Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2010 Jun 14;107(27):12110–12115. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1006991107

Consolation as possible expression of sympathetic concern among chimpanzees

Teresa Romero a,b,1, Miguel A Castellanos c, Frans B M de Waal a,1
PMCID: PMC2901437  PMID: 20547864

Abstract

Chimpanzees are known to spontaneously provide contact comfort to recent victims of aggression, a behavior known as consolation. Similar behavior in human children is attributed to empathic or sympathetic concern. In line with this empathy hypothesis, chimpanzee consolation has been shown to reduce the recipient's state of arousal, hence to likely alleviate distress. Other predictions from the empathy hypothesis have rarely been tested, however, owing to small sample sizes in previous studies. An exceptionally large database of spontaneous consolation in two outdoor-housed groups of chimpanzees lends further support to the empathy hypothesis in that consolation occurred disproportionally between individuals that are socially close (i.e., kin and affiliation partners) and was more typical of females than males, which differences are also known of human empathy. These effects were demonstrated using generalized linear mixed models, which control multiple variables at once. An exception to the above pattern was formed by the highest-ranking males, which frequently offered consolation to victims of aggression, probably as part of their general policing function in chimpanzee society. Consolation occurred more frequently in the absence of reconciliation between former opponents, suggesting that actors are sensitive to the contact need of victims of aggression, which may be greater if the aggressor ignores them. That consolation is an integrated part of close mutual relationships is supported by the tendency for it being reciprocated.

Keywords: empathy, Pan troglodytes, postconflict affiliation


Providing contact comfort to distressed others has been studied in human children, in whom it is generally classified as an expression of empathic or sympathetic concern for the other (13). The same behavior has received less attention in animal research, perhaps owing to its restricted distribution. Despite ample attention to postconflict behavior in more than 30 primate species, both in captivity and in the field (46), as well as several nonprimates (79), only a handful of species regularly shows reassurance of distressed conspecifics. The term consolation has been proposed for interactions in which an uninvolved bystander initiates friendly contact with a recent victim of aggression (10) (Fig. 1). This definition excludes other forms of postconflict contact, such as reconciliation (i.e., a reunion between former opponents) (10) and third-party contacts sought by the conflict participants themselves or made with the aggressor.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Chimpanzees spontaneously console distressed individuals by means of friendly body contact. Loudly screaming after having lost a fight against a rival, an adult male (right) is approached by a juvenile who puts an arm around him. Photograph by Frans de Waal.

Thus defined, spontaneous consolation has been documented or suggested only for the great apes [Pan troglodytes (1016), P. paniscus (17), Gorilla gorilla (18, 19)], canids [Canis spp (20, 21)], and corvids [Corvus spp (22, 23)] and has been studied with similar ethological methods in human children (24). That consolation has a much more limited distribution than some of the other forms of postconflict behavior has been related to the assumed underlying empathic capacity (11). If this behavior indeed serves to reassure distressed parties, as its label suggests, bystanders need to be affected by the distress of others and be inclined to provide succor. The reactions of chimpanzees to distressed others resemble those of humans in both social context and morphology (e.g., touching, hugging, kissing). Given the close genetic relation between both species, the most parsimonious assumption is a common motivation for both, hence to apply to chimpanzees the same explanation as for human sympathetic concern, which is that of empathy with the predicament of another. At the same time, we realize that an observational study such as the present one cannot demonstrate underlying mechanisms.

Recent evidence has shown that chimpanzee consolation (i) reduces the recipient's stress, and (ii) is provided mainly by individuals socially close to the recipient (25, 26; but see ref. 15). Both findings are consistent with an empathy-based explanation given that similarity, familiarity, and social closeness are known to facilitate empathy in both humans and other animals (2731). It remains unclear, however, whether other factors that facilitate or inhibit human empathy also modulate consolation in apes. Researchers have reported that human empathy and prosocial behavior tend to increase with age, at least up to adolescence, and that females are generally more empathic than males. Only two studies have examined the effect of these variables on consolation in other primates, however, and neither study reported an effect, perhaps owing to small sample sizes (11, 25).

It is assumed that empathy originally evolved in the context of maternal care, in which immediate reactions to the distress or alarm of others are highly adaptive (27, 32). This origin would explain the facilitating role of oxytocin on human empathy (33) as well as the generally greater sensitivity of women and girls to emotional signals. Indeed, using a wide range of assessments, human studies have consistently found females to score higher on empathy than males (3436). For example, girls express more interpersonal concern and caring behaviors than do boys (37).

By reducing the stress of a victim of aggression, consolation confers benefits to another, as indicated for chimpanzees (25), while carrying the risk for the performer of getting drawn into the conflict that caused the recipient's distress (38). Consolation thus fits the definition of altruistic behavior, which is generally thought to evolve through kin selection or reciprocity. We therefore expect this behavior to be biased toward relatives and individuals inclined to return the favor. While evidence for kin selection in the evolution of altruistic behavior is strong (39), evidence for reciprocal altruism is weaker. Although consolation may be expected to be reciprocated or exchanged for other valuable services (31), no study has investigated correlations between given and received consolation.

The present study is unusual in its sample size, which is many times larger than that of any previous study of animal consolation, hence allowing an exploration of all of the above questions in a single analysis. In two large outdoor-living groups of chimpanzees (FS1 and FS2), a total of 3,003 aggressive conflicts and postconflict periods were recorded. This database served to investigate how consolation is affected by variables that typically modulate human empathic responses, such as social closeness and actor sex. We also measured the characteristics of the previous conflict, the relation between conflict participants, and the conflict participants’ relation with third parties, as well as the reciprocity of consolations (i.e., do chimpanzees preferentially console those individuals that console them most?).

Results

Social Determinants.

The effects of a variety of variables on the likelihood of consolation (i.e., the first contact initiated by a bystander with an individual recently targeted by aggression, which individual will be called the “victim”; Materials and Methods) was measured using generalized linear mixed models (GLMM). Among the characteristic of the previous conflict (Materials and Methods and Table 1), the only variables remaining in the best model were the occurrence of reconciliation between both opponents, and the victim's rank (Fig. 2 and Table S1). Consolation seemed to compete with or replace reconciliation between the opponents, in that consolation occurred more often after unreconciled conflicts (β = −0.781, P < 0.001). We also found that low-ranking victims of aggression were consoled significantly less often than high-ranking victims (high vs. low ranking: β = 0.724, P = 0.040; medium vs. low ranking: β = 0.849, P = 0.004).

Table 1.

Description of variables used in GLMM analyses

Name Type
Dependent variable
 Consolation behavior Dichotomous (1 = yes, 0 = no)
 Frequency of consolation Continuous
Fixed explanatory variables
 Conflict characteristics
  Outcome Dichotomous (1 = decided, 0 = undecided)
  Intensity Ordinal (1 = low, 2 = medium, 3 = high)
  Directionality Dichotomous (1 = unidirectional, 0 = bidirectional)
 Reconciliation Dichotomous (1 = yes, 0 = no)
 Appeasement Dichotomous (1 = yes, 0 = no)
 Individual characteristics
  Sex Dichotomous (1 = male, 2 = female)
  Rank Ordinal (1 = high, 2 = medium, 3 = low)
 Relationship characteristics
  Kinship Dichotomous (1 = kin, 0 = no kin)
  Affiliation level Dichotomous (1 = strong, 0 = no-strong)
Random variables
 Aggressor's, recipient's, and consoler's identity Nominal
 Group Nominal

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Mean proportion of occurrence of consolation (±95% confidence intervals) corrected by the number of conflicts for each category in relation to (A) occurrence of reconciliation and (B) victim's dominance rank. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.001.

Triadic Relations.

Because consolation involves three parties (i.e., the consoling bystander and the two opponents, aggressor and victim, of the original conflict), there are three relationships to be considered. We investigated how relational variables between consoler and aggressor and between consoler and victim (Table 1) determine the occurrence of consolation by running GLMM (Materials and Methods). Whereas none of the aggressor's individual or relationship characteristics affected the occurrence of consolation, the variables kinship and affiliation between consoler and victim remained significant in the best model (Fig. 3 and Table S2). Consolation was directed more often at kin (β = 1.189, P < 0.001) and at unrelated individuals with whom the actor had a strong affinitive tie (strong vs. no-strong affiliation: β = 0.370, P = 0.005). Thus, victims of aggression were more likely to be consoled by individuals with whom they enjoyed a close social tie.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

Mean consolation rate (±95% confidence intervals) in relation to (A) genetic relationship and (B) affiliative relationship between consoler and recipient. Consolation rate was calculated as the number of consolations corrected for the total number of opportunities to receive consolation (Materials and Methods). *P < 0.05, **P < 0.001.

Sex Differences.

Sex of the victim was not among the variables remaining in the best model, which suggests that male and female victims of aggression were consoled to a similar degree. On the actor's side, though, the sex of consolers and the interaction between the consoler's sex and rank remained in the best model (Table S2). Overall, female bystanders provided consolation more often than did males (β = 1.507, P < 0.001). This difference held only for individuals of medium and low rank, however, because high-ranking males offered significantly more affiliative contacts to victims of aggression than did females (Fig. 4 and Table S2).

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

Mean consolation rate (±95% confidence intervals) in relation to the sex and rank of consolers. Consolation rate was calculated as the number of consolations corrected by the total number of opportunities to receive consolation (Materials and Methods).

Reciprocity.

In both study groups, individuals disproportionally consoled individuals from whom they received consolation. This was measured by a rowwise matrix correlation (40) between consolation given and consolation received (i.e., between the consolation matrix and its transposition), which controls for interindividual variation (FS1: τrw = 0.142, P = 0.002; FS2: τrw = 0.149, P < 0.0001). Because previous studies have shown that reciprocity may be affected by symmetrical aspects of the social relationship, such as proximity (41), we controlled for the proportion of scans samples in which two individuals were in close social contact (Materials and Methods) as well as for kinship (entered in a separate 1/0 matrix). After having controlled for these variables, the partial correlation between given and received consolation remained significant in both study groups (FS1: τrw;XY.ZW = 0.101, P = 0.028; FS2: τrw;XY.ZW = 0.089, P = 0.047), suggesting that the observed reciprocity was unexplained by symmetrical aspects of the social relationships.

Discussion

According to our analysis, in which the effect of each variable on consolation was measured while controlling for the effects of all other variables, this behavior followed predictions from an empathy-based explanation in that it occurred disproportionately between socially close individuals and was more typical of females. Consolation also tended to be reciprocated, suggesting that it is an integrated part of mutually beneficial relationships.

Consistent with previous research (14, 17, 25), consolation was more often directed at victims who had not reconciled their conflict than at those who had. Although reconciliation is likely the most effective way to reduce relationship damage caused by aggression (4, 5), it has been argued that consolation provides advantages when reconciliation is either not beneficial or too risky (12). There is no evidence, however, that consolation is a true alternative to reconciliation, which serves to repair relationships (42, 43). Consolers might be reconciling with the victim “on behalf” of the aggressor, but then one would expect these bystanders to be close to the aggressor, such as immediate kin (44). Yet the relationship between aggressors and bystanders failed to affect consolation in the present study. Therefore, the more likely explanation focuses on the need for stress reduction in the victim, which need may be greater if reconciliation with the aggressor has failed to take place. As predicted if empathy is the main motivation, the increased rate of consolation in the absence of reconciliation may be a response to greater distress.

Consolation was more common if the victim of aggression was of high or medium rank. When dominant individuals find themselves on the receiving end of a fight, possibly losing to a subordinate (or a coalition of subordinates), they may show greater distress given that this is an unusual situation for them. Previous studies have shown that chimpanzee distress vocalizations, such as screams, can be flexibly modified and convey a rich array of social information (45), while also eliciting consolation by bystanders (14). Further studies are needed to evaluate how individuals modify the acoustic structure of their calls according to their emotional state and whether bystanders are responsive to the vocalizations or other distress behaviors when victims perceive the conflict outcome as a threat to their status. Such sensitivity has been suggested for a massive consolation response to a dethroned alpha male in a zoo colony (46).

Empathy is not equally aroused by the emotional signals of any individual, but rather is in both humans and other animals biased toward parties close to the observer (27, 28, 30). Empathy has been proposed as the proximate mechanism of directed altruism, because its manifestations rather precisely follow predictions from kin selection and reciprocal altruism theories (31). That consolation in this study was disproportionally aimed at close social partners, including genetic relatives, fits the predictions of the empathy hypothesis, as did the only other chimpanzee study to analyze the effect of relationship quality (25). This may also hold for other species in which postconflict affiliation by bystanders is typically provided by close partners, such as mates in rooks (22) or individuals with a high degree of familiarity in wolves (Canis lupus) (21).

Another point in favor of the empathy hypothesis is the observed sex difference, which matches that found in human studies. Overall, female chimpanzees offered more consolation to recent victims of aggression than males, suggesting that females were particularly responsive to the distress of others. Furthermore, this postconflict pattern did not reflect typical sex differences in affiliation during nonconflict situations because in chimpanzee society in general (38, 47), and in our two study groups in particular, males are on average more affiliative than females (mean ± SD percentage of scan samples individuals spent grooming: females, 5.50 ± 0.81%; males, 7.10 ± 1.20%; ANOVA, F1,25 = 62.28, P < 0.001).

An unanticipated outcome was that the tendency of females to show more consolation behavior than males reversed when the analysis is limited to high-ranking individuals, because the most dominant males (e.g., alpha males) frequently reassured distressed parties. This may relate to the special political role of these males in a chimpanzee community, which is that of mediating and controlling open conflict. Adult male chimpanzees often intervene in ongoing conflicts and perform pacifying interventions (46, 48). Postconflict affiliative contacts by high-ranking males toward conflict participants may be part of these policing strategies, which reduce the probability of further aggression and/or social tensions within the group. In both study groups, high-ranking males not only affiliated frequently with recent victims of aggression, but also performed the vast majority of impartial interventions during ongoing conflicts (FS1: 63%; FS2: 68%). A recent experimental study on a different primate species showed that policing males exert a major positive effect on overall intragroup harmony (49).

An observational study cannot determine the exact empathy mechanism operative in chimpanzees. Researchers typically distinguish multiple levels (27). If perception of another's distress merely leads to a matching state in the observer, the latter is expected to selfishly seek alleviation of their own distress, probably turning away from the victim. This personal distress explanation does not seem to apply to our observations because it would preclude other-orientation. In contrast, when vicarious distress leads to sympathetic concern, observers are expected to seek out the other and perform prosocial acts. This explanation fits our observations in which the most typical chimpanzee consolation behaviors were grooming (28.1%), embracing (26.8%), gentle touching (19.2%), and kissing (9.5%). Future research should focus on the bystander's emotional state to evaluate their motivation to contact recipients of aggression. It should be kept in mind, however, that the first expressions of consolation in humans, around 1 y of age, have elements of both providing and receiving comfort (2), and that both elements may be also present in chimpanzee consolation.

Expressions of empathy are not immune to strategic decision making. Like all behavior, actors may learn about its costs and benefits, which in turn influence their propensity to show the behavior. Thus, consolation may become part of an exchange system among partners consistent with reciprocal altruism theory (31). In our data, significant reciprocity correlations persisted after controlling for symmetrical relationship characteristics, such as mutual association and kinship. This suggests that chimpanzees do not simply provide consolation to distressed others with whom they frequently interact but selectively offer this benefit to those partners that console them in return. The receipt of past favors may thus enter into the decision of whether consolation will be provided, which is consistent with experimental empathy studies on humans, in which the degree of empathy with others varies with the history of positive or negative interactions (50, 51).

In short, consolation among chimpanzees fits predictions derived from the empathy hypothesis. Until and unless contrary evidence is produced, this behavior should therefore be considered a likely expression of “sympathetic concern.”

Materials and Methods

Study Population.

The present study was conducted on two groups of chimpanzees (FS1 and FS2) housed at the field station of the Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Lawrenceville, GA. Each group had access to larger outdoor compounds (750 and 520 m2) connected to indoor areas. The demographic composition of groups varied slightly during the study period because of births, deaths, and several removals for veterinary reasons and management purposes. Most of the time, both groups included multiple adult males and at least twice as many adult females (26). The analysis has been limited to individuals at least 10 y old: a total of 8 males and 21 females.

Data Collection.

Since the formation of the two groups, controlled observation sessions have been conducted with regularity (≈1 per week). Data presented here refer to a period from 1992 to 2000 for FS1 and from 1994 to 2000 for FS2. During 90-min observation sessions (52), affiliative and sexual interactions (including kiss, embrace, grooming, gentle touch, finger/hand-in-mouth, mounting) and agonistic interactions (which by definition include at least one of the following behavior elements: tug, brusque rush, trample, bite, grunt-bark, shrill-bark, flight, crouch, shrink/flinch, or bared-teeth scream) (53, 54) were recorded by a trained research technician, Mike Seres, using an all-occurrence sampling technique. Additionally, scan samples of state behaviors (e.g., contact-sitting, grooming, play) were taken at regular intervals (i.e., every 5 min through 1993 and every 10 min in the years thereafter).

Because the observation sessions were not designed to study postconflict interactions, formal postconflict and matched control observations were not conducted. However, because the observations were continuous, the behavior following aggression can be considered postconflict data (55). We focused on the immediate 10-min postconflict period on the basis of previous investigations (e.g., refs. 13, 14). Following de Waal and van Roosmalen (10), an interaction was considered an agonistic conflict if at least one of the strictly agonistic patterns previously listed occurred. Polyadic conflicts (i.e., those involving more individuals than the two original opponents) were divided into dyadic components (53), and for each agonistic dyad the identities of the initial aggressor and recipient of aggression were recorded along with the intensity, directionality, and outcome of the conflict. The intensity was scored as low if the conflict included a threat, chase, and/or brusque rush, as medium if it included hit, punch, push, and pull, and as high if it involved trample or bite. A conflict was considered unidirectional if all aggressive behavior was directed toward the initial recipient of aggression and no counteraggression occurred. The outcome of the conflict was recorded as decided if only one of the parties showed signs of submission (e.g., screaming, teeth-baring, fleeing, or pant-grunt) and as undecided in the remaining cases.

Data Analysis.

A total of 1,676 and 1,327 valid 10-min postconflict periods were collected for FS1 and FS2 groups, respectively. From postconflict periods we extracted the information concerned with all interactions involving the opponents as well as the time of the interaction, the identity of the interaction partners, and the identity of the initiator of the interactions. For the purpose of this study, reconciliation was operationally defined as the fist affiliative contact between former opponents after a conflict, appeasement as the first affiliative contact directed from a third party to the initial aggressor, and consolation as the first affiliative contact directed from a third party to the recipient of aggression. Third-party individuals were defined as those individuals who were neither involved in the conflict or in any agonistic interaction in a time window of ±2 min from the occurrence of the conflict.

To examine whether the occurrence of consolation was affected by several factors, GLMM with a binomial error structure and logit link function were used. When evaluating the effect of the characteristic of the previous conflict on the occurrence of consolation, the dependent variable was a binary term (binomial error structure) of whether consolation behavior was present or absent. Conflict characteristics (i.e., intensity, directionality, outcome), sex and dominance rank of both opponents, relationship characteristics between aggressors and recipients, and the occurrence of reconciliation and appeasement were entered as fixed variables (Table 1). Dominance was defined by the direction of submissive signals, such as pant-grunt and bobbing movements, and by nonagonistic approach/retreat interactions. Rank was determined by the relative number of dominated individuals: individuals dominating at least 75% of group mates were classified as high ranking, those dominated by 75% of individuals as low ranking, and the remaining individuals as middle ranking. Kinship was based on maternal lineages, and only (grand)-mother-offspring and maternal siblings were considered related individuals. The affiliation level between dyads was categorized using a combined measure of four state behaviors collected during scans (i.e., contact sitting, sitting within arm's reach, grooming, and mutual grooming), and calculating the quartile points of dyadic scores for each focal individual. Only dyads with scores higher than the top quartile were considered to have a strong affiliative relationship. Because dominance rank and affiliative relationships could vary along years, we calculated dyadic values for each year independently. The identity of aggressors and recipients of aggression, as well as the study group name (i.e., FS1 and FS2) were entered as random variables.

A second set of analyses was performed to examine the effect of individual characteristics of participants and relationship characteristics between opponents and third parties on the occurrence of consolation. The dependent variable was the frequency of giving consolation corrected by the number of opportunities. In the first analysis, the frequency of giving consolation equaled the number of times each potential consoler initiated the affiliative interaction toward a particular recipient of aggression. To correct for the opportunity each potential consoler had to console the victim, we included as an offset variable the number of postconflicts in which one individual was the recipient, excluding those in which the partner was an involved individual in the conflict (i.e., the aggressor or a supporter of either opponent). GLMM was then run with individual characteristics of recipients of aggression and consolers (i.e., sex and rank) and relationship characteristics between recipients and potential consolers (i.e., kinship, affiliation level) as fixed terms (Table 1). In the second analysis, the frequency of consolation equaled the number of times each potential consoler offered consolation when a particular individual was the aggressor. We corrected for the opportunity to offer consolation, including as an offset variable the number of postconflicts in which one individual was the aggressor, excluding those in which the partner was an involved individual in the conflict (i.e., the recipient or a supporter of either opponent). Then, the GLMM was run including the aggressors’ variables (Table 1). As random terms we included the identity of opponents and consolers and the group name.

For all GLMM analyses we used restricted maximum likelihood methods for model estimation. A step-up strategy (i.e., fixed factors were added to the model sequentially) was used, and the selection of the model was based on Akaike's information criterion (AIC). A model with a lower value of AIC is considered to be a better model. The assumptions of the GLMM analyses of the models were checked by visual inspection of the residuals and of predicted random variables. GLMM did not show overdispersion. GLMM analyses were run on R version 2.8.1 (56, 57) using the lmer function included in the lme4 package.

To test for reciprocity and interchange we used the rowwise matrix correlation method (40, 58), which is a distribution-free test taking into account that in interaction matrices data are not independent. The number of consolations received by third parties (corrected by the number of opportunities of receiving consolation) was entered into directional, square, interaction matrices. Interindividual proximity was defined by the percentage of scan samples during which a dyad was in close social contact (i.e., contact sitting, sitting within arm's reach, and grooming). Kendall's form of rowwise matrix correlations (τrw) and second-order partial rowwise matrix correlations (τrw;XY.ZW) were used. The calculation was implemented in R software (57) with the recursive partial correlation formula, and exact probability values were calculated on the basis of 10,000 permutations. All analyses were two-tailed, and the significance level was set at 0.05.

Supplementary Material

Supporting Information

Acknowledgments

We thank Michael Seres for the behavioral data collection of this study in the 1990s; Carolyn Zahn-Waxler and Elisabetta Palagi for constructive comments on a previous version of the manuscript; and the animal care and veterinary staff at the Yerkes National Primate Research Center (YNPRC) for maintaining the health and well being of the chimpanzees. This work was supported by Emory's College of Arts and Sciences, National Institutes of Health Base Grant RR-00165 to the YNPRC, and funding from the Living Links Center. The YNPRC is fully accredited by the American Association for Accreditation for Laboratory Animal Care.

Footnotes

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1006991107/-/DCSupplemental.

References

  • 1.Zahn-Waxler C, Hollenbeck B, Radke-Yarrow M. The origins of empathy and altruism. In: Fox MW, Mickley LD, editors. Advances in Animal Welfare Science. Washington, DC: Humane Society; 1984. pp. 21–39. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Zahn-Waxler C, Radke-Yarrow M. The origins of empathic concern. Motiv Emot. 1990;14:107–130. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Eisenberg N. Empathy and sympathy. In: Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM, editors. Handbook of Emotion. New York: Guilford Press; 2000. pp. 677–691. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.de Waal FB. Primates—a natural heritage of conflict resolution. Science. 2000;289:586–590. doi: 10.1126/science.289.5479.586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Aureli F, Cords M, van Schaik CP. Conflict resolution following aggression in gregarious animals: A predictive framework. Anim Behav. 2002;64:325–343. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Arnold K, Aureli F. In: Primates in Perspective. Campbell CJ, Fuentes A, MacKinnon KC, Panger M, Bearder SK, editors. Oxford: Oxford Univ Press; 2007. pp. 592–608. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Schino G. In: Natural Conflict Resolution. Aureli F, de Waal FBM, editors. Berkeley, CA: Univ California Press; 2000. pp. 225–242. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Wahaj SA, Guse KR, Holekamp KE. Reconciliation in the spotted hyena (Crocuta crocuta) Ethology. 2001;107:1057–1074. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Cordoni G, Palagi E. Reconciliation in wolves (Canis lupus): New evidence for a comparative perspective. Ethology. 2008;114:298–308. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.de Waal FBM, van Roosmalen A. Reconciliation and consolation among chimpanzees. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1979;5:55–66. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.de Waal FBM, Aureli F. In: Reaching into Thought: The Minds of Great Apes. Russon AE, Bard KA, Taylor Parker S, editors. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ Press; 1996. pp. 80–110. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Wittig RM, Boesch C. The choice of post-conflict interactions in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Behaviour. 2003;140:1527–1559. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kutsukake N, Castles DL. Reconciliation and post-conflict third-party affiliation among wild chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Primates. 2004;45:157–165. doi: 10.1007/s10329-004-0082-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Palagi E, Cordoni G, Borgognini Tarli S. Possible roles of consolation in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Am J Phys Anthropol. 2006;129:105–111. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.20242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Koski SE, Sterck EHM. Triadic postconflict affiliation in captive chimpanzees: Does consolation console? Anim Behav. 2007;73:133–142. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Fraser ON, Aureli F. Reconciliation, consolation and postconflict behavioral specificity in chimpanzees. Am J Primatol. 2008;70:1114–1123. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Palagi E, Paoli T, Tarli SB. Reconciliation and consolation in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus) Am J Primatol. 2004;62:15–30. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Cordoni G, Palagi E, Tarli S. Reconciliation and consolation in captive western gorillas. Int J Primatol. 2006;27:1365–1382. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Mallavarapu S, Stoinski TS, Bloomsmith MA, Maple TL. Postconflict behavior in captive western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) Am J Primatol. 2006;68:789–801. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Cools AKA, van Hout AJM, Nelissen MHJ. Canine reconciliation and third-party initiated postconflict affiliation: Do peacemaking social mechanisms in dogs rival those of higher primates? Ethology. 2008;114:53–63. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Palagi E, Cordoni G. Postconflict third-party affiliation in Canis lupus: Do wolves share similarities with the great apes? Anim Behav. 2009;78:979–986. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Seed AM, Clayton NS, Emery NJ. Postconflict third-party affiliation in rooks, Corvus frugilegus. Curr Biol. 2007;17:152–158. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.11.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Fraser ON, Bugnyar T. Do ravens show consolation? Responses to distressed others. PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e10605. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010605. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Fujisawa KK, Kutsukake N, Hasegawa T. Peacemaking and consolation in Japanese preschoolers witnessing peer aggression. J Comp Psychol. 2006;120:48–57. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.120.1.48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Fraser ON, Stahl D, Aureli F. Stress reduction through consolation in chimpanzees. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:8557–8562. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0804141105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Romero T, de Waal FBM. Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) consolation: Third party identity as a window on possible function. J Comp Psychol. doi: 10.1037/a0019144. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Preston SD, de Waal FB. Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behav Brain Sci. 2002;25:1–20. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x02000018. discussion 20–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Cialdini RB, Brown SL, Lewis BP, Luce C, Neuberg SL. Reinterpreting the empathy-altruism relationship: When one into one equals oneness. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1997;73:481–494. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Langford DJ, et al. Social modulation of pain as evidence for empathy in mice. Science. 2006;312:1967–1970. doi: 10.1126/science.1128322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Hoffman ML. In: Empathy and Moral Development: Implications for Caring and Justice. Hoffman ML, editor. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ Press; 2000. pp. 197–220. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.de Waal FBM. Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of empathy. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:279–300. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.MacLean PD. Brain evolution relating to family, play, and the separation call. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1985;42:405–417. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1985.01790270095011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Zak PJ, Stanton AA, Ahmadi S. Oxytocin increases generosity in humans. PLoS ONE. 2007;2:e1128. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Eisenberg N, Lennon R. Sex differences in empathy and related capacities. Psychol Bull. 1983;94:100–131. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Baron-Cohen S, Knickmeyer RC, Belmonte MK. Sex differences in the brain: Implications for explaining autism. Science. 2005;310:819–823. doi: 10.1126/science.1115455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Han S, Fan Y, Mao L. Gender difference in empathy for pain: An electrophysiological investigation. Brain Res. 2008;1196:85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.12.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Zahn-Waxler C, Radke-Yarrow M, Wagner E, Chapman M. Development of concern for others. Dev Psychol. 1992;28:126–136. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Goodall J. The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behaviour. Cambridge, MA: Belknap; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Griffin AS, West SA. Kin selection: Fact and fiction. Trends Ecol Evol. 2002;17:15–21. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.de Vries H. The rowwise correlation between two proximity matrices and the partial rowwise correlation. Psychometrika. 1993;58:53–69. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.de Waal FBM, Luttrell LM. Mechanisms of social reciprocity in three primate species: Symmetrical relationship characteristics or cognition? Ethol Sociobiol. 1988;9:101–118. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Cords M. Post-conflict reunions and reconciliation in long-tailed macaques. Anim Behav. 1992;44:57–61. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Koyama NF. The long-term effects of reconciliation in Japanese macaques Macaca fuscata. Ethology. 2001;107:975–987. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Wittig RM, Crockford C, Wikberg E, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. Kin-mediated reconciliation substitutes for direct reconciliation in female baboons. Proc Biol Sci. 2007;274:1109–1115. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2006.0203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Slocombe KE, Zuberbühler K. Chimpanzees modify recruitment screams as a function of audience composition. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104:17228–17233. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0706741104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.de Waal FBM. Chimpanzee Politics. New York: Harper; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Arnold K, Whiten A. Grooming interactions among the chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest, Uganda: Tests of five explanatory models. Behaviour. 2003;140:519–552. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Boehm C. In: Chimpanzee Cultures. Wrangham RW, McGrew WC, de Waal FBM, Heltne PG, editors. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ Press; 1994. pp. 211–226. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Flack JC, Girvan M, de Waal FB, Krakauer DC. Policing stabilizes construction of social niches in primates. Nature. 2006;439:426–429. doi: 10.1038/nature04326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Lanzetta JT, Englis BG. Expectations of cooperation and competition and their effects on observers’ vicarious emotional responses. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989;56:543–554. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Singer T, et al. Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature. 2006;439:466–469. doi: 10.1038/nature04271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.de Waal FBM. Food sharing and reciprocal obligations among chimpanzees. J Hum Evol. 1989;18:433–459. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.de Waal FBM, van Hooff JARAM. Side-directed communication and agonistic interactions in chimpanzees. Behaviour. 1981;77:164–198. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.van Hooff JARAM. In: Social Communication and Movement. Von Cranach M, Vine I, editors. London: Academic Press; 1974. pp. 75–162. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.de Waal FBM, Yoshihara D. Reconciliation and redirected affection in rhesus monkeys. Behaviour. 1983;85:224–241. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Schall R. Estimation in generalized linear models with random effects. Biometrika. 1991;78:719–727. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.R Development Core Team . R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Hemelrijk CK. A matrix partial correlation test used in investigation of reciprocity and other social interaction patterns at group level. J Theor Biol. 1990;143:405–420. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supporting Information

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES