Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Jun 25.
Published in final edited form as: Arch Neurol. 2008 Feb;65(2):244–248. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2007.34

Increased Frequency of Learning Disability in Patients With Primary Progressive Aphasia and Their First-Degree Relatives

Emily Rogalski 1, Nancy Johnson 1, Sandra Weintraub 1, Marsel Mesulam 1
PMCID: PMC2892116  NIHMSID: NIHMS209522  PMID: 18268195

Abstract

Background

Although risk factors for Alzheimer disease have been well studied, much less is known about risk factors for primary progressive aphasia (PPA).

Objective

To demonstrate that learning disabilities (LDs) are more common in patients with PPA and their first-degree family members.

Design, Setting, and Patients

Self-report endorsement of an individual and family history of an LD in a sample of 699 subjects from the Northwestern Alzheimer’s Disease Center registry. We compared 3 dementia groups (PPA, typical amnestic Alzheimer disease, and the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia) and 1 elderly control group. A retrospective medical record review in the PPA probands was used to obtain additional information.

Main Outcome Measure

Prevalence of LDs among probands and their first-degree relatives.

Results

The patients with PPA and their first-degree family members had a significantly higher frequency of LD compared with the other dementia groups and the controls. Some of the families of patients with PPA displayed unusual concentrations of LD, especially dyslexia.

Conclusion

These results suggest that LD may constitute a risk factor for PPA, providing additional clues concerning the determinants for the selective vulnerability of the language network in this syndrome.


Primary progressive aphasia (PPA) is a neurodegenerative syndrome characterized by the progressive loss of language functioning over time with relative preservation of other cognitive domains within the first 2 years of symptom onset.1,2 Neuroanatomically, the pathological changes in PPA are frequently asymmetric and most severe in the hemisphere (usually left) dominant for language. 36 Neuropathologically, most of the patients show changes similar to those of frontotemporal lobar degeneration,7 but nearly 30% show changes similar to those of Alzheimer disease (AD).8

Although risk factors for AD have been well studied, much less is known about risk factors for PPA. Our group has previously shown that the apolipoprotein E4 allele, a known genetic risk factor for AD, is not associated with increased risk of PPA.9 Demographic estimates of the disease suggest that symptom onset is generally in the presenium (before 65 years of age) and that a slightly greater incidence of the disease occurs in men than in women.5,10 This represents a profile different from that of AD, in which most patients are older than 65 years and female.11,12

Initial observations on a small set of subjects suggested a higher frequency of learning disabilities (LDs) in patients with PPA and their first-degree relatives than in patients with clinical AD.13 To test the validity of this observation in a larger set of subjects, the prevalence of self-reported LDs was compared among 4 different groups in the Northwestern Alzheimer’s Disease Center registry: PPA, typical amnestic AD, behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia (FTD), and healthy elderly control subjects.

METHODS

On enrollment into the Northwestern Alzheimer’s Disease Center registry, subjects gave written informed consent and completed a detailed demographic and medical history interview. During this initial interview, subjects were asked 2 questions about a history of LD: “Do you have a history of a learning disability?” and “Have any of your first-degree family members (eg, mother, father, siblings, and children) had a history of a learning disability?” Based on the initial results, we also conducted a retrospective medical record review of the patients with PPA who gave affirmative answers to either of the 2 questions. Some of the reviewed medical records included additional illustrative information on unusual familial concentrations of LD in these families.

Information was available on 699 individuals who responded to the questions about LD. Of these, 353 individuals were classified as controls, 154 as having typical amnestic AD according to the criteria of McKhann et al,14 84 as having the behavioral variant of FTD according to the criteria of Neary et al,15 and 108 as having PPA according to the criteria of Mesulam.1 Clinical diagnoses were made by the consensus of a neurologist (including M.M.) and a neuropsychologist (including N.J. and S.W.) at the Northwestern Alzheimer’s Disease Center. Although the tendency now is to classify PPA into agrammatic/dysfluent, semantic, and logopenic variants,16 we did not make such distinctions for the purpose of this study. The demographic characteristics for the diagnostic groups are described in Table1. Separate χ2 tests were conducted to determine whether the prevalence of LD differed between the patient history of each diagnostic group and/or the patient’s family history.

Table 1.

Demographic Information

Characteristic Group
Control Subjects
(n=353)
Typical Amnestic AD
(n=154)
Behavioral Variant of FTD
(n=84)
PPA
(n=108)
Age at onset, mean (SD), y 69.4 (8.2)a 71.2 (9.5) 60.3 (9.2) 62.9 (8.3)
Education, mean (SD), y 15.8 (2.6) 14.0 (3.0) 14.9 (2.8) 15.3 (2.8)
IQ, mean (SD)b 112.1 (6.6) 109.1 (8.3) 111.9 (6.8) 112.9 (6.5)
Sex, No.
  Female 274 102 38 56
  Male 79 52 46 52

Abbreviations: AD, Alzheimer disease; FTD, frontotemporal dementia; PPA, primary progressive aphasia.

a

Because there was no age at disease onset for the controls, the age at their first visit is provided.

b

The IQs were determined according to Barona et al.17

RESULTS

Our results indicated that the patients with PPA and their first-degree family members had a significantly higher frequency of LDs compared with the other dementia and control groups (Pearson χ2 for individual history, 33.15; P<.001; Pearson χ2 for first-degree family members, 41.57; P<.001) (Table 2). Table 3 provides more specific personal and family history information from a subset of 23 patients with PPA to illustrate the unusually high concentration of LDs, especially dyslexia, within some of the families.

Table 2.

Percentage of Individual and Family History of a Learning Disability

History of
Learning
Disability
Group, No. (%)

Control
Subjects
(n=353)
Typical
Amnestic AD
(n=154)
Behavioral
Variant of FTD
(n=84)
PPA
(n=108)
Individual 5 (1.4) 7 (4.5) 6 (7.1) 16 (14.8)a
Family 24 (6.8) 16 (10.4) 12 (14.3) 32 (29.6)a

Abbreviations: AD, Alzheimer disease; FTD, frontotemporal dementia; PPA, primary progressive aphasia.

a

The individual and family history of learning disability is significantly elevated in the PPA group compared with the other groups, P<.001.

Table 3.

Concentration of LDs in a Subset of Patients With PPA and Their Family Members

Patient
No.
LD Total No.
of 1st-Degree
Family Members
With LD
Total No. of
Family Members
With LD
LD Description

In
Patient
In 1st-Degree
Family
1 No Yes 1 1 of 4 Children, 0 of 1 sibling Son was diagnosed as having dyslexia
2 No Yes 1 1 of 4 Children, 0 of 4 siblings Son had problems with speech, reading, spelling, writing, and
    arithmetic
3 Yes Yes 5 3 of 3 Children, no siblings,
    2 of 2 granddaughters
Patient did not learn to read until age 9 or 10 y and his spelling
    was always poor; despite trying, patient was unable to learn a
    foreign language; all of the patient’s children and 2
    grandchildren were described as dyslexic; only 1 of the
    patient’s sons was able to graduate from high school
4 Yes Yes 3 3 of 3 Children 3 Sons had problems with speech, reading, spelling, writing,
    arithmetic, and learning language
5 Yes Yes 1 1 of 3 Children, 0 of 1 sibling Patient reports always having difficulty spelling; 1 son had
    difficulty with spelling and reading
6 No Yes 1 1 of 2 Children, 0 of 1 sibling Daughter had a positive LD history consistent with mild dyslexia;
    patient reports she was never “good at languages” and her
    mother had difficulty pronouncing long words
7 Yes Yes 2 1 of 4 Children, 1 of 3 siblings Patient reported having persistent difficulty with arithmetic;
    history of dyslexia reported in patient’s younger sister,
    daughter, paternal uncle, and nephew
8 Yes Yes 3 3 of 4 Children, 0 of 1 sibling
    (died at age 10 y)
Patient reportedly did well in mathematics and science but
    struggled with English and literature; patient received therapy
    for early speech problem and was a below-average speller; 3
    children (2 sons, 1 daughter) had difficulty with spelling and 1
    of the sons also had difficulty with reading
9 No Yes 2 1 of 1 Sibling, 1 of 2 parents Patient’s father stuttered as a child; brother had a history of LD
    and was held back in school for poor spelling; 1 nephew
    diagnosed as having dyslexia
10 Yes No 0 0 of 1 Children, 0 of 1 sibling Patient always had difficulty spelling
11 Yes No 0 0 of 2 Children, 0 of 1 sibling Patient reports persistent spelling difficulties since childhood;
    she attended a spelling course as a child
12 Yes Yes 1 1 of 5 Children, 0 of 3 siblings,
    0 of 2 parents
Patient had difficulty with mathematics and writing as a child;
    patient’s daughter had a similar history of LD
13 Yes Yes 2 2 of 3 Children, 0 of 1 sibling Patient was a slow reader and had difficulty with mathematics
    and spelling as a child; 2 of patient’s sons were diagnosed as
    having ADD
14 No Yes 5 5 of 5 Siblings All of patient’s siblings had persistent difficulty with reading and
    spelling
15 Yes Yes 1 1 of 2 Children, 0 of 1 sibling Patient had difficulty reading, consistent with dyslexia, and was
    never good at spelling; his son also has a history of dyslexia
16 No Yes 1 0 of 1 Children, 1 of 1 sibling Brother had difficulty with speech as a child and needed speech
    therapy; brother’s 2 children also have difficulty with speech
17 Yes No 0 0 of 4 Children, 3 of 9
    grandchildren
Patient was never a good speller; 3 of patient’s grandsons have a
    positive history of LD, 1 of which has severe dyslexia
18 Yes Yes 1 1 of 3 Children Son has LD for language
19 No Yes 1 0 of 4 Children, 1 of 1 sibling Brother is reported to have LD consistent with mild dyslexia
20 Yes Yes 1 1 of 2 Children, 0 of 4 siblings,
    0 of 2 parents
Patient had trouble with speech as a child and took special
    speech classes; patient’s son also received speech therapy
21 Yes No 0 0 of 3 Children, 0 of 2 siblings,
    0 of 2 parents
Patient was enrolled in special education class for kindergarten
    and has LDs consistent with dyslexia
22 Yes Yes 1 0 of 6 Children, 1 of 13
    siblings, 0 of 2 parents
1 Brother has reported dyslexia
23 Yes Yes 5 2 of 2 Children, 3 of 3 siblings 3 Brothers and 2 sons have reported a history of language LD

Abbreviations: ADD, attention deficit disorder; LD, learning disability; PPA, primary progressive aphasia.

COMMENT

Responses to questions about the presence of an LD in 699 probands and their first-degree relatives showed the prevalence of LDs to be higher in PPA than in controls, typical amnestic AD, and the behavioral variant of FTD. A retrospective medical record review in a subset of the patients with PPA showed families with unusually high concentrations of LD. For example, in 3 cases (patients 3, 4, and 8) 9 of the 10 children (and in patients 14 and 23, all 8 siblings) of the probands were reported to have a history of specific LD in the area of language (Table 3). These results suggest that LD may constitute a risk factor for PPA, providing additional clues concerning the selective vulnerability in this syndrome.

All neurodegenerative syndromes, including AD, FTD, and PPA, are clinically characterized by exquisite specificity, especially during the initial stages. In the case of PPA, neuroimaging and neuropsychological examination results show a selective impairment of word usage and a corresponding concentration of atrophy and hypometabolism within the left hemisphere language network. 1 How does a disease process become distributed asymmetrically, and how does it target the language network? An exploration of specific risk factors for PPA may help to address these questions.

Several differential risk factors set PPA apart from other degenerative syndromes. For example, the apolipoprotein E4 allele is a risk factor for AD but not PPA.9 Furthermore, the H1/H1 haplotype of the tau gene and the MV polymorphism in codon 129 of the prion protein gene have emerged as potential risk factors for PPA.18,19 In rare cases, PPA may be associated with causative genetic mutations. For example, in 2 families in which all affected members (5 of 7 siblings in 2 kindreds) displayed the typical PPA phenotype, mutations in the progranulin gene (PGRN) segregated with the clinical disease.20 However, such mutations are detected in only a few patients with PPA. Similar PGRN mutations can give rise to the behavioral variant of FTD in other families,20 making it necessary to look for additional risk factors that determine the clinical selectivity of the PPA syndrome.

Some potential risk factors for PPA may be developmental or environmental rather than genetic. In 2 patients who experienced the onset of PPA in their seventh decade of life, brain imaging showed left hemicraniosynostosis and a decreased size of the left frontal and temporal lobes.21 In another case, a man who experienced onset of typical PPA at age 70 years had a history of an abscess that had been surgically removed from the left temporal lobe at age 11 years.13 In these 3 patients, an early injury to the left hemisphere that was neurologically compensated for through much of adulthood seemed to have provided a “locus of least resistance” for the concentration of neurodegeneration within the language-dominant left hemisphere. There are other examples of analogous phenomena. Women who recover from Sydenham chorea in childhood, a disease thought to be associated with antibodies to the basal ganglia, can experience chorea gravidarum during pregnancy in response to alterations of the hormonal milieu.22 Patients who have recovered from poliomyelitis can develop, decades later, a progressive motoneuron disease in the previously affected muscles.23 Finally, patients who have recovered from childhood hemiplegia can develop, later in life, a progressive hemiparkinsonism on the side of the recovered weakness.24

Questions may be raised about the specificity and reliability of these findings. For example, subjects with relatively uncommon diagnoses such as PPA might be more likely to report a history of LD because they may be more inclined to explore their family history for similar disorders. This phenomenon is well described in Parkinson disease.25 Although this possibility of biased reporting needs to be considered, it may be no more relevant to PPA than to FTD and AD in this study. The question of specificity is more difficult to address definitively. Our survey of the literature did not produce evidence of a relationship between premorbid LD and Huntington disease or Parkinson disease. However, there are indications that a subset of schizophrenia characterized by receptive language dysfunction may be associated with a premorbid LD for language.26 Specific LDs may therefore constitute premorbid phenotypical markers for other neuropsychiatric disorders as well.

The results of our study add further credence to earlier observations13 that patients with PPA and their first-degree relatives display a higher frequency of LD, especially of the dyslexic type. The higher frequency of LD in the PPA group may provide a marker for preexisting developmental or acquired susceptibilities targeting the language network. Primary progressive aphasia may thus represent the tardive manifestation of an antecedent vulnerability that remains neurologically compensated for during much of adulthood but that eventually becomes a nidus for the anatomical distribution of a degenerative disease that would have had a different distribution in individuals with different susceptibilities. The higher frequency of LD not only in probands but also in first-degree relatives raises the possibility that this risk factor has a genetic component that is developmentally expressed as dyslexia in some individuals and as a neurodegenerative disease, also affecting language, in others. This relationship may exist in only a small subgroup of persons with dyslexia without necessarily implying that the entire population with dyslexia or their family members are at higher risk of PPA.

In our clinical practice, we encounter many patients with PPA who report that spelling was never their “strong suit” or that they could not learn new languages, but who would not have identified themselves as having an LD. Furthermore, family history in these patients is likely to be incomplete, especially in a complex area such as LD. It is therefore reasonable to assume that the frequency of LD shown in Table 1 may be an underestimation. This possibility could be addressed in a larger epidemiological study that also determines the nature of the LD in each proband and family member.

The clinical classification of PPA is moving toward the identification of 3 variants—agrammatic/dysfluent, semantic, and logopenic—each associated with a slightly different set of underlying molecular neuropathologic features. 8,16 We did not subdivide our patients into these variants because most were entered into the study before the acceptance of this classification. Although the details of the aphasia and the underlying neuropathologic features in our patients with PPA may be heterogeneous, the group has the common denominator of having the language network of the brain as the principal locus of degeneration. The common feature, for which LD may be an antecedent risk factor, is the anatomical locus of involvement at the level of a specific interconnected neural network rather than the nature of the underlying disease. Future studies, however, may show that the incidence of LD is higher in some variants of PPA than in others.

Acknowledgments

Funding/Support: This study was supported by the National Institute on Aging, by Alzheimer’s Disease Core Center grant AG 13854, and by grant DC008552 from the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders.

Footnotes

Author Contributions: Study concept and design: Johnson, Weintraub, and Mesulam. Acquisition of data: Rogalski, Johnson, Weintraub, and Mesulam. Analysis and interpretation of data: Rogalski, Johnson, Weintraub, and Mesulam. Drafting of the manuscript: Rogalski, Johnson, Weintraub, and Mesulam. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Rogalski, Johnson, Weintraub, and Mesulam. Statistical analysis: Rogalski and Johnson. Obtained funding: Mesulam. Administrative, technical, and material support: Rogalski and Mesulam. Study supervision: Johnson, Weintraub, and Mesulam.

Financial Disclosure: None reported.

Additional Contributions: Katie Cohen helped organize the data for this project.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Mesulam MM. Primary progressive aphasia—a language-based dementia. NEngl J Med. 2003;349(16):1535–1542. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra022435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Mesulam MM. Slowly progressive aphasia without generalized dementia. Ann Neurol. 1982;11(6):592–598. doi: 10.1002/ana.410110607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Chawluk JB, Mesulam MM, Hurtig H, et al. Slowly progressive aphasia without generalized dementia: studies with positron emission tomography. Ann Neurol. 1986;19(1):68–74. doi: 10.1002/ana.410190112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kirshner HS, Tanridag O, Thurman L, Whetsell WO., Jr Progressive aphasia without dementia: two cases with focal spongiform degeneration. Ann Neurol. 1987;22(4):527–532. doi: 10.1002/ana.410220413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Mesulam MM, Weintraub S. Primary progressive aphasia: sharpening the focus on a clinical syndrome. In: Boller F, Forette F, Khachaturian Z, Poncet M, Christen Y, editors. Heterogeneity of Alzheimer’s Disease. Berlin, Germany: Springer Verlag; 1992. pp. 43–66. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Turner RS, Kenyon LC, Trojanowski JQ, Gonatas N, Grossman M. Clinical, neuroimaging, and pathologic features of progressive nonfluent aphasia. Ann Neurol. 1996;39(2):166–173. doi: 10.1002/ana.410390205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.McKhann GM, Albert MS, Grossman M, Miller B, Dickson D, Trojanowski JQ. Work Group on Frontotemporal Dementia and Pick’s Disease. Clinical and pathological diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia: report of the Work Group on Frontotemporal Dementia and Pick’s Disease. Arch Neurol. 2001;58(11):1803–1809. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.11.1803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Knibb JA, Xuereb JH, Patterson K, Hodges JR. Clinical and pathological characterization of progressive aphasia. Ann Neurol. 2006;59(1):156–165. doi: 10.1002/ana.20700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Mesulam MM, Johnson N, Grujic Z, Weintraub S. Apolipoprotein E genotypes in primary progressive aphasia. Neurology. 1997;49(1):51–55. doi: 10.1212/wnl.49.1.51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Westbury C, Bub D. Primary progressive aphasia: a review of 112 cases. Brain Lang. 1997;60(3):381–406. doi: 10.1006/brln.1997.1840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Fratiglioni L, Viitanen M, von Strauss E, Tontodonati V, Herlitz A, Winblad B. Very old women at highest risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: incidence data from the Kungsholmen Project, Stockholm. Neurology. 1997;48(1):132–138. doi: 10.1212/wnl.48.1.132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Gao S, Hendrie HC, Hall KS, Hui S. The relationships between age, sex, and the incidence of dementia and Alzheimer disease: a meta-analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55(9):809–815. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.9.809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Mesulam MM, Weintraub S. Spectrum of primary progressive aphasia. Baillieres Clin Neurol. 1992;1(3):583–609. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology. 1984;34(7):939–944. doi: 10.1212/wnl.34.7.939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Neary D, Snowden JS, Gustafson L, et al. Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology. 1998;51(6):1546–1554. doi: 10.1212/wnl.51.6.1546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Gorno-Tempini ML, Dronkers NF, Rankin KP, et al. Cognition and anatomy in three variants of primary progressive aphasia. Ann Neurol. 2004;55(3):335–346. doi: 10.1002/ana.10825. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Barona A, Reynolds CR, Chastain R. A demographically based index of premorbid intelligence for the WAIS-R. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1984;52:885–887. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Sobrido MJ, Abu-Khalil A, Weintraub S, et al. Possible association of the tau H1/H1 genotype with primary progressive aphasia. Neurology. 2003;60(5):862–864. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000049473.36612.f2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Li X, Rowland LP, Mitsumoto H, et al. Prion protein codon 129 genotype prevalence is altered in primary progressive aphasia. Ann Neurol. 2005;58(6):858–864. doi: 10.1002/ana.20646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Mesulam M, Johnson N, Krefft TA, et al. Progranulin mutations in primary progressive aphasia: the PPA1 and PPA3 families. Arch Neurol. 2007;64(1):43–47. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.1.43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Alberca R, Montes E, Russell E, Gil-Néciga E, Mesulam M. Left hemicranial hypoplasia in 2 patients with primary progressive aphasia. Arch Neurol. 2004;61(2):265–268. doi: 10.1001/archneur.61.2.265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Cardoso F. Chorea gravidarum. Arch Neurol. 2002;59(5):868–870. doi: 10.1001/archneur.59.5.868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Cashman NR, Maselli R, Wollmann RL, Roos R, Simon R, Antel JP. Late denervation in patients with antecedent paralytic poliomyelitis. N Engl J Med. 1987;317(1):7–12. doi: 10.1056/NEJM198707023170102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Klawans HL. Hemiparkinsonism as a late complication of hemiatrophy: a new syndrome. Neurology. 1981;31(5):625–628. doi: 10.1212/wnl.31.5.625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Elbaz A, McDonnell SK, Maraganore DM, et al. Validity of family history data on PD: evidence for a family information bias. Neurology. 2003;61(1):11–17. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000068007.58423.c2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Condray R. Language disorder in schizophrenia as a developmental learning disorder. Schizophr Res. 2005;73(1):5–20. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.05.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES