Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Jun 21.
Published in final edited form as: Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009 Aug;66(8):811–822. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.91

Meta-analysis of 41 Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Executive Function in Schizophrenia

Michael J Minzenberg 1, Angela R Laird 1, Sarah Thelen 1, Cameron S Carter 1, David C Glahn 1
PMCID: PMC2888482  NIHMSID: NIHMS199763  PMID: 19652121

Abstract

Context:

Prefrontal cortical dysfunction is frequently reported in schizophrenia. It remains unclear whether this represents the coincidence of several prefrontal region- and process-specific impairments or a more unitary dysfunction in a superordinate cognitive control network. Whether these impairments are properly considered reflective of hypofrontality vs hyperfrontality remains unresolved.

Objectives:

To test whether common nodes of the cognitive control network exhibit altered activity across functional neuroimaging studies of executive cognition in schizophrenia and to evaluate the direction of these effects.

Data Sources:

PubMed database.

Study Selection:

Forty-one English-language, peer-reviewed articles published prior to February 2007 were included. All reports used functional neuroimaging during executive function performance by adult patients with schizophrenia and reported whole-brain analyses in standard stereotactic space. Tasks primarily included the delayed match-to-sample, N-back, AX-CPT, and Stroop tasks.

Data Extraction:

Activation likelihood estimation modeling reported activation maxima as the center of a 3-dimensional gaussian function in the meta-analysis, with statistical thresholding and correction for multiple comparisons.

Data Synthesis:

In within-group analyses, healthy controls and patients activated a similarly distributed cortical-subcortical network, prominently including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC), ventrolateral PFC, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and thalamus. In between-group analyses, patients showed reduced activation in the left dorsolateral PFC, rostral/dorsal ACC, left thalamus (with significant co-occurrence of these areas), and inferior/ posterior cortical areas. Increased activation was observed in several midline cortical areas. Activation within groups varied modestly by task.

Conclusions:

Healthy adults and schizophrenic patients activate a qualitatively similar neural network during executive task performance, consistent with the engagement of a general-purpose cognitive control network, with critical nodes in the dorsolateral PFC and ACC. Nevertheless, patients with schizophrenia show altered activity with deficits in the dorsolateral PFC, ACC, and mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus. Increases in activity are evident in other PFC areas, which could be compensatory in nature.


Impaired cognition is a core, disabling feature of schizophrenia, with no established treatment. Although numerous deficits have been described across perceptual, attentional, mnemonic, linguistic, and intellectual functions, impaired executive functions are among the most widely observed, and they are consistently associated with impaired function of the prefrontal cortex (PFC).1 Different aspects of executive dysfunction have been examined, including multiple facets of working memory, response inhibition, conflict processing, and problem solving, demonstrating deficits across a range of circumscribed PFC regions, such as the ventrolateral PFC (VLPFC), dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC), ventromedial PFC, and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). A recent quantitative meta-analysis of 12 N-back studies found alterations among schizophrenic subjects compared with healthy controls in several nodes of this neural network, including reduced activity in the DLPFC and increased activity in the VLPFC, ACC, and left frontal pole.2 Other studies have reported reduced activity in the VLPFC during other working memory tasks3 and in the ACC during conflict processing.4,5

One possible interpretation of these findings is that there is a general deficit in PFC function in schizophrenia, with findings in a given task reflecting a distinct underlying circuitry that supports each particular function, eg, the VLPFC for working memory maintenance, the DLPFC for working memory manipulation or interference control, and the frontopolar cortex for response inhibition. Alternatively, there could be a set of regions that show impaired function in schizophrenia across a range of tasks and other areas with preserved or compensatory function. One hypothesis suggests that there is impaired DLPFC activity in the presence of an intact VLPFC.6 Together with reports of selective loss of neuropil in dorsal vs ventral regions of the PFC, this hypothesis might imply some subregion specificity to frontal dysfunction, ie, the DLPFC is impaired while the VLPFC is not. An influential model of PFC function assigns the dorsolateral regions a critical superordinate role in regulating cognitive control.7 According to this model, the DLPFC maintains the context or task set to ensure accurate and flexible performance during higher-level cognition, while medial frontal regions (eg, the ACC) support dynamic adjustments in control in concert with the DLPFC.8,9 According to this view, dysfunction in general-purpose, high-level cognitive control functions of the dorsolateral and medial PFC could result in a wide range of deficits in different executive functions in schizophrenia.

In this article, we use meta-analysis to test whether the various executive function deficits observed across functional neuroimaging studies in schizophrenia represent the coincidence of several PFC subregion- and task-specific dysfunctions or a more unitary dysfunction of a general-purpose, DLPFC/ACC-based cognitive control network. In this context, the meta-analytic approach is uniquely valuable, as it allows us to test a research problem that is not easily addressed in a single study; to overcome equivocation or inconsistencies in an existing literature; and to provide the reader with the landscape of a research domain, which in contemporary biomedical research may be more highly valued than the results of an individual study.10 Activation likelihood estimation (ALE) is a meta-analytic tool that models 3-dimensional coordinates (from reported activations in a standard space) as the center of a 3-dimensional gaussian distribution.11 This obviates the need for raw data and thus increases the potential set of studies subject to meta-analysis and whole-brain analyses corrected for multiple comparisons.11 Activation likelihood estimation has been implemented to address a variety of research questions in both healthy subjects and clinical samples.12

We used ALE to test the largest sample to date of studies of PFC-dependent executive cognition in schizophrenia. We hypothesized that, across a range of discrete PFC-dependent executive tasks, schizophrenic patients would show a deficit in a general-purpose DLPFC/ACC control/ conflict-processing system. In contrast, if schizophrenia is the expression of multiple “hits” against a number of distinct component executive functions, it would be difficult to detect robust between-group differences when these varied executive function studies are integrated in a single analysis. Relatively distinct task-dependent patterns of activation should be observed within tests of these functions and not across different executive functions.

METHODS

STUDY SELECTION

A PubMed literature search was performed to identify English-language, peer-reviewed studies that investigated executive function in schizophrenic patients and healthy control subjects using functional magnetic resonance imaging or positron emission tomography. Executive functions can be defined as processes necessary to control or regulate other cognitive processes in the service of goal-directed behavior. The term cognitive control is often used to describe executive functions to emphasize the regulatory component of this aspect of information processing. For this analysis, we searched for studies that used task paradigms that are typically associated with executive functions or cognitive control. These included delayed match-to-sample or delayed response (including Sternberg item recognition), go/no-go (including AX-CPT), mental arithmetic, N-back, oddball, sequence recall, Stroop, Wisconsin Card Sort, and word generation tasks. Studies that did not report results as 3-dimensional coordinates in standard stereotactic space or that reported only data from individual subjects or deactivations were excluded. Forty-one studies published prior to February 2007 met these criteria (Table 1).3,5,13-51 Study design features are indicated in Table 1, including clinical characteristics like illness duration and symptom severity; sex ratio among the patient group; medication status; block vs event-related design; and performance-matched vs nonmatched groups for analysis. From this group of studies, coordinate results of within-group activations and between-group differences were divided into 4 groups: activations in schizophrenic patients, activations in normal control subjects, increases in schizophrenic patients relative to controls, and increases in controls relative to schizophrenic patients. To evaluate potential task-specific patterns of activation, we also repeated the within-group analyses in an identical manner but with the subsets of studies segregated by task type.

Table 1.

Published Studies Included in the ALE Meta-analysis of Executive Function by Task

Sample Size, No.
Mean, y
Source Contrasts SCZ (Male
Patients)
CON Age Duration
of Illness
Patient
Medication
Mean Symptom
Scores
Block
Design
Performance
Matched
PFC in
SCZa
Delayed Match to Sample
Crespo-Facorro et al,13 2001 SCZ, CON, SCZ>CON, CON>SCZ 19 (14) 34 29.6 8 Naive; 11 free for 3 weeks SANS/SAPS item score, negative, 2.53; positive, 2.55; disorganized, 1.26 Yes No
Manoach et al,14 2000 CON, SCZ 9 (7) 9 42.4 25.0 1 Atypical; 7 typical PANSS, positive, 14.4; negative, 21.6; BPRS total, 20.1 Yes Both matched and unmatched ↑/−
Manoach et al,15 2005 SCZ, SCZ>CON, CON>SCZ 19 (14) 12 42 18 9 Atypical; 2 typical; 2 both; 2 none PANSS, positive, 13; negative, 14; BPRS total, 16; SANS total, 26 Yes No ↓ ↑
Quintana et al,16 2003 SCZ, CON 8 (6) 8 29.3 8.5 All atypical NA Yes No ↓↑ (ROI/ANOVA)
Tan et al, 200517 SCZ>CON, CON>SCZ 11 (5) 11 25.0 0.2b 5 Olanzapine; 6 risperidone PANSS, positive, 10.2; negative, 17.5; general, 28.4 Yes SCZ=CONc ↓ ↑

Go/No-Go
Carter et al,18 2001 CON>SCZ 17 (12) 18 33.5 16.0 6 Typical; 11 atypical PANSS, total, 25.7; positive, 7.1; negative, 10.7 No No
Laurens et al,19 2003 SCZ>CON 10 (12) 16 32.9 11 All atypical SSPI total, 8.3; positive, 2.0; negative, 1.0; disorganized, 0.9 No No ↓ ↑
MacDonald and Carter,20 2003 SCZ, CON 17 (12) 17 34.2 11 Atypical; 6 typical PANSS, positive, 7.1; negative, 10.7; disorganized, 7.9 No No ↓ (ROI/ANOVA)
MacDonald et al,21 2005 SCZ, CON, CON>SCZ 18 (12) 28 27.5b All naive SAPS/SANS, positive, 15.7; negative, 7.3; disorganized, 7.3 No No ↓ ↑
Rubia et al,22 2001 SCZ>CON, CON>SCZ 6 (6) 7 40 15.7 All taking medications NA Yes SCZ=CONc

Mental Arithmetic
Hugdahl et al,23 2004 SCZ, CON, SCZ>CON, CON>SCZ 12 (6) 12 32.4 8.7 All taking medications PANSS total, 58.2; BPRS total, 45.2 Yes No ↓ ↑

N-Back
Callicott et al,24 2000 SCZ>CON, CON>SCZ 13 (10) 18 33.0 10 NA PSAS, 15.4 Yes Both matched and unmatched ↓↑/↓↑
Callicott et al,25 2003 SCZ>CON, CON>SCZ 14 (11) 14 31.5 Mean 476 CPZ NA Yes Both matched and unmatched ↓/↓↑
Honey et al,26 1999 SCZ, CON 20 (20) 10 37.2 12.9 Mean 194.5 CPZ PANSS, positive 8.4; negative 10.7; general, 20.2 Yes SCZ=CONc NA
Honey et al,27 2002 SCZ, CON 20 (20) 20 34.6 11.8 Mean 299 CPZ PANSS, positive, 10.2; negative, 15.0; general, 26.9 Yes SCZ=CONc NA
Honey et al,28 2003 SCZ, CON, CON>SCZ 30 (27) 27 36.9 13 Mean 327.3 CPZ PANSS, positive, 11.7; negative, 14.6; general, 26.3 Yes No
Jacobsen et al,29 2004 SCZ>CON 13 (9) 13 42.9 11 Atypical; 1 typical; 1 both NA Yes No
Jansma et al,30 2004 SCZ>CON 10 (8) 10 27.2 8 Clozapine; 2 olanzapine PANSS, positive, 13.9; negative, 15.4; general, 31.0 Yes No
Kim et al,31 2003 SCZ, CON 12 (6) 12 26.2 2.8 All atypical PANSS, positive, 14.1; negative, 17.6; general, 31.3 Yes SCZ=CONc NA
Meisenzahl et al,32 2006 CON, CON>SCZ 12 (11) 12 33.6 9 Naive; 3 postwashout PANSS total, 54.6; SANS total, 63.9; BPRS total, 56.6 Yes No
Mendrek et al,33 2004 SCZ, CON, CON>SCZ 8 (6) 8 30 All atypical SSPI total, 19.85 Yes No ↓ ↑
Mendrek et al,34 2005 SCZ, CON, SCZ>CON 12 (9) 12 28.8 All atypical SSPI total, 8.58 Yes No ↓ ↑
Meyer-Lindenberg et al,35 2001 SCZ>CON, CON>SCZ 13 (10) 13 32.5 Postwashout for 2 weeks NA Yes Both matched and unmatched ↓↑/↓↑
Sabri et al,36 2003 SCZ>CON 12 (7) 10 30.6 All taking medication NA Yes SCZ=CONc
Salgado-Pineda et al,37 2004 CON>SCZ 14 (7) 14 25.1 All risperidone SAPS total, 9.8; SANS total, 21.2 Yes No
Schneider et al,38 2007 SCZ>CON, CON>SCZ 48 (26) 57 31.0 1.9b Mean 66-day treatment history; medications NA PANSS, positive, 9.2; negative, 13.5; general, 25.3 Yes No
Tan et al,39 2006 SCZ>CON 15 (12) 26 32.7 Mean 501 CPZ PANSS, positive, 11.6; negative, 16.6; general, 25.8 Yes Both matched and unmatched ↑/↑
Walter et al,40 2003 SCZ>CON 15 (8) 15 28.7 5.5 13 Atypical; 1 typical; 1 none PANSS, positive, 23.8; negative, 19.4; BPRS total, 53.9 Yes Both matched and unmatched ↑/↑
Wykes et al,41 2002 SCZ>CON 6 (6) 6 35 >67%>10 9 Typical (mean 643 CPZ), 3 atypical NA Yes No
Yoo et al,42 2005 SCZ, CON, SCZ>CON, CON>SCZ 10 (8) 10 24.9 2.4 All atypical BPRS total, 24.7 Yes No ↓ ↑

Oddball
Heckers et al,43 2004 SCZ, CON 19 (NA) 15 46.6 NA PANSS, 64.6; SANS total, 38.5 Yes SCZ=CONc NA
Laurens et al,44 2005 SCZ, CON, CON>SCZ 29 (20) 28 31.6 7 27 Atypical; 1 typical; 1 no medication SSPI total, 12.7; positive, 2.8; negative, 4.0; disorganized, 1.6 No No

Sequence Recall
Stevens et al,3 1998 SCZ, CON 12 (8) 10 39.6 All taking medication PANSS, positive, 16.1; negative, 17.2; general, 32.1 Yes SCZ=CONc ↓ (ANOVA)
Johnson et al,45 2006 CON, SCZ>CON, CON>SCZ 18 (16) 18 36.9 All taking medication NA Yes Both matched and unmatched ↓/↓↑

Stroop
Seok Jeong et al,46 2005 SCZ, CON 10 (3) 10 29.2 4.3 7 Olanzapine; 6 risperidone PANSS, positive, 13.2; negative, 15.9; general, 29.3 No No ↓ (ANOVA)
Kerns et al,5 2005 SCZ, CON, SCZ>CON, CON>SCZ 13 (9) 13 35.6 2 Haloperidol; 11 atypical PANSS total, 8.0; BPRS total, 28.1 No No
Weiss et al,47 2003 SCZ, CON, SCZ>CON, CON>SCZ 13 (13) 13 32.7 6.2 All atypical PANSS, positive, 11.4; negative, 14.2; general, 23.3 Yes SCZ=CONc
Weiss et al,48 2007 SCZ, CON, SCZ>CON, CON>SCZ 8 (8) 8 29.5 2.4d 6 Naive; 2 taking medication PANSS, positive, 20.0; negative, 17.0; general, 35.2 No No

Wisconsin Card Sorting
Ragland et al,49 1998 SCZ, CON 15 (7) 15 33.5 11.1 6 Not taking medication; 9 with mean 631 CPZ SAPS total, 27.2; SANS total, 29.8, BPRS total, 37.1 Yes Both matched and unmatched ↓/↓

Word Generation
Curtis et al,50 2001 SCZ, CON 5 (5) 5 29.6 NA Total SANS subscale, negative, 23.2; positive, 20.8 Yes No
Weiss et al,51 2004 SCZ, CON 9 (9) 9 31.4 All atypical PANSS, positive, 10.6; negative, 14.2; general, 23.5 Yes SCZ=CONc (pre-scan) No differences

Abbreviations: ALE, activation likelihood estimation; ANOVA, analysis of variance; BPRS, Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale; CON, control group; CON>SCZ, more activity in CON than SCZ; CPZ, chlorpromazine equivalents; NA, not available; PANSS, Positive and Negative Symptom Scale; PFC, prefrontal cortex; PSAS, Psychiatric Symptom Assessment Scale; ROI, region of interest; SANS, Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms; SAPS, Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms; SCZ, schizophrenia group; SCZ>CON, more activity in SCZ than CON; SSPI, Signs and Symptoms of Psychotic Illness; ↑, increased PFC neural activity in SCZ compared with CON; ↓ decreased PFC neural activity in SCZ compared with CON; ↓↑, both increased and decreased activity in PFC areas compared with CON; –, no change in PFC activity for SCZ compared with CON.

a

Arrows separated by a slash indicate the direction of PFC effects as a function of performance matching (right) or not (left).

b

All patients were having their first psychotic episode.

c

Without performance matching.

d

Six patients were having their first psychotic episode.

ACTIVATION LIKELIHOOD ESTIMATION

Activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis was conducted.11,52 All data processing was performed in the BrainMap environment.53,54 On insertion into the database, the spatial normalization template of each article was noted and the coordinates were automatically transformed to allow analysis relative to a single template.55 Coordinates were converted using the icbm2tal transformation,56 which has shown to provide improved fit over the mni2tal transformation.57 Included foci were smoothed with a full-width at half-maximum of 12 mm, and the ALE statistic was computed for every voxel in the brain. Separate ALE maps were created for each of the 4 types of statistical comparisons. Statistical significance was determined using a permutation test of randomly generated foci, corrected for multiple comparisons. Five thousand permutations were computed using the same full-width at half-maximum value and the same number of foci used in computing ALE values. The final ALE maps were thresholded at P<.05 (false discovery rate–corrected) with an extent threshold greater than 400 mm3 and overlaid onto a template generated by spatially normalizing the International Consortium for Brain Mapping template to Talairach space.58

FRACTIONAL SIMILARITY NETWORK ANALYSIS

To expand on our basic meta-analysis and to determine if specific brain regions co-occur frequently across studies, fractional similarity network analysis was used.59 Fractional similarity network analysis identifies subordinate networks within a larger network by creating a co-occurrence matrix, in which each element indicates how often a given pair of regions is co-activated in a given study. This is accomplished with a general similarity coefficient, the fraction of 1-1 and 0-0 binary matches between 2 patterns. Using this coefficient, fractional similarity network analysis iteratively groups regions into subnetworks based on the likelihood that those regions co-occur across studies. A cluster threshold of 100 mm3 was applied to ALE results prior to fractional similarity network analysis. No adjustment was made for varying significance thresholds across studies, as P values were highly variable in value; correction for multiple comparisons; or application at the voxel vs cluster level. As in all previously published ALE analyses, we chose to include all available articles (with statistically significant results) rather than exclude some on the basis of the statistical methods used. These were, however, conducted only on reports of whole-brain analyses. We used fractional similarity network analysis to evaluate whether, among those brain regions exhibiting significant activity changes in the patients relative to controls, some might co-occur together across the full (global) set of studies. This would suggest the presence of a deficit in a distinct subnetwork that mediates PFC-dependent cognitive processes.

RESULTS

GLOBAL ANALYSIS OF ALL STUDIES

Within-Group Analysis of Controls

In reporting these data, we emphasize functional brain regions (eg, DLPFC) to complement the tabulation of anatomic descriptions (eg, gyri or Brodmann areas). This analysis revealed robust activation of a broad cortical-subcortical network, including bilateral DLPFC (extending from the mid-DLPFC posteriorly into the dorsal pre-motor cortex); bilateral VLPFC (anterolateral areas and frontal opercular cortical areas, extending into subjacent insular cortex); and a large area centered on the dorsal ACC (extending superiorly into the supplementary motor area [SMA] and pre-SMA) (Figure 1 and Table 2). More posterior neocortical areas were observed in the temporal and parietal lobes. Subcortical areas of activation notably included a large area that covered much of the left thalamus (centered on anterolateral nuclei but including the extent of the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus) and the left cerebellar declive.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Global analysis of executive function studies in schizophrenia. A, Brain regions with significant activation across executive function task types. In the bottom row, clusters in which controls showed more activation than schizophrenic patients are in red and clusters in which schizophrenic patients showed more activation than controls are in blue. B, Three-dimensional rendering of areas with more activation in controls than in schizophrenic patients across task types (global). L indicates left; R, right.

Table 2.

Brain Regions Exhibiting Significant Activity Across the Full Set of Cognitive Tasks

Brain Regiona Volume, mm3 Brodmann
Area
Talairach Coordinates,
x, y, z
Healthy Controls
Right superior frontal gyrus 400 9 40, 38, 32
Right middle frontal gyrus 1104 10 36, 44, 14
688 6 32, −6, 54
Left superior frontal gyrus 5400 6 0, 8, 48
Left precentral gyrus 11 584 6 −42, 2, 36
Right precentral gyrus 6720 9 42, 6, 32
Left superior temporal gyrus 1288 22 −58, −46, 18
Right middle temporal gyrus 512 22 56, −38, 6
Left inferior parietal lobule 4608 40 −38, −54, 38
Right precuneus 3120 19 32, −62, 40
Left claustrum 4496 −30, 18, 2
Right insula 2056 34, 16, 2
Left thalamus 1464 −14, −6, 10
Left cerebellum (declive) 1464 −38, −70, −14

Schizophrenic Patients
Right middle frontal gyrus 5856 9 36, 32, 28
4104 9 44, 8, 32
Left middle frontal gyrus 2472 6 −30, 0, 52
Left inferior frontal gyrus 9736 9 −44, 8, 28
Right cingulate gyrus 5800 32 4, 16, 40
Right superior temporal gyrus 1240 22 60, −36, 8
Left superior temporal gyrus 1048 22 −54, −40, 10
Right superior parietal lobule 1400 7 24, −64, 42
Left superior parietal lobule 1176 7 −30, −60, 44
Left inferior parietal lobule 592 40 −48, −48, 46
Left thalamus 464 −12, −12, 4
Left cerebellum (declive) 824 −36, −62, −16

Greater Activity in Controls Than Schizophrenic Patients
Left middle frontal gyrus 3096 9 −38, 30, 30
664 6 −30, −6, 44
Right middle frontal gyrus 712 8 32, 24, 42
Right medial frontal gyrus 1480 9 6, 42, 18
Right cingulate gyrus 1704 32 2, 18, 34
Right claustrum 1776 26, 22, 2
Left middle occipital gyrus 416 19 −42, −70, 6
Right inferior parietal lobule 792 7 36, −58, 42
Left claustrum 880 −28, 24, 0
Right putamen 448 20, −4, 14
Left mediodorsal thalamus 1736 −4, −14, 10

Greater Activity in Schizophrenic Patients Than Controls
Left superior frontal gyrus 440 6 −8, −14, 68
1320 9 −2, 52, 24
Left inferior frontal gyrus 656 46 −40, 36, 12
Right medial frontal gyrus 424 10 8, 44, −12
Left precentral gyrus 752 6 −54, 4, 30
Left cingulate gyrus 2208 32 −2, 10, 40
Right superior temporal gyrus 584 41 38, −36, 6
Left inferior parietal lobule 1200 40 −54, −42, 42
Right lingual gyrus 800 18 14, −74, 6
Right insula 1136 13 38, 16, 4
Right amygdala 592 18, −4, −12

Greater Activity in Controls Than Schizophrenic Patientsb (n=22)
Right medial frontal gyrus 360 9 6, 42, 18
Left medial frontal gyrus 184 8 −12, 34, 36
Left middle frontal gyrus 240 6 −30, −6, 42
Left middle frontal gyrus 120 9 −40, 32, 28
Right cingulate gyrus 848 32 2, 18, 34
Left superior parietal lobule 224 7 −28, −70, 46
Right inferior parietal lobule 368 7 36, −60, 42
a

Regions are listed in hierarchical order based on the following nested criteria: cortical to cerebellar to subcortical, rostral to caudal, dorsal to ventral, left to right, and large to small.

b

Without N-back studies.

Within-Group Analysis of Schizophrenic Patients

This analysis also revealed robust activation of a broad cortical-subcortical network, very similar in qualitative pattern to that of control subjects. This included areas of activation in the DLPFC, which was somewhat more restricted than that for the controls but also covered the mid-DLPFC, dorsal premotor cortex, and VLPFC, and a midline frontal cortical area extending from the dorsal ACC into the SMA. While the extent of activation in the ACC/SMA regions was roughly comparable between groups, the peak activation in the patients was displaced 8 mm posteriorly and inferiorly from that of the controls (Table 2). More posterior neocortical areas of activation were found in the temporal and parietal cortex; subcortical areas, including a left thalamus cluster that was considerably smaller than that found in controls; and the left cerebellar declive.

Between-Group Comparisons

In these direct comparisons, a number of the brain areas from the cortical-subcortical network described above were significantly reduced in patients relative to controls. These included the bilateral DLPFC, right VLPFC (extending from the right claustrum), right ventral premotor cortex, and 2 large midline frontal cortical areas, including the dorsal ACC and a more anterior and inferior area that peaked in the medial frontal gyrus but also extended into the adjacent ACC. Posterior neocortical areas were also observed in the parietal and occipital cortex. Subcortical areas included the right putamen and a large area in the left thalamus (prominently including the mediodorsal nucleus). This between-group analysis was repeated excluding the N-back studies (leaving 22 remaining studies). All differences in increases in controls relative to schizophrenic patients in the PFC remained significant except in the right middle frontal gyrus (Table 2), suggesting that differences in the full 41-study sample were not inordinately determined by the N-back studies.

A large area near the rostral pole of the left PFC and areas in the left dorsal and ventral premotor cortex were activated to a greater degree in patients than in control subjects. This area is considerably smaller and located posterolateral to the left DLPFC area, which is impaired in patients. A restricted area in the left VLPFC and 2 midline frontal cortical areas were also observed. The largest of these was located in the dorsal ACC, posterior and dorsal to the ACC area where the patients were impaired, extending primarily into the suprajacent SMA. A smaller area was located in the ventromedial PFC. Posterior neocortical areas were found in the temporal and parietal cortex. Subcortical areas included the insula and amygdala, both in the right hemisphere.

TASK-WISE WITHIN-GROUP RESULTS

We identified which sets of tasks were associated with significant activity in a given brain region, within a given subject group (Figure 2 and Table 3). We present the results in this manner, rather than enumerate separate active brain regions by task, to emphasize task-related neuroanatomic differences.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Brain regions with significant within-group activation by executive function task. L indicates left; R, right; 1, global; 2, N-back; 3, go/no-go; 4, Stroop; 5, delayed match to sample.

Table 3.

Brain Regions With Significant Activation Within Healthy Control and Schizophrenic Patient Groups by Task

Brain Regiona Volume, mm3 Brodmann
Area
Talairach Coordinates,
x, y, z
N-Back in Controls
Right superior frontal gyrus 1600 6 2, 8, 48
Left middle frontal gyrus 4888 6 −42, 2, 40
720 10 −36, 40, 24
408 6 −22, −12, 52
Right middle frontal gyrus 1104 10 36, 44, 16
Right inferior frontal gyrus 2072 9 46, 14, 22
Right precentral gyrus 1072 6 32, −8, 54
Right anterior cingulate gyrus 952 32 2, 24, 38
Left inferior parietal lobule 3384 40 −38, −54, 38
Right precuneus 2360 19 32, −62, 38
Left cerebellum declive 3024 −36, −68, −14
Left cerebellum, cerebellar tonsil 608 −36, −54, −38
Right cerebellum declive 1048 30, −64, −16
Right cerebellum anterior lobe 832 32, −56, −28
568 0, −54, −28
Left claustrum 2240 −30, 20, 2
Left thalamus ventral anterior nucleus 1760 −12, −8, 12
Right thalamus 816 4, −22, 8

N-Back in Schizophrenic Patients
Left middle frontal gyrus 1616 6 −32, −2, 48
1568 9 −42, 14, 28
Right middle frontal gyrus 3272 9 46, 8, 34
Right anterior cingulate gyrus 1648 32 6, 18, 42
Right superior parietal lobule 1952 7 24, −64, 42
Left inferior parietal lobule 1240 40 −38, −48, −40
Right inferior parietal lobule 736 40 40, −46, 40
Left cerebellum declive 944 −36, −62, −16
672 −6, −72, −14
Right cerebellum culmen 1304 38, −52, −18
Left insula 664 13 −36, 14, −2

Delayed Match to Sample in Controls
Right middle frontal gyrus 840 11 28, 44, −10
Left inferior frontal gyrus 592 45 −40, 20, 4

Delayed Match to Sample in Schizophrenic Patients
Left middle frontal gyrus 728 46 −50, 28, 20
Right middle frontal gyrus 1000 9 36, 30, 28
Left inferior frontal gyrus 856 9 −58, 6, 22
Left superior parietal lobule 520 7 −36, −70, 46
Right insula 688 30, 22, 2

Go/No-Go in Controls
Left superior frontal gyrus 576 9 −28, 50, 34
Left middle frontal gyrus 648 9 −40, 14, 28
Right middle frontal gyrus 640 9 42, 22, 28
640 6 42, 6, 48
Right inferior frontal gyrus 448 13 36, 22, 6
Right precentral gyrus 552 9 40, 8, 36
Left postcentral gyrus 464 3 −42, −22, 40
Right postcentral gyrus 512 2 52, −22, 34
Left claustrum 544 −30, 14, 0
Right thalamus 736 14, −14, 2

Go/No-Go in Schizophrenic Patients
Left middle frontal gyrus 1776 9 −36, 12, 26
Right middle frontal gyrus 664 10 38, 36, 24
Right inferior frontal gyrus 792 9 40, 8, 30
Right anterior cingulate gyrus 712 32 2, 14, 38
Right inferior parietal lobule 752 40 42, −32, 38

Stroop in Controls
Left precentral gyrus 728 6 −42, 0, 34
Right anterior cingulate gyrus 464 32 2, 14, 40
Left middle temporal gyrus 1072 21 −58, −48, 6
Right supramarginal gyrus 680 40 54, −50, 24
Left precuneus 560 7 −24, −64, 30
416 7 −14, −66, 44
Right precuneus 680 31 26, −72, 18
Left claustrum 688 −28, 18, 4

Stroop in Schizophrenic Patients
Right middle frontal gyrus 1840 46 40, 28, 14
Right precentral gyrus 696 6 36, 2, 36
Left postcentral gyrus 528 5 −20, −42, 66
Right cuneus 1048 18 24, −76, 18
Right insula 464 13 32, 16, −6
Left thalamus 672 −12, −12, 2
a

Regions are listed in hierarchical order based on the following nested criteria: cortical to cerebellar to subcortical, rostral to caudal, dorsal to ventral, left to right, and large to small.

Controls

Subregions within the DLPFC were activated in the N-back and go/no-go but not in the delayed match-to-sample or Stroop tasks. The VLPFC was activated in the delayed match-to-sample task. The ACC was activated in the N-back and Stroop tasks. A large midline SMA/preSMA region was activated solely in the N-back task. The premotor cortex was activated in the N-back, go/no-go, and Stroop tasks. The primary somatosensorimotor cortex was activated only in the go/no-go task. In the temporal lobe, only the left middle temporal gyrus was activated during the Stroop task. In the parietal lobe, significant activation was observed in the right supramarginal gyrus in the Stroop tasks and in the left inferior parietal lobule in the N-back task. The precuneus was activated in the N-back and Stroop tasks. The left claustrum was activated in the N-back, go/no-go, and Stroop tasks. Cerebellar activation was observed (bilaterally) only in the N-back task. The thalamus was activated in the N-back (bilateral) and go/no-go (right thalamus) tasks.

Schizophrenic Patients

Subregions within the DLPFC were activated in the N-back, delayed match-to-sample, and go/no-go tasks. Conversely, the VLPFC was activated only in the Stroop task. The ACC was activated in the N-back and go/no-go tasks. The premotor cortex was activated in the N-back and Stroop tasks. The postcentral gyrus was activated in the Stroop task. In the parietal lobe, significant activation was observed in the superior parietal lobule in the N-back and delayed match-to-sample, in the inferior parietal lobule bilaterally in the N-back, and in the right hemisphere on the go/no-go tasks. The cuneus was activated in the Stroop task. Unlike controls, the patients showed activation in the insula with the N-back, delayed match-to-sample (right), and Stroop tasks. Cerebellar activation was observed only in the N-back task. The left thalamus was activated only in the Stroop task.

BETWEEN-GROUP GLOBAL RESULTS OF FRACTIONAL SIMILARITY NETWORK ANALYSIS

Areas of schizophrenic hypoactivation that co-occurred across the full set of studies included the bilateral DLPFC, the right ACC, and the left mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus (Figure 3 and Table 4). The bilateral claus-trum represented a second set of co-occurring brain regions that were impaired in the patients.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Co-occurring brain areas with significant differences between controls and schizophrenic patients across executive function studies. The 2 sets of clusters are arbitrarily indicated by color to distinguish them. L indicates left; R, right.

Table 4.

Brain Regions With Significant Between-Group Differences in a Co-occurring Manner

Brain Regiona Volume,
mm3
Brodmann
Area
Talairach
Coordinates,
x, y, z
Greater activity in controls than schizophrenic patients
 Left middle frontal gyrus 1456 9 −38, 30, 30
 Right middle frontal gyrus 696 9 6, 42, 18
 Right anterior cingulate gyrus 792 32 2, 18, 34
 Left claustrum 488 −28, 24, 0
 Right claustrum 936 26, 22, 2
 Left thalamus, medial dorsal nucleus 760 −4, −14, 10
Greater activity in schizophrenic patients than controls
 Left anterior cingulate gyrus 1256 32 −2, 10, 40
 Left inferior parietal lobule 584 40 −54, −42, 42
a

Regions are listed in hierarchical order based on the following nested criteria: cortical to subcortical, rostral to caudal, dorsal to ventral, left to right, and large to small.

Analysis revealed 2 regions where patients activated more than controls across all studies, including the left ACC and left inferior parietal lobule (Figure 3 and Table 4). As with the between-group analyses reported above, the ACC sub-region was dorsal and posterior to the ACC subregion, where the patients exhibited impaired activity.

COMMENT

Using a quantitative meta-analysis of 41 functional neuroimaging studies of executive functioning in schizophrenia, we found evidence of a superordinate, general-purpose cognitive control network that is associated with executive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Within-group analysis of all of the 41 studies indicated that healthy controls and schizophrenic patients activated a similarly distributed cortical-subcortical network while performing executive tasks, including the DLPFC, ACC, VLPFC, pre-motor cortex, lateral temporal cortical areas, parietal areas, cerebellum, and thalamus. Nevertheless, in direct between-group comparisons, schizophrenic patients exhibited reduced activation in several key nodes of this network, including the bilateral DLPFC, right VLPFC, right dorsal ACC, pre-SMA, left ventral premotor cortex, posterior areas in the temporal and parietal cortex, and sub-cortical areas, such as the mediodorsal thalamus and putamen. These results did not appear to reflect the inordinate influence of N-back studies. Of these regions, the DLPFC, ACC, and mediodorsal thalamus showed significant co-occurrence in between-group comparisons.

Increased activation in a frontocingulate network has been widely reported during normal executive functions60 and is consistent with models of cognitive control,7,8 which propose that the lateral PFC provides top-down control to establish an optimal pattern of processing across the brain to support task-appropriate responding. Consistent with this view, individual differences in DLPFC activation often correlate with superior task performance among the healthy subjects in these studies.61-63 Within this model, the ACC monitors performance, is sensitive to levels of conflict present during information processing, and serves to modulate the level of DLPFC task–related engagement in a dynamic manner.8,9 The consistent reduction of DLPFC and ACC activity observed in this meta-analysis is consistent with impairment in this dynamic cognitive control-related circuitry in schizophrenia. As noted, a broad network of frontal, subcortical, and posterior brain regions that support task performance were reduced in schizophrenic disrupted frontal-based top-down control functions (elaborated on in the Miller and Cohen “guided activation” model7) that lead to a disruption of processing across the distributed brain network supporting task performance.

In contrast, patients with schizophrenia showed relatively greater activity in a region in the VLPFC; a midline cortical region located in the ACC extending into the SMA, which was dorsal and posterior to the ACC area showing reduced activity; posterior and inferior cortical areas (in the temporal and parietal cortex); the insula; and the amygdala. It is possible that these regions are associated with a compensatory response and/or are recruited to support alternate strategies to support task performance. With impaired DLPFC regulation of the distributed network engaged by task demands, patients may increase engagement of other processes to maintain task performance, such as attentional, mnemonic, and performance monitoring functions. These would be expected to manifest as relative hyperactivations in the ventral, medial, or posterior cortical regions. In addition, amygdala and insula activation clusters could reflect a differing emotional reactivity to task demands. This account could be compatible with a popular inefficiency hypothesis, as the compensatory hyperactivations could reasonably be viewed as reflecting an excessive distribution of cortical activity that is more restricted to the DLPFC and its tight control of these areas under normal (ie, healthy) conditions. A second possibility is that this profile of activity increases and decreases constitutes a disease-specific variation in the topographic basis for cognitive control and related executive functions. In this scenario, the topography of activity engaged during performance of these tasks is displaced for patients, giving rise to areas of relative hypoactivity adjacent to those with relative hyperactivity. This pattern of results does suggest a few adjacent regions with this pattern, notably in the medial wall of the PFC; however, this is not a comprehensive pattern in the present results, which suggests that other factors are at work. In any event, the present results taken together strongly argue against asimplehypofrontalityvshyperfrontalityaccountofthe altered function of the frontal cortex in schizophrenia.

Within the healthy control group, the distributed cognitive control network was engaged comparably across various executive function tasks, including the lateral PFC; premotor cortex; posterior neocortical areas, such as the parietal cortex and precuneus; and the thalamus. There were nonetheless some interesting task-specific areas of activation, which may be related to particular demands on certain component cognitive processes in these tasks. These include medial PFC (ACC and SMA/pre-SMA) activation in the Stroop and N-back tasks, potentially a function of conflict-processing demands; lateral (neocortical) temporal lobe and supramarginal gyrus activation in the Stroop task, both likely a function of linguistic processing involved in this task; and cerebellar activation in the N-back task, which may be related to the degree of temporal sequencing in processing of stimuli in this task. Notably, delayed match-to-sample performance was unassociated with above-threshold DLPFC activation (though significant VLPFC activity was evident), suggesting that these tasks were effectively performed by controls using simple maintenance strategies, obviating the need for higher-order dorsal PFC–mediated control.

The degree of task-specific variation appeared roughly comparable in the schizophrenia group, with lateral PFC activation in each task and similar variation in the observation of activation in midline PFC areas; in posterior cortical areas, such as the parietal cortex and cuneus; and in other elements of this distributed circuit, such as the cerebellum and thalamus. Inferences regarding which task-related areas of activation are significantly different between the 2 subject groups are best appreciated in the direct between-group comparisons described above.

A few limitations in this study are apparent. Activation likelihood estimation requires that source reports present data in 3-dimensional coordinates in a standard brain space and excludes studies that report only region-of-interest findings. However, the vast majority of published neuroimaging studies, including those focused on schizophrenia, report voxel-wise analyses in standard brain space.12,64 As a result, we included the largest set of studies of this kind to date in a quantitative meta-analysis. A further limitation is the relatively small set sizes for individual task types (other than the N-back). Therefore, results of the other major task types should be interpreted with caution. The future expansion of this primary source literature should enhance the reliability of meta-analytic approaches to these studies. Finally, in this type of meta-analysis, it would be generally desirable to have the capability to evaluate a range of study-wise factors that may be associated with variation in reported effects. These factors may include subject-specific factors, such as clinical or demographic factors or variation in sample size, and variation in data acquisition and analysis, which may affect both effect sizes and the brain topography of these effects. The considerable variation in study design and analysis and clinical measures used among the source studies (Table 1) unfortunately precludes a quantitative assessment of these factors. Given this variation, it is remarkable that a number of reasonably predictable and coherent results were found. This suggests a degree of robustness in the present results and that we have achieved a fair view of the landscape of this literature, which is a distinct advantage of meta-analysis in general.10

CONCLUSIONS

The association of deficits in executive cognitive functions with reduced functions of a frontal-cortical–based cognitive control system in the brain has important implications for both the pathophysiology of cognitive pathology in the illness as well as for the development of therapies targeting this disabling aspect of the illness. For instance, there could be a single or a few pathologic processes that manifest throughout this network that give rise to the observed findings. These processes could include (but are not limited to) developmental processes, which might include genes regulating neural development, and neurotransmitter elements that regulate signaling activity throughout this network, which could include systems like glutamate, γ-aminobutyric acid, and/or monoamines. These results do not rule out a multiple-hit model of underlying cellular/molecular pathology; rather, it seems unlikely that this pathology would manifest in a widely coincident yet independent gross anatomic pattern. An alternative account of the present findings, which emphasizes the fractional similarity network analysis results, posits the DLPFC/ACC/mediodorsal thalamus triad as a core deficit, with the dysfunction elsewhere in the network as a downstream functional consequence of this disturbance. In any event, the robust meta-analytic results found across this heterogeneous set of studies reaffirms the reliability of functional magnetic resonance imaging to assess the functional neuroanatomy of schizophrenia. Treatment implications suggest that, to the extent that a unitary pathophysiological process is evident, a more unitary intervention strategy might be adopted to target the discrete neural system serving these general-purpose cognitive control functions.

Acknowledgments

Funding/Support: This work was supported by grants R01-MH074457- 01A1 from the Human Brain Project of the National Institute of Mental Health, UL1 RR024146 from the National Center for Research Resources (Dr Minzenberg), and MH059883 from the National Institutes of Health (Dr Carter).

Footnotes

Financial Disclosure: None reported.

Previous Presentation: Portions of this work were presented at the annual meetings of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, Boca Raton, Florida, December 12, 2007; and the Society of Biological Psychiatry, Washington, DC, May 3, 2008.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Zec RF. Physiologic dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia, I: regional cerebral blood flow evidence. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1986;43(2):114–124. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1986.01800020020004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Glahn DC, Ragland JD, Abramoff A, Barrett J, Laird AR, Bearden CE, Velligan DI. Beyond hypofrontality: a quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of working memory in schizophrenia. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25(1):60–69. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Stevens AA, Goldman-Rakic PS, Gore JC, Fulbright RK, Wexler BE. Cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia during auditory word and tone working memory demonstrated by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55(12):1097–1103. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.12.1097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Carter CS, Perlstein W, Ganguli R, Brar J, Mintun M, Cohen JD. Functional hypofrontality and working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155(9):1285–1287. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.9.1285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW, III, Johnson MK, Stenger VA, Aizenstein H, Carter CS. Decreased conflict- and error-related activity in the anterior cingulate cortex in subjects with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(10):1833–1839. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Barch DM, Csernansky JG, Conturo T, Snyder AZ. Working and long-term memory deficits in schizophrenia: is there a common prefrontal mechanism? J Abnorm Psychol. 2002;111(3):478–494. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.111.3.478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW, III, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science. 2004;303(5660):1023–1026. doi: 10.1126/science.1089910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol Rev. 2001;108(3):624–652. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.3.624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Rosenthal R, DiMatteo MR. Meta-analysis: recent developments in quantitative methods for literature reviews. Annu Rev Psychol. 2001;52:59–82. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Laird AR, Fox PM, Price CJ, Glahn DC, Uecker AM, Lancaster JL, Turkeltaub PE, Kochunov P, Fox PT. ALE meta-analysis: controlling the false discovery rate and performing statistical contrasts. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25(1):155–164. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Fox PT, Laird AR, Lancaster JL. Coordinate-based voxel-wise meta-analysis: dividends of spatial normalization: report of a virtual workshop. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25(1):1–5. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Crespo-Facorro B, Wiser AK, Andreasen NC, O'Leary DS, Watkins GL, Boles Ponto LL, Hichwa RD. Neural basis of novel and well-learned recognition memory in schizophrenia: a positron emission tomography study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2001;12(4):219–231. doi: 10.1002/1097-0193(200104)12:4&#x0003c;219::AID-HBM1017&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-L. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Manoach DS, Gollub RL, Benson ES, Searl MM, Goff DC, Halpern E, Saper CB, Rauch SL. Schizophrenic subjects show aberrant fMRI activation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia during working memory performance. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;48(2):99–109. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00227-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Manoach DS, White N, Lindgren KA, Heckers S, Coleman MJ, Dubal S, Goff DC, Holzman PS. Intact hemispheric specialization for spatial and shape working memory in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2005;78(1):1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Quintana J, Wong T, Ortiz-Portillo E, Kovalik E, Davidson T, Marder SR, Mazziotta JC. Prefrontal-posterior parietal networks in schizophrenia: primary dysfunctions and secondary compensations. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53(1):12–24. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01435-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Tan HY, Choo WC, Fones CS, Chee MW. fMRI study of maintenance and manipulation processes within working memory in first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(10):1849–1858. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Carter CS, MacDonald AW, III, Ross LL, Stenger VA. Anterior cingulate cortex activity and impaired self-monitoring of performance in patients with schizophrenia: an event-related fMRI study. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(9):1423–1428. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.9.1423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Laurens KR, Ngan ET, Bates AT, Kiehl KA, Liddle PF. Rostral anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction during error processing in schizophrenia. Brain. 2003;126(pt 3):610–622. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.MacDonald AW, III, Carter CS. Event-related FMRI study of context processing in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of patients with schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2003;112(4):689–697. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.MacDonald AW, III, Carter CS, Kerns JG, Ursu S, Barch DM, Holmes AJ, Stenger VA, Cohen JD. Specificity of prefrontal dysfunction and context processing deficits to schizophrenia in never-medicated patients with first-episode psychosis. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(3):475–484. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.3.475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Rubia K, Russell T, Bullmore ET, Soni W, Brammer MJ, Simmons A, Taylor E, Andrew C, Giampietro V, Sharma T. An fMRI study of reduced left prefrontal activation in schizophrenia during normal inhibitory function. Schizophr Res. 2001;52(1-2):47–55. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(00)00173-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hugdahl K, Rund BR, Lund A, Asbjornsen A, Egeland J, Ersland L, Landrø NI, Roness A, Stordal KI, Sundet K, Thomsen T. Brain activation measured with fMRI during a mental arithmetic task in schizophrenia and major depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(2):286–293. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.2.286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Callicott JH, Bertolino A, Mattay VS, Langheim FJ, Duyn J, Coppola R, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR. Physiological dysfunction of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia revisited. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10(11):1078–1092. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.11.1078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Callicott JH, Mattay VS, Verchinski BA, Marenco S, Egan MF, Weinberger DR. Complexity of prefrontal cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia: more than up or down. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(12):2209–2215. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.12.2209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Honey GD, Bullmore ET, Soni W, Varatheesan M, Williams SC, Sharma T. Differences in frontal cortical activation by a working memory task after substitution of risperidone for typical antipsychotic drugs in patients with schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1999;96(23):13432–13437. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.23.13432. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Honey GD, Bullmore ET, Sharma T. De-coupling of cognitive performance and cerebral functional response during working memory in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2002;53(1-2):45–56. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00154-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Honey GD, Sharma T, Suckling J, Giampietro V, Soni W, Williams SC, Bullmore ET. The functional neuroanatomy of schizophrenic subsyndromes. Psychol Med. 2003;33(6):1007–1018. doi: 10.1017/s0033291703007864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Jacobsen LK, D'Souza DC, Mencl WE, Pugh KR, Skudlarski P, Krystal JH. Nicotine effects on brain function and functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55(8):850–858. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.12.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Jansma JM, Ramsey NF, van der Wee NJ, Kahn RS. Working memory capacity in schizophrenia: a parametric fMRI study. Schizophr Res. 2004;68(2-3):159–171. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00127-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Kim JJ, Kwon JS, Park HJ, Youn T, Kang DH, Kim MS, Lee DS, Lee MC. Functional disconnection between the prefrontal and parietal cortices during working memory processing in schizophrenia: a[15(O)]H2O PET study. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(5):919–923. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.5.919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Meisenzahl EM, Scheuerecker J, Zipse M, Ufer S, Wiesmann M, Frodl T, Koutsouleris N, Zetzsche T, Schmitt G, Riedel M, Spellmann I, Dehning S, Linn J, Brückmann H, Möller HJ. Effects of treatment with the atypical neuroleptic quetiapine on working memory function: a functional MRI follow-up investigation. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;256(8):522–531. doi: 10.1007/s00406-006-0687-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Mendrek A, Laurens KR, Kiehl KA, Ngan ET, Stip E, Liddle PF. Changes in distributed neural circuitry function in patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. 2004;185:205–214. doi: 10.1192/bjp.185.3.205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Mendrek A, Kiehl KA, Smith AM, Irwin D, Forster BB, Liddle PF. Dysfunction of a distributed neural circuitry in schizophrenia patients during a working-memory performance. Psychol Med. 2005;35(2):187–196. doi: 10.1017/s0033291704003228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Meyer-Lindenberg A, Poline JB, Kohn PD, Holt JL, Egan MF, Weinberger DR, Berman KF. Evidence for abnormal cortical functional connectivity during working memory in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(11):1809–1817. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Sabri O, Owega A, Schreckenberger M, Sturz L, Fimm B, Kunert P, Meyer PT, Sander D, Klingelhöfer J. A truly simultaneous combination of functional transcranial Doppler sonography and H(2)(15)O PET adds fundamental new information on differences in cognitive activation between schizophrenics and healthy control subjects. J Nucl Med. 2003;44(5):671–681. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Salgado-Pineda P, Junque C, Vendrell P, Baeza I, Bargallo N, Falcon C, Bernardo M. Decreased cerebral activation during CPT performance: structural and functional deficits in schizophrenic patients. Neuroimage. 2004;21(3):840–847. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.10.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Schneider F, Habel U, Reske M, Kellermann T, Stocker T, Shah NJ, Zilles K, Braus DF, Schmitt A, Schlösser R, Wagner M, Frommann I, Kircher T, Rapp A, Meisenzahl E, Ufer S, Ruhrmann S, Thienel R, Sauer H, Henn FA, Gaebel W. Neural correlates of working memory dysfunction in first-episode schizophrenia patients: an fMRI multi-center study. Schizophr Res. 2007;89(1-3):198–210. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.07.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Tan HY, Sust S, Buckholtz JW, Mattay VS, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Egan MF, Weinberger DR, Callicott JH. Dysfunctional prefrontal regional specialization and compensation in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(11):1969–1977. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.11.1969. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Walter H, Wunderlich AP, Blankenhorn M, Schafer S, Tomczak R, Spitzer M, Grön G. No hypofrontality, but absence of prefrontal lateralization comparing verbal and spatial working memory in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2003;61(2-3):175–184. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(02)00225-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Wykes T, Brammer M, Mellers J, Bray P, Reeder C, Williams C, Corner J. Effects on the brain of a psychological treatment—cognitive remediation therapy: functional magnetic resonance imaging in schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. 2002;181:144–152. doi: 10.1017/s0007125000161872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Yoo SS, Choi BG, Juh RH, Park JM, Pae CU, Kim JJ, Lee SJ, Lee C, Paik IH, Lee CU. Working memory processing of facial images in schizophrenia: fMRI investigation. Int J Neurosci. 2005;115(3):351–366. doi: 10.1080/00207450590520957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Heckers S, Weiss AP, Deckersbach T, Goff DC, Morecraft RJ, Bush G. Anterior cingulate cortex activation during cognitive interference in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(4):707–715. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.4.707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Laurens KR, Kiehl KA, Ngan ET, Liddle PF. Attention orienting dysfunction during salient novel stimulus processing in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2005;75(2-3):159–171. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Johnson MR, Morris NA, Astur RS, Calhoun VD, Mathalon DH, Kiehl KA, Pearlson GD. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of working memory abnormalities in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(1):11–21. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.11.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Seok Jeong B, Kwon JS, Yoon Kim S, Lee C, Youn T, Moon CH, Yoon Kim C. Functional imaging evidence of the relationship between recurrent psychotic episodes and neurodegenerative course in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2005;139(3):219–228. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2004.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Weiss EM, Golaszewski S, Mottaghy FM, Hofer A, Hausmann A, Kemmler G, Kremser C, Brinkhoff C, Felber SR, Fleischhacker WW. Brain activation patterns during a selective attention test: a functional MRI study in healthy volunteers and patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2003;123(1):1–15. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(03)00019-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Weiss EM, Siedentopf C, Golaszewski S, Mottaghy FM, Hofer A, Kremser C, Felber S, Fleischhacker WW. Brain activation patterns during a selective attention test: a functional MRI study in healthy volunteers and unmedicated patients during an acute episode of schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2007;154(1):31–40. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Ragland JD, Gur RC, Glahn DC, Censits DM, Smith RJ, Lazarev MG, Alavi A, Gur RE. Frontotemporal cerebral blood flow change during executive and declarative memory tasks in schizophrenia: a positron emission tomography study. Neuropsychology. 1998;12(3):399–413. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.12.3.399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Curtis VA, Dixon TA, Morris RG, Bullmore ET, Brammer MJ, Williams SC, Sharma T, Murray RM, McGuire PK. Differential frontal activation in schizophrenia and bipolar illness during verbal fluency. J Affect Disord. 2001;66(2-3):111–121. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(00)00240-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Weiss EM, Hofer A, Golaszewski S, Siedentopf C, Brinkhoff C, Kremser C, Felber S, Fleischhacker WW. Brain activation patterns during a verbal fluency test-a functional MRI study in healthy volunteers and patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004;70(2-3):287–291. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Turkeltaub PE, Eden GF, Jones KM, Zeffiro TA. Meta-analysis of the functional neuroanatomy of single-word reading: method and validation. Neuroimage. 2002;16(3, pt 1):765–780. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Laird AR, Lancaster JL, Fox PT. BrainMap: the social evolution of a human brain mapping database. Neuroinformatics. 2005;3(1):65–78. doi: 10.1385/ni:3:1:065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Fox PT, Lancaster JL. Opinion: mapping context and content: the BrainMap model. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(4):319–321. doi: 10.1038/nrn789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Talairach J, Tournoux P. Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. Thieme; New York, NY: 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Lancaster JL, Tordesillas-Gutierrez D, Martinez M, Salinas F, Evans A, Zilles K, Mazziotta JC, Fox PT. Bias between MNI and Talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM-152 brain template. Hum Brain Mapp. 2007;28(11):1194–1205. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Brett M, Christoff K, Cusack R, Lancaster J. Using the Talairach atlas with the MNI template [poster] Neuroimage. 2001;13(6):S85. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Kochunov P, Lancaster J, Thompson P, Toga AW, Brewer P, Hardies J, Fox P. An optimized individual target brain in the Talairach coordinate system. Neuroimage. 2002;17(2):922–927. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Lancaster JL, Laird AR, Fox PM, Glahn DE, Fox PT. Automated analysis of meta-analysis networks. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25(1):174–184. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Smith EE, Jonides J. Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science. 1999;283(5408):1657–1661. doi: 10.1126/science.283.5408.1657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.MacDonald AW, III, Cohen JD, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science. 2000;288(5472):1835–1838. doi: 10.1126/science.288.5472.1835. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Snitz BE, MacDonald A, III, Cohen JD, Cho RY, Becker T, Carter CS. Lateral and medial hypofrontality in first-episode schizophrenia: functional activity in a medication-naive state and effects of short-term atypical antipsychotic treatment. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(12):2322–2329. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.12.2322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Yoon JH, Minzenberg MJ, Ursu S, Walters BS, Wendelken C, Ragland JD, et al. Association of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex dysfunction with disrupted coordinated brain activity in schizophrenia: relationship with impaired cognition, behavioral disorganization, and global function. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(8):1006–1014. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07060945. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Fox PT, Parsons LM, Lancaster JL. Beyond the single study: function/location metanalysis in cognitive neuroimaging. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1998;8(2):178–187. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(98)80138-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES