Skip to main content
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience logoLink to Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience
. 2009 Aug 27;5(2-3):194–202. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp026

Assortative human pair-bonding for partner ancestry and allelic variation of the dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) gene

Dan T A Eisenberg 1,, Coren L Apicella 2, Benjamin C Campbell 3, Anna Dreber 4,5, Justin R Garcia 6, J Koji Lum 6
PMCID: PMC2894670  PMID: 19713452

Abstract

The 7 repeat (7R) allele of the dopamine receptor D4 gene has been associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and risk taking. On the cross-population scale, 7R allele frequencies have been shown to be higher in populations with more of a history of long-term migrations. It has also been shown that the 7R allele is associated with individuals having multiple ancestries. Here, we conduct a replication of this latter finding with two independent samples. Measures of subjects’ ancestry are used to examine past reproductive bonds. The individuals’ history of interracial/ancestral dating and their feelings about this are also assessed. Tentative support for an association between multiple ancestries and the 7R allele was found. These results are dependent upon the method of questioning subjects about their ancestries, with only finer-scale measures of ancestry being associated with 7R. Interracial dating and feelings about interracial pairing were not related to the presence of the 7R allele. This study provides continued support for a role for the 7R allele in migration and/or mate choice patterns. However, replications and extensions of this study are needed and the way ancestry/race is assessed must be carefully considered.

Keywords: DRD4, assortative mating, race, ancestry, pair-bonds

INTRODUCTION

The cosmopolitan nature of humans is a defining characteristic of our evolutionary history and our present conditions. Mixing of human populations with accompanying political changes has long been recognized as a characteristic of historic and contemporary human populations (Boas, 1909). In characterizing human migration, Charles Darwin went so far as to speculate that, ‘The restless who will not follow any steady occupation … emigrate to newly settled countries, where they prove useful pioneers’ (Darwin, 1977). Human mating patterns are well known to be influenced by geographical propinquity (Peach and Mitchell, 1988; Rosenberg et al., 2005) as well as homogamy (Kalmijn, 1998; Smits et al., 1998).

While migration propensity is likely due to a suite of biological traits, cultural traits and genes, there is some evidence that particular traits and genes are of increased importance in explaining human migrations. In particular, the 7 repeat (7R) allele of the 48bp VNTR site in the dopamine receptor D4 gene (DRD4) is present at higher frequencies in populations that have migrated farther in the past 1000 to 30 000 years (Chen et al., 1999).

The genetic structure of this same ‘migratory’ 7R allele suggests that it originated and was positively selected for between 40 000 and 50 000 years ago (Wang et al., 2004). The 7R allele of the DRD4 gene has been associated with behavioral traits such as attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Li et al., 2006), impulsivity (Eisenberg et al., 2007), financial risk-taking (Dreber et al., 2009; Kuhnen and Chiao, 2009) and novelty seeking (Kluger et al., 2002; Schinka et al., 2002; Savitz and Ramesar, 2004). It has also been shown that the 7R allele is associated with more nomadic lifestyles (Chen et al., 1999) and potentially greater success in this nomadic lifestyle (Eisenberg et al., 2008). Thus, the 7R allele might be related to decreased assortative mating via propinquity because of its association with migration and via homogamy because of its association with novelty seeking. Further, interracial romantic relationships face a number of cultural barricades (Harris and Kalbfleisch, 2000), which 7R individuals might more readily overcome.

While a good case has been made that DRD4 played a salient role in pre-historic population structure through its association with migration, it is less clear if DRD4 is a correlate of current population structure. Key determinants of population structure that likely have salient behavioral components include migration and mating patterns (Nalls et al., 2009). On the one hand, Chen and colleagues (1999) found no DRD4 allele frequency differences between migrants and their source populations (however, this analysis was exploratory and of limited power). On the other hand, Eisenberg and colleagues (2007) recently found that 7R alleles were associated with having multiple ancestries in a group of US (Binghamton, NY) undergraduates. This is consistent with other results that found that emigrants are more extroverted and open to experience than natives (Ciani et al., 2007; Jokela, 2009). For example, Dutch couples born in different geographic regions and their offsprings are more sensation seeking than those couples born in the same geographic region and their children (Rebollo and Boomsma, 2007). Similarly, in Finland, migration from rural to urban areas is associated with increased trait level sociability, and migration more generally is associated with increased trait level activity (Jokela et al., 2008).

The dopamine system has also been implicated in sexual (Melis and Argiolas, 1995; Dominguez and Hull, 2005; Melis et al., 2006; Succu et al., 2007) and pair bonding behaviors (Gingrich et al., 2000; Curtis and Wang, 2005; Fisher et al., 2005; Aragona et al., 2006; Smeltzer et al., 2006), including romantic love in humans (Fisher, 2004). DRD4 has specifically been associated with sexual desire, sexual arousal, sexual function (Zion et al., 2006), sexual novelty (Hamer, 2002), and age at first sexual intercourse (Miller et al., 1999; Guo and Tong, 2006; Eisenberg et al., 2007). Additionally, 7R alleles have been associated with a desire for children and marriage earlier in life (Eisenberg et al., 2007).

Here, we explore the previous findings of an association between DRD4 and assortative mating for partner ancestry in two new datasets. In addition to evaluating whether DRD4 is related to past cross-cultural pairing (having ancestors from multiple geographic regions/cultures), we also examine whether current/recent pair-bonding behaviors as well as planned future pair-bonding behaviors are related to DRD4. Given abundant failures to replicate findings of gene association studies (Lucentini, 2004), replications, like that conducted here, are very important.

METHODS

Data collection

This study incorporates participants from two separate samples: the first includes 98 male undergraduates from Harvard University between the ages of 18 and 23 years (mean 20.07) and the second includes 181 undergraduate students (118 females and 63 males) from Binghamton University, State University of New York between the ages of 18 and 28 years (mean 20.11). Harvard subjects were all male because an aim of gathering the dataset was to analyze correlates of testosterone levels (Apicella et al., 2008). Harvard University participants were recruited by fliers distributed on the Harvard campus, as well as via e-mail solicitation to undergraduate residential houses. Harvard subjects were excluded if they responded affirmatively to questions about current use of psychotropic medication or having been diagnosed with bipolar depression, pathological gambling and/or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Harvard subjects completed the study in small group sessions (between one and twelve individuals) at a central location in the Department of Anthropology during spring 2007. Harvard subjects answered the questions in privacy and were told that all data would be confidential. Binghamton University participants were recruited from the Department of Psychology's Human Subject Pool. Data were collected in a reserved lecture hall where measures were taken to ensure participant privacy. All participants from both samples were asked to complete questionnaires and provide a saliva buccal wash sample using 10 ml of Scope™ mouthwash for later DNA extraction. Research procedures were conducted under the respective approval of Harvard University's Institutional Review Board and Binghamton University's Human Subjects Research Review Committee. Written consent was obtained from all subjects before participating in the study.

Genotyping

Buccal cell samples for DNA analysis (Feigelson et al., 2001) were obtained from participants and processed in the Laboratory of Evolutionary Anthropology and Health at Binghamton University, New York. DNA was extracted using an abbreviated version of the silica extraction protocol (Boom et al., 1990) previously described by Lum et al., (1998).

DRD4 VNTR

The DRD4 48-bp VNTR polymorphism is in exon 3 of the gene coding for the dopamine receptor D4. The VNTR polymorphism varies between 2 and 11 repeats of a similar 48 bp coding region sequence, with a tri-modal distribution of 2, 4 and 7 repeat alleles (2R, 4R and 7R) in most, but not all, populations (Ding et al., 2002). Although the functional significance of the DRD4 VNTR polymorphism has not been definitively characterized, long alleles (typically 7R as opposed to 4R) have been generally found to be functionally less reactive in in vitro expression experiments (Van Tol et al., 1992; Asghari et al., 1995; Schoots and Van Tol, 2003; Van Craenenbroeck et al., 2005; Czermak et al., 2006), with some heterogeneity (Asghari et al., 1994; Jovanovic et al., 1999; Watts et al., 1999; Oak et al., 2001; Cho et al., 2006). Additionally, in vivo human pharmacological studies are also generally consistent with the notion that 7R alleles are associated with less responsive D4 receptors than 4R alleles (Hutchison et al., 2003; Hamarman et al., 2004; Brody et al., 2006; Hutchison et al., 2006; McGough et al., 2006). The DRD4 gene codes for a receptor for dopamine that is particularly expressed in the prefrontal cortex (Lahti et al., 1995; Matsumoto et al., 1996; Meador-Woodruff et al., 1996; Defagot and Antonelli, 1997; Mulcrone and Kerwin, 1997; De la Garza and Madras, 2000; Zhang et al., 2002; Callier et al., 2003; Durston et al., 2005; Diamond, 2005; Noain et al., 2006; Beazely et al., 2006).

Sufficient DNA for DRD4 PCR amplification was extracted from 166 Binghamton University and 95 Harvard University buccal cell samples. All samples that were initially scored as homozygotes were reanalyzed two additional times with different starting template concentrations to decrease the likelihood of allelic dropout and other errors (for more details on genotyping issues at this locus see: Hamarman et al., 2004; Eisenberg et al., 2007; Eisenberg et al., 2008). The PCR reaction consisted of 1× Q-Solution (Qiagen), 1× Buffer (Qiagen), 1 µM Primer 1 (5′ GCGACTACGTGGTCTACTCG 3′), 1 µM Primer 2 (5′ AGGACCCTCATGGCCTTG 3′), 200 µM dATP, 200 µM dTTP, 200 µM dCTP, 100 µM dITP, 100 µM dGTP, 0.3 units HotStar Taq (Qiagen), and 1 µl of DNA template, in a total volume of 10 µl. The PCR profile began with 15 min at 95°C for enzyme activation and denaturing of template DNA followed by 40 cycles consisting of 1 min denaturation at 94°C, 1 min annealing at 55°C, 1.5 min extension at 72°C, and finished with a 10 min extension at 72°C. Amplicons were electrophoresed through 1.4–2.0% agarose gels containing ethidium bromide, and genotypes were determined by comparison with a 100 bp ladder.

DRD4 allele and genotype frequencies are given in Table 1. DRD4 in the Harvard dataset was consistent with Hardy–Weinberg Equilibrium (HWE; Markov Chain algorithm, P = 0.717), while in the Binghamton subject pool, HWE was violated (Markov Chain algorithm, P = 0.006). Some caution should be used in interpreting the Binghamton results because its deviations from HWE might suggest genotyping errors (Hosking et al., 2004). While HWE violations have been shown to have the potential to bias results in case–control studies, it seems less likely that this would bias the current type of association study. Since HWE assumes a large, randomly mating population, our use of a small sample of a relatively narrow cohort of young individuals who might assortatively mate with respect to the allele in question might account for the HW disequilibrium instead of genotyping error.

Table 1.

DRD4 allele and genotype frequencies by each independent sample

Harvard
Binghamton
Allele/genotype n % n % Expected %
Allele
2 24 12.6 38 11.0
3 6 3.2 9 2.6
4 129 67.9 246 71.1
5 4 2.1 4 1.2
6 2 1.1 0 0.0
7 25 13.2 47 13.6
9 0 0.0 2 0.6
Total 190 100.0 346 100.0
Genotype classification
2/2 1 1.1 7 4.0 1.2
2/3 1 1.1 1 0.6 0.6
2/4 20 21.1 22 12.7 15.6
2/7 1 1.1 1 0.6 3.0
3/3 0 0.0 1 0.6 0.1
3/4 3 3.2 6 3.5 3.7
3/7 2 2.1 0 0.0 0.7
4/4 41 43.2 90 52.0 50.5
4/5 3 3.2 4 2.3 1.6
4/6 2 2.1 0 0.0 0.0
4/7 19 20.0 33 19.1 19.3
4/9 0 0.0 1 0.6 0.8
5/7 1 1.1 0 0.0 0.3
7/7 1 1.1 6 3.5 1.8
7/9 0 0.0 1 0.6 0.2
7– 71 74.7 132 76.3
7+ 24 25.3 41 23.7
Total 95.0 100.0 173.0 100.0 ∼99.5

Since the Binghamton sample is in Hardy–Weinberg disequilibrium, expected genotype percentages are given. ∼ does not sum to 100% because expected frequencies of genotypes not found in the sample are not shown.

Measures of cross-cultural pairing

Three different types of measures of assortative mating for ancestry were used: (i) ancestral partnering patterns, (ii) current/recent partner patterns and (iii) expected future partnering patterns.

Ancestral partnering patterns

These were evaluated with different means in each dataset. In the Harvard dataset, subjects were asked the ethnicities of each of their four grandparents (ancestry I). They were instructed not to answer if they did not know and to circle as many choices for each grandparent as necessary. Choices were semi-structured with options: European, East Asian, Hispanic/Latino, African American and an other category that allowed free responses. Only one subject reported more than two ancestries and he was combined with those reporting two ancestries for analysis. In the Binghamton survey, subjects were given a question identical to that of above (ancestry I), and because of a printing error, the ‘other’ category contained a very small area for free responses that subjects did not take advantage of or realize the purpose of (Harvard participants consistently filled in free responses in their questionnaires where this error did not appear). This question was thus effectively a simple multiple-choice question. Since those indicating an ‘other’ ancestry are also more likely to have multiple ancestries than the general sample populations in both the Harvard dataset (not shown) and previous study (Eisenberg et al., 2007, unpublished data), this error likely decreases power to detect a DRD4 multiple-ancestry association and might even bias the analysis (e.g. if lack of a free-response option changes subjects’ selection). Subjects in the Binghamton survey were also asked to respond freely by listing their ‘ethnic group background/identification (please be as specific as possible)’. The number of mutually exclusive categories listed was then counted (ancestry II). Ancestry II was also reduced to a dichotomous variable representing having multiple ancestries or not (ancestry III). Those who only listed their ethnic group as ‘American’ were not analyzed. Ancestry I tends to measure partnering patterns across continents/regions, while ancestry II and III tend to also measure more fine-grained differences across countries (e.g. having Irish and German roots). It is important to note that ancestry I is the measure that most closely parallels the measure used in the only previous study of DRD4 and multi-racial ancestry (Eisenberg et al., 2007).

Recent partnering patterns

These were evaluated by asking, ‘What are the ethnicities of your three most important sexual partners (most important first)?’. Choices were: European, East Asian, Hispanic/Latino, African American and Other. Partner ethnicities were matched to the subjects’ ethnicities and scored dichotomously as all partners congruent with subject ancestry, or not. This measure parallels ancestry I, in that ethnic groups are assessed in the same manner and it tends to measure partnering across continents/regions.

Expected future partners

Subjects were asked to rate their agreement with two statements on a 1–5 scale (1 being strongly disagree and 5 strongly agree): (i) ‘I would be willing to have a romantic relationship with someone from a different race than myself.’ and (ii) ‘I would be willing to get married to someone from a different race than myself?’.

It should be noted that the questions in the Binghamton study came after a long series of questions about sexual behaviors, sexual expectations and sexual feelings. This may have affected responses to our measures of cross-cultural pairing. A previous study has shown that DRD4/7R individuals are less likely to answer the Sociosexual Orientation Inventory (Eisenberg et al., 2007), a questionnaire with fewer and less-in-depth questions about sexual behaviors. If this is a factor, it seems most likely that it would bias the sample by producing more missing values in 7R individuals. However, this is not seen in our analysis of missing values (not shown).

Data analysis

HW equilibriums were tested with the HWE program (J. Brzustowski, http://www2.biology.ualberta.ca/jbrzusto/hwenj.html) using a Markov Chain algorithm (Guo and Thompson, 1992). All other statistical analyses utilized STATA/IC 10.0. Distributions in regressions were homoskedastic (using the Breusch–Pagan/Cook–Weisberg test for heteroskedasticity). DRD4 genotypes were parsed by the number of 7R alleles for regressions and 7R− vs 7R+ in cross tabulations. In cross tabulations, Pearson chi-square tests were used when expected cell frequencies exceeded 10, and Fisher's exact tests when below 10. An α value of 0.05 was used throughout. Since we have clear a priori predictions, one-sided significance values are used where appropriate to the statistical test.

Due to the complexity of parsing ethnicity, as well as DRD4 genotypes, the raw data used for this analysis are available upon request or from www.dtae.net.

RESULTS

Pair-wise correlations between measures of assortative mating for ancestry are given in Table 2 for the Harvard and Binghamton studies, respectively. The remainder of the analysis looks at these traits individually in an exploratory fashion because correlations are generally not high, and the measures likely represent distinct facets of the traits of interest. Of important note, in the Binghamton study (Table 2), ancestry I is barely correlated with ancestry II or ancestry III. Ancestry I has a strong correlation with recent partners in both studies, probably because subjects identifying as having multiple ancestries are unlikely to meet people of the same multiple ancestries as themselves. Correlations between variables are generally consistent between the Harvard and Binghamton (Table 2) subsamples (e.g. significant correlations between ancestry 1 and recent partners, recent partners and future romantic, and future romantic and future marriage), suggesting that these measures and populations are generally similar.

Table 2.

Pair-wise correlations between measures of assortative mating for ancestry

Harvard study 1 2 3 4
1 Ancestry I 1.000
2 Recent partners 0.249* 1.000
3 Future romantic 0.134 0.226* 1.000
4 Future marriage 0.156 0.187 0.645* 1.000
Binghamton study 1 2 3 4 5 6
1 Ancestry I 1.000
2 Ancestry II 0.014 1.000
3 Ancestry III 0.010 0.849* 1.000
4 Recent partners 0.505* −0.110 −0.088 1.000
5 Future romantic 0.038 0.094 0.025 0.261* 1.000
6 Future marriage 0.024 0.140 0.087 0.280* 0.847* 1.000

In Binghamton study, ancestry I is from the multiple choice measure, ancestry II from the free-response measure and ancestry III is a dichotomized version of ancestry II.

*P < 0.05.

Ancestral partnering patterns

There was no association between having diverse ancestry and being 7R+ in the Harvard dataset using the semi-structured measure (ancestry I; Table 3; n = 95, one-sided Fisher's Exact, P = 0.562) or the Binghamton dataset using an effectively multiple-choice measure (ancestry I; Table 3; n = 173, one-sided Fisher's Exact, P = 0.104).

Table 3.

Relationships between DRD4 and ancestral diversity

Harvard study (using the semi-structured ancestry I measure)
Binghamton study (using a multiple-choice version of ancestry I)
Binghamton dataset (using a free-response measure (ancestry III)
7R– 7R+ Total 7R– 7R+ Total 7R– 7R+ Total
1 ancestry Obs. 63 21 84 102 36 138 93 22 115
Exp. 62.8 21.2 84 105.3 32.7 138 87.3 27.7 115
>1 ancestry Obs. 8 3 11 30 5 35 36 19 58
Exp. 8.2 2.8 11 26.7 8.3 35 41.7 13.3 58
Total 71 24 95 132 41 173 132 41 173
One-sided Fisher's exact = 0.562 One-sided Fisher's exact = 0.104 Pearson's chi-square = 4.83, P = 0.028

In the Binghamton survey, using the second ancestral partnering evaluation (ancestry II) means simply asking subjects for their ethnic background in one free-response question; ancestry varies from one reported ancestry to seven with a median of one. Those who were 7R+ in the Binghamton dataset were more likely to have diverse ancestries (ancestry III; Table 3; n = 170, Pearson chi-square = 4.83, P = 0.028). Results were similar when the diversity of ancestry was regressed against the number of 7R alleles (ancestry II; n = 170, t = 3.73, β = 0.321, R2 = 0.022, one-sided P = 0.028). Each additional 7R allele was associated with reporting 0.321 more ancestry groups. Similarly, when the sample was restricted to only those with ancestry in Europe, in an effort to eliminate the effect of dramatically different DRD4 allele frequencies in places such as Asia and South America, a stronger relationship was found, despite decreased power (n = 115, t = 2.32, β = 0.450, R2 = 0.05, one sided P = 0.011). When the scale of ancestral diversity was categorized by whether diversity occurred at the intra-continental or inter-continental scales, there was a near significant trend towards increasing 7R allele frequencies with an increasingly large scale of ancestral diversity (n = 169, t = 1.62, β = 0.145, R2 = .016, one sided P = 0.054). Ancestry II and ancestry III variables were not available in the Harvard survey.

Recent partnering patterns

Incongruities in ancestries between subjects and their past/present sexual partners were near significantly related to the presence of 7R alleles in the Harvard study (Table 4; one-sided Fisher's exact, P = 0.081), but not in the Binghamton study (Table 4; Pearson chi-square = 0.432, P = 0.511). It should be noted that the current/recent partnering patterns from the Binghamton survey are based upon the same partially flawed question as seen above in Table 3. That is, because subjects specifying a grandparent from an ‘other’ category generally did not specify what this ‘other’category was, we were unable to distinguish whether these subjects matched their partners or not.

Table 4.

Relationship between current/recent partnering patterns and DRD4

Harvard study
Binghamton study
7R– 7R+ Total 7R– 7R+ Total
Matched Obs. 29 6 35 57 22 79
Exp. 25.7 9.3 35 58.7 20.3 79
Unmatched Obs. 37 18 55 47 14 61
Exp. 40.3 14.7 55 45.3 15.7 61
Total 66 24 90 104 36 140
One-sided Fisher's exact = 0.081 Pearson's chi-square = 0.432, P = 0.511

Expected future partners

The distribution of the two Likert Scale questions, ‘I would be willing to have a romantic relationship with someone from a different race than myself’ and ‘I would be willing to get married to someone from a different race than myself?’ were heavily skewed towards complete agreement with the respective statements. For this reason, and because the scale is ordinal, Kruskal–Wallis tests were used to test if scores differed by 7R allele presence. No significant or near significant associations were found between 7R+ presence and willingness to either have romantic relationships with those from different races (Harvard: n = 93, df = 1, chi-square = 1.218, P = 0.270; Binghamton: n = 168, df = 1, chi-square = 0.104, P = 0.747), or marry those from different races (Harvard: n = 93, df = 1, chi-square = 0.048, P = 0.827; Binghamton: n = 168, df = 1, chi-square = 0.607, P = 0.436).

DISCUSSION

The findings of this study of two independent samples coupled with the similar past study of Eisenberg et al. (2007) suggest partial support for the hypothesis that DRD4/7R is associated with having multiple ancestries. The current study particularly illustrates the difficulties of measuring ancestry and how sensitive results can be to differences in question phrasing and layout.

Ancestral partnering was not associated with 7R in the Harvard dataset and only associated with 7R by the finer scale measure of ancestry in the Binghamton dataset. We note that the finer scale ancestry measure used in the Binghamton dataset (ancestry II/III) was not available in the Harvard dataset. Recent relationships with partners from differing ancestries was near significantly associated with 7R+ in the Harvard study, but not in the Binghamton one (however, a finer scale measure for recent relationships analogous to ancestry II/III was not available in either study). It should be noted that DRD4 genotype associations with subjects’ ancestries are actually proxy measures for the associations of the behaviors of the subjects’ ancestors with DRD4 genotypes. Since a subject with a 7R allele by definition had more ancestors with 7R alleles, the subject's genotype serves as a rough proxy for ancestral genotypes. Expected future partnering patterns were not related to DRD4/7R. The fact that only ancestral partnering patterns and not recent partnering or planned future partnering were significantly associated with DRD4 suggests some explanations including: type II error, that most measures were insufficiently specific, that the nature of mating based upon ancestry has become less taboo in recent years, or perhaps that ancestral interracial pairing was a greater reflection of traveling out of one's country, while multi-cultural college communities afford much more mixing.

The variation of results by different ancestry measures as well as their low correlations (Table 2) warrant further discussion. As noted above, the first measure of multiple ancestries in the Binghamton dataset (ancestry I) was likely inadequate because it did not leave space for subjects to define their ancestry in a free response. As such, we believe that the second ancestry measure (ancestry II) was superior in that it elicited a finer scale response of ancestral backgrounds. In fact, this free-response method is probably superior to the measure used in previous study (Eisenberg et al., 2007) in that it was better able to quantify multiple ancestries from different countries and cultures in the same continent (the measure used in the 2007 study most closely parallels the ancestry I measure). If the ancestry II variable was assessed in the Harvard study, the results between surveys likely would have been more consistent with our hypothesis.

Regardless of the validity of ancestry scales I and II, the low pair-wise correlations between the two suggests that they are measuring substantively different factors. Since marriage practices and genetic similarities between populations tend to be highly correlated with geographical distance between populations (isolation by distance) as well as exhibiting genetic discontinuities between continents (Peach and Mitchell, 1988; Rosenberg et al., 2005), measuring ancestral differences across both the intra- and inter-continental scales is likely an important distinction to retain.

We note that the young age of participants limits the conclusions we can draw from the ancestries of their past sexual partners. Sexual partners at this age might more reflect experimentation than who a subject will actually have children with. Similarly, self-reported feelings about romantic relationships and marriage with those of other races might be a greater reflection of explicit social norms (especially on the relatively liberal college campuses where these studies were conducted), rather than implicit biases and actual behavior (e.g. Cronk, 1999, p. 9). It is also likely that actual behaviors have changed much over the past few generations, such that interracial dating and marriage are currently much more acceptable/prevalent (Johnson et al., 1980; Ahern et al., 1983; Nalls et al., 2009; however linear trends should not be assumed: Halpin and Chan, 2003). In support of the notion that there might be a secular trend in inter-ethnic partnering, there is evidence that ‘close, positive interracial contact’ decreases racial prejudice (Blascovich et al., 2001; Olsson et al., 2005).

From these two studies of DRD4 and ancestry and the previous one (Eisenberg et al., 2007), we are struck by how carefully researchers must phrase questions about ancestry in order to gain the necessary information. We suggest that future studies that analyze ancestry for similar purposes use free-response questions to ask specifically about the ancestry of each grandparent. Subjects should be given examples of possible answers (e.g. ‘Western European’, ‘Scottish-Irish-German’, ‘Ashkenazi Jewish’, ‘Korean’ etc.) and instructed to be as specific as their knowledge allows.

It might also be beneficial to explore family histories more deeply to understand the contexts that lead to partnering patterns (e.g. Gal, 1979). Did grandparents and parents of different ancestries meet because one or both partners were traveling/immigrating? What roles, if any, do the stigmatization or positive-prejudices of out-groups play? What about the economic status of partners? How do these patterns in past generations compare and contrast with those seen today? Differences in DRD4 responses to different races should be further analyzed. Perhaps DRD4 influences mating patterns via altering affective conditioning (Livingston and Drwecki, 2007).

Genetic measures of individual admixture (e.g. heterozygosities) might also provide a more objective measure of multiple ancestries (Nalls et al., 2009). We predict that these genetic measures will correlate more strongly with DRD4/7R alleles than more subjective and memory-constrained self-report measures.

We wish to be clear that while genetic factors might play a role in behavioral differences in human populations and propensities for cross-population mating, this does not preclude the importance of other developmental, political, economic and social factors. Given the small effect sizes and R2 values observed here, it is clear that DRD4 accounts for only a small part of the additive variance (if any). We suspect that other factors might be more important proximate determinants that in some cases feedback and select for particular genes (Richerson and Boyd, 2005). While the theoretical and evolutionary implications of this corpus of literature is compelling for the understanding of human diversity and our evolutionary history (e.g. the implications of assortative mating for multi-level selection and altruism as in Sober and Wilson, 1998), we are not aware of any legitimate (never-mind moral) policy or other practical implications of this line of research.

Conflict of Interest

None declared.

Footnotes

The authors thank Peter Ellison for his help in making this study possible and Richard Zamore, Edward Aller and Rita Spathis for valuable laboratory assistance. The Newton Free Library provided research assistance. D.T.A.E. is funded by a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship and A.D. by the Jan Wallander and Tom Hedelius Foundation. The Binghamton study and all genotyping was supported in part by the Binghamton University Foundation's Research Awards (to J.R.G.) and by startup funds (to J.K.L.) from Binghamton University.

REFERENCES

  1. Ahern FM, Johnson RC, Cole RE. Generational differences in spouse similarity in education attainment. Behavior Genetics. 1983;13:95–8. doi: 10.1007/BF01071747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Apicella CL, Dreber A, Campbell B, Gray PB, Hoffman M, Little AC. Testosterone and financial risk preferences. Evolution and Human Behavior. 2008;29:384–90. [Google Scholar]
  3. Aragona BJ, Liu Y, Yu YJ, et al. Nucleus accumbens dopamine differentially mediates the formation and maintenance of monogamous pair bonds. Nature Neuroscience. 2006;9:133–9. doi: 10.1038/nn1613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Asghari V, Sanyal S, Buchwaldt S, Paterson A, Jovanovic V, Vantol HHM. Modulation of intracellular cyclic-amp levels by different human dopamine D4 receptor variants. Journal of Neurochemistry. 1995;65:1157–65. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1995.65031157.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Asghari V, Schoots O, Vankats S, et al. Dopamine D4 receptor repeat—analysis of different native and mutant forms of the human and rat genes. Molecular Pharmacology. 1994;46:364–73. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Beazely MA, Tong A, Wei WL, Van Tol H, Sidhu B, MacDonald JF. D2-class dopamine receptor inhibition of NMDA currents in prefrontal cortical neurons is platelet-derived growth factor receptor-dependent. Journal of Neurochemistry. 2006;98:1657–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2006.04064.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Blascovich J, Mendes WB, Hunter SB, Lickel B, Kowai-Bell N. Perceiver threat in social interactions with stigmatized others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;80:253–67. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Boas F. Race problems in America. Science. 1909;29:839–849. doi: 10.1126/science.29.752.839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Boom R, Sol C, Salimans M, Jansen C, Wertheim-van Dillen P, van der Noordaa J. Rapid and simple method for purification of nucleic acids. Journal of Clinical Microbiology. 1990;28:495–503. doi: 10.1128/jcm.28.3.495-503.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brody AL, Mandelkern MA, Olmstead RE, et al. Gene variants of brain dopamine pathways and smoking-induced dopamine release in the ventral caudate/nucleus accumbens. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63:808–16. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.7.808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Callier S, Snapyan M, Crom SL, Prou D, Vincent JD, Vernier P. Evolution and cell biology of dopamine receptors in vertebrates. Biology of the Cell. 2003;95:489–502. doi: 10.1016/s0248-4900(03)00089-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chen CS, Burton M, Greenberger E, Dmitrieva J. Population migration and the variation of dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) allele frequencies around the globe. Evolution and Human Behavior. 1999;20:309–24. [Google Scholar]
  13. Cho DI, Beorn S, Van Tol HHM, Caron MG, Kim KM. Characterization of the desensitization properties of five dopamine receptor subtypes and alternatively spliced variants of dopamine D-2 and D-4 receptors. Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications. 2006;350:634–40. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2006.09.090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Ciani ASC, Capiluppi C, Veronese A, Sartori G. The adaptive value of personality differences revealed by small island population dynamics. European Journal of Personality. 2007;21:3–22. [Google Scholar]
  15. Cronk L. That Complex Whole: Culture and the Evolution of Human Behavior. Boulder, CO: Westview Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  16. Curtis JT, Wang Z. Glucocorticoid receptor involvement in pair bonding in female prairie voles: the effects of acute blockade and interactions with central dopamine reward systems. Neuroscience. 2005;134:369–76. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Czermak C, Lehofer M, Liebmann PM, Traynor J. [S-35]GTP gamma S binding at the human dopamine D4 receptor variants hD4.2, hD4.4 and hD4.7 following stimulation by dopamine, epinephrine and norepinephrine. European Journal of Pharmacology. 2006;531:20–4. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2005.11.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Darwin C. The Origin of Species and the Descent of Man. New York: Random House; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  19. De la Garza R, Madras BK. [H-3]PNU-101958, a D-4 dopamine receptor probe, accumulates in prefrontal cortex and hippocampus of non-human primate brain. Synapse. 2000;37:232–44. doi: 10.1002/1098-2396(20000901)37:3<232::AID-SYN7>3.0.CO;2-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Defagot MC, Antonelli MC. Autoradiographic localization of the putative D4 dopamine receptor in rat brain. Neurochemical Research. 1997;22:401–7. doi: 10.1023/a:1027399408608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Diamond A. Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (with hyperactivity) Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17:807–25. doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Ding YC, Chi HC, Grady DL, et al. Evidence of positive selection acting at the human dopamine receptor D4 gene locus. Proceedings of the National Academy of the USA. 2002;99:309–14. doi: 10.1073/pnas.012464099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dominguez JM, Hull EM. Dopamine, the medial preoptic area, and male sexual behavior. Physiology & Behavior. 2005;86:356–68. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dreber A, Apicella CL, Eisenberg DTA, et al. The 7R polymorphism in the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) is associated with financial risk-taking in men. Evolution and Human Behavior. 2009;30:85–92. [Google Scholar]
  25. Durston S, Fossella JA, Casey BJ, et al. Differential effects of DRD4 and DAT1 genotype on fronto-striatal gray matter volumes in a sample of subjects with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, their unaffected siblings, and controls. Molecular Psychiatry. 2005;10:678–85. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Eisenberg DTA, Campbell B, Gray PB, Sorenson MD. Dopamine receptor genetic polymorphisms and body composition in undernourished pastoralists: an exploration of nutrition indices among nomadic and recently settled Ariaal men of northern Kenya. BMC Evolutionary Biology. 2008;8 doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-8-173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Eisenberg DTA, Campbell B, MacKillop J, et al. Polymorphisms in the dopamine D2 and D4 receptor genes and reproductive, sexual and life history behaviors. Evolutionary Psychology. 2007;5:696–715. [Google Scholar]
  28. Eisenberg DTA, MacKillop J, Modi M, et al. Examining impulsivity as an endophenotype using a behavioral approach: a DRD2 TaqI A and DRD4 48-bp VNTR association study. Behavioral and Brain Functions. 2007;3(2) doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-3-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Feigelson H, Rodriguez C, Robertson A, et al. Determinants of DNA yield and quality from buccal cell samples collected with mouthwash. Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention. 2001;10:1005–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Fisher H, Aron A, Brown LL. Romantic love: an fMRI study of a neural mechanism for mate choice. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2005;493:58–62. doi: 10.1002/cne.20772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Fisher HE. Why We Love: The Nature and Chemistry of Romantic Love. New York: Holt; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  32. Gal S. Peasant men can’t get wives: language change and sex roles in a bilingual community. Language in Society. 1979;7:1–16. [Google Scholar]
  33. Gingrich B, Liu Y, Cascio C, Wang ZX, Insel TR. Dopamine D2 receptors in the nucleus accumbens are important for social attachment in female prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) Behavioral Neuroscience. 2000;114:173–183. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.114.1.173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Guo G, Tong YY. Age at first sexual intercourse, genes, and social context: evidence from twins and the dopamine D4 receptor gene. Demography. 2006;43:747–69. doi: 10.1353/dem.2006.0029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Guo SW, Thompson EA. Performing the exact test of Hardy–Weinberg proportion for multiple alleles. Biometrics. 1992;48:361–72. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Halpin B, Chan TW. Educational homogamy in Ireland and Britain: trends and patterns. The British Journal of Sociology. 2003;54:473–95. doi: 10.1080/0007131032000143546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hamarman S, Fossella J, Ulger C, Brimacombe M, Dermody J. Dopamine receptor 4 (DRD4) 7-repeat allele predicts methylphenidate dose response in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a pharmacogenetic study. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology. 2004;14:564–74. doi: 10.1089/cap.2004.14.564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hamer DH. Genetics of Sexual Behavior. In: Benjamin J, Ebstein R, Belmaker R, editors. Molecular Genetics and the Human Personality. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2002. p. 265. [Google Scholar]
  39. Harris TM, Kalbfleisch PJ. Interracial dating: the implications of race for initiating a romantic relationship. Howard Journal of Communications. 2000;11:49–64. [Google Scholar]
  40. Hosking L, Lumsden S, Lewis K, et al. Detection of genotyping errors by Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium testing. European Journal of Human Genetics. 2004;12:395–9. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hutchison KE, Ray L, Sandman E, et al. The effect of olanzapine on craving and alcohol consumption. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:1310–7. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hutchison KE, Wooden A, Swift RM, et al. Olanzapine reduces craving for alcohol: A DRD4 VNTR polymorphism by pharmacotherapy interaction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28:1882–8. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Johnson RC, Ahern FM, Cole RE. Secular change in degree of assortative mating for ability? Behavior Genetics. 1980;10:1–8. doi: 10.1007/BF01067315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Jokela M. Personality predicts migration within and between U.S. states. Journal of Research in Personality. 2009;43:79–83. [Google Scholar]
  45. Jokela M, Elovainio M, Kivimäki M, Keltikangas-Järvinen L. Temperament and migration patterns in Finland. Psychological Science. 2008;19:831–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02164.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Jovanovic V, Guan HC, Van Tol HHM. Comparative pharmacological and functional analysis of the human dopamine D-4.2 and D-4.10 receptor variants. Pharmacogenetics. 1999;9:561–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kalmijn M. Intermarriage and homogamy: causes, patterns, trends. Annual Review of Sociology. 1998;24:395–421. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.24.1.395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kluger AN, Siegfried Z, Ebstein RP. A meta-analysis of the association between DRD4 polymorphism and novelty seeking. Molecular Psychiatry. 2002;7:712–7. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Kuhnen CM, Chiao JY. Genetic determinants of financial risk taking. PLoS ONE. 2009;4:e4362. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lahti RA, Roberts RC, Tamminga CA. D-2-family receptor distribution in human postmortem tissue: an autoradiographic study. Neuroreport. 1995;6:2505–12. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199512150-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Li D, Sham PC, Owen MJ, He L. Meta-analysis shows significant association between dopamine system genes and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Human Molecular Genetics. 2006;15:2276–84. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddl152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Livingston RW, Drwecki BB. Why are some individuals not racially biased? Susceptibility to affective conditioning predicts nonprejudice toward blacks. Psychological Science. 2007;18:816–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01985.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Lucentini Gene association studies typically wrong. Scientist. 2004;18:20. [Google Scholar]
  54. Lum J, Cann R, Martinson J, Jorde L. Mitochondrial and nuclear genetic relationships among Pacific island and Asian populations. American Journal of Human Genetics. 1998;63:613–24. doi: 10.1086/301949. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Matsumoto M, Hidaka K, Tada S, Tasaki Y, Yamaguchi T. Low levels of mRNA for dopamine D4 receptor in human cerebral cortex and striatum. Journal of Neurochemistry. 1996;66:915–9. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1996.66030915.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. McGough J, McCracken J, Swanson J, et al. Pharmacogenetics of methylphenidate response in preschoolers with ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2006;45:1314–22. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000235083.40285.08. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Meador-Woodruff J, Damask S, Wang J, Haroutunian V, Davis K, Watson S. Dopamine receptor mRNA expression in human striatum and neocortex. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1996;15:17–29. doi: 10.1016/0893-133X(95)00150-C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Melis MR, Argiolas A. Dopamine and sexual-behavior. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 1995;19:19–38. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(94)00020-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Melis MR, Succu S, Sanna F, et al. PIP3EA and PD-168077, two selective dopamine D4 receptor agonists, induce penile erection in male rats: site and mechanism of action in the brain. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;24:2021–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05043.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Miller WB, Pasta DJ, MacMurray J, Chiu C, Wu H, Comings DE. Dopamine receptor genes are associated with age at first sexual intercourse. Journal of Biosocial Science. 1999;31:43–54. doi: 10.1017/s0021932099000437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Mulcrone J, Kerwin RW. The regional pattern of D4 gene expression in human brain. Neuroscience Letters. 1997;234:147–50. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(97)00702-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Nalls MA, Simon-Sanchez J, Gibbs JR, et al. Measures of autozygosity in decline: globalization, urbanization, and its implications for medical genetics. PLoS Genetics. 2009;5:e1000415. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1000415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Noain D, Avale ME, Wedemeyer C, Calvo D, Peper M, Rubinstein M. Identification of brain neurons expressing the dopamine D4 receptor gene using BAC transgenic mice. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;24:2429–38. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05148.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Oak JN, Lavine N, Tol HHMV. Dopamine D4 and D2L receptor stimulation of the mitogen-activated protein kinase pathway is dependent on trans-activation of the platelet-derived growth factor receptor. Molecular Pharmacology. 2001;60:92–103. doi: 10.1124/mol.60.1.92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Olsson A, Ebert JP, Banaji MR, Phelps EA. The role of social groups in the persistence of learned fear. Science. 2005;309:785–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1113551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Peach C, Mitchell JC. Human Mating Patterns. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1988. Marriage distance and ethnicity. In: Mascie-Taylor, C.G.N., Boyce, A.J., editors; p. 237. [Google Scholar]
  67. Rebollo I, Boomsma DI. Personality: possible effects of inbreeding depression on sensation seeking. European Journal of Personality. 2007;21:621–3. [Google Scholar]
  68. Richerson PJ, Boyd R. Not By Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  69. Rosenberg NA, Mahajan S, Ramachandran S, Zhao C, Pritchard JK, Feldman MW. Clines, clusters, and the effect of study design on the inference of human population structure. PLoS Genetics. 2005;1:e70. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.0010070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Savitz JB, Ramesar RS. Genetic variants implicated in personality: a review of the more promising candidates. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B—Neuropsychiatric Genetics. 2004;131B:20–32. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.20155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Schinka JA, Letsch EA, Crawford FC. DRD4 and novelty seeking: results of meta-analyses. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 2002;114:643–8. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.10649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Schoots O, Van Tol HHM. The human dopamine D4 receptor repeat sequences modulate expression. Pharmacogenomics Journal. 2003;3:343–8. doi: 10.1038/sj.tpj.6500208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Smeltzer MD, Curtis JT, Aragona BJ, Wang ZX. Dopamine, oxytocin, and vasopressin receptor binding in the medial prefrontal cortex of monogamous and promiscuous voles. Neuroscience Letters. 2006;394:146–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.10.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Smits J, Ultee W, Lammers J. Educational homogamy in 65 countries: an explanation of differences in openness using country-level explanatory variables. American Sociological Review. 1998;63:264–85. [Google Scholar]
  75. Sober E, Wilson DS. Unto Others: The Evolution and Psychology of Unselfish Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  76. Succu S, Sanna F, Melis T, Boi A, Argiolas A, Melis MR. Stimulation of dopamine receptors in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus of male rats induces penile erection and increases extra-cellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens: involvement of central oxytocin. Neuropharmacology. 2007;52:1034–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2006.10.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Van Craenenbroeck K, Clark SD, Cox MJ, Oak JN, Liu F, Van Tol HHM. Folding efficiency is rate-limiting in dopamine D4 receptor biogenesis. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 2005;280:19350–7. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M414043200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Van Tol HHM, Wu CM, Guan HC, et al. Multiple dopamine D4 receptor variants in the human population. Nature. 1992;358:149–52. doi: 10.1038/358149a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Wang E, Ding YC, Flodman P, et al. The genetic architecture of selection at the human dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) gene locus. American Journal of Human Genetics. 2004;74:931–44. doi: 10.1086/420854. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Watts VJ, Vu MN, Wiens BL, Jovanovic V, Tol HHMV, Neve KA. Short- and long-term heterologous sensitization of adenylate cyclase by D4 dopamine receptors. Psychopharmacology. 1999;141:84–92. doi: 10.1007/s002130050810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Zhang KH, Tarazi FI, Davids E, Baldessarini RJ. Plasticity of dopamine D-4 receptors in rat forebrain: temporal association with motor hyperactivity following neonatal 6-hydroxydopamine lesioning. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26:625–33. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00404-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Zion IB, Tessler R, Cohen L, et al. Polymorphisms in the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) contribute to individual differences in human sexual behavior: desire, arousal and sexual function. Molecular Psychiatry. 2006:1–5. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001832. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES