Abstract
OBJECTIVES
To determine whether the offspring of centenarians have personality characteristics that are distinct from the general population.
DESIGN
Case-control.
SETTING
Nationwide U.S. sample.
PARTICIPANTS
Unrelated offspring of centenarians (n = 246, mean age 75) were compared with published norms.
MEASUREMENTS
Using the NEO-Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) questionnaire, measures of the personality traits neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness were obtained. T-scores and percentiles were calculated according to sex and used to interpret the results.
RESULTS
Male and female offspring of centenarians scored in the low range of published norms for neuroticism and in the high range for extraversion. The women also scored comparatively high in agreeableness. Otherwise, both sexes scored within normal range for conscientiousness and openness, and the men scored within normal range for agreeableness.
CONCLUSION
Specific personality traits may be important to the relative successful aging demonstrated by the offspring of centenarians. Similarities across four of the five domains between male and female offspring is noteworthy and may relate to their successful aging. Measures of personality are an important phenotype to include in studies that assess genetic and environmental influences of longevity and successful aging.
Keywords: personality, longevity, centenarian, extraversion, neuroticism, agreeableness
Research on siblings and offspring of centenarians has documented that exceptional longevity runs strongly in families.1–4 Studies of the offspring of centenarians have shown that their mortality is lower that that of other members of their birth cohort5 and that they have lower prevalence1,6 and delayed onset of cardiovascular disease, hypertension, and diabetes mellitus.7 Thus, they are regarded as a model of healthy aging. Because personality traits have been shown to have substantial heritable components, 8 it was hypothesized that certain personality features may be important to the healthy aging observed in the offspring of centenarians.
Several studies have assessed the effect of personality traits on survival using the Five-Factor Model (FFM) of Personality. Studies have used the Revised NEO Personality Inventory, a 240-item measure of the five broad factors and the 30 narrower facet scales or the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI), a 60-item self-report questionnaire that measures only the five personality factors: neuroticism (emotional reactivity and distress), extraversion (tendency to be outgoing, active), openness (flexibility and openness to new ideas), agreeableness (cooperation and empathy), and conscientiousness (social responsibility and self-discipline).9 Findings from longitudinal studies have, to some extent, been consistent, indicating that high conscientiousness, low neuroticism, and perhaps high extraversion are predictive of lower mortality and longevity in aging populations.10–13 Prior research suggests that offspring of centenarians are a model of healthy aging and that certain personality traits are associated with mortality and longevity. Thus, this study aimed to test the hypothesis that above-average levels of health-promoting traits (e.g., conscientiousness and extraversion) and below-average levels of health-damaging traits (e.g., neuroticism) characterize offspring of centenarians.
Using the NEO-FFI from the FFM, this study set out to measure the personality characteristics of offspring of centenarians and to compare NEO-FFI scores of centenarians’ offspring with normative scores.
METHODS
Study Participants
Participants were biological offspring of centenarians enrolled in the New England Centenarian Study, an ongoing nationwide study of centenarians and their families. From 560 sibships, the oldest living sibling was chosen, yielding 352 female and 208 male centenarian offspring. A power analysis revealed that a total sample of 200 subjects was needed to detect a difference of 2.5 points or more in mean scores between the centenarian offspring and reported normative mean scores with an α = 0.05 and β = 0.80. Thus, anticipating some losses to follow-up or refusals to participate, a random sample of 308 unrelated subjects was selected (168 women and 140 men, reflecting the expected proportions of women and men for an average age of 75). Of the women, 24 were unavailable, 14 had died, and five refused, resulting in an analytical sample of 125 women. Of the males, 15 had died, and four others could not be reached, resulting in an analytical sample of 121 men.
Measures
Participants completed Form S (self-report version) of the NEO-FFI,9 a 60-item version of the NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R), which operationalizes the FFM. Means and standard deviations for each of the five NEO-FFI personality factors were calculated separately in the men and women. Raw scores were converted into sex-specific standardized T-scores with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10. T-scores were calculated using within-sex published NEO-FFI means and standard deviations that represent population norms published in Table B-4 on page 78 of the NEO PI-R manual.9 The following guide was used to interpret the T-scores; T-scores between 45 and 54 are considered average, scores of 44 or less are low, and scores of 55 or greater are high. The raw scores were also converted into percentiles using the scales provided in Table C-7 on page 85 of the manual.9
RESULTS
Age, ethnicity, and education level according to sex are displayed in Table 1. The education level of the present sample is comparable with the education level of the NEO-FFI norm group. Table 2 contains NEO-FFI mean raw domain scores, T-scores, and percentiles comparing the offspring with a sex-matched referent cohort aged 21 to 96. Within the offspring of centenarian group, men and women did not differ significantly in their mean raw scores for the five factors, except for agreeableness, which was higher for the women (Student t-test = 4.5, P < .001). According to T-scores, male offspring of centenarians were low in neuroticism; high in extraversion; and average in openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness. Percentile scores showed that men scored in the bottom third percentile for neuroticism and the top third percentile for extraversion. Female offspring, per T-scores, were also low in neuroticism and high in extraversion but also high in agreeableness. Otherwise, they were average in openness and conscientiousness. Women scored in the bottom third percentile for neuroticism and the top third percentiles for extraversion and agreeableness.
Table 1.
Characteristic | Men n = 121 | Women n = 125 |
---|---|---|
Age, mean ± SD | 75.6 ± 6.5 | 74.9 ± 7.6 |
Caucasian, % | 99 | 97 |
Education, years, mean ± SD | 16.7 ± 3.4 | 15.2 ± 2.3 |
SD = standard deviation.
Table 2.
Domain | Raw Score Mean ± Standard Deviation |
T- Score |
Percen- tile |
Inter- pretation |
---|---|---|---|---|
Men | ||||
Neuroticism | 13.1 ± 5.9 | 44.0 | 32 | Low |
Extraversion | 30.8 ± 6.5 | 56.1 | 75 | High |
Openness | 27.5 ± 6.0 | 50.7 | 58 | Average |
Agreeableness | 34.4 ± 5.3 | 54.9 | 70 | Average |
Conscientiousness | 34.7 ± 6.0 | 51.0 | 58 | Average |
Women | ||||
Neuroticism | 14.4 ± 6.8 | 41.9 | 22 | Low |
Extraversion | 31.3 ± 6.0 | 55.4 | 70 | High |
Openness | 27.5 ± 6.2 | 50.9 | 59 | Average |
Agreeableness | 37.3 ± 4.7 | 57.5 | 81 | High |
Conscientiousness | 36.0 ± 6.0 | 51.7 | 59 | Average |
DISCUSSION
In this study of personality traits, the offspring of centenarians appear to have distinctive characteristics that may have important implications for their longevity. The data presented here suggest that the offspring of centenarians demonstrate greater extraversion and lower neuroticism and that the female subjects also reveal greater agreeableness. The high levels of extraversion are all the more impressive given that the extraversion scores were higher than in the full adult sample, which had a lower mean age (~ 50 vs 73). Lower extraversion, not higher, would normally be expected with older age.14 Generally speaking, women usually score higher than men in neuroticism, particularly in Western and European cultures,15 yet among the offspring of centenarians, the male and female mean T-scores for neuroticism were similar.
It is possible that the low neuroticism and higher extraversion will confer health benefits to these subjects. For example, people who are lower in neuroticism may be able to manage or regulate stressful situations more effectively than those with higher neuroticism levels. Similarly, high extraversion levels have been associated with greater subjective well-being, vitality, and longevity. A limitation of the current study is that it does not include facet-level data. Future research might employ the 30 facets of the NEO-PI-R, which could help identify which lower-order traits are driving the low neuroticism and high extraversion. Also, larger samples and prospective follow-up of offspring could facilitate the comparison for age-related diseases and mortality of offspring and other referent cohorts with and without personality traits (and facets) associated thus far with longevity. The findings of the current study suggest that personality is an important phenotype to include in studies that assess genetic and environmental determinants of longevity. Such studies will be possible soon with the availability of fruitful genomics technologies, ongoing recruitment of ever-growing samples of long-lived families (https://longlifefamilystudy.wustl.edu/), and collaborating centenarian studies that also enroll offspring.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We are grateful to the children of centenarians who participated in this study. This work was supported in part by grants from the National Institute on Aging (NIA): K-24, AG025727 (TP), K23 AG026754 (Paul Beeson Physician Faculty Scholar in Aging Award, DT), and the Intramural Research Program of the NIA.
Sponsor’s Role: The sponsor, NIA, had no role in any of the above activities.
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest: The editor in chief has reviewed the conflict of interest checklist provided by the authors and has determined that the authors have no financial or any other kind of personal conflicts with this manuscript. Paul Costa receives royalties from the NEO-FFI.
Author Contributions: Jane Givens and Dellara Terry: study concept and design, analysis, and preparation of manuscript. Maureen Frederick and Margery Silver: acquisition of data, preparation of manuscript. Leanne Silverman: analysis and preparation of manuscript. Stacy Anderson, Paola Sebastiani, and Joanna Senville: acquisition of data. Thomas Perls: study concept and design and preparation of manuscript. Paul Costa received the data from Tom Perls and compared them against normative data provided in the Psychological Assessment Resources Inc., manual. He also played a substantial role in the writing of the manuscript.
REFERENCES
- 1.Atzmon G, Schechter C, Greiner W, et al. Clinical phenotype of families with longevity. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2004;52:274–277. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52068.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Perls T, Shea-Drinkwater M, Bowen-Flynn J, et al. Exceptional familial clustering for extreme longevity in humans. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2000;48:1483–1485. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Perls TT, Wilmoth J, Levenson R, et al. Life-long sustained mortality advantage of siblings of centenarians. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2002;99:8442–8447. doi: 10.1073/pnas.122587599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Terry DF, Evans JC, Pencina MJ, et al. Characteristics of Framingham offspring participants with long-lived parents. Arch Intern Med. 2007;167:438–444. doi: 10.1001/archinte.167.5.438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Terry DF, Wilcox MA, McCormick MA, et al. Lower all-cause, cardiovascular, and cancer mortality in centenarians’ offspring. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2004;52:2074–2076. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52561.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Terry DF, Wilcox M, McCormick MA, et al. Cardiovascular advantages among the offspring of centenarians. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2003;58A:M425–M431. doi: 10.1093/gerona/58.5.m425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Terry D, Wilcox M, McCormick M, et al. Cardiovascular disease delay in the offspring of centenarians. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2004;59A:M385–M389. doi: 10.1093/gerona/59.4.m385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Jang KL, Livesley WJ, Vernon PA. Heritability of the big five personality dimensions and their facets: A twin study. J Pers. 1996;64:577–591. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1996.tb00522.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Costa P, McCrae R. Revised NEO Personality Inventory and NEO Five-Factor Inventory: Professional Manual, PAR. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Terracciano A, Lockenhoff CE, Zonderman AB, et al. Personality predictors of longevity: Activity, emotional stability, and conscientiousness. Psychosom Med. 2008;70:621–627. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31817b9371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Weiss A, Costa PT., Jr Domain and facet personality predictors of all-cause mortality among Medicare patients aged 65 to 100. Psychosom Med. 2005;67:724–733. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000181272.58103.18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Wilson RS, Krueger KR, Gu L, et al. Neuroticism, extraversion, and mortality in a defined population of older persons. Psychosom Med. 2005;67:841–845. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000190615.20656.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Kern ML, Friedman HS. Do conscientious individuals live longer? A quantitative review. Health Psychol. 2008;27:505–512. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.5.505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Terraciano A, McCrae RR, Brant LJ, et al. Hierarchical linear modeling analyses of NEO-PI-R scales in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Psychol Aging. 2005;20:493–506. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.3.493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Costa PTTA, McCrae RR. Gender differences in personality traits across cultures: Robust and surprising findings. J Personality Soc Pyschol. 2001;81:322–331. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]