Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Jul 2.
Published in final edited form as: J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009 Jan;15(1):130–136. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708090024

White matter and neurocognitive changes in adults with chronic traumatic brain injury

Mary RT Kennedy 1, Jeffrey R Wozniak 2, Ryan L Muetzel 2, Bryon A Mueller 2, Hsin-Huei Chiou 1, Kari Pantekoek 1,2, Kelvin O Lim 2
PMCID: PMC2895769  NIHMSID: NIHMS210877  PMID: 19128536

Abstract

Diffusion tensor imaging was used to investigate white matter (WM) integrity in adults with traumatic brain injury (TBI) and healthy adults as controls. Adults with TBI had sustained severe vehicular injuries on the average of 7 years earlier. A multivariate analysis of covariance with verbal IQ as the covariate revealed that adults with TBI had lower fractional anisotropy and higher mean diffusivity than controls, specifically in the three regions of interest (ROIs), the centrum semiovale (CS), the superior frontal (SPF), and the inferior frontal (INF). Adults with TBI averaged in the normal range in motor speed and two of three executive functions and were below average in delayed verbal recall and inhibition, whereas controls were above average. Time since injury, but not age, was associated with WM changes in the SPF ROI, whereas age, but not time since injury, was associated with WM changes in the INF ROI, suggesting that the effects of WM on time since injury may interact with age. To understand the utility of WM changes in chronic recovery, larger sample sizes are needed to investigate associations between cognition and WM integrity of severely injured individuals who have substantial cognitive impairment compared to severely injured individuals with little cognitive impairment.

Keywords: Diffusion tensor imaging, Anisotropy, Brain injury, Executive functions, Cognition

INTRODUCTION

Impairments after traumatic brain injury (TBI) include deficits in motor skills, processing speed, attention, word finding, memory, social skills, and executive functioning. These impairments interfere with successful return to work, school, family, and community—often in relation to the level of injury severity (e.g., Millis et al., 2001; Rohling et al., 2003; Schretlen & Shapiro, 2003; Serino et al., 2006).

TBI results in both gray matter and white matter (WM) tissue loss. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies help to clarify relationships between cognitive deficits and underlying neuropathology (Bigler, 2003; Gale et al., 2005; Johnson et al., 1994), even in cases of mild TBI (Kurca et al., 2006). Diffuse axonal injury (DAI) to WM occurs during rapid acceleration/deceleration and rotation of variously dense brain structures (Adams et al., 1982; Strich, 1961). Conventional MRI is insensitive to DAI except when accompanied by visible bleeds (e.g., Rugg-Gunn et al., 2001) and has limited ability to detect subtle WM injury.

Diffusion Tensor Imaging

Diffusion of water molecules in healthy WM is highly directional or anisotropic, occurring more readily along axons because of natural barriers (cell membranes, myelin, and adjacent axons) (Le Bihan, 1995). Damaged WM bundles have increased diffusion perpendicular to axons and lower anisotropy relative to healthy WM. Two commonly used diffusion measures are fractional anisotropy (FA) and mean diffusivity (MD). FA is a relative measure of diffusion directionality along the axon relative to the perpendicular axis (Pierpaoli & Basser, 1996). Lower FA is observed in TBI, especially in regions where DAI occurs, whereas MD is the average diffusion in all directions and should be higher in TBI.

There is now general consensus that diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) is more sensitive than conventional MRI for detecting DAI in acute and chronic stages of TBI (e.g., Rugg-Gunn et al., 2001). For example, DTI detects changes in WM within hours or days of injury. In humans, anisotropy changes have been detected within 24 (Arfanakis et al., 2002) and 32 (Newcombe et al., 2007) hours and within 6 days in mildly injured adolescents (Wilde et al., 2008). Huisman et al. (2004) found lower FA after TBI within 7 days in the internal capsule and corpus callosum, and these were correlated with injury severity and early outcomes.

Because Wallerian degeneration continues after initial recovery, it is important to describe long-term WM changes in TBI. Within the past year, a few studies have used DTI to document changes in WM in adults with TBI in chronic recovery (e.g., Mathias et al., 2004; Xu et al., 2007; Yuan et al., 2007). Salmond et al. (2006) found widespread decreases in cerebral WM FA in adults with TBI who were at least 6 months postinjury. A small sample of severely injured adults who were 4–6 years postinjury had significant decreased FA in the major WM tracts (Xu et al., 2007). Benson et al. (2007) found that changes in WM FA in a chronically injured group predicted severity of injury, in particular posttraumatic amnesia better than Glasgow Coma Scale scores (GSC). Kraus et al. (2007) examined WM and cognitive changes in adults with mild, moderate, or severe TBI who averaged 10 years postinjury. Compared to healthy adults, FA was decreased in all 13 regions of interest (ROIs) in adults with moderate-to-severe TBI. A general WM load measure was correlated with executive functions, attention, and memory domains.

In the current study, we use an ROI approach to compare WM FA and MD of severely injured adults with TBI who averaged 7 years postinjury to healthy controls. We expected lower WM FA and higher WM MD in the group with TBI. We were particularly interested in the centrum semiovale (CS), superior frontal (SPF), and inferior frontal (INF) ROIs due to the diffuse nature of TBI and the long-lasting executive function difficulties. Additionally, we explored associations between time since injury and WM integrity.

METHODS

Participants

Nine adults with TBI were recruited from rehabilitation programs, newsletters, and support groups. Ten control participants were recruited through advertisements. All were native English speakers without a history of neurological disease, substance abuse, learning difficulties, or psychiatric disorder. One TBI participant was excluded because of an unavailable medical record. One control was dropped for partial completion and another for being an outlier on vocabulary—a verbal IQ estimate. Thus, data from eight adults with TBI and eight controls who were close in age and education were included (Table 1). Groups did not approach significant differences for age and education (p < .05). No participants had aphasia (i.e., aphasia quotient <93.8 on the Western Aphasia Battery; Kertesz, 1982), and all successfully read sentences and paragraphs (reading subtest). Verbal IQ was estimated with the National Adult Reading Test (NART; Nelson & Willison 1991), a stable estimate when administered at least 1 year postinjury (Riley & Simmonds, 2003). Controls averaged higher verbal IQ than adults with TBI, whereas years of education did not statistically differ (p = .10) (Table 1).

Table 1.

Means, SDs, and statistical comparisons of demographics and neurocognitive performance for adults with TBI (n = 8) and healthy controls (n = 8)

Demographics TBI (mean ± SD) Control (mean ± SD) t d
Age at MRI scan (years) 39.07 ± 12.37 40.11 ± 14.69 0.15 0.08
Gender: male/female 6/2 5/3 na na
Education (years) 13.00 ± 1.41 14.38 ± 1.69 1.77 0.88
NART verbal score 105.00 ± 9.62 113.625 ± 4.03 2.34* 1.16

Neurocognitive measures TBI (mean ± SD) Control (mean ± SD) F
ηp2

Executive functions: alternating trails (D-KEFS)
  Number-letter switching 9.5 ± 3.0 11.9 ± 1.4 1.91 .13
  Combined number and letter sequencing 9.8 ± 1.9 12.8 ± 1.7 5.17* .28
  Number-letter switching vs. speed 9.3 ± 3.9 9.5 ± 1.7 0.09 .01
Executive functions: verbal fluency (D-KEFS)
  Letter fluency 10.6 ± 2.9 12.8 ± 2.1 0.08 .01
  Category fluency 9.5 ± 3.7 13.1 ± 3.3 1.45 .10
  Category switching—correct responses 9.8 ± 1.5 14.0 ± 3.3 5.57* .30
  Repetition errors 9.3 ± 2.3 11.1 ± 2.5 1.99 .13
Executive functions: inhibition (go-no-go)
  Omission errors (raw scores) 18.5 ± 18.3 7.8 ± 6.3 4.84* .27
  Commission errors (raw scores) 8.8 ± 4.0 5.9 ± 3.8 5.18* .29
Long-delayed recall: CVLT-II
  Free 7.9 ± 3.4 12.5 ± 3.0 6.43* .33
  Cued 9.5 ± 2.8 12.4 ± 3.0 2.72 .17
Speed: simple trails (D-KEFS)
  Number sequencing 9.3 ± 2.4 11.6 ± 1.7 0.56 .04
  Letter sequencing 9.4 ± 2.1 12.3 ± 1.2 12.39** .49
  Motor speed 10.3 ± 1.6 12.4 ± 0.7 4.63* .26

Note. Group comparisons of neurocognitive performance were analyzed with MANCOVAs in which estimates of verbal intelligence quotient (IQ) were covaried. ηp2 effect size values are interpreted as follows: large is >.2, medium is >.1, and small is >.05. d effect size values are interpreted as follows: large is >.8, medium is >.5, and small is >.2 (Cohen, 1988). CVLT-II, California Verbal Learning Test—II; D-KEFS, Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

All participants with TBI had sustained severe injuries in vehicular accidents that averaged 7.0 years (standard deviation [SD] = 8.6) prior to the study. Early clinical MRIs or CT reports were available and with the exception of one participant, all had substantial cortical neuropathology. Length of coma ranged from 5 to 90 days (mean = 21.75, SD = 28.69). Injury severity was based on coma length, GCS scores when available, and initial neuropathology (Teasdale & Jennett, 1974).

Neurocognitive Measures

All experimental procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the university. The study included two sessions: neurocognitive assessment and MRI. The average time between sessions was 14.6 days (SD = 15.6). Neurocognitive tests were selected based on their known sensitivity to memory and executive functions associated with TBI.

Verbal fluency and trail subtests were administered from the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) (Delis et al., 2001), including letter fluency, category fluency, category switching, and trail making. The go/no-go task, a measure of motor response execution and inhibition, was based on a task described by Braver et al. (2001); individuals with TBI have been shown to be impulsive on this task (Dockree et al., 2004). The California Verbal Learning Test—II (Delis et al., 2000) measures verbal learning during immediate and delayed recall and recognition memory and assesses learning strategies.

Imaging Procedures

MRI data acquisition

MRI was performed on a 3Tesla Trio scanner using an eightchannel array head coil (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). T1 images were acquired coronally using a three-dimensional magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo (MP-RAGE) sequence [repetition time (TR) = 2530 ms, echo time (TE) = 3.65 ms, inversion time (TI) = 1100 ms, 240 slices, 1 × 1 × 1–mm voxel, flip angle = 7°, field of view (FOV) = 256 × 256 mm]. Proton density (PD)–weighted images were acquired axially using a hyperecho turbo spin echo (TSE) sequence (TR) = 8550 ms, (TE) = 14 ms, 80 slices, 1 × 1 × 2–mm voxel, flip angle = 120°, FOV = 256 mm). DTI data were acquired axially, aligned with the TSE images, using a dual–spin echo, single-shot, pulsed-gradient, echo planar imaging sequence (TR = 8300 ms, TE = 86 ms, 64 contiguous slices, 2 × 2 × 2–mm voxel, FOV = 256 mm, two averages, b value = 1000 s/mm2). Thirteen unique volumes were collected to compute the tensor: a b = 0 s/mm2 image and 12 images with diffusion gradients applied in noncollinear directions. A dual–echo flash field map sequence with voxel parameters common to the DTI was used to correct the DTI data for geometric distortion caused by magnetic field inhomogeneity (TR = 700 ms, TE = 4.62 ms/7.08 ms, flip angle = 90°, magnitude, and phase difference contrasts).

Anatomical image processing

Imaging data were processed using tools from the FMRIB software library (FSL, http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/). Nonbrain tissue was removed using brain extraction tool (BET). The PD brain was aligned to the T1 brain using the FMRIB’s linear registration tool (FLIRT). Dual-channel segmentation was performed on the T1 and aligned PD brains using FMRIB’s automated segmentation tool (FAST), producing four tissue classes (cerebral spinal fluid, white, gray, and blood). The T1 brain was registered to the FMRIB software library (FSL) template brain (MNI-152 brain, Montreal Neurological Institute) using a 12-parameter affine transformation.

DTI processing

FMRIB’s diffusion toolbox (FDT) was used to correct the diffusion-weighted images for motion and eddy current distortion and then compute the diffusion tensor. Maps of MD and FA were derived. FA is the anisotropic component of the tensor (Basser, 1995), ranging between 0 (perfectly isotropic) and 1 (diffusion occurring in only one direction). The geometric distortion caused by the magnetic field inhomogeneity was determined from the field map image, and FMRIB’s utility for Geometrically Unwarping EPIs (FUGUE) was used to dewarp the b = 0 diffusion image and the scalar maps (FA, MD).

ROI definition

The same semiautomated procedures reported in Wozniak et al. (2007) were used in this work to define ROIs. Subject-specific WM masks for the dewarped DTI scalar maps were determined by transforming the partial volume estimate (PVE) WM map from the anatomic segmentation onto the distortion-corrected DTI image. Voxels in the dewarped DTI images were classified as WM if the DTI-aligned PVE value exceeded 50% in that voxel.

Three ROIs were selected for evaluation because of the diffuse nature of injury and incidence of executive dysfunction in TBI: the CS, which is all WM voxels superior to corpus callosum; the INF region, which is all WM voxels rostral to the genu of the callosum and inferior (ventral) to the anterior and posterior commisure (AC–PC) plane; and the SPF region, which is all WM voxels rostral to the genu of the callosum and superior to the AC–PC plane (Figure 1). A trained operator determined the boundary of the ROIs on each MNI-aligned T1 image; these planes were aligned on the DTI and convolved with the WM mask to define the WM ROI masks. Mean FA and MD values were computed for the WM voxels within each of the three ROIs.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

(a) The CS region is superior to the corpus callosum. (b) The INF region is rostral to the genu of the corpus callosum and inferior (ventral) to the AC–PC plane. The SPF region is rostral to the genu of the corpus callosum and superior to the AC–PC plane.

RESULTS

Multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was used to compare groups’ neurocognitive performance and DTI measures. NART verbal scores served as the covariate to statistically account for any variance that could be attributed to differences in verbal IQ. Tables 1 and 2 contain univariate F tests and effect sizes, whereas the omnibus results are reported below. Associations between FA, MD, and time since injury were explored using Pearson partial correlations in which verbal IQ was partitioned out. since injury were explored using Pearson partial correlations in which verbal IQ was partitioned out.

Table 2.

Means, SDs, and statistical comparisons of FA and MD for adults with TBI (n = 8) and healthy controls (n = 8)

DTI measures TBI (mean ± SD) Control (mean ± SD) F
ηp2
FA (×10−3)
  CS 343.3 ± 18.6 382.2 ± 11.6 31.28**** .72
  SPF 327.6 ± 32.0 368.7 ± 27.9 9.86** .45
  INF 342.9 ± 36.8 370.1 ± 29.7 10.45** .47
MD (×10−6 mm2/s)
  CS 814.6 ± 45.9 748.2 ± 21.7 17.32*** .59
  SPF 883.3 ± 47.8 796.7 ± 25.6 15.07** .32
  INF 908.3 ± 64.5 850.5 ± 71.8 5.75* .56

Note. Group comparisons were analyzed with MANCOVAs in which estimates of verbal IQ were covaried. ηp2 effect size values are interpreted as follows: >.2 is large, >.1 is medium, and >.05 is small.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001.

****

p < .0001.

Neurocognitive Findings

Three neurocognitive domains were examined to describe the population samples: executive functions (alternating trails, inhibition, and verbal fluency), delayed recall, and speed (Table 1).

Executive functions

The MANCOVA revealed that adults with TBI were slower than controls at alternating trails (Wilks’ Lambda = 8.21, p = .004). Specifically, they were significantly slower when combining number and letter sequences. Adults with TBI tended to perform worse than controls on verbal fluency tasks revealed by a statistical trend (Wilks’ Lambda = 2.80, p = .09). Univariate tests revealed that they had fewer correct responses when switching categories than controls. Groups did not differ significantly on letter fluency, category fluency, or repetition errors. On the go/no-go task, a test of inhibition, adults with TBI made more errors (Wilks’ Lambda = 4.44, p = .04). Thus, as groups, adults with TBI were within average, whereas controls were just above average, resulting in significant differences overall in two of the three executive domains and in several specific tasks.

Delayed recall

Overall, adults with TBI recalled significantly less than controls under delayed conditions (Wilks’ Lambda = 4.18, p = .04). As expected, adults with TBI recalled less than controls without cues, whereas recall by adults with TBI improved when provided with cues.

Speed: Simple trails

Adults with TBI were significantly slower than controls overall (Wilks’ Lambda = 9.39, p = .002). Specifically, they were slower when sequencing letters and tracing lines.

DTI Findings

FA and MD differed between groups in the MANCOVA omnibus comparisons when verbal IQ was controlled for (FA: Wilks’ Lambda = 8.83, p = .004; MD: Wilks’ Lambda = 8.93, p = .004).a FA was significantly lower and MD was significantly higher in adults with TBI compared to controls in all three ROIs in the univariate comparisons at varying levels of probability ranging from p < .05 to p < .0001 (Table 2). Thus, even when accounting for any differences in estimates of verbal IQ, differences in WM in adults with TBI were significantly apparent in the expected direction.

Association Findings for Adults With TBI

Negative associations between FA and MD across ROIs are reported here to validate the integrity of these measures. Pearson partial correlation coefficients in which verbal IQ was controlled revealed that CS FA was negatively correlated with CS MD (r = −.94, p = .002), SPF FA was negatively correlated with SPF MD (r = −.88, p = .02), and INF FA was negatively correlated with INF MD (r = −.89, p = .02). There were no significant correlations of FA or MD between ROIs.

Using partial correlations, time since the injury was strongly and negatively associated with SPF FA (r = −.96, p = .002) and positively associated with SPF MD (r = .83, p = .04) but was not significantly associated with FA or MD of the CS or INF. Thus, those who had lived with chronic TBI longer had lower FA and higher MD in the SPF ROI.

DISCUSSION

This study sought to identify changes in cerebral WM in adults with severe TBI in the chronic recovery stage. While DTI may be a biomarker for DAI in acute recovery (Arfanakis et al., 2002; Huisman et al., 2004; Newcombe et al., 2007; Wilde et al., 2008), few others have identified WM changes years after injury (e.g., Kraus et al., 2007). Our results provide additional evidence that DTI is a useful technique for describing WM integrity, and compared to controls, FA and MD differ as hypothesized when statistically controlling for any difference due to verbal IQ (Kraus et al., 2007; Xu et al., 2007). That is, adults who averaged 7 years after the injury demonstrated decreased FA and increased MD in the three ROIs.

WM changes observed in this chronic sample likely represent multiple injury processes over time including direct, primary mechanisms (e.g., DAI/shearing, hemorrhages, contusions/contra-coup injury); secondary mechanisms (e.g., swelling); and tissue atrophy from Wallerian degeneration. Salmond et al. (2006) showed changes in FA and MD of adults beyond 6 months postinjury in 10 ROIs, and Kraus et al. (2007) found similar changes in adults who averaged 10 years postinjury. The current study showed robust group differences in FA and MD within the CS, similar to findings from other studies. In our study, the CS region was dorsal to corpus callosum (e.g., motor and sensory tracts, frontobasal ganglia tracts, arcuate fasciculus, longitudinal fasciculus) as was the superior parietal/SPF regions in the study of Salmond et al. (2006). In the study of Kraus et al. (2007), the CS included the corona radiata, superior longitudinal fasciculus, and corticospinal tracts. Similarly, group differences were also found in FA and MD in the SPF and INF regions, although our frontal ROIs were not comparable to the frontal regions of Salmond et al. (2006) or Kraus et al. (2007).

Associations of FA and MD within ROIs were included to verify the integrity of our DTI measures. FA and MD should be negatively associated, and indeed, our results support this. However, unlike the findings of other studies (e.g., Kraus et al., 2007), no associations were found between ROIs. Possible reasons for this are the small sample and the limited range of severity. Our study was limited to those with severe injury, whereas Kraus et al. (2007) included those with mild, moderate, and severe injuries.

A noteworthy finding in the current study was the relationship between time since injury and WM integrity in the SPF ROI. Given that typical aging is associated with changes in WM in frontal lobes and CS, one might suspect that this was due to aging (e.g., Pfefferbaum et al., 2000). Post hoc Pearson partial correlation that accounted for verbal IQ revealed that age was not associated with time since injury (r = .03, p = .95), the SPF FA or MD (r = −.15, p = .77; r = −.12, p = .82), or the CS FA or MD (r = .48, p = .34; r = −.35, p = .50). Age tended to be negatively associated with the INF FA (r = −.77, p = .07) and was positively associated with the INF MD (r = .90, p = .01). Thus, associations between FA and MD in the SPF ROI and time since injury were not due to aging, though aging did have its effect on the INF region. Relationships between time since injury and WM integrity in the SPF region could represent compounding effects of multiple injury processes over time that interact with age. We do not know whether WM in the SPF region is particularly susceptible to injury-related changes over time, yet this finding certainly warrants further consideration given the propensity of long-lasting executive dysfunction in the population with TBI.

The performance of those with TBI was significantly lower than the performance of healthy adults on several neurocognitive measures even when controlling for verbal IQ; however, their average performance was within the normal range and would not necessarily mean that they were impaired. Indeed, these adults with TBI were all employed, either full time or part time, and living in the community. Thus, in the presence of significant reductions in WM integrity, those who sustain severe injuries may still perform quite well over time. We could find only one published case study that found similar results (Skoglund et al., 2008). Additional studies with larger sample sizes of severely injured adults who display more severe neurocognitive impairments are needed to clarify whether WM changes are predictive of eventual neurocognitive and long-term functional outcomes. Future studies should include larger sample sizes that allow one to model relationships between demographics, FA and MD, and neurocognitive performance reliably. Until then, we cannot be certain that WM integrity and eventual, long-term neurocognitive performance are related.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors thank the Center for Magnetic Resonance Research at the University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, for the use of time, space, equipment, and personnel to operate the 3Tesla magnet during this study. Acknowledgments are also extended to Pradeep Ramanathan and Casey Tuck for their assistance in scanning participants. Importantly, we want to thank the Brain Injury Association—Minnesota (www.braininjurymn.org) for assisting us in recruiting volunteers for the study. Edward Carney assisted us with statistics and for this we are grateful. A special “thank you” is extended to all of those with TBI, who so willingly gave us their time and energy to participate in this study.

Footnotes

a

A TBI participant’s MD in two ROIs, INF and SPF, were more than 2 and 3 SDs below the control groups’ means, respectively. These two data points were removed prior to analysis. Regardless, group differences did not change when analyzed with these outlying data points.

REFERENCES

  1. Adams JH, Graham DI, Murray LS, Scott G. Diffuse axonal injury due to non-missile head injury in humans: An analysis of 45 cases. Annals of Neurology. 1982;12:557–563. doi: 10.1002/ana.410120610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arfanakis K, Haughton JD, Carew JD, Rogers BP, Dempsey RJ, Meyerand ME. Diffusion tensor MR imaging in diffuse axonal injury. American Journal of Neuroradiology. 2002;23:794–802. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Basser PJ. Inferring microstructural features and the physiological state of tissues from diffusion-weighted images. NMR in Biomedicine. 1995;8(7–8):333–344. doi: 10.1002/nbm.1940080707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Benson RR, Meda SA, Vasudevan S, Kou Z, Koushik GA, Hanks RA, Millis SR, Makke M, Latif Z, Coplin W, Maythaler J, Haacke EM. Global white matter analysis of diffusion tensor images is predictive of injury severity in traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurotrauma. 2007;24:466–459. doi: 10.1089/neu.2006.0153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bigler ED. Neurobiology and neuropathology underlie the neuropsychological deficits associated with traumatic brain injury. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2003;18(6):595–621. discussion 623–597. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Braver TS, Barch DM, Gray JR, Molfese DL, Snyder A. Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: Effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. Cerebral Cortex. 2001;11(9):825–836. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.9.825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  8. Delis DC, Kaplan E, Kramer JH. Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  9. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. California Verbal Learning Test. 2nd Ed. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  10. Dockree PM, Kelly SP, Roche RA, Hogan MJ, Reilly RB, Robertson IH. Behavioural and physiological impairments of sustained attention after traumatic brain injury. Brain Research/Cognition Brain Research. 2004;20:403–414. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.03.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Gale SD, Baxter L, Roundy N, Johnson SC. Traumatic brain injury and gray matter concentration: A preliminary voxel based morphometry study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry. 2005;76(7):984–988. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2004.036210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Huisman TAGM, Schwamm LH, Schaefer PW, Koroshetz WJ, Shetty-Alva N, Ozsunar Y, Wu O, Sorensen AG. Diffusion tensor imaging as potential biomarker of white matter injury in diffuse axonal injury. American Journal of Neuroradiology. 2004;25:370–376. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Johnson SC, Bigler ED, Burr RB, Blatter DD. White matter atrophy, ventricular dilation, and intellectual functioning following traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology. 1994;8:307–315. [Google Scholar]
  14. Kertesz A. Western Aphasia Battery. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  15. Kraus MF, Susmaras T, Caughlin BP, Walker CJ, Sweeney JA, Little DM. White matter integrity and cognition in chronic traumatic brain injury: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Brain. 2007;130:2508–2519. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Kurca E, Sivak S, Kucera P. Impaired cognitive functions in mild traumatic brain injury patients with normal and pathologic magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroradiology. 2006;48(9):661–669. doi: 10.1007/s00234-006-0109-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Le Bihan D, editor. Diffusion and Perfusion Magnetic Resonance Imaging. New York: Raven Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  18. Mathias JL, Bigler ED, Jones NR, Bowden SC, BarrettWoodbridge M, Brown GC, Taylor DJ. Neuropsychological and information processing performance and its relationship to white matter changes following moderate and severe traumatic brain injury: A preliminary study. Applied Neuropsychology. 2004;11:134–152. doi: 10.1207/s15324826an1103_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Millis SR, Rosenthal M, Novack TA, Sherer M, Nick TG, Kreutzer JS, High WM, Ricker JH. Long-term neuropsychological outcome after traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation. 2001;16(4):343–355. doi: 10.1097/00001199-200108000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Nelson HE, Willison J. National Adult Reading Test. 2nd ed. Windsor, UK: MFER-NELSON; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  21. Newcombe VFJ, Williams GB, Nortje J, Bradley PG, Harding SG, Smielewski P, Coles JP, Malya B, Gillard JH, Hutchinson PJ, Pickard JD, Carpenter TA, Menon DK. Analysis of acute traumatic axonal injury using diffusion tensor imaging. British Journal of Neurosurgery. 2007;21:340–348. doi: 10.1080/02688690701400882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Pfefferbaum A, Sullivan EV, Hedehus M, Lim KO, Adalsteinsson E, Moseley M. Age-related decline in brain white matter anisotropy measured with spatially corrected echoplanar diffusion tensor imaging. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2000;44:259–268. doi: 10.1002/1522-2594(200008)44:2<259::aid-mrm13>3.0.co;2-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Pierpaoli C, Basser PJ. Toward a quantitative assessment diffusion anisotropy. Magnetic Resonance Medicine. 1996;36:893–906. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910360612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Riley GA, Simmonds LV. How robust is performance on the National Adult Reading Test following traumatic brain injury? British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2003;42(Pt 3):319–328. doi: 10.1348/01446650360703410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Rohling ML, Meyers JE, Millis SR. Neuropsychological impairment following traumatic brain injury: A dose-response analysis. Clinical Neuropsychology. 2003;17(3):289–302. doi: 10.1076/clin.17.3.289.18086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Rugg-Gunn FJ, Symms MR, Barker GJ, Greenwood R, Duncan JS. Diffusion imaging shows abnormalities after blunt head trauma when conventional magnetic resonance imaging is normal. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 2001;70:530–533. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.70.4.530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Salmond CH, Menon DK, Chatfield DA, Williams GB, Pena BJ, Sahakian BJ, Pickard JD. Diffusion tensor imaging in chronic head injury survivors: Correlations with learning and memory indices. Neuroimage. 2006;29:117–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Schretlen DJ, Shapiro AM. A quantitative review of the effects of traumatic brain injury on cognitive functioning. International Review of Psychiatry. 2003;15(4):341–349. doi: 10.1080/09540260310001606728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Serino A, Ciaramelli E, Di Santantonio A, Malagu S, Servadei F, Ladavas E. Central executive system impairment in traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury. 2006;20(1):23–32. doi: 10.1080/02699050500309627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Skoglund TS, Nilsson D, Ljunberg M, Jonsson L, Rydenhag B. Long-term follow-up of a patient with traumatic brain injury using diffusion tensor imaging. Acta Radiologica. 2008;49:98–100. doi: 10.1080/02841850701561372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Strich SJ. Shearing of nerve fibres as a cause of brain damage due to head injury: A pathological study of twenty cases. Lancet. 1961;2:443–448. [Google Scholar]
  32. Teasdale G, Jennett B. Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness: A practical scale. Lancet. 1974;2:81–84. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(74)91639-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Wilde EA, McCauley SR, Hunter JV, Bigler ED, Chu Z, Wang ZJ, Hanten GR, Troyanskaya M, Yallampalli R, Li X, Chia J, Levin HS. Diffusion tensor imaging of acute mild traumatic brain injury in adolescents. Neurology. 2008;70:948–955. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000305961.68029.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Wozniak JR, Krach L, Ward E, Mueller B, Muetzel R, Schnoebelen S, Kriel R, Kiragu A, Lim KO. Neurocognitive and neuroimaging correlates of pediatric traumatic brain injury: A diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) study. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2007;22:555–568. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Xu J, Rasmussen I-A, Lagopoulos J, Haberg A. Diffuse axonal injury in severe traumatic brain injury visualized using high-resolution diffusion tensor imaging. Journal of Neuro-trauma. 2007;24:753–765. doi: 10.1089/neu.2006.0208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Yuan W, Holland SK, Schmithorst VJ, Walz NC, Cecil KM, Jones BV, Karunanayaka P, Michaud L, Wade SL. Diffusion tensor MR imaging reveals persistent white matter alteration after traumatic brain injury experienced during early childhood. American Journal of Neuroradiology. 2007;28:1919–1925. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A0698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES