Abstract
Compared to other specialties, the field of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine (PRM) has not received the deserved recognition from clinicians and researchers in the scientific community. One of the reasons is the lack of sound evidence to support the traditional PRM treatments. The best way to change this disadvantage is through well-conducted clinical research, such as the standard placebo or sham-controlled randomized clinical trials. Therefore, having placebo groups in clinical trials is essential to improve the level of evidence-based practice in PRM that ultimately translates in a better clinical care. To address the challenges for the use of placebo in PRM randomized clinical trials, and to create useful recommendations, we convened a working group during the inaugural International Symposium in Placebo (February 2009, in Sao Paulo, Brazil) in which the following topics were discussed: (1) current status of randomized clinical trials in PRM, (2) challenges for the use of placebo in PRM, (3) bioethical issues, (4) use of placebo in acupuncture trials and for the treatment of low-back pain, (5) mechanisms of placebo, and (6) insights from other specialties. The current article represents the consensus report from the working group.
Keywords: Placebo, Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine, Randomized Clinical Trials, Methodology, Clinical Trials Design, Ethics, Acupuncture, Low-Back Pain
Although Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine (PRM) is an established medical specialty, it has not received the deserved recognition from clinicians and researchers in the scientific community. There are several reasons that might explain this unfavorable scenario; but one that seems to stand out is the lack of high quality clinical research in PRM due to the inherent difficulty in the specialty to conduct double-blinded placebo-controlled clinical trials. Most of the studies in PRM are either based on clinical observation (naturalistic studies), uncontrolled observational studies, or clinical trials comparing two active interventions. However these study designs do not provide sufficient proof of efficacy of interventions, which is only achieved in high quality double-blinded placebo-controlled trials.
The best way to improve the recognition of PRM is by improving clinical research and facilitating the conduct of high quality clinical trials such as the placebo or sham-controlled randomized clinical trials. Therefore the understanding of placebo use in clinical research and the development of reliable placebos or sham interventions are necessary steps to improve the level of evidence in PRM. The ultimate goal is to improve patient care in PRM.
To understand the challenges for placebo use in randomized clinical trials in PRM, we convened a working group of established clinical researchers in PRM and experts in placebo and clinical research methodology on Feb 12–14, 2009 to discuss the main challenges of placebo use in PRM and propose potential solutions and recommendations. A summary of the discussion is provided below.
METHODS
The education committee of the international Society of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine (ISPRM) and the Brazilian Association of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation (ABMFR) were the sponsors of the inaugural International Symposium in Placebo held in Sao Paulo, Brazil, February 12 14, 2009. The local host was Dr Paulo Boggio from Mackenzie University, and the scientific program was organized by Dr Felipe Fregni, Dr. Marta Imamura and Dr. Ted Kaptchuk. The purpose of the symposium was to highlight the main aspects of placebo in clinical research with a special emphasis to PRM. There were a total of 14 presentations given by some members of this working group. In addition 18 abstracts were submitted and presented at this symposium. Two hundred and twenty six participants attended to this event.
The scientific program was structured in two sessions: (i) Basics mechanisms and discussion of placebo effect; (ii) Placebo effect in different specialties. In addition, there was a special panel composed by three PRM researchers (MI, AF, MR) to discuss the challenges of placebo in PRM.
The current article represents the consensus report from the working group of PRM clinical researchers in addition to experts in placebo and clinical research methodology. Participants from the working group were asked to previously submit the material to the working group coordinator (FF). During the consensus meeting each participant presented their designated topics with input and discussion by all working group participants. The topics covered different aspects of placebo in PRM including two sections on a case discussion in acupuncture and low back pain as to give the readers practical examples. The meeting lasted 3 hours and was video-recorded. After the meeting, the reports from each participant were compiled by the coordinator using the same format and sections as it was used during our working meeting. The final report was reviewed by each member via e-mail interactions.
Current State of Placebo-Controlled Clinical Trials in PRM
In the recent years, the interest in controlled studies in PRM has increased. Conducting randomized controlled trials has become the goal of clinical researchers involved in PRM, especially when dealing with non-pharmacological treatments. In fact, the growing interest in controlled trials can be observed in MEDLINE. Figure 1 shows the increase in the number of placebo and sham-controlled studies published since 1964 in rehabilitation, physical medicine, and physical therapy journals. In the last decade (1999–2008) there were 8680 placebo or sham controlled trials in PRM, which indicated an increase of 3743 from the prior decade (1989–1998). The search strategy is shown in appendix 1.
Figure 1.
Number of articles when using the following search strategy in Pubmed:- Search with the following limits: (1) Period (e.g.: 2000–2009); (2) We searched in the 5 main clinical journals in PRM: (“Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation”, “American journal of physical medicine & rehabilitation/Association of Academic Physiatrists”, “Physical therapy”, “Restorative neurology and neuroscience” and “Journal of rehabilitation medicine: official journal of the UEMS European Board of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine”). This search yielded the total number of articles in each respective period in these 5 journals. We then added the word “placebo” for the total number of articles including placebo as a keyword.
There is also an increasing interest in trials evaluating therapeutic effects of alternative medicine and physical modalities such as acupuncture, electrical stimulation and application of heat. These topics have had a large number of publications in the field of PRM recently.
The increase in the number of placebo-controlled studies in PRM in the last decade is a result of several factors:
The evidence-based medicine movement that started in the early 90s, in which controlled clinical trials are used as best evidence to support novel interventions as to improve the level of quality care
An increased interest in rehabilitation modalities, with emphasis on conditions usually treated by PRM physicians, such as chronic diseases encountered in the aging population (e.g., arthritis and chronic pain).
Reimbursement decisions are usually dependent on high-level of evidence.
Although there is a clear increase in the number of placebo-controlled studies in PRM, there is still a number of important challenges that are responsible for the delay in the development of high-quality evidence in this specialty. These challenges were discussed in our group meeting and will be presented in this article.
The Need for Sham or Placebo-Controlled Trials in PRM
In PRM, the most commonly used control group includes interventions as simple as non-participation (waiting list), usual care or another intervention. These control interventions are associated with non-specific physiological effects that can affect the active intervention, especially when patients and care providers are not blinded to the interventions. Patient’s expectation may also significantly interfere with treatment results1 and nocebo effects may also be observed2.
Placebo controlled trials ensure blinding and reduce the risk of potential bias that can also interfere with the observed outcomes. The type of biases that can be minimized by using adequate placebos are3:
Performance bias defined as systematic differences between groups in the care that is provided, or due to exposure to factors other than the interventions of interest.
Detection bias defined as systematic differences between groups in how outcomes are determined. This may pose a special challenge since most PRM interventions, by their nature, are associated with the building of relationships between the patient, rehabilitation team members, family members, and care providers.
Although there is a critical need for randomized, placebo-controlled clinical trials in PRM, in some situations, the use of placebo or sham device might not be practical. For instance, there are situations in which it is not possible to use of placebo such as in a trial testing the effectiveness of lower limb prosthesis for ambulation after amputation, or use of sham-orthosis for drop foot; or situations in which is difficult to sort out the placebo from the real effect and thus it is problematic to design an adequate placebo intervention such as for hydrotherapy. In the next section, we discuss specific challenges for the use of placebo in PRM clinical trials.
Challenges of Placebo Use in PRM Clinical Trials
We identified nine important challenges for the use of placebo in PRM clinical trials:
-
Development of placebo and sham devices:
Placebos are for drugs as shams are for devices. PRM is a specialty in which most of the treatments are based on devices. Two typical sham treatments in PRM are sham ultrasound4,5,6 and “off-TENS (transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation)”7–15.
The main issue with sham devices is the difficulty to blind subjects as effectively because the use of a device is usually associated with a sensation; such as during TENS stimulation. Therefore, subjects with prior experience with that device might be easily unblinded. Even in naive subjects, active stimulation might induce a larger placebo effect due to the sensations associated with the active treatment.
Interestingly, despite widely used in physical and rehabilitation medicine, the efficacy of therapeutic ultrasound and TENS remains controversial4–15 and current evidence does not support their use in routine management due to limited, conflicting and inconclusive data. On the other hand, there are examples of PRM interventions with proven efficacy over sham devices: radial shockwave therapy16 and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS)17–20. One important difference is that for the latter two interventions, the sham method is a reliable method21.
There is a need for further engineering development for reliable methods of sham device. An ideal sham device should have the same appearance and induce similar feelings as the real device. However, here one important point that needs to be considered is whether the feelings associated with a given treatment are also correlates of the active ingredient; for instance, a perfect placebo for acupuncture would induce the qi sensation; however it is unclear whether inducing the qi sensation regardless of the stimulation point is associated with the physiological effects of acupuncture. Another example here would be designing a perfect placebo for tricyclic antidepressant in this case, the placebo would ideally also be associated with sleepiness; but then the same question applies: is some of the effects of tricyclic antidepressants associated with sleepiness?.
Finally, a reliable sham device should also allow reproducibility of the results in various patient populations. Finally, because devices have different regulatory requirements (often less restrict than drugs), development of effective sham methods are not sufficiently supported.
-
Lack of standards in PRM therapies
Several PRM interventions are not standardized or are delivered differently according to each patient or the person administering it. For example, exercises. There are different types of exercise such as stretching, strengthening, endurance, which can be applied separately or in conjunction. Because many PRM therapies are difficult to be standardized, it is even more difficult to design a “standard” placebo therapy in these situations.
-
Treatment heterogeneity due to therapist skill differences
A great number of interventions used in PRM depend on the technician or physician’s skills such as the application of acupuncture, injections, and nerve blocks. Therefore controlling for these interventions becomes difficult with this important source of variability. This makes it even more difficult to design an appropriate placebo in these situations. In order to control for skills and levels of experience it would be necessary to conduct multicentric studies with various levels of skills and experiences and perform multivariate analyses to adjust for these variables. In this scenario, large number of patients would be necessary; increasing the difficulties to conduct such studies.
-
Issues with adequate masking
For a placebo/sham controlled trial to be valid, it has to ensure adequate blinding: patients, therapists, outcome assessors, and data analysts must all be adequately blinded22. However many interventions in PRM are impossible to blind. For example: occupational therapy for stroke. In fact, when compared to pharmacological studies, trials involving non-pharmacological interventions report less blinding of care providers, patients and outcome assessors23. If blinding is impossible, it does not make sense to design a placebo-controlled trial; however alternative methods to analyze bias should be then carried out.
Another important issue associated with adequate masking is subjective outcomes. Several conditions in PRM have subjective outcomes such as chronic pain. Usually subjective outcomes have a higher magnitude of placebo effect24, causing the difference between active and sham group to be smaller.
In chronic pain studies, for example, the most commonly used primary outcome is the subjective visual analogue scale16,17,19,25. In fact, inadequate sham devices (that do not induce the same sensation as the active treatment) might unblind patients; therefore modifying their pain ratings on this subjective scale.
-
Personal interaction between therapist and patient
Several PRM interventions involve a close relationship between the patient and the therapist, For example: speech and language therapy after brain injury. Because placebo effect is directly associated to the degree of such interaction, this factor complicates the design of clinical trials in PRM26 as it is not possible to isolate this contact completely.
There are other important issues of the relationship between patient and therapist for some interventions such as cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT). For instance the degree of interaction is important not only to determine the degree of placebo effect but also to induce the real effects. Therefore motivation is an important part of the active aspects of CBT as this therapy involves relearning strategies. In fact motivation and preference present an important challenge. For example, randomly assigning exercise or CBT in a randomized clinical trial does not account for the fact that in practice one would first determine whether a patient has sufficient motivation and thus makes practical sense to administer a therapy such as CBT– not doing this as for instance in RCT might assign CBT to patients with lack of motivation and therefore results be biased27,28. Therefore in certain circumstances the specific and unspecific aspects of the intervention cannot be detangled– therefore decreasing the chances of designing an effective placebo intervention.
-
Personal beliefs, previous experience and motivation
Patients have different expectation levels with non-pharmacological treatments in PRM. Because expectation plays a critical role for the effects of placebo, placebo effect might be elevated in some treatments in PRM and therefore difficult to control for, such as in occupational therapy, home exercises, and cognitive-behavioral interventions25.
-
Small effect sizes
Some of the PRM interventions induce small improvements (effect sizes), therefore studies to prove efficacy require large sample sizes, which implies increased costs.
-
Long follow-up
Because many PRM interventions treat chronic conditions, the studies involve long follow-up periods (increasing the costs and complexity of the study). This makes more difficult the use placebo due to ethical concerns of a long period of exposure to a placebo intervention.
-
Lack of training to conduct clinical research
Many PRM practitioners lack appropriate training in clinical research; therefore the importance and methods of placebo use are sometimes not appreciated.
-
Use of medical devices
PRM is a specialty that largely uses medical devices as compared with other specialties such as infectious diseases or pediatrics. This also has an important impact for placebo use as there is a notion that sham devices would have a higher placebo response than placebo pills30. In fact, a sham intervention showed increased response in a sham device vs. inert pill trial31 and in a meta-analysis comparing subcutaneous placebo with oral placebo from acute migraine32. A recent meta-analysis on major depression, however, did not find differences between placebo pill and use of a sham device (transcranial magnetic stimulation)33. Similar contradictory result was found in an acute care study that found no difference between parenteral medication and oral medication35. Therefore it is still unclear whether sham device induces a larger response. But given it does (or at least a different placebo response), then one challenge is that clinicians should be aware that the number needed to treat (NNT) in randomized clinical trials (that take into account placebo effect and especially when using sham devices) might be different than the NNT in the clinical practice.
Ethical Issues for the Use of Placebo in Randomized Clinical Trials
The use of placebo in randomized clinical trial (RCT) generates an intense debate and is considered an ethical dilemma. As in any ethical dilemma, benefits in one area can automatically imply shortcomings in another area. A central ethical tension is whether the researcher-clinician should be guided by the ethics of therapeutic medicine or the one underlying research. In this context, the clinical investigator has a different role as compared to a clinician. These two roles need to be differentiated.
In this scenario, an important debate has been around the concept of equipoise. The first principle is that a clinical trial begins with an honest null hypothesis–that there is no difference between the two treatments being tested. In fact, defenders of equipoise argue that if a researcher understands that the arms of a trial are not equivalent, then the inferior arm must be excluded or even the entire trial interrupted– it is conceived that all subjects must receive the superior treatment. In 1987, Benjamin Freedman35, in The New England Journal of Medicine, proposed the concept of equipoise arguing that “the requirement is satisfied if there is genuine uncertainty within the expert medical community– not necessarily on the part of the individual investigator– about the preferred treatment”. For instance, although patients and some physicians might believe that acupuncture is effective for fibromyalgia, there is not enough evidence supporting its use; therefore a trial comparing placebo vs. acupuncture for fibromyalgia is considered to have equipoise.
Another important issue is when it is ethically justified to use placebo in a clinical trial. The response might be included in the Declaration of Helsinki. This document states that:
“The physician may combine medical research with medical care only to the extent that the research is justified by its potential preventive, diagnostic or therapeutic value and if the physician has good reason to believe that participation in the research study will not adversely affect the health of the patients who serve as research subjects. … The benefits, risks, burdens and effectiveness of a new intervention must be tested against those of the best current proven intervention, except in the following circumstances: The use of placebo, or no treatment, is acceptable in studies where no current proven intervention exists; or where for compelling and scientifically sound methodological reasons the use of placebo is necessary to determine the efficacy or safety of an intervention and the patients who receive placebo or no treatment will not be subject to any risk of serious or irreversible harm. Extreme care must be taken to avoid abuse of this option.”
Therefore, it is important that there is a real need to determine the efficacy or safety of a new treatment if this active treatment in the given condition being investigated does not cause any serious or irreversible harm.
In fact, lack of placebo studies might also be associated with uneffective and harmful treatments. The surgical treatment of angina pectoris is a good example of this risk. In the past, internal mammary artery ligation by surgical procedures was considered an effective therapeutic approach relieving the related-symptoms. Data supporting this therapeutic procedure was based on uncontrolled trials. Nevertheless, when this method was compared with a sham procedure, no differential effects were observed between groups and this intervention was discontinued.
In fact, such debate is intense in another specialty: psychiatry. In 2000, Khan et al.36 published their analysis of the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) database on antidepressant clinical trials. The use of a placebo in an antidepressant clinical trial usually generates concerns specially associated with suicide risk. In this article, the authors showed, based on RCT of seven new antidepressant drugs, that there was no difference between placebo and experimental group with regard to rates of suicide and attempted of suicide. They also found a symptom reduction of 40.7% with investigational drugs and 30.9% with placebo. These data raised an intense debate. Leber37 considered that although these data could not be assumed as a final proof that placebo do not result in risk for the subjects; the spontaneous improvement related to placebo administration is an indicator for the necessity of placebo RCT.
Another important aspect of placebo-controlled trials is that the sample sizes are usually smaller than when an active intervention is used in the control group, therefore the number of subjects exposed to an experimental intervention is reduced. Leon et al.38 showed that a study comparing an investigational drug with placebo needs a smaller number of subjects resulting in smaller number of non-responders compared to the alternative of using an active control.
However, some researchers consider that statistical arguments should not be used as a way to justify ethical issues. Kraemer39 commented on the premise that patients do not have full comprehension on the type of medical support they will have when participating in a placebo-controlled study.
The debate becomes more intense when the placebo-controlled group is not an inert placebo but an active one such as surgical procedures or pills that mimic some of the side effects of the pharmacological therapy under evaluation. Edward et al.40 discussed this issue considering what they called “three ethical hurdles”. For these authors the “evaluation methods must be the best or only scientific option available to get valid data; acceptable to participants in terms of a utilitarian calculation of risks and benefits; and respectful of the needs of individuals and communities to control their own destinies”.
Another important issue is cultural aspects when designing placebo-controlled trials. For instance, a placebo-controlled trial of acupuncture might not be accepted in China because it might be an established treatment in Eastern countries, while it might not be accepted as an effective treatment is some Western countries. This increases the challenges for placebo use for global clinical trials especially in PRM where there is a lack of consensus regarding the use of medical devices for several conditions
Finally, although any research involving human subjects are abide to the use of informed consent form and that theoretically patients can decide to participate or not in a given trial based on the knowledge of risks involving participation; it should be noted that in several circumstances patients do not understand the concept of placebo and in addition a level of cohercion to participate in a study might be present, especially in PRM patients who often have a close relationship with their physician.
Case Study 1: Placebo in Acupuncture Clinical Trials
Because many PRM physicians are recommending acupuncture treatment, it is important to discuss the challenges and potential solutions for the use of placebo in acupuncture. In fact, results from several RCT showed that the clinical effect of acupuncture is inconclusive and one of the reasons is the challenge for the use of placebo in this condition. Here we list the main issues with placebo use in acupuncture trials:
Mechanisms of action: although the neurophysiological mechanisms of acupuncture are well established in research for experimental pain41,42, it is still unclear the exact mechanisms underlying the action of acupuncture in clinical practice; therefore making the development of effective and reliable sham methods difficult. In fact we still do not know the relative efficacy of specific acupuncture approaches. Is dry needling technique equivalent to needling on the real acupuncture point? Do we need to obtain the qi sensation (the sensation following the needling of the specific acupuncture point)? Should we select the point according to standardized traditional Chinese medicine (TCM) formula or should we allow selection of points according to the experiences of the practitioner? Does the insertion technique provoke different results? All these questions exemplify the need of mechanistic studies in order to develop an effective sham method.
Different sham methods: clinical trials in acupuncture use different methods of sham as there is still controversy on the best sham method. In addition some of the methods of sham might induce physiological effects. For instance, it is possible that sham controlled acupuncture trials compare two active forms of needling since most of the sham techniques often consist of misplaced needling (superficial or off point needling), which may have a physiological effects43–45. The search for an ideal and physiological inert acupuncture placebo has involved a variety of different techniques, such as the non-insertive stimulation (using the stage dagger effect) and controls which do not involve needling (mock transcutaneous nerve stimulation).
It appears that these different methods are associated with physiological effects. A recent study has shown that light touch of the skin by a placebo acupuncture needle (touches the skin without penetrating it) activates mechanoreceptors and slow conducting unmyelinated (C) afferents inducing activity in the insula; thus potentially modulating chronic pain46. Therefore it is possible that this method of placebo acupuncture has also physiological effects beyond those induced by placebo administration. Similarly, misplaced needling is also associated with significant physiological effects that are also difficult to detangle from the experience of receiving acupuncture.
The existence of several methods of sham results in an additional methodological issue as results from different acupuncture trials cannot be easily compared, and therefore conclusions on this technique cannot be easily made47,48.
Increased expectancy: patients who seek acupuncture treatment usually have high levels of expectancy. This fact might increase placebo response and therefore decrease the effect sizes in acupuncture trials.
Therapist’s skills and interaction: acupuncture as with other techniques involving devices depends on the acupuncturist skills and also on the interaction between patient and therapist and therefore making the control more difficult as the active and placebo acupuncture treatment are highly dependent on these variable factors.
Acupuncture is an example of a treatment frequently utilized in PRM that presents several challenges for the placebo use in RCTs that have critically contributed to the uncertainty regarding the evidence to support this therapeutic method.
Case Study 2: Placebo Effect in Low-Back Pain Clinical Trials
Low-back pain (LBP) was selected for a case study because it is an example of a very prevalent condition with a wide range of possible treatments that therefore can generate a discussion on the challenges of placebo research.
Halderman and Dagenai49 cite a list of available and commonly used treatment interventions for chronic low back pain which resembles a shopping list in a foreign supermarket since many techniques are unfamiliar for the patients and anyone involved in the management of low back pain. In addition, an analysis of 126 randomized trials for LBP by Machado et al.50, identified over 25 different placebo interventions, and concluded that in only 13% of trials the adequacy of blinding was assessed. Furthermore, in 20% of trials, placebo intervention was a potentially genuine treatment (not inert). These results demonstrate that critical challenges for placebo use are common in low back pain studies.
To illustrate the most commonly used placebo and sham-interventions for the management of low back pain, we used the reviews produced by the Cochrane collaboration Back Review Group and cite some of the treatments used and the rate of placebo response. Antidepressants are highly utilized for the treatment of chronic pain and their mean placebo response in patients with LBP is 11.7%51. In other treatments such as herbal medicines, muscle relaxants, and NSAIDs, the placebo response rate varies from 26% to 41.3%52–54. Similarly, placebo effect in chronic back pain is also elevated for other non-pharmacological interventions.
For non-pharmacological treatments, the difficulty for placebo use increases as there is a lack of understanding of their basic mechanisms of action, such as low-level laser therapy for pain. Therefore it is difficult to design an effective placebo method if the underlying mechanism is not clear. In addition, interventions such as cognitive behavioral therapies or exercises pose additional difficulty because the placebo arm needs to have the same characteristics as the active arm.
Besides all these difficulties, LBP is a condition with many subjective outcomes such as pain and functional assessment. It has been shown in a recently meta-analysis that placebo response is significantly higher as compared to non-treatment in subjective (but not objective) outcomes24.
In summary, LBP is an example that placebo response might dramatically change according to the placebo method and expectations of patients especially in conditions in which the main outcome is subjective.
Brief Summary of Placebo mechanisms: Insights for PRM Placebos
In this section we will discuss the potential biological mechanisms by which a placebo intervention may cause an effect. These mechanisms are classified into: cognitive and neural theory, conditioning, and expectation. This discussion might be helpful for PRM researchers to understand better the placebo effect and therefore design their RCTs appropriately.
In order to understand placebo effects, it is important to discuss (and differentiate) its two main components: methodological artifacts (or non-specific effects) vs. specific effects. The methodological artifacts need to be considered as part of the placebo effects such as the natural history of disease. The following non-specific effects can be listed: (i) natural history of disease as diseases naturally fluctuates and have cyclical variations, therefore a spontaneous remission is possible; (ii) regression to the mean -defined as the statistical phenomenon in which there is a random variability in the measurement and if the population to be studied has severe symptoms at the start of the trial, it is expected that there will be a regression to the mean in the scores of symptoms; (iii) therapist and observer bias - if therapists and observers are not blinded, they can therefore influence the treatment and rating scores; (iv) patient biases -if patients are not adequately blinded, they might also influence the results, especially the subjective scores55,56.
On the other hand, there is a variety of cognitive and neural theory to explain the potential specific, biological effects of placebo. There is a large body of literature showing that changes in the limbic system (associated with emotional and affective processing) might be associated with extensive changes in neural activity and therefore might induce changes in hormonal levels (through modulation of the neuroendocrine axis), immune system (also through modulation of the sympathetic and parasympathetic systems). Therefore changes in hope associated with treatment expectations can induce meaningful changes in neural activity and therefore induce biological changes57–59.
Patient’s beliefs and expectancies are critically important for the biological effects of placebo and need to be carefully considered in the PRM field, especially due to the intensive interaction between the rehabilitation team, the patients and their families. Finally another model that needs to be considered in this context is conditioning (the Pavlovian conditioning models). In fact, several studies have shown that if a repeated stimulus (for instance water and sugar) is linked to a physiological response; for instance, an injection that can change the immune system; then the water and sugar alone will have by itself an effect in the immune system. This effect is likely a consequence of plastic brain changes induced during the period of conditioning and shares similar mechanisms to the effects induced by learning..
It is already known that not all placebo interventions will have the same effects57. Evidence exists that sham devices and procedures have “enhanced” placebo effects compared to placebo pills58,59. For example, a RCT prospectively compared the two different placebo controls- a sham device (a validated sham acupuncture needle) and an oral placebo pill– and found that the sham device reduced pain significantly more than an inert pill31. Elaborate rituals can produce effects that are greater than simple pill ingestion. Furthermore, the accompanying efforts of participation (for instance– PRM procedures such as laser therapy) are much more evocative and potentially potent than medication. Whether assigned to a genuine or dummy treatment, patients have to make a commitment to travel and therefore do the potentially valuable exercise for just getting to the treatment. Transportation often requires the assistance of family and friends that can provide tacit social support. In a trial, these factors, which will accounted as “non-specific,” are all likely to increase the effect size of the placebo arm and show a difference from the genuine treatment which is more difficult to detect.
Performing genuine or sham PRM active treatment requires mutual understanding and agreement on completing the procedures. Patients and practitioners often need to negotiate and reach mutual agreement on how to cooperate and work together in a manner far more complex than what is required to take medications. These interactions often require explanations, assurances, opportunities for dialogue and a high degree of trust. Such genuine patient-physicians encounters, necessary in both the genuine and placebo arms, have demonstrated their ability contribution to positive health outcomes60–63. For example, a recent RCT demonstrated that augmenting the patient-practitioner with a patient-centered approach in sham acupuncture treatment can significantly enhance clinical outcomes compared to sham acupuncture performed in a business-like disease-centered clinical encounter64.
What Have We Learned from Other Specialties?
The challenges observed for placebo use in PRM occurs across other specialties. However different solutions have been proposed and adopted, providing useful insights for placebo trials in PRM.
Ethical issues and use of placebo in psychiatry: Psychiatry has similar challenges as PRM for placebo use. 1) For instance, major depression is similar to chronic pain, since both are associated with a large placebo response. 2) They also have to deal with the ethical issue of offering placebo to depressed patients when there is an (evidence-based proven) effective treatment. In this case, the argument for defending placebo-controlled trials in psychiatry is that there is a risk that a trial comparing a new intervention to an established intervention might show no differences in results if the established intervention is not effective either. In this case the conclusion that a new intervention is as effective as compared to the established intervention is not valid. 3) In addition, there is the issue of assay sensitivity, where a lack of difference between two active interventions might be the result of a poorly executed trial (such as due to lack of power). A compromised solution is to include patients with mild and moderate depression (to increase sample size) and have a relatively short trial (to decrease the exposure of patients to a less effective therapy). Similarly, in PRM, the same issues should be taken into account when considering testing two active interventions for the treatment of chronic pain, for instance.
Testing of implantable devices in Neurology: PRM is a specialty that uses many devices and physical modalities. Exploring the trials that use devices from other specialties might provide useful insights. Parkinson’s disease is a condition that has received FDA approval for the treatment with deep brain stimulation (DBS) since 1997. Because the unethical use of placebo device implantation due to the invasiveness of the procedure, one strategy is to implant the device in all the patients and have it turned ON or OFF during different time periods and compare the results. Therefore a cross-over study is an interesting possibility for using an implantable device. The main issue is the carry-over effect. In these cases, treatments that induce long-lasting effects might not be candidate for cross-over design, alternatively, a longer interval period between conditions will be required.
Use of placebo in Children –experience with pediatrics: PRM is a condition that has also a large proportion of pediatric patients. One interesting aspect is the ethics and difficulties of placebo use in children. As with any other clinical trial, pediatric trials should also involve placebo use as much as possible as to assure the highest trial quality. In addition, because of the mechanisms of disease are different in children and adult, it is important to conduct sound research in children. In fact, children have not benefited from advances in drug and device development to the same extent as adults. Thus the same standards should be followed to children as compared to adults. In a recent article, Flynn65 discusses the use of placebo in children in antihypertensive trials and proposes the following recommendations for the ethical use of placebo: “(i) The potential subjects have asymptomatic, mild-to-moderate primary hypertension; (ii) The potential subjects do not have hypertension-related target organ damage; (iii) Placebo treatment will be of short duration (generally <4–8 weeks)”. Similar principles could be easily translated to PRM interventions in children.
Use of sham procedures/surgical trials– insights from surgical trials: Because some treatments in PRM might involve the need of surgery or invasive procedures, this issue needs to be visited. The main issue against the use of placebo in surgery is that in a standard, placebo-controlled drug trial, the inert substance used in the placebo group is known to have no adverse effects; however this is not true for sham surgery. The potential harm in a sham surgery procedure might be ethically unacceptable. However, not performing sham-controlled trials might promote the use of procedures that are not effective. For instance, surgical research with placebo eliminated a common surgical intervention, internal mammary artery ligation, for the treatment of chest pain66. In addition, a recent trial on arthroscopy for knee osteoarthritis has shown that outcomes after arthroscopic surgery are not better as compared with placebo surgery67. Therefore alternative procedures such as a small incision for the sham surgery group might be necessary as to assess the real effectiveness of interventions associated with the need of surgery. Despite of these issues, the need for placebo-controlled trials using surgical techniques is as important as the test of the clinical pharmacological procedures.
General Recommendations to Improve the Quality of Placebo and Sham-Controlled Clinical Trials in PRM
Based on the working group’s discussion, it is clear that the use of placebo in PRM is challenging for several reasons therefore resulting in less placebo-controlled RCTs being conducted. This fact in addition to the issues of PRM as a specialty (e.g., less research, lower impact factor journals) lead to the use of treatments that do not always have the necessary evidence to support them. We propose then 14 recommendations and alternatives to address the challenges for placebo use in PRM and therefore encourage whenever possible well designed studies and use of more evidence-based medicine. These recommendations are grouped in 5 different categories as shown below:
Develop a better understanding of placebos
There is a lack of understanding of the mechanisms associated with placebo effects. Therefore it is critical to also investigate the biological mechanisms of placebo effects as to plan better placebo treatments.
Patients might respond differently to placebo. Therefore studies should be conducted to find predictors for placebo response. In fact, some of these studies might be based on secondary analyses of placebo arm of previous RCTs in the field of PRM.
Some placebos might have larger effects than others. It is important, therefore to learn more about the effect sizes of different placebos before designing a given trial (as we discussed in the case study of low back pain).
Challenges particular to the population seen in PRM
PRM is a specialty that has a large number of chronic conditions such as chronic pain and sequela post stroke, brain injury, spinal cord injury, etc. Therefore for these conditions, a placebo treatment needs to be considered for a longer period of time and in addition it is important that the researcher is aware of small effect sizes to detect a meaningful difference since improvement is less dramatic than with acute conditions.
Patients in PRM trials might have had previous experiences with the intervention under study (such as who received acupuncture before participating in a new acupuncture trial) and therefore might have a different expectation regarding treatments that will influence placebo response. The recommendation of our working group was to use naive subjects whenever possible. However this approach might be difficult to follow in some populations (for ex: low-back pain) which is often a chronic condition and patients are influenced by media, health professionals and personal contacts opinions, which introduce beliefs that can either favors or hinder the implementation of a given intervention.
Challenges particular to the type of interventions commonly used in PRM
Large placebo effects might be observed due to more interaction between care providers and patients. A potential alternative is that instructions might be given by a computer (such as in a video game), or use of a standard protocol in which investigators would follow a specific protocol. Efforts should be made to try to reduce variability of interventions both in the real and in the placebo groups. The variability might be due to the 1) amount of time spent in each session, 2) number and intensity of sessions, 3) too many providers, 4) different settings (private clinic, academic hospital, inpatient, outpatient, etc), 5) verbal and non-verbal clues, 6) educational materials or packages of information, and 6) rewards or re-imbursements. Therefore controlling for these factors is recommended to decrease variability.
For multi-component interventions, for ex: multidisciplinary rehabilitation treatments. Our recommendation is to study the isolated components of rehabilitation; however this will depend on research question and might not be feasible. When the investigation of the multidisciplinary treatments is needed, some alternatives can be considered: (i) consider waiting list control group (when placebo and real interventions are similar); (ii) consider virtual reality in situations where designing a placebo is difficult and also to avoid provider patient interaction, and (iii), the use of structural equivalence - a method useful in psychotherapy trials where the experimental and placebo groups have similar degree of therapeutic contact.
Development of sham in exercise studies is challenging. Our recommendation is that a better understanding of various mechanisms and types of exercises is necessary to allow the development of a standard sham exercise therapy. This might include studying unique components of exercise at each time, for example using factorial design.
Challenges for the development of sham devices. Our recommendation is to partner with engineers and with industry to develop valid and reliable sham devices.
Challenges for the development of sham injection techniques. The interventions using injection raise an additional question as to whether the therapeutic mechanism involves the drug or the procedure. In fact, should the control be the same injection with placebo content (corticoid versus saline)68 or a control procedure (fake injection versus dry needling in a different location). We recommend that study designs should allow the comparisons of two variables (for ex: drug and the procedure). In fact, when injection versus pill administration of the same medication is being tested the adoption of a “double-dummy” procedure - one arm has real injection + placebo pill and the other arm has fake injection + real pill– can be valuable.
Use of subjective outcomes. Some conditions in PRM such as chronic pain have subjective outcomes as the main endpoints. One alternative here is the use of surrogate outcomes if the clinical outcome is a subjective outcome. For instance, ongoing research has attempted to identify brain activity signatures using methods of neuroimaging and EEG69. In addition, models to induce pain might also be valuable as method to quantify pain in subjects with chronic pain70.
Bioethical considerations
Beliefs regarding treatments vary across different cultures. This is important for global clinical trials. Therefore it is critical to respect the specific cultural beliefs. Patients need to be asked before they are randomized, and researchers need to be aware of potential biases of including subjects who have a strong belief in favor of one intervention.
Patients rights cannot be jeopardized by the scientific interest. Amdur and Biddle71 proposed a decision algorithm to evaluate ethical aspects involved in a placebo-controlled RCT. In this algorithm, decision points have the following questions: “Is placebo being used in place of standard therapy? Is standard therapy considered to be effective? Is the toxicity of standard therapy such that patients routinely refuse this treatment? Could the use of placebo instead of standard treatments cause irreversible health problems or extreme suffering? Is it possible to predict the placebo response rate in this study with a reasonable degree of accuracy? Could this trial benefit future patients to the point that a reasonable person with an average degree of altruism and risk-aversiveness would consent to being randomized in this trial?”.
One important issue when considering the ethics is the concept of equipoise (when there is uncertainty regarding the comparative therapeutic merits of two treatments) that is not usually appreciated by investigators. If a treatment is not proven to be efficacious, then a comparison against placebo is indeed ethical (in addition to the standard treatment if this is the case) and should be the first option.
Methodological considerations
There is a critical need to develop an effective method of blinding in trials in PRM especially as some treatments might not be easy to blind. Our recommendation is to assess the effectiveness of blinding during and at the end of the trial; however the researcher needs to be aware that this procedure might be biased when patients guess correctly due to the therapeutic effects of the intervention.
In order to design proper placebo-controlled RCTs, PRM researchers need also to have a strong foundation in the methodology of clinical research. Therefore clinical researchers in the field of PRM need to receive appropriate training and education to conduct better clinical trials.
Acknowledgments
The authors are grateful to Barbara Bonetti for her assistance to organize this meeting.
Footnotes
Disclosures:
This work was supported by the David Rockefeller Center for Latin American Studies –Harvard University - conference grant and the Brazilian Association of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. We are grateful to Scala Institute, Mackenzie University (Sao Paulo, Brazil) and Brazilian Medical College of Acupuncture for the additional support to organize the I International Symposium in Placebo. TJK was supported by NIH grant # K24 AT004095.
References
- 1.Kalauokalani D, Cherkin DC, Sherman KJ, et al. Lessons from a trial of acupuncture and massage for low back pain. Patient expectations and treatment effects. Spine. 2001;26:1418–24. doi: 10.1097/00007632-200107010-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Turner JA, Jensen MP, Warms CA, et al. Blinding effectiveness and association of pretreatment expectations with pain improvement in a double-blind randomized controlled trial. Pain. 2002;99:91–9. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(02)00060-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Stewart LA, Parmar MK. Bias in the analysis and reporting of randomized controlled trials. Int J Technol Assess Health Care. 1996;12:264–75. doi: 10.1017/s0266462300009612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Van Der Windt DA, Van Der Heijden GJ, Van Den Berg SG, et al. Ultrasound therapy for acute ankle sprains. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2002;(1):CD001250. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD001250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Robinson V, Brosseau L, Casimiro L, et al. Thermotherapy for treating rheumatoid arthritis. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2002;(2):CD002826. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD002826. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ozgonenel L, Aytekin E, Durmu oglu G. A double-blind trial of clinical effects of therapeutic ultrasound in knee osteoarthritis. Ultrasound Med Biol. 2009;35:44–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ultrasmedbio.2008.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Brosseau L, Casimiro L, Robinson V, et al. Therapeutic ultrasound for treating patellofemoral pain syndrome. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2001;4:CD003375. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Osiri M, Welch V, Brosseau L, Shea B, et al. Transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation for knee osteoarthritis. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2000;(4):CD002823. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD002823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Brosseau L, Judd MG, Marchand S, et al. Transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation (TENS) for the treatment of rheumatoid arthritis in the hand. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2003;3:CD004377. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD004377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kroeling P, Gross A, Goldsmith C. Electrotherapy for neck disorders. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2005;2:CD004251. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD004251.pub3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Khadilkar A, Odebiyi DO, Brosseau L, et al. Transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation (TENS) versus placebo for chronic low back pain. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2008;4:CD003008. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003008.pub3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Nnoaham KE, Kumbang J. Transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation (TENS) for chronic pain. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2008;(3):CD003222. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003222.pub2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Robb KA, Bennett MI, Johnson MI, et al. Transcutaneous electric nerve stimulation (TENS) for cancer pain in adults. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2008;(3):CD006276. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD006276.pub2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Bjordal JM, Johnson MI, Lopes-Martins RA, et al. Short-term efficacy of physical interventions in osteoarthritic knee pain. A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised placebo-controlled trials. BMC Musculoskelet Disord. 2007;8:51. doi: 10.1186/1471-2474-8-51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Johnson M, Martinson M. Efficacy of electrical nerve stimulation for chronic musculoskeletal pain: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Pain. 2007;130:157–65. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gerdesmeyer L, Frey C, Vester J, et al. Radial extracorporeal shock wave therapy is safe and effective in the treatment of chronic recalcitrant plantar fasciitis: results of a confirmatory randomized placebo-controlled multicenter study. Am J Sports Med. 2008;36:2100–2109. doi: 10.1177/0363546508324176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Fregni F, Boggio PS, Lima MC, et al. A sham-controlled, phase II trial of transcranial direct current stimulation for the treatment of central pain in traumatic spinal cord injury. Pain. 2006;122:197–209. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.02.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Fregni F, Boggio PS, Mansur CG, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation of the unaffected hemisphere in stroke patients. Neuroreport. 2005;16:1551–5. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000177010.44602.5e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Fregni F, Gimenes R, Valle AC, et al. A randomized, sham-controlled, proof of principle study of transcranial direct current stimulation for the treatment of pain in fibromyalgia. Arthritis Rheum. 2006;54:3988–98. doi: 10.1002/art.22195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Lima MC, Fregni F. Motor cortex stimulation for chronic pain: systematic review and meta-analysis of the literature. Neurology. 2008;70:2329–37. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000314649.38527.93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gandiga PC, Hummel FC, Cohen LG. Transcranial DC stimulation (tDCS): a tool for double-blind sham-controlled clinical studies in brain stimulation. Clin Neurophysiol. 2006;117:845–50. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Boutron I, Guittet L, Estellat C, et al. Reporting methods of blinding in randomized trials assessing nonpharmacological treatments. PLoS Med. 2007;4:e61. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0040061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Boutron I, Tubach F, Giraudeau B, et al. Methodological differences in clinical trials evaluating nonpharmacological and pharmacological treatments of hip and knee osteoarthritis. JAMA. 2003;290:1062–70. doi: 10.1001/jama.290.8.1062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hrobjartsson A, Gotzsche PC. Is the placebo powerless? An analysis of clinical trials comparing placebo with no treatment. N Engl J Med. 2001;344:1594–1602. doi: 10.1056/NEJM200105243442106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Targino RA, Imamura M, Kaziyama HH, et al. A randomized controlled trial of acupuncture added to usual treatment for fibromyalgia. J Rehabil Med. 2008;40:582–8. doi: 10.2340/16501977-0216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Miller FG, Kaptchuk TJ. Sham procedures and the ethics of clinical trials. J Roy Soc Med. 2004;97:576–8. doi: 10.1258/jrsm.97.12.576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kaptchuk TJ. The double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial: gold standard or golden calf? J Clin Epidemiol. 2001;54:541–9. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(00)00347-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Horwitz RI, Horwitz SM. Adherence to treatment and health outcomes. Arch Intern Med. 1993;153:1863–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kirsch I. Specifying nonspecifics: psychological mechanisms of placebo effects. In: Harrington A, editor. The Placebo Effect. An Interdisciplinary Exploration. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Walach H. The efficacy paradox in randomized controlled trials of CAM and elsewhere: beware of the placebo trap. J Altern Complement Med. 2001;7:213–8. doi: 10.1089/107555301300328070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kaptchuk TJ, Stason WB, Davis RB. Sham device v inert pill: randomised controlled trial of two placebo treatments. BMJ. 2006;332:391–7. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38726.603310.55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.de Craen AJ, Tijssen JG, de Gans J, et al. Placebo effect in the acute treatment of migraine: subcutaneous placebos are better than oral placebos. J Neurol. 2000;247:183–8. doi: 10.1007/s004150050560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Brunoni AR, Lopes M, Kaptchuk TJ, et al. Placebo response of non-pharmacological and pharmacological trials in major depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One. 2009;4:e4824. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Schwartz NA, Turturro MA, Istvan DJ, et al. Patients’ perceptions of route of nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drug administration and its effect on analgesia. Acad Emerg Med. 2000;7:857–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1553-2712.2000.tb02061.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Freedman B. Equipoise and the ethics of clinical research. N Engl J Med. 1987;317:141–5. doi: 10.1056/NEJM198707163170304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Khan A, Warner HA, Brown WA. Symptom reduction and suicide risk in patients treated with placebo in antidepressant clinical trials: an analysis of the Food and Drug Administration database. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:311–7. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.4.311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Leber P. The use of placebo control groups in the assessment of psychiatric drugs: an historical context. 1. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;15;47:699–706. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00321-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Leon AC. Placebo protects subjects from nonresponse: a paradox of power. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:329–30. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.4.329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Kraemer HC. Statistical analysis to settle ethical issues? Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:327–8. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.4.327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Edward SJ, Stevens AJ, Braunholtz DA, et al. The ethics of placebo-controlled trials: a comparison of inert and active placebo controls. World J Surg. 2005;29:610–4. doi: 10.1007/s00268-005-7621-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Han JS. Acupuncture and endorphins. Neurosci Lett. 2004;361:258–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.12.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Wang SM, Kain ZN, White P. Acupuncture analgesia: I. Scientific basis. Anesth Analg. 2008;106:602–10. doi: 10.1213/01.ane.0000277493.42335.7b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Lund I, Lundeberg T. Are minimal, superifical or sham acupuncture procedures acceptable as inert placebo controls? Acupuncture in Med. 2006;26:12–5. doi: 10.1136/aim.24.1.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Assefi NP, Sherman KJ, Jacobsen C, et al. A randomized clinical trials of acupuncture compared to sham acupuncture in fibromyalgia. Ann Intern Med. 2005;143:10–9. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-143-1-200507050-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Sherman KJ, Hogenboom CJ, Cherkin DC, et al. Description and validation of a noinvasive placebo acupuncture procedure. J Altern Complem Med. 2002;8:11–9. doi: 10.1089/107555302753507140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Olausson H, Lamarre Y, Backlund H, et al. Unmyelinated tactile afferents signal touch and project to insular cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:900–4. doi: 10.1038/nn896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kim S. Creating an instrument for a successful double-blind acupuncture placebo. J Acupuncture Meridian Stud. 2008;1:36–41. doi: 10.1016/S2005-2901(09)60005-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Madsen MV, Gotzsche PC, Hrobjartsson A. Acupuncture treatment for pain: systematic review of randomised clinical trials with acupuncture, placebo acupuncture, and no acupuncture groups. BMJ. 2009;338:a3115. doi: 10.1136/bmj.a3115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Haldeman S, Dagenais S. A supermarket approach to the evidence-informed management of chronic low back pain. Spine J. 2008;8:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.spinee.2007.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Machado LA, Kamper SJ, Herbert RD, et al. Imperfect placebos are common in low back pain trials: a systematic review of the literature. Eur Spine J. 2008;17:889–904. doi: 10.1007/s00586-008-0664-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Urquhart DM, Hoving JL, Assendelft WW, et al. Antidepressant for non-specific low back pain. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2008;(1):CD001703. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD001703.pub3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Gagnier JJ, van Tulder M, Berman B, et al. Herbal medicine for low back pain. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2006;(2):CD004504. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD004504.pub3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.van Tulder MW, Touray T, Furlan AD, et al. Muscle relaxants for non-specific low back pain. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2003;(2):CD004252. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD004252. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Roelofs PD, Deyo RA, Koes BW, et al. Nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs for low back pain: an updated Cochrane review. Spine. 2008;33:1766–74. doi: 10.1097/BRS.0b013e31817e69d3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Hrobjartsson A. What are the main methodological problems in the estimation of placebo effects. J Clin Epidemiol. 2002;55:340–5. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(01)00496-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Kaptchuk TJ. Powerful placebo: the dark side of the randomized controlled trial. Lancet. 1998;351:1722–5. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(97)10111-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Vickers AJ, de Craen AJ. Why use placebos in clinical trials? A narrative review of the methodological literature. J Clin Epidemiol. 2000;53:157–61. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(99)00139-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Kaptchuk TJ, Goldman P, Stone DA, et al. Do medical devices have enhanced placebo effects? J Clin Epidemio. 2000;53:786–92. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(00)00206-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Kaptchuk TJ. The placebo effect in alternative medicine: can the performance of a healing ritual have clinical significance? Ann Intern Med. 2002;136:817–25. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-136-11-200206040-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Di Blasi Z, Harkness E, Ernst E, et al. Influence of context effects on health outcomes: a systematic review. Lancet. 2001;357:757–62. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(00)04169-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Steward MA. Effective physician-patient communication and health outcomes: a review. Can Med Assoc J. 1995;152:1423–33. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Ong LM, de Haes JC, Hoos AM, et al. Doctor-patient communication: a review of the literature. Soc Sci Med. 1995;40:903–18. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)00155-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Kaplan Sh, Greenfield S, Ware JE. Assessing the effects of physician-patient interactions on the outcomes of chronic disease. Med Card. 1989;27:S110–27. doi: 10.1097/00005650-198903001-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Kaptchuk TJ, Kelley JM, Conboy LA, et al. Components of the placebo effect: randomized controlled trial in patients with irritable bowel syndrome. BMJ. 2008;336:998–1003. doi: 10.1136/bmj.39524.439618.25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Flynn JT, Newburger JW, Daniels SR, et al. A randomized, placebo-controlled trial of amlodipine in children with hypertension. J Pediatr. 2004;145:353–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2004.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Dimond EG, Kittle CF, Crockett JE. Comparison of internal mammary artery ligation and sham operation for angina pectoris. Am J Cardiol. 1960 Apr;5:483–6. doi: 10.1016/0002-9149(60)90105-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Moseley JB, O’Malley K, Petersen NJ, et al. A controlled trial of arthroscopic surgery for osteoarthritis of the knee. N Engl J Med. 2002;347:81–8. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa013259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Fuchs S, Erbe T, Fischer HL, et al. Intraarticular hyaluronic acid versus glucocorticoid injections for nonradicular pain in the lumbar spine. J Vasc Interv Radio. 2005;16:1493–8. doi: 10.1097/01.RVI.0000175334.60638.3F. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Jensen MP, Hakimian S, Sherlin LH, et al. New insights into neuromodulatory approaches for the treatment of pain. J Pain. 2008;9:193–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2007.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Klein T, Magerl W, Rolke R, et al. Human surrogate models of neuropathic pain. Pain. 2005;115:227–33. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2005.03.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Amdur RJ, Biddle CJ. An algorithm for evaluating the ethics of a placebo-controlled trial. Int JCancer. 2001;96:261–9. doi: 10.1002/ijc.1026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]