian criterion values are stronger: weights for the dominant models are .65 or .66 in Experiment 1 and .73 or .74 in Experiment 2. The evidence ratios for the winning models range from 3 to over 8, which correspond to probabilities of .76 to .89. According to BIC, therefore, we can be fairly confident that our testing procedure has chosen the correct model, whereas AIC comparisons are less decisive. Recall that BIC tends to favor simpler models (those with fewer parameters). The two dominant models have fewer parameters than some of their competitors (see Table 2), but because they each have 8 parameters neither has an advantage in computing evidence ratios.

Simulations

Another way to assess the confusability of these models was employed by Wixted (2007). He examined a (slightly different) implementation of the HTSDT and SDT models by simulating each and fitting the resulting data to both the model of origin and its competitor. The result was that in 20 of 30 cases the model that generated the data was better fit, and this proportion was the same for both models. According to this result, we may expect that 1/3 of the subjects were misclassified, but there is no bias – that is, the proportion allocated to the two models is approximately correct. A similar result was obtained by Cohen, Rotello, and Macmillan (2008), who compared remember-know extensions of HTSDT and SDT models. The HTSDT model was found to be more flexible, that is, it could account for data generated by SDT assumptions more often that the reverse. To the degree our designs are similar, this result suggests that the true partition of subjects has more SDT than HTSDT subjects.

Past attempts to classify subjects

The possibility that subjects differ in the models to which they adhere is supported by past recognition studies in our laboratory. In several experiments, we have fit HTSDT and SDT models to ratings or ratings-plus-remember-know data. Table 8, which displays the number of subjects whose data were best-described by each of these models, tells a consistent story: both models are supported by some subjects, with the SDT model gaining more adherents than HTSDT. In none of these past studies does the proportion of HTSDT subjects reach the levels observed in the present data; it may be that plurals paradigm, in providing something concrete to recollect (namely, an “s”) is more amenable to an HTSDT strategy.

Table 8.

Number of Subjects in Previous Remember-know Experiments Best Described by HTSDT and SDT.

Experimental Condition Reference HTSDT or Process-pure SDT
Emotional words Dougal & Rotello (2007) 8 22
Emotional words – younger subjects Kapucu et al. (2008) 3 19
Emotional words – older subjects Kapucu et al. (2008) 7 16
Remember-first paradigm Rotello & Macmillan (2006) 2 13
Trinary paradigm Rotello & Macmillan (2006) 2 35
30% old items Rotello et al. (2006) 3 19
70% old items Rotello et al. (2006) 5 19
Conservative bias Rotello et al. (2006) 2 17
Liberal bias Rotello et al. (2006) 3 15
Total 35 175

Note: An additional 23 subjects were best fit by STREAK (Rotello et al., 2004). However, 18 of these fits were for the alternative remember-know designs studied by Rotello et al. (2006) and one of those (the trinary paradigm) has been shown to be particularly poor at distinguishing models (Cohen, Rotello, & Macmillan, 2008).

Two other paradigms, associative recognition and source recognition, bear both formal and empirical similarities to the plurals paradigm. Formally, in all cases discrimination depends on a single, well-identified piece of information, whereas in a typical recognition memory test with random lures, many features distinguish test words. Empirically, all three of these designs lead to flattened ROCs, and these ROCs have been interpreted as providing support for dual-process theories (Parks & Yonelinas, 2007), models in which inattention mixes with attention (DeCarlo, 2002), or models with nonlinear decision bounds (Hautus, Macmillan, & Rotello, 2008).

To our knowledge, three papers have compared competing model fits for individual subjects in these designs. In their study of associative recognition, Kelley and Wixted (2001, Exp. 4) found that 14 out of 18 subjects were better fit by SDT than by a threshold model; the two models provided essentially equivalent fits for 6 other participants. They interpreted this division to the over-flexibility of the threshold model. Healy, Light, and Chung (2005) fit versions of many models, including SDT, HTSDT, and Kelley and Wixted’s (2001) SON model, to individual subject data in associative recognition. They found SDT and SON to be favored over HTSDT and the others. Slotnick and Dodson (2005) fit versions of continuous SDT and high-threshold models to source memory data from individual subjects data and found that 11 of 15 subjects were better fit better by an unequal-variance SDT model than by a two-high threshold model. It appears that unanimity in the best-fitting model is not found when tested for, but that SDT models perform as well as or better than other models in these competitions.

A single representation for all, but a variety of decision rules?

Does our conclusion – that subjects presented with the same experimental problem can be described by different models – make sense? Must we conclude that there are two kinds of people with distinct memory representations? To the contrary, we believe the data are best interpreted to mean that two different response strategies are available to apply to a common representation. We have previously proposed such a representation for conventional recognition experiments (Macmillan & Rotello, 2006), and extend it here to apply to the plurals paradigm.

Two kinds of information are available; following common usage, we term these familiarity and recollection. A two-dimensional space defined by these dimensions (both of which are assumed to be continuous) is shown in Figure 4. Bivariate distributions of information for targets, similar lures, and new lures are located in the space, represented by circles. Targets naturally have higher values on both dimensions than either type of lures. We conjecture that the similar items are intermediate in familiarity between targets and lures, but differ from the lures on recollection in the opposite direction from targets. As illustrated in Figure 4a, HTSDT subjects respond “old” for observations above a criterion on the recollection axisv; for lower values of recollection, they rely on familiarity. The dashed lines in the figure are decision boundaries: observations in the lower-left corner of the decision space are labeled “new,” the rest “old.”

Figure 4.

Figure 4

An interpretation of the HTSDT and SDT models in a common decision space. The F-and R-axes quantify familiarity and recollection, and bivariate normal distributions for new and old items are indicated by constant-likelihood circles. Dashed lines are decision boundaries dividing “old” and “new” responses. (a) A “process-pure” model that resembles HTSDT; an “old” response is made if either dimension exceeds a criterion. (b) A one-dimensional model in which decisions are based on the sum of F and R. (c) A one-dimensional model in which decisions are based on F. (d) A one-dimensional model in which decisions are based on R.

SDT subjects instead base their ratings judgments (and remember-know decisions) on a single axis. The most nearly optimal such dimension is a sum of the two strengths, as proposed by Wixted and Stretch (2004). A decision bound that accomplishes this rule is shown in Figure 4b. Two additional rules are also illustrated: A subject relying on pure familiarity bases decisions on the horizontal axis (Fig. 4c), whereas someone relying on pure (continuous) recollection bases decisions on the vertical axis (Fig. 4d). The projected location of the similar items differs for these last two rules. For subjects relying on familiarity, similar items are intermediate in strength between targets and lures; for subjects relying on recollection, such items are more discriminable from lures than are targets. Recall that some of our SDT subjects were best described by each of these inferred arrangements.

The models we have tested can thus be viewed as strategies available to all subjects, the choice being influenced by experimental factors including instructions. Banks (2000) demonstrated exactly such an effect in the context of source memory. This interpretation implies that an individual might be encouraged to shift between strategies with the proper incentives or instructions, but there are no overall differences in accuracy between the subject groups and it is not clear what kinds of incentives might accomplish such a shift.

Implications for conventional data analysis

Most recognition experiments, including many of ours, base their conclusions on group data. What would we have concluded had we analyzed the present data in this conventional way? We evaluated the group data (displayed in Figure 2) by adding log likelihoods for all subjects for each model, then examining AIC and BIC as before (see Heathcote et al., 2006, for details of this procedure). In Experiment 1, AIC chose the HTSDT Equal-Variance Model whereas BIC selected the SDT Two-Variance Model. The positive and negative remember rates were .24 and .20, close to the RO and RN estimates (for the AIC best model) of .23 and .16. In Experiment 2, SDT Two-Variance was selected by both criteria, and of course the remember rates (.22 and .21) were not close to the model recollection values of 0. If the true situation is as we claim – different models applying to different subjects – then this analysis is quite meaningless.

The take home message is that this paradigm cannot be used in any single simple way to extract different memory components as has sometimes been done in the past. For example, Curran (2000) proposed that particular ERP components are identified with familiarity and recollection processes based on contrasts between different conditions of the plurals paradigm – that is, the contrast between targets and similar lures identifies recollection, while the contrast between similar lures and unrelated lures identifies familiarity. Our analyses suggest that at the very least this may be true for only a subset of participants while for another subset, the same contrasts imply different aspects of memory.vi

The discrepancy between individual and group data analysis has been noted many times (e.g., Cohen, Sanborn, & Shiffrin, 2008; Estes & Maddox, 2005; Lee & Webb, 2005). The plurals paradigm may be particularly vulnerable to strategy differences. Subjects can adopt different methods for remembering plurality information, be more or less willing to engage in recall-to-reject reasoning, and have different interpretations of the “negative remembering” response. However, a variety of decision rules are available in many recognition designs – the remember-know literature, for instance, abounds in conflicting proposals about how information is used to generate responses. Whenever there are options, it is risky to assume that all participants will choose to participate in the same way.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by an NIMH grant to Rotello and Macmillan (R01 MH60274) and is the basis of an MS thesis by Kapucu. A preliminary version of this article was presented at the 2006 meeting of the Psychonomic Society in Houston, TX. We thank John Dunn and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments.

Appendix: Equations for the models

The parent model is a version of HTSDT in which the variances are unconstrained. In all conditions, recollection leads to a “sure-yes” response for targets with probability RO and a “sure-no” response for similar lures with probability RN. When recollection fails a signal-detection process occurs with the following parameters: distribution means 0 for new lures, dL for similar lures, and dT for the targets; corresponding standard deviations 1, sL, and sT; and criteria c1, c2, c3, c4, and c5, in increasing order. Submodels are created by fixing the values of parameters (see Table 2).

When recollection does not occur (RO = RN = 0), a pure signal-detection process results.

Targets

P(6)=Ro+(1Ro)[1Φ(c5dTsT)]P(i)=(1Ro)[Φ(cidTsT)Φ(ci1dTsT)],i=25P(1)=(1Ro)Φ(c1dTsT)

Lures (plurality-changed)

P(6)=(1RN)[1Φ(c5dLsL)]P(i)=(1RN)[Φ(cidLsL)Φ(ci1dLsL)],i=25P(1)=RN+(1RN)Φ(c1dLsL)

Lures (new)

P(6)=(1RN)[1Φ(c5)]P(i)=(1RN)[Φ(ci)Φ(ci1)],i=25P(1)=RN+(1RN)Φ(c1)

Footnotes

i

Yonelinas and colleagues generally refer to this model as “dual-process,” and when it was first formulated that term served to distinguish it from other models. Since then, a number of other models postulating two processes have been developed (Kelley & Wixted, 2001; Rotello, Macmillan, & Reeder, 2004; Murdock, 2006), and we use the term “HTSDT” to be explicit about the two processes in question.

ii

Another class of models assumes that performance is a mixture of attended and less-attended trials (DeCarlo, 2002). We developed a model of this type for our design, but it provided a poor fit for almost all subjects relative to the models in the HTSDT class listed in Table 2.

iii

Alternatively, one might include remember responses at all correct ratings, that is, 4, 5, 6 for old items and 1, 2, and 3 for new items. We adopted the method preferred by HTSDT proponents. A few subjects who did not use the extreme ratings were dropped from the analysis.

iv

In some interpretations, SDT predicts a correlation between remember hits and remember false-alarms and HTSDT does not. In Experiment 1 these correlations are .09 (for similar false-alarms) and −.12 (for new false-alarms); in Experiment 2 the values are .34 and .28. This is not a revealing pattern.

v

The figure implies that recollection is continuous, as has been argued recently (Mickes, Wais, & Wixted, 2009), whereas HTSDT posits that it is a threshold process. The figure is most consistent with the process-pure model of Murdock (2006), but does illustrate the possibility of partitioning the decision space in different ways. The linear decision bounds imply SDT-like ROCs and would therefore require modification in an actual implementation of the model. Likelihood-ratio decision bounds, as employed in Hautus et al. (2008), can produce appropriately flattened ROCs.

vi

We thank John Dunn for this point.

References

  1. Akaike H. Information theory as an extension of the maximum likelihood principle. In: Petrov BN, Csaki F, editors. Second International Symposium on Information Theory. Akademiai Kiado; Budapest: 1973. pp. 267–281. [Google Scholar]
  2. Banks WP. Recognition and source memory as multivariate decision processes. Psychological Science. 2000;11:267–273. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Cohen AL, Rotello CM, Macmillan NA. Evaluating models of remember-know judgments: Complexity, mimicry, and discriminability. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2008;15:906–926. doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.5.906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Cohen AL, Sanborn AN, Shiffrin RM. Model evaluation using grouped or individual data. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2008;15:692–712. doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.4.692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Coltheart M. The MRC Psycholinguistic Database. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1981;33A:497–505. [Google Scholar]
  6. Curran T. Brain potentials of recollection and familiarity. Memory & Cognition. 2000;28:923–938. doi: 10.3758/bf03209340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. DeCarlo LT. Signal detection theory with finite mixture distributions: Theoretical developments with applications to recognition memory. Psychological Review. 2002;109:710–721. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.109.4.710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Donaldson W. The role of decision processes in remembering and knowing. Memory & Cognition. 1996;24:523–533. doi: 10.3758/bf03200940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Dougal S, Rotello CM. “Remembering” emotional words is based on response bias, not recollection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2007;14:423–429. doi: 10.3758/bf03194083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Dunn JC. Remember-know: A matter of confidence. Psychological Review. 2004;111:524–542. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.111.2.524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Estes WK, Maddox WT. Risks of drawing inferences about cognitive processes from model fits to individual versus average performance. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2005;12:403–408. doi: 10.3758/bf03193784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gardiner JM, Richardson-Klavehn A. Remembering and knowing. In: Tulving E, Craik FIM, editors. The Oxford Handbook of Memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2000. pp. 229–244. [Google Scholar]
  13. Hautus MJ, Macmillan NA, Rotello CM. Toward a complete decision model of item and source recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2008;15:889–905. doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.5.889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Healy MR, Light LL, Chung CC. Dual-process models of associative recognition in younger and older adults: Evidence from receiver operating characteristics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2005;31:768–788. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.4.768. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Heathcote A, Raymond R, Dunn J. Recollection and familiarity in recognition memory: Evidence from ROC curves. Journal of Memory and Language. 2006;55:495–514. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.07.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Jacoby LL. A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language. 1991;30:513–541. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(91)90025-F. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Jones TC, Atchley P. Conjunction errors, recollection-based rejections, and forgetting in a continuous recognition task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2006;32:70–78. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.1.70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Kapucu A, Rotello CM, Ready RE, Seidl KN. Response bias in ‘remembering’ emotional stimuli: A new perspective on age differences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2008 doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kelley R, Wixted JT. On the nature of associative information in recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2001;27:701–722. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.27.3.701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kucera H, Francis WN. Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press; 1967. [Google Scholar]
  21. Lee MD, Webb MR. Modeling individual differences in cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2005;12:605–621. doi: 10.3758/bf03196751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Macmillan NA, Rotello CM. Deciding about decision models of remember and know judgments: A reply to Murdock (2006) Psychological Review. 2006;113:657–665. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.113.3.657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Mickes L, Wais PE, Wixted JT. Recollection is a continuous process: Implications for dual-process theories of recognition memory. Psychological Science. 2009;20:509–515. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02324.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Migo E, Montaldi D, Norman KA, Quamme J, Mayes A. The contribution of familiarity to recognition memory is a function of test format when using similar foils. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2009;62:1198–1215. doi: 10.1080/17470210802391599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Murdock B. Decision-making models of remember-know judgments: Comment on Rotello, Macmillan, and Reeder (2004) Psychological Review. 2006;113:648–656. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.113.3.648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Parks CM, Yonelinas AP. Moving beyond pure signal-detection models: Comment on Wixted (2007) Psychological Review. 2007;114:188–202. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Pitt MA, Myung IJ, Zhang S. Toward a method of selecting among computational models of cognition. Psychological Review. 2002;109:472–491. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Rajaram S. Remembering and knowing: Two means of access to the personal past. Memory & Cognition. 1993;21:89–102. doi: 10.3758/bf03211168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Rotello CM. Recall processes in recognition memory. In: Medin DL, editor. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Vol. 40. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 2000. pp. 183–221. [Google Scholar]
  30. Rotello CM, Macmillan NA. Remember-know models as decision strategies in two experimental paradigms. Journal of Memory and Language. 2006;55:479–494. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.08.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Rotello CM, Macmillan NA, Hicks JL, Hautus MJ. Interpreting the effects of response bias on remember-know judgments using signal-detection and threshold models. Memory & Cognition. 2006;34:1598–1614. doi: 10.3758/bf03195923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Rotello CM, Macmillan NA, Reeder JA. Sum-difference theory of remembering and knowing: A two-dimensional signal detection model. Psychological Review. 2004;111:588–616. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.111.3.588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Rotello CM, Macmillan NA, Reeder JA, Wong M. The remember response: Subject to bias, graded, and not a process-pure indicator of recollection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2005;12:865–873. doi: 10.3758/bf03196778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Rotello CM, Macmillan NA, Van Tassel G. Recall-to-reject in recognition: Evidence from ROC curves. Journal of Memory and Language. 2000;43:67–88. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1999.2701. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. Rotello CM, Masson MEJ, Verde MF. Type I error rates and power analyses for single-point sensitivity measures. Perception & Psychophysics. 2008;70:389–401. doi: 10.3758/PP.70.2.389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Schwarz G. Estimating the dimension of a model. The Annals of Statistics. 1978;6:461–464. [Google Scholar]
  37. Slotnick SD, Dodson CS. Support for a continuous (single-process) model of recognition memory and source memory. Memory & Cognition. 2005;33:151–170. doi: 10.3758/bf03195305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Tulving E. Memory and consciousness. Canadian Journal of Psychology. 1985;26:1–12. doi: 10.1037/h0080017. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  39. Wagenmakers EJ, Farrell S. AIC model selection using Akaike weights. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2004;11:192–195. doi: 10.3758/bf03206482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Wixted JT. Dual-process theory and signal-detection theory of recognition memory. Psychological Review. 2007;114:152–176. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Wixted JT, Stretch V. In defense of the signal detection interpretation of remember/know judgments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2004;11:616–641. doi: 10.3758/bf03196616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Yonelinas AP. Receiver-operating characteristics in recognition memory: Evidence for a dual-process model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition. 1994;20:1341–1354. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.20.6.1341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Yonelinas AP. Recognition memory ROCs for items and associative information: The contribution of recollection and familiarity. Memory & Cognition. 1997;25:747–763. doi: 10.3758/bf03211318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Yonelinas AP. Consciousness, control, and confidence: The 3 Cs of recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2001;130:361–379. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Yonelinas AP, Dobbins I, Szymanski MD, Dhaliwal HS, King L. Signal-detection, threshold, and dual-process models of recognition memory: ROCs and conscious recollection. Consciousness & Cognition: An International Journal. 1996;5:418–441. doi: 10.1006/ccog.1996.0026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES