Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Nov 15.
Published in final edited form as: Neuroimage. 2009 Dec 21;53(3):935–942. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.041

Dopamine transporter gene variation modulates activation of striatum in youth with ADHD

Anne-Claude Bédard 1, Kurt P Schulz 1, Edwin H Cook Jr 2, Jin Fan 1,3, Suzanne M Clerkin 1, Iliyan Ivanov 1, Jeffrey M Halperin 1,4, Jeffrey H Newcorn 1
PMCID: PMC2891221  NIHMSID: NIHMS166340  PMID: 20026227

Abstract

Polymorphisms in the 3′ UTR variable number tandem repeat (VNTR) of exon 15 of the dopamine transporter gene (DAT1) have been linked to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD); moreover, variability in DAT1 3′UTR genotype may contribute to both heterogeneity of the ADHD phenotype and differences in response to stimulant medications. The impact of this VNTR on neuronal function in individuals with ADHD remains unclear despite evidence that the polymorphisms influence dopamine transporter expression. Thus, we used event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging to examine the impact of DAT1 3′UTR genotype on brain activation during response inhibition in unmedicated children and adolescents with ADHD. Twenty-one youth with ADHD who were homozygous for the 10-repeat (10R) allele of the DAT1 3′UTR and 12 youth who were carriers of the 9-repeat (9R) allele were scanned while they performed a Go/No-Go task. Response inhibition was modeled by contrasting activation during correct No-Go trials versus correct Go trials. Participants who were homozygous for the DAT1 3′UTR 10R allele and those who had a single 9R allele did not differ on percent of trials with successful inhibition, which was the primary measure of inhibitory control. Yet, youth with the DAT1 3′UTR 10R/10R genotype had significantly greater inhibitory control-related activation than those with one 9R allele in the left striatum, right dorsal premotor cortex, and bilaterally in the temporoparietal cortical junction. These findings provide preliminary evidence that neural activity related to inhibitory control may differ as a function of DAT1 3′UTR genotype in youth with ADHD.

Introduction

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most common neurobehavioral disorders of childhood, with worldwide prevalence rates estimated at 4–10% (as reviewed by Skounti et al., 2007). Genetic studies have demonstrated that ADHD is highly heritable, with estimates ranging from 0.60 to 0.90 (Faraone et al., 2005). Candidate gene studies of ADHD have implicated several dopamine genes in the etiology of the disorder. Recent meta-analyses reported that several genes showed statistically significant evidence of association with ADHD: four of which were dopamine system genes (the dopamine D4 and D5 receptor genes, the dopamine transporter gene (DAT1), and the dopamine beta-hydroxylase gene) (Faraone and Khan, 2006; Gizer et al., 2009). Of these, the dopamine transporter is a principal target of stimulant medications (Zhu and Reith, 2008), which are the primary treatments for ADHD.

DAT1 (SLC6A3) is a membrane protein that binds dopamine and provides the primary mechanism through which dopamine is cleared from synapses (Bannon et al., 2001). DAT1 is found in brain regions where dopamine signaling is widespread, including the striatum, nucleus accumbens, anterior cingulate, posterior parietal cortex, and hippocampus (Ciliax et al., 1999; Lewis et al., 2001). The gene for DAT1 contains 15 exons, is located on chromosome 5p15.3, and has a protein encoding region which is over 64 kb long (Kawarai et al., 1997). Of note, the DAT1 gene has a polymorphic variable number tandem repeat (VNTR) which is 40 bp in length and is located in the 3′ UTR of exon 15. The 10-repeat (10R) and nine-repeat (9R) alleles of this VNTR are the most frequently occurring (Kang et al., 1999; Vandenbergh et al., 1992). Although several studies have reported a significant association between the DAT1 3′ UTR 10R allele and ADHD (e.g., Cook et al., 1995; Hawi et al., 2009; see meta-analysis by Yang et al., 2007), others have suggested that it may be the 9R allele that is preferentially transmitted in individuals with ADHD (Franke et al., 2008), and some have reported no association between DAT1 and ADHD (Johansson et al., 2008; Langley et al., 2005) (as reviewed by Plomp et al., 2009).

Although the functional significance of the different DAT1 3′ UTR polymorphisms is still unclear, it has been shown that the 10R variant is associated with greater levels of dopamine transporter protein than the 9R form (Brookes et al., 2007; Fuke et al., 2001; Heinz et al., 2000; Mill et al., 2002; VanNess et al., 2005). Because the variation is located in the 3′ UTR, the reported increase in protein expression is likely a result of changes in mRNA stability or protein translation. Accordingly, the increased levels of dopamine transporter in individuals who carry the 10R allele may directly lead to the decreased levels of synaptic dopamine that have been linked to ADHD (Volkow et al., 2007). However, this mechanism of action has been questioned by reports of both lower dopamine transporter density for individuals with 10R-homozygosity compared to carriers of a 9R allele in healthy subjects (Jacobsen et al., 2000; van Dyck et al., 2005), and no difference in dopamine transporter density with differing DAT1 3′ UTR genotypes in both ADHD and normal control samples (Krause et al., 2006; Martinez et al., 2001).

Recent neuroimaging studies have provided preliminary evidence that the DAT1 3′ UTR genotype influences cognition-related brain activity. However, the functional impact of the various DAT1 3′ UTR alleles is not fully understood, with reports linking the 10R allele to both lower activation during working memory in the prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices in healthy adults (Bertolino et al., 2006) and greater activation during executive functions in the anterior insula and caudate nucleus in adults with and without schizophrenia (Prata et al., 2009). The impact of the DAT1 3′ UTR genotype is similarly variable in individuals with ADHD. Adults with ADHD homozygous for the 10R allele showed dorsal anterior cingulate hypoactivation during executive function compared to 9R-carriers (Brown et al., 2009), while boys with ADHD and their unaffected siblings who were 10R homozygotes had reduced striatal activity but increased cerebellar activation during response inhibition compared to carriers of the 9R allele (Durston et al., 2008). In addition, there were no differences in activation in other brain regions where DAT1 is known to be expressed such as the posterior parietal cortex and hippocampus (Ciliax et al., 1999; Lewis et al., 2001). These intriguing, albeit somewhat inconsistent findings clearly warrant further investigation.

The current study was designed to further investigate differential brain activation as a function of the DAT1 3′ UTR polymorphism in a larger sample of youth with ADHD. Based on the findings of Durston et al. (2008), we hypothesized that youth with ADHD who were homozygous for the DAT1 3′ UTR 10R allele would show reduced striatal activity compared to those who were heterozygous carriers of the 9R allele. We also hoped to identify other dopaminergically-innervated brain regions that might be differentially affected by DAT1 3′UTR genotype. Specifically, we used fMRI to compare brain activation during successful inhibition in a Go/No-Go task in 33 children with ADHD.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Thirty-three children and adolescents (24 males, 9 females), aged 7 to 16 years, were participants of an NIH-funded treatment study examining the differential effects of treatment with methylphenidate and atomoxetine (Table 1). All participants met Diagnostic and Statistical Manual-4th Edition (DSM-IV) (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria for ADHD using the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Aged Children-Present and Lifetime Versions (K-SADS-PL) (Kaufman et al., 1997), and were rated at least one and a half standard deviations above age and gender norms on the ADHD Rating Scale-IV-Parent Version: Investigator Administered (ADHD-RS-IV) (DuPaul et al., 1998). Children who did not respond to prior treatment with methylphenidate or atomoxetine, or had experienced adverse effects, were excluded from the study. Other exclusion criteria included substance abuse history or a positive urine screen, participation in a treatment study in the past 30 days, a past or present diagnosis of a psychotic disorder, a history of head injury with loss of consciousness, neurological or cardiovascular disease, IQ < 75 and any other condition that could affect brain function. Twenty-one participants were medication-naïve; all other participants were medication-free for a minimum of two weeks prior to the fMRI scan. There was no significant difference in the severity of symptoms on the ADHD-RS-IV between the treatment-naïve and previously treated participants (p > .05).

Table 1.

Demographic and clinical characteristics of youth with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) by DAT1 3′UTR genotype

DAT1 3′UTR 9R
DAT1 3′UTR 10R/10R
Characteristic n = 12 n = 21
Age (years) 11.0 ± 2.6 11.2 ± 2.4
Malea 5 (42%) 19 (90%)
Right handed 10 (83%) 18 (90%)
Ethnicity
 African-American 3 (25%) 11 (52%)
 Hispanic 4 (33%) 6 (29%)
 Caucasian 3 (25%) 4 (19%)
 Asian 0 (0%) 1 (5%)
 Biracial 2 (17%) 2 (9%)
ADHD subtype
 Combined 6 (50%) 11 (52%)
 Inattentive 5 (42%) 10 (48%)
 Hyperactive-Impulsive 1 (8%) 0 (0%)
Comorbid diagnosis of ODD 4 (33%) 8 (38%)
ADHDRS-IV
 Hyperactivity-impulsivity 17.0 ± 5.6 15.2 ± 8.5
 Inattention 21.2 ± 4.5 22.5 ± 3.1
Prior treatment for ADHD 6 (50%) 6 (29%)
a

Significant gender difference (χ2 = 9.2, p = 0.002).

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Mount Sinai School of Medicine. Written informed consent was obtained from parents; child assent was also obtained, and certified by a witness unaffiliated with the study. Participants and their parents received modest compensation for participation. Consent for the study was obtained for 29 additional youth with ADHD who did not successfully complete the procedures, 19 for excessive motion or anxiety during the scan, five who did not have enough correct trials, four for whom DAT1 3′UTR genotype was not available and one who had a DAT1 3′ UTR 8R/10R genotype and did not fit into either of the genotype groups under investigation. The youth who did not successfully complete the study procedures were younger than the 33 study participants (p < 0.01), but did not otherwise differ in gender or ADHD severity (p > 0.05).

Genotyping

Saliva samples for DAT1 3′UTR genotyping were obtained using Oragene (DNA Genotek, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada) self-collection vials. After extraction, DNA was quantitated with Quant-iT PicoGreen dsDNA Assay (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA) and normalized to a concentration of 10ng/uL. DNA was amplified with NED labeled forward primer 5′-NED-TGT GGT GTA GGG AAC GGC CTG AG-3′ (AppliedBiosystems, Foster City, CA) and reverse primer (with pigtail sequence in parentheses) 5′-(GTTTCTT) GGT CTG CGG TGG AGT CTG-3′ (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA) using Dynazyme EXT Polymerase (Finnzymes, Espoo, Finland) with an initial denaturation step of 96°C for 12min followed by 45 cycles of 96°C for 30sec, 68°C for 45sec, 72°C for 3min, one hold at 72°C for 10min, and a final hold at 10°C. Products were separated on a 3730 Genetic Analyzer (Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA) in the UIC Research Resources Center DNA Services Facility. Alleles were called blind to phenotype data using Genemapper v 3.7.

The participants were classified as either homozygous carriers of the DAT1 3′UTR 10R allele (n = 21) or heterozygous carriers of a 9R allele (n = 12; 11 with a 9R/10R genotype, one with a 7R/9R genotype). The two groups did not differ in age, ADHD subtype, comorbidity, severity of ADHD, handedness or prior history of stimulant treatment (Table 1). However, the homozygous 10R group had a lower ratio of females: males than the heterozygous 9R group (p < 0.01).

Go/No-Go Task

The Go/No-Go task was designed to measure the ability to inhibit responses to rare non-targets (No-Go trials) in the context of frequent targets (Go trials) (Durston et al., 2002). This task has previously been used in longitudinal studies of children (Durston et al., 2006a), to test children with ADHD (Durston et al., 2006b), and to examine differential brain activation as a function of the DAT1 3′ UTR polymorphism in adolescents with ADHD (Durston et al., 2008). The task consisted of six runs that each lasted three minutes and 58 seconds. Each run began with 10 seconds of fixation and contained 57 trials, with 43 (75%) Go trials and 14 (25%) No-Go trials, yielding a total of 84 No-Go trials across the task. Trial order was pseudo randomized so that the occurrence of No-Go trials was jittered from four to 20 seconds (i.e., preceded by one to five Go trials). Stimuli were presented for 500 ms with an inter-stimulus interval of 3500 ms. To make the task more appealing to children, images of Spiderman and the Green Goblin from the “Spiderman” movie (© Columbia Pictures Industries, Inc.) were used as stimuli for Go and No-Go trials, respectively (Figure 1). Fixation was depicted by a small image of a spider presented at the center of the screen. Participants were reminded at the beginning of each block to respond as quickly as possible while trying not to make mistakes. The Go/No-Go task was compiled and run using E-Prime software (Schneider et al., 2002). Stimuli were projected via an SVGA projector system onto a rear-projection screen mounted at the head of the magnet bore that was viewed through a mirror on the head coil. Participants responded with the right hand using the BrainLogics fiber optic button system (Psychology Software Tools, Inc.). Responses were recorded on a desktop computer and provided measures of reaction time (RT) and accuracy.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Schematic diagram of the Go/No-Go task. The diagram illustrates a No-Go trial (Green Goblin) preceded by three Go trials (Spiderman). Stimuli were presented individually at fixation for 500ms, followed by a 3500ms interstimulus interval demarcated by a small spider. Trial order was pseudorandomized so that the occurrence of No-Go trials was jittered from 4 to 20 seconds (i.e., preceded by 1 to 5 Go trials). Participants were instructed to “press the button for all Spiderman pictures and not press for Green Goblin.” The images of Spiderman and Green Goblin were adapted from promotional images for the “Spiderman” movie (©Columbia Pictures Industries, Inc.).

Image Acquisition

Participants were scanned on a 3.0 Tesla Siemens Allegra (Siemens Medical Systems) head-dedicated MRI scanner. Scan sessions began with shimming and sagittal localization. A high-resolution T2-weighted anatomical volume of the brain was then acquired in the axial plane with a turbo spin-echo (TSE) pulse sequence (TR = 5380 ms, TE = 99 msec, flip angle = 170°, FOV = 210 mm, matrix = 512 × 336, 28 slices, slice thickness = 4 mm contiguous, in-plane resolution = 0.41 mm2). Functional T2*-weighted images depicting the blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) signal were acquired at the same 28 slice locations using gradient-echo echo-planar images (TR = 2000 ms, TE = 40 ms, flip angle = 90°, FOV = 210 mm, matrix = 64 × 64, slice thickness = 3 mm, gap = 1mm, in-plane resolution = 3.28 mm2). All images were acquired with slices positioned parallel to the anterior commissure – posterior commissure line.

Data Analysis

The percent of successful inhibitions on No-Go trials served as the measure of response inhibition on the Go/No-Go task. Responses on Go trials faster than 150 ms were considered anticipatory and classified as fast Go responses, while responses with RTs greater than three standard deviations above the individual participant’s mean RT were classified as slow Go responses. Responses on Go trials with a RT between these two values were defined as correct hits and served as the primary measure of habitual motor responding. Mean RT was calculated only for correct hits. The effects of DAT1 3′ UTR genotype on Go/No-Go task performance were analyzed with independent samples t tests, in which the behavioral performance of 9R participants was compared to those homozygous for the 10R allele. All reported p values are two-tailed, with a significance level of 0.05.

Functional MR images were preprocessed and analyzed using statistical parametric mapping (SPM2; Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, UK). The six functional time series acquired for each participant were realigned to the first volume in the first series (motion-corrected) and the resultant realignment parameters were examined for motion during each of the time series. Functional series with more than one voxel (4 mm) of motion were dropped from the analyses. The two groups did not differ in the mean number of functional time series or correct Go and No-Go trials (all p < 0.05). The remaining functional time series were coregistered to the T2 anatomical images, normalized to the standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template, and re-sampled using a sinc interpolation, which yielded a voxel size of 2 mm3. The images were then spatially smoothed with an isotropic 8 mm full-width-at-half-maximum Gaussian kernel.

A general linear model (GLM) was conducted to determine the relationship between observed event-related BOLD signals and regressors that represented expected neural responses to events. Regressors were created by convolving a train of delta functions that represented the individual trial events with the SPM2 base function, which consisted of a synthetic hemodynamic response function, composed of two gamma functions and their derivatives (Friston et al., 1998). There were six regressors that corresponded to the behavioral analyses, representing: (1) correct No-Go events; (2) No-Go errors (false alarms); (3) correct Go events; (4) Go errors (misses); (5) fast Go responses; and (6) slow Go responses. The six parameters created during motion correction and the global mean of all voxels were entered as covariates of no interest in the GLM (Johnstone et al., 2006). A 0.0125 Hz high-pass filter, a first-order autoregressive function, and global mean scaling of the signal value across scans were applied to the GLM. The neural effect of response inhibition was tested by applying appropriate linear contrasts to the parameter estimates for correct No-Go events minus correct Go events, resulting in a contrast map for each participant.

The contrast images of all participants were entered into second level group analyses that used random-effects statistical models. An initial analysis used a one-sample t-test to confirm the neural effects of response inhibition in the whole sample of youth with ADHD. The a priori hypotheses were tested with planned contrasts that used two-sample t-tests to compare inhibitory-related neural activation in homozygous carriers of the DAT1 3′UTR 10R allele versus individuals who carried the 9R allele. The height (intensity) threshold for each activated voxel was set at an uncorrected p value of 0.05 and the resultant voxel-wise statistical maps were thresholded for significance using a cluster-size algorithm that protects against false-positive results (Hayasaka et al., 2004). A Monte Carlo simulation established that a cluster extent of 100 contiguous resampled voxels (2 × 2 × 2 mm3) was necessary to correct for multiple voxel comparisons at p < 0.05. The MNI coordinates of any significant activations were converted to the Talairach and Tournoux (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988) atlas system using a nonlinear transformation (http://www.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/Imaging/mnispace.html). To illustrate the significant DAT1 3′UTR genotype X trial type interaction in the left striatum, volumes of interest (VOI) were extracted from a priori activated regions of interest with a radius of 6 mm centered at the local maximum of the mass, located at Talaraich coordinates: x = −16, y = −1, and z = 9.

Results

Task Performance

Performance measures on the Go/No-Go task by DAT1 3′UTR genotype are presented in Table 2. There were no differences between the groups on the percent of successful inhibitions on No-Go trials or any other measures of performance, with one exception. Mean RT on Go trials was faster in youth homozygous for the 10R allele compared to those with the 9R allele (t1,31 = 2.40, p = 0.02). To ensure that there was no confound of gender, a 2 × 2 factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to examine the influence of gender and DAT1 3′UTR genotype on mean RT. This analysis demonstrated no main effects of gender (F1, 29 = 0.97, p = 0.35) and no gender X DAT1 3′UTR interaction (F1, 29 = 0.20, p = 0.66) on mean RT. Further, the ANOVA, in contrast to the t test, revealed no main effect of DAT1 3′UTR genotype on RT (F1, 29 = 1.44, p = 0.24).

Table 2.

Behavioral performance on the Go/No-Go Task of youth with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) by DAT1 3′UTR genotype

DAT1 3′UTR 9R
DAT1 3′UTR 10R/10R
Measure n = 12 n = 21
Successful inhibition (%) 79.6 ± 13.5 74.6 ± 14.0
Accurate responses (%) 92.9 ± 8.4 94.0 ± 5.8
Mean RT (ms)a 530 ± 117 460 ± 51
RTSD (ms) 149 ± 60 117 ± 61

RT, reaction time; RTSD, reaction time standard deviation.

a

Significant group difference (t1,31 = 2.40, p = .02).

Functional Imaging

The successful inhibition of responses on No-Go trials produced significant neural activation in the whole sample of youth with ADHD in a frontostriatal network implicated in response inhibition, including the left inferior frontal gyrus, right middle frontal gyrus, anterior cingulate gyrus, and anterior insula cortex, and bilateral striatum (see Supplementary Table 1). Robust activation was also seen more dorsally in the right posterior cingulate cortex, left precuneus, and bilaterally in the temporoparietal cortical junction (TPJ).

Direct comparison of youth heterozygous for the DAT1 3′UTR 9R allele and those homozygous for the 10R allele identified significant differences in inhibition-related neural activity in several a priori regions of interest (Table 3). As shown in Figure 2, youth who were homozygous for the 10R allele showed greater task-related activation in the left striatum, right dorsal premotor cortex, and bilaterally in the TPJ compared to individuals with a 9R allele. The significant DAT1 3′UTR genotype X trial type interaction in the striatum is illustrated in Figure 3. Youth homozygous for the 10R allele also had greater activation in the left midcingulate cortex, left inferior parietal lobule, right angular gyrus, left middle occipital gyrus and right thalamus. Conversely, youth with a 9R allele showed greater task-related activation in the left inferior frontal gyrus than those homozygous for the 10R allele (Figure 2 and Table 3). The 9R allele carriers also had greater activity in the left middle frontal gyrus and inferior parietal lobule, posterior and superior to the TPJ.

Table 3.

BOLD signal increases for successful response inhibition in youth with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) who are homozygous carriers of the DAT1 3′UTR 10R allele versus those have a DAT1 3′UTR 9R allele

Talaraich coordinates
Region BA x y z Voxels t p
DAT1 3′ UTR 10R/10R > DAT1 3′UTR 9R
 R dorsal premotor cortex 6 34 −4 39 522 3.37 0.001
 L midcingulate cortex 32 −6 −1 47 396 3.26 0.001
 R temporoparietal cortical junction 40 46 −37 31 329 3.00 0.003
 L temporoparietal cortical junction 22 −55 −44 15 342 2.99 0.001
 L Inferior parietal lobule 40 −40 −24 27 196 3.10 0.002
 L inferior parietal lobule 40 −30 −35 46 351 3.45 0.001
 R angular gyrus 39 38 −70 27 1,415 3.47 0.001
 L middle occipital gyrus 19 −34 −78 24 263 2.80 0.004
 R thalamus -- 6 −21 1 353 2.95 0.003
 L striatum -- −16 −1 9 375 2.94 0.003
DAT1 3′UTR 9R > DAT1 3′UTR 10R/10R
 L inferior frontal gyrus 47 −42 37 −4 162 2.98 0.003
 L middle frontal gyrus 8 −24 33 41 131 3.45 0.001
 L inferior parietal lobule 40 −30 −50 45 125 2.84 0.004

L, left; R, right; BA, Brodmann area.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Regions showing significant differences in BOLD responses to successful response inhibition (correct No-Go events minus correct Go events) in youth with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) who were homozygous for the DAT1 3′UTR 10-repeat (10R) allele compared to those who are heterozygous for the 9-repeat (9R) allele. Arrows indicate the temporoparietal cortical junction (left section), striatum (middle section), and inferior frontal gyrus (right section). The activations were significant at p < 0.05 corrected with a voxel extent > 100 voxels. The inset depicts the position and Talairach coordinates for the sections.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Percent BOLD signal change in the striatum for successful response inhibition. Bars represent mean signal change for correct No-Go and Go events in youth with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) who were homozygous for the DAT1 3′UTR 10-repeat (10R) allele versus those heterozygous for the 9-repeat (9R) allele. Error bars represent ± 1 standard error.

Post hoc analyses were conducted with ANOVA models to examine any potential confound of gender. There were significant main effects of gender in the left midcingulate and right thalamic regions that were identified in the direct comparison of the two DAT1 3′UTR genotypes (Supplementary Table 2). There were no gender X DAT1 3′UTR genotype interactions in any region identified in the a priori contrasts of the two DAT1 3′UTR genotypes. Main effects of DAT1 3′UTR genotype were again seen in the left striatum, as well as in the other regions identified in Table 3..

Discussion

These results provide further evidence that the DAT1 3′ UTR polymorphism influences neural activity in a corticostriatal circuit implicated in the pathophysiology of ADHD. Children and adolescents with ADHD who were homozygous for the DAT1 3′UTR 10R allele showed greater activation during response inhibition in the left striatum, right dorsal premotor cortex, and bilateral TPJ than youth with ADHD who were heterozygous for the DAT1 3′UTR 9R allele despite similar behavioral task performance. Homozygosity for the 10R allele was also associated with hyperactivation in other regions that express dopamine transporters but are generally not engaged by Go/No-Go tasks, like the left middle occipital gyrus (Ciliax et al., 1999), as well as with hypoactivity in inferior frontal regions that are central to the performed task but are sparse in dopamine transporters (Ciliax et al., 1999). These findings together suggest that DAT1 3′UTR genotype influences neural activity through a combination of direct effects on dopamine function and indirect effects on the function of “downstream” regions.

The differential impact of DAT1 3′UTR genotype on activation in the striatum is of particular interest theoretically and clinically. The striatum is a major target of the dopaminergic nigrostriatal (A9) pathway (Smith and Kieval, 2000) and contains the richest concentration of dopamine transporters in the brain (Ciliax et al., 1999). Dopamine released from these nigrostriatal fibers has a critical modulatory influence on striatal medium spiny neuron signaling by boosting the effective excitatory drive of glutamatergic corticostriatal inputs (Rebec, 1998). Genetically altering dopamine transporter function has profound effects on this corticostriatal signaling (Ghisi et al., 2009; Wu et al., 2007). Overexpression of the dopamine transporter produces a marked reduction in synaptic dopamine that results in an adaptive upregulation of dopamine receptors (Ghisi et al., 2009), which in turn, strengthens corticostriatal glutamate signaling (Ghisi et al., 2009; Wu et al., 2007). These adaptive mechanisms may explain the current finding of elevated striatal activation in youth with ADHD who were homozygous for the DAT1 3′UTR 10R allele, which presumably produces more dopamine transporter protein than the 9R allele (VanNess et al., 2005). The DAT1 3′UTR genotype X trial type interaction in striatal activation in the current study suggests that these adaptive mechanisms have a particular impact on inhibitory mechanisms in youth with ADHD, consistent with both the inhibitory functions of the striatum (Aron et al., 2007) and the effects of adaptive upregulation of dopamine receptors in animals (Breese et al., 1987; Hu et al., 1990). Youth homozygous for the 10R allele used greater striatal activity to inhibit than execute simple motor responses compared to carriers of the 9R allele, who showed similar levels of activation for the two actions. Further, the 10R homozygotes required more striatal activation to achieve a similar level of inhibitory performance as carriers of the 9R allele. Although the clinical implication of these findings remains uncertain, it is possible that genetically-determined differences in striatal function contribute to heterogeneity of the ADHD phenotype, individual differences in response to stimulant medication, or individual differences in the dose required to bring about improvement – all of which have been observed in ADHD populations (as reviewed by Halperin et al., in press; Hermens et al., 2006).

The extensive output system of the striatum provides a neural substrate for the influence of DAT1 3′UTR genotype on neural activity in brain regions that are not directly innervated by dopaminergic pathways (Hoover and Strick, 1993; Tomasi et al., 2009). For example, corticostriatal excitation of striatal medium spiny neurons releases striatal output neurons from tonic inhibition, which in turn, disinhibits thalamic relay nuclei that project to premotor and other cortical areas (Kelly and Strick, 2004). Thus, alterations in striatal dopamine signaling produced by specific DAT1 3′UTR polymorphisms could have resulted in the increased parietal and temporal activation seen in youth homozygous for the 10R allele compared to those heterozygous for the 9R allele. Alternatively, increased activation in regions such as the middle occipital gyrus may reflect the effect of homozygosity for the DAT1 3′UTR 10R allele on intrinsic dopamine signaling (Ciliax et al., 1999).

The reduced task-related activation of inferior frontal gyrus seen in youth homozygous for the DAT1 3′UTR 10R allele is more difficult to comprehend. The reduced inferior frontal activation may indirectly reflect the effect of 10R-homozygosity on striatal feedback to the prefrontal cortex (Kelly and Strick, 2004). Alternately, this may reflect the uneven distribution of dopamine transporters in the prefrontal cortex (Ciliax et al., 1999; Lewis et al., 2001). This finding warrants further investigation given that this rostral-most region of the inferior frontal gyrus has been shown to convert sensory and contextual inputs into behavioral codes (Sakagami et al., 2001), to be engaged by Go/No-Go tasks (Chikazoe et al., 2007; Schulz et al., 2009), and to be hypoactivated during response inhibition in individuals with ADHD (Rubia et al., 2005; Schulz et al., 2004).

Results support the contention that striatal functioning in youth with ADHD varies as a function of genotype. However, the direction of the finding (i.e., increased or decreased activation) differs here compared to that of the Durston et al. (2008) study, despite the fact that the two studies used the same Go/No-Go task and parameters. In the latter study, inhibition-related striatal activation was lower in ten boys with ADHD who were homozygous for the DAT1 3′UTR 10R allele (Durston et al., 2008), which was interpreted as consistent with findings that homozygosity for the 10R allele produces lower dopamine transporter density in healthy subjects (van Dyck et al., 2005), possibly because reduced dopamine transporter translation produces an adaptive diminution of corticostriatal glutamate signaling (Wu et al., 2007). Discrepancies regarding the direction of striatal activation as a function of DAT1 3UTR 10R polymorphisms parallels the debate over the clinical (e.g., Cook et al., 1995; Franke et al., 2008; Hawi et al., 2009; Johansson et al., 2008) and functional significance of these genotypes (Heinz et al., 2000; Martinez et al., 2001; van Dyck et al., 2005; VanNess et al., 2005). In addition, differences in the direction of striatal activation across the studies may reflect the fact that our sample was somewhat younger, more ethnically mixed, comprised both boys and girls, and included a substantial number of subjects with predominantly inattentive ADHD compared to the sample in Durston et al. (2008). Moreover, differences in the duration of the wash-out period between the two studies (two weeks in this study vs. 24 hours in Durston et al. [2008]) should be carefully noted, since stimulant treatment discontinuation produces an upregulation of dopamine transporter density (Feron et al., 2005) and neuronal activity (Langleben et al., 2002).

Results from this study should be understood in the context of some methodological limitations. First, our sample size was modest relative to samples used in genetic studies, but it is larger than other samples that have examined imaging in the context of genetics. In addition, it is the first study of imaging genetics in youth with ADHD to include females (Durston et al., 2008; Szobot et al., 2005). Second, the study consisted of an ethnically diverse sample (Table 1), which may not be optimal for examining genetic differences in brain activation, since allelic frequencies of the DAT1 3′UTR polymorphism differ widely across ethnic populations (e.g., Kang et al., 1999). Third, we included a wide age range of youth with ADHD, potentially confounding our data with multiple stages of brain development and gene expression. To partially account for this, we excluded outliers and calculated data based on individual within-subject performance (rather than a group mean). Future studies with better power should examine the effect of brain development on genotype effects in individuals with ADHD. Also, this study was limited to a sample of youth with ADHD. As a result, we were unable to determine how the DAT1 3′UTR polymorphism-related effects we found would compare to that of typically developing youth. Accordingly, it may be that the DAT1 3′UTR effect we found is limited to only youth with ADHD, or that the magnitude of the effect could differ from that of typically developing youth. In this regard, it might potentially be interesting to examine how corticostriatal circuitry is modulated by ADHD symptom severity either independent of genotype or through interactions with genotype. This is likely to require a larger sample than we utilized in the current investigation, and therefore beyond the scope of this study. However, this would be a fertile topic for future research.

In addition to these overall design considerations, these findings must also be considered in the context of the methodological limitations of the Go/No-Go task used in this study. First, the comparison of Go trials, which required motor responses, and No-Go trials, which did not involve responses, introduced motor activity as a potential confounding factor in the analyses. This is less of an issue in group comparisons like those in the current study than in single-group designs, since the two groups serve as controls for each other. However, we cannot completely rule out that our findings reflect the effects of motor control processes rather than inhibitory control processes. Second, although the current study was designed to test inhibitory control, it is possible that differential affective encoding of the stimuli for the Go and No-Go trials may have contributed to study results.

In conclusion, findings from the present study indicate that youth with polymorphisms of DAT1 3′UTR differentially recruit the striatum during the successful implementation of inhibitory control. Heterogeneity in the association between DAT1 genotype and brain activation within ADHD raises the possibility that genotype may contribute to the observed inconsistent findings of striatal hypoactivation during Go/No-Go tasks in children and adolescents with ADHD (e.g., Durston et al., 2003; Durston et al., 2006b), and differential response to medication, and in particular, to stimulants (Gruber et al., 2009; Joober et al., 2007; Kirley et al., 2003; Lott et al., 2005; Roman et al., 2002; Stein et al., 2005; Winsberg and Comings, 1999). Although more research is needed in this area, findings from the present study add to those already published in describing the heterogeneity of DAT neuroanatomy and neurophysiology in ADHD (Volkow et al., 2007a), and illustrating the functional consequences of polymorphisms of DAT1 3′UTR.

Supplementary Material

01

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants R01 MH070935 (JHN) and K01 MH070892 (KPS) and by a Canadian Institutes of Health Research Fellowship (ACB). We are grateful for the assistance of Gregory Moy, who ran the genotyping assays for us at the University of Illinois at Chicago and for Frank Macaluso, Hanna Oltarzewska, and Paul Wisdom for helping with the collection of the fMRI scans.

Jeffrey Newcorn receives research support from Eli Lilly, Ortho-McNeil-Janssen and Shire, and is an advisor/consultant for Eli Lilly, Novartis, McNeil, Shire, Astra-Zeneca, Schering-Plough and BioBehavioral Diagnostics Company. The other authors reported no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4. American Psychiatric Press; Washigton, D.C: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  2. Aron AR, Durston S, Eagle DM, Logan GD, Stinear CM, Stuphorn V. Converging evidence for a fronto-basal-ganglia network for inhibitory control of action and cognition. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2007;27:11860–11864. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3644-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bannon MJ, Michelhaugh SK, Wang J, Sacchetti P. The human dopamine transporter gene: gene organization, transcriptional regulation, and potential involvement in neuropsychiatric disorders. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2001;11:449–455. doi: 10.1016/s0924-977x(01)00122-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bertolino A, Blasi G, Latorre V, Rubino V, Rampino A, Sinibaldi L, Caforio G, Petruzzella V, Pizzuti A, Scarabino T, Nardini M, Weinberger DR, Dallapiccola B. Additive effects of genetic variation in dopamine regulating genes on working memory cortical activity in human brain. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2006;26:3918–3922. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4975-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Breese GR, Duncan GE, Napier TC, Bondy SC, Iorio LC, Mueller RA. 6-hydroxydopamine treatments enhance behavioral responses to intracerebral microinjection of D1- and D2-dopamine agonists into nucleus accumbens and striatum without changing dopamine antagonist binding. The Journal of pharmacology and experimental therapeutics. 1987;240:167–176. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brookes KJ, Neale BM, Sugden K, Khan N, Asherson P, D’Souza UM. Relationship between VNTR polymorphisms of the human dopamine transporter gene and expression in post-mortem midbrain tissue. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2007;144B:1070–1078. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brown AB, Biederman J, Valera EM, Doyle AE, Bush G, Spencer T, Monuteaux MC, Mick E, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Makris N, Laviolette PS, Oscar-Berman M, Faraone SV, Seidman LJ. Effect of dopamine transporter gene (SLC6A3) variation on dorsal anterior cingulate function in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. American journal of medical genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric genetics : the official publication of the International Society of Psychiatric Genetics. 2009 doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chikazoe J, Konishi S, Asari T, Jimura K, Miyashita Y. Activation of right inferior frontal gyrus during response inhibition across response modalities. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19:69–80. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Ciliax BJ, Drash GW, Staley JK, Haber S, Mobley CJ, Miller GW, Mufson EJ, Mash DC, Levey AI. Immunocytochemical localization of the dopamine transporter in human brain. J Comp Neurol. 1999;409:38–56. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19990621)409:1<38::aid-cne4>3.0.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cook EH, Jr, Stein MA, Krasowski MD, Cox NJ, Olkon DM, Kieffer JE, Leventhal BL. Association of attention-deficit disorder and the dopamine transporter gene. Am J Hum Genet. 1995;56:993–998. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. DuPaul GJ, Power TJ, Anastopoulos AD, Reid R. ADHD Rating Scale - IV: Checklists, Norms, and Clinical Interpretation. The Guilford Press; New York: 1998. [Google Scholar]
  12. Durston S, Thomas KM, Worden MS, Yang Y, Casey BJ. The effect of preceding context on inhibition: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2002;16:449–453. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Durston S, Tottenham NT, Thomas KM, Davidson MC, Eigsti IM, Yang Y, Ulug AM, Casey BJ. Differential patterns of striatal activation in young children with and without ADHD. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53:871–878. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01904-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Durston S, Davidson MC, Tottenham N, Galvan A, Spicer J, Fossella JA, Casey BJ. A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development. Dev Sci. 2006a;9:1–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00454.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Durston S, Mulder M, Casey BJ, Ziermans T, van Engeland H. Activation in ventral prefrontal cortex is sensitive to genetic vulnerability for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2006b;60:1062–1070. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.12.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Durston S, Fossella JA, Mulder MJ, Casey BJ, Ziermans TB, Vessaz MN, Van Engeland H. Dopamine transporter genotype conveys familial risk of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder through striatal activation. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;47:61–67. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e31815a5f17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Faraone SV, Perlis RH, Doyle AE, Smoller JW, Goralnick JJ, Holmgren MA, Sklar P. Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:1313–1323. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Faraone SV, Khan SA. Candidate gene studies of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2006;67(Suppl 8):13–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Feron FJ, Hendriksen JG, van Kroonenburgh MJ, Blom-Coenjaerts C, Kessels AG, Jolles J, Weber WE, Vles JS. Dopamine transporter in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder normalizes after cessation of methylphenidate. Pediatr Neurol. 2005;33:179–183. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2005.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Franke B, Hoogman M, Arias Vasquez A, Heister JG, Savelkoul PJ, Naber M, Scheffer H, Kiemeney LA, Kan CC, Kooij JJ, Buitelaar JK. Association of the dopamine transporter (SLC6A3/DAT1) gene 9–6 haplotype with adult ADHD. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008;147B:1576–1579. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Friston KJ, Fletcher P, Josephs O, Holmes A, Rugg MD, Turner R. Event-related fMRI: characterizing differential responses. Neuroimage. 1998;7:30–40. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fuke S, Suo S, Takahashi N, Koike H, Sasagawa N, Ishiura S. The VNTR polymorphism of the human dopamine transporter (DAT1) gene affects gene expression. Pharmacogenomics J. 2001;1:152–156. doi: 10.1038/sj.tpj.6500026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Ghisi V, Ramsey AJ, Masri B, Gainetdinov RR, Caron MG, Salahpour A. Reduced D2-mediated signaling activity and trans-synaptic upregulation of D1 and D2 dopamine receptors in mice overexpressing the dopamine transporter. Cell Signal. 2009;21:87–94. doi: 10.1016/j.cellsig.2008.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gizer IR, Ficks C, Waldman ID. Candidate gene studies of ADHD: a meta-analytic review. Human Genetics. 2009;126:51–90. doi: 10.1007/s00439-009-0694-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gruber R, Joober R, Grizenko N, Leventhal BL, Cook EH, Jr, Stein MA. Dopamine transporter genotype and stimulant side effect factors in youth diagnosed with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2009;19:233–239. doi: 10.1089/cap.2008.0133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Halperin JM, Bedard AV, Berwid OG. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a lifespan synthesis. In: Hunter SJ, Donders J, editors. Principles and Practice of Lifespan Developmental Neuropsychology. Camrbidge University Press; United Kingdom: in press. [Google Scholar]
  27. Hawi Z, Kent L, Hill M, Anney RJ, Brookes KJ, Barry E, Franke B, Banaschewski T, Buitelaar J, Ebstein R, Miranda A, Oades RD, Roeyers H, Rothenberger A, Sergeant J, Sonuga-Barke E, Steinhausen HC, Faraone SV, Asherson P, Gill M. ADHD and DAT1: Further evidence of paternal over-transmission of risk alleles and haplotype. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2009 doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30960. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hayasaka S, Phan KL, Liberzon I, Worsley KJ, Nichols TE. Nonstationary cluster-size inference with random field and permutation methods. Neuroimage. 2004;22:676–687. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Heinz A, Goldman D, Jones DW, Palmour R, Hommer D, Gorey JG, Lee KS, Linnoila M, Weinberger DR. Genotype influences in vivo dopamine transporter availability in human striatum. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;22:133–139. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00099-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hermens DF, Rowe DL, Gordon E, Williams LM. Integrative neuroscience approach to predict ADHD stimulant response. Expert Rev Neurother. 2006;6:753–763. doi: 10.1586/14737175.6.5.753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hoover JE, Strick PL. Multiple output channels in the basal ganglia. Science. 1993;259:819–821. doi: 10.1126/science.7679223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hu XT, Wachtel SR, Galloway MP, White FJ. Lesions of the nigrostriatal dopamine projection increase the inhibitory effects of D1 and D2 dopamine agonists on caudate-putamen neurons and relieve D2 receptors from the necessity of D1 receptor stimulation. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 1990;10:2318–2329. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-07-02318.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jacobsen LK, Staley JK, Zoghbi SS, Seibyl JP, Kosten TR, Innis RB, Gelernter J. Prediction of dopamine transporter binding availability by genotype: a preliminary report. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:1700–1703. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.10.1700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Johansson S, Halleland H, Halmoy A, Jacobsen KK, Landaas ET, Dramsdahl M, Fasmer OB, Bergsholm P, Lundervold AJ, Gillberg C, Hugdahl K, Knappskog PM, Haavik J. Genetic analyses of dopamine related genes in adult ADHD patients suggest an association with the DRD5-microsatellite repeat, but not with DRD4 or SLC6A3 VNTRs. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008;147B:1470–1475. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Johnstone T, Ores Walsh KS, Greischar LL, Alexander AL, Fox AS, Davidson RJ, Oakes TR. Motion correction and the use of motion covariates in multiple-subject fMRI analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. 2006;27:779–788. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Joober R, Grizenko N, Sengupta S, Amor LB, Schmitz N, Schwartz G, Karama S, Lageix P, Fathalli F, Torkaman-Zehi A, Ter Stepanian M. Dopamine transporter 3′-UTR VNTR genotype and ADHD: a pharmaco-behavioural genetic study with methylphenidate. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32:1370–1376. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kang AM, Palmatier MA, Kidd KK. Global variation of a 40-bp VNTR in the 3′-untranslated region of the dopamine transporter gene (SLC6A3) Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46:151–160. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00101-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, Williamson D, Ryan N. Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36:980–988. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kawarai T, Kawakami H, Yamamura Y, Nakamura S. Structure and organization of the gene encoding human dopamine transporter. Gene. 1997;195:11–18. doi: 10.1016/s0378-1119(97)00131-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kelly RM, Strick PL. Macro-architecture of basal ganglia loops with the cerebral cortex: use of rabies virus to reveal multisynaptic circuits. Prog Brain Res. 2004;143:449–459. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(03)43042-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kirley A, Lowe N, Hawi Z, Mullins C, Daly G, Waldman I, McCarron M, O’Donnell D, Fitzgerald M, Gill M. Association of the 480 bp DAT1 allele with methylphenidate response in a sample of Irish children with ADHD. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2003;121B:50–54. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.20071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Krause J, Dresel SH, Krause KH, La Fougere C, Zill P, Ackenheil M. Striatal dopamine transporter availability and DAT-1 gene in adults with ADHD: no higher DAT availability in patients with homozygosity for the 10-repeat allele. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2006;7:152–157. doi: 10.1080/15622970500518444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Langleben DD, Acton PD, Austin G, Elman I, Krikorian G, Monterosso JR, Portnoy O, Ridlehuber HW, Strauss HW. Effects of methylphenidate discontinuation on cerebral blood flow in prepubescent boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Nucl Med. 2002;43:1624–1629. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Langley K, Turic D, Peirce TR, Mills S, Van Den Bree MB, Owen MJ, O’Donovan MC, Thapar A. No support for association between the dopamine transporter (DAT1) gene and ADHD. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2005;139B:7–10. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lewis DA, Melchitzky DS, Sesack SR, Whitehead RE, Auh S, Sampson A. Dopamine transporter immunoreactivity in monkey cerebral cortex: regional, laminar, and ultrastructural localization. J Comp Neurol. 2001;432:119–136. doi: 10.1002/cne.1092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lott DC, Kim SJ, Cook EH, Jr, de Wit H. Dopamine transporter gene associated with diminished subjective response to amphetamine. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:602–609. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Martinez D, Gelernter J, Abi-Dargham A, van Dyck CH, Kegeles L, Innis RB, Laruelle M. The variable number of tandem repeats polymorphism of the dopamine transporter gene is not associated with significant change in dopamine transporter phenotype in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;24:553–560. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00216-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Mill J, Asherson P, Browes C, D’Souza U, Craig I. Expression of the dopamine transporter gene is regulated by the 3′ UTR VNTR: Evidence from brain and lymphocytes using quantitative RT-PCR. Am J Med Genet. 2002;114:975–979. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.10948. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Plomp E, van Engeland H, Durston S. Understanding genes, environment, and their interaction in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: is there a role for neuroimaging? Neuroscience. 2009 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.07.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Prata DP, Mechelli A, Picchioni MM, Fu CH, Toulopoulou T, Bramon E, Walshe M, Murray RM, Collier DA, McGuire P. Altered effect of dopamine transporter 3′UTR VNTR genotype on prefrontal and striatal function in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:1162–1172. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Rebec GV. Real-time assessments of dopamine function during behavior: single-unit recording, iontophoresis, and fast-scan cyclic voltammetry in awake, unrestrained rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1998;22:32–40. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1998.tb03614.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Roman T, Szobot C, Martins S, Biederman J, Rohde LA, Hutz MH. Dopamine transporter gene and response to methylphenidate in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pharmacogenetics. 2002;12:497–499. doi: 10.1097/00008571-200208000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Rubia K, Smith AB, Brammer MJ, Toone B, Taylor E. Abnormal brain activation during inhibition and error detection in medication-naive adolescents with ADHD. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:1067–1075. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.6.1067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Sakagami M, Tsutsui K, Lauwereyns J, Koizumi M, Kobayashi S, Hikosaka O. A code for behavioral inhibition on the basis of color, but not motion, in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex of macaque monkey. J Neurosci. 2001;21:4801–4808. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-13-04801.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Schneider W, Eschman A, Zuccolatto A. E-Prime User’s Guide. Psychology Software Tools, Inc; Pittsburgh, PA: 2002. [Google Scholar]
  56. Schulz KP, Fan J, Tang CY, Newcorn JH, Buchsbaum MS, Cheung AM, Halperin JM. Response inhibition in adolescents diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during childhood: an event-related FMRI study. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:1650–1657. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.9.1650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Schulz KP, Clerkin SM, Halperin JM, Newcorn JH, Tang CY, Fan J. Dissociable neural effects of stimulus valence and preceding context during the inhibition of responses to emotional faces. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009 doi: 10.1002/hbm.20706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Skounti M, Philalithis A, Galanakis E. Variations in prevalence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder worldwide. Eur J Pediatr. 2007;166:117–123. doi: 10.1007/s00431-006-0299-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Smith Y, Kieval JZ. Anatomy of the dopamine system in the basal ganglia. Trends Neurosci. 2000;23:S28–33. doi: 10.1016/s1471-1931(00)00023-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Stein MA, Waldman ID, Sarampote CS, Seymour KE, Robb AS, Conlon C, Kim SJ, Cook EH. Dopamine transporter genotype and methylphenidate dose response in children with ADHD. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:1374–1382. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300718. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Szobot C, Roman T, Renato C, Acton P, Hutz M, Rohde LA. Brain perfusion and dopaminergic genes in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med Genet: Part B. 2005;132B:53–58. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Talairach J, Tournoux M. Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. Thieme Medical; New York: 1988. [Google Scholar]
  63. Tomasi D, Volkow ND, Wang R, Telang F, Wang GJ, Chang L, Ernst T, Fowler JS. Dopamine transporters in striatum correlate with deactivation in the default mode network during visuospatial attention. PLoS One. 2009;4:e6102. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. van Dyck CH, Malison RT, Jacobsen LK, Seibyl JP, Staley JK, Laruelle M, Baldwin RM, Innis RB, Gelernter J. Increased dopamine transporter availability associated with the 9-repeat allele of the SLC6A3 gene. J Nucl Med. 2005;46:745–751. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Vandenbergh DJ, Persico AM, Hawkins AL, Griffin CA, Li X, Jabs EW, Uhl GR. Human dopamine transporter gene (DAT1) maps to chromosome 5p15.3 and displays a VNTR. Genomics. 1992;14:1104–1106. doi: 10.1016/s0888-7543(05)80138-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. VanNess SH, Owens MJ, Kilts CD. The variable number of tandem repeats element in DAT1 regulates in vitro dopamine transporter density. BMC Genet. 2005;6:55. doi: 10.1186/1471-2156-6-55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Newcorn J, Telang F, Solanto MV, Fowler JS, Logan J, Ma Y, Schulz K, Pradhan K, Wong C, Swanson JM. Depressed dopamine activity in caudate and preliminary evidence of limbic involvement in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:932–940. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.8.932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Winsberg BG, Comings DE. Association of the dopamine transporter gene (DAT1) with poor methylphenidate response. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999;38:1474–1477. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199912000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Wu N, Cepeda C, Zhuang X, Levine MS. Altered corticostriatal neurotransmission and modulation in dopamine transporter knock-down mice. J Neurophysiol. 2007;98:423–432. doi: 10.1152/jn.00971.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Yang B, Chan RC, Jing J, Li T, Sham P, Chen RY. A meta-analysis of association studies between the 10-repeat allele of a VNTR polymorphism in the 3′-UTR of dopamine transporter gene and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2007;144B:541–550. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Zhu J, Reith ME. Role of the dopamine transporter in the action of psychostimulants, nicotine, and other drugs of abuse. CNS Neurol Disord Drug Targets. 2008;7:393–409. doi: 10.2174/187152708786927877. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

01

RESOURCES