Skip to main content
International Journal of Endocrinology logoLink to International Journal of Endocrinology
. 2010 Jun 13;2010:326151. doi: 10.1155/2010/326151

Modulation of Sleep Homeostasis by Corticotropin Releasing Hormone in REM Sleep-Deprived Rats

Ricardo Borges Machado 1, Sergio Tufik 1, Deborah Suchecki 1,*
PMCID: PMC2902042  PMID: 20628511

Abstract

Studies have shown that sleep recovery following different protocols of forced waking varies according to the level of stress inherent to each method. Sleep deprivation activates the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and increased corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) impairs sleep. The purpose of the present study was to evaluate how manipulations of the CRH system during the sleep deprivation period interferes with subsequent sleep rebound. Throughout 96 hours of sleep deprivation, separate groups of rats were treated i.c.v. with vehicle, CRH or with alphahelical CRH9−41, a CRH receptor blocker, twice/day, at 07:00 h and 19:00 h. Both treatments impaired sleep homeostasis, especially in regards to length of rapid eye movement sleep (REM) and theta/delta ratio and induced a later decrease in NREM and REM sleep and increased waking bouts. These changes suggest that activation of the CRH system impact negatively on the homeostatic sleep response to prolonged forced waking. These results indicate that indeed, activation of the HPA axis—at least at the hypothalamic level—is capable to reduce the sleep rebound induced by sleep deprivation.

1. Introduction

One of the most interesting sleep phenomena is its homeostatic regulation, which can be manifest by the rebound in sleep that ensues after total or partial sleep deprivation, for example, increased time spent in sleep during the recovery period [1]. This phenomenon, known as sleep rebound, is also observed after deprivation of selected sleep stages, when recovery of the suppressed stage is observed [2]. However, sleep deprivation is considered a form of stress both in humans [3] and rats (for review, see [4]), although there is not complete agreement on the matter [5]. Animal models of sleep deprivation indicate that not only the loss of sleep per se, but also the method employed to induce sleep deprivation generates stress, resulting in increased activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, with elevated corticosterone (CORT) and adrenocorticotropic (ACTH) plasma levels and adrenal hypertrophy [69]. Additional data demonstrate that sleep deprivation induces increased immunoreactivity [10] and expression of hypothalamic CRH [11].

Interestingly stressors can induce specific changes in sleep patterns, including increased REM sleep after immobilization stress [12], increased slow wave sleep after social defeat stress [13], and decreased REM sleep after footshock [1416]. When associated with sleep deprivation, however, immobilization stress inhibits the homeostatic REM sleep rebound [17], whereas intermittent chronic foostshock exacerbates the expression of REM sleep during recovery, in an apparently prolactin- and CORT-dependent effect [18]. Curiously, both exogenous corticosterone administration and dexamethasone treatment inhibit sleep in unstressed rats [19, 20] or after immobilization stress [21], indicating that other mediators participate in this phenomenon. Corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH), the primary orchestrator of the endocrine stress response, is synthesized in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus [22] and is a major regulator of waking in rats [2325]. It increases neuronal excitability and convulsions [26], and stimulates the locus coeruleus noradrenergic neurons [27]. CRH receptors are densely distributed in basal prosencephalic areas, thalamus, hypothalamus, mesencephalus, brainstem, and pons [28], areas which are involved in cerebral activation and waking maintenance [29].

The pioneering study by Ehlers and coworkers [25] on the effects of CRH on sleep in rats demonstrates that low doses of this peptide reduce slow wave sleep (NREM) and low frequency activity. However, in high doses, CRH exhibits an opposite effect and reduces fast frequency activity (32–64 Hz). In human beings, however, peripheral administration of CRH does not significantly alter REM sleep [30]. Moreover, CRH modulates the homeostatic rebound induced by sleep deprivation, by increasing REM sleep rebound when administered immediately after the sleep deprivation procedure [31]. In contrast, α-helical-CRH9−41 (αhCRH), a CRH receptor blocker, prevents immobilization stress-induced sleep rebound and does not influence sleep in stress-free conditions [32].

In an attempt to determine the role of hormones of the HPA axis, we recently demonstrated that 96 hours of REM sleep deprivation together with repeated administrations of metyrapone, a corticosterone synthesis inhibitor, impaired sleep deprivation-induced NREM compensation [33]. We then hypothesized that increased production of CRH elicited by removal of the CORT negative feedback signal at the hypothalamic level [34] could be, at least in part, responsible for this effect. If this hypothesis were correct, then i.c.v. administration of CRH in sleep-deprived rats should produce similar results to those obtained with metyrapone, and α-helical-CRH9−41 and should increase sleep rebound in these animals.

2. Methods

2.1. Subjects

Male adult Wistar rats (350–450 g) from the animal facility of the Department of Psychobiology—UNIFESP—were used (eight to ten animals per group) and prior approval from the Ethics Research Committee of Universidade Federal de São Paulo was obtained in accordance with international guidelines for care in animal research (CEP 0125/04). Constant 12 hours light-dark cycle (fluorescent white lamps-lights on at 7:00 h) and 20–22°C temperature were maintained in all experimental rooms throughout the experimental protocol. Rats were allowed free access to food and water.

2.2. Electrophysiological Procedures

Under ketamine-xylazine anaesthesia (9.0–10.5 mg/kg, i.p.), rats were fitted with electrodes to monitor the sleep-wake cycle: two bipolar electrodes placed ipsilaterally with stainless-steel micro-screws (0.9 mm in diameter) were used for EEG monitoring: one pair on the right lateral parietoparietal (for minimum theta activity EEG) and the other on the left medial frontoparietal (for maximum theta activity EEG) areas [35, 36]. One pair of insulated nickel-chromium flexible fine wire electrodes was implanted in the dorsal neck muscle for EMG recording. For intracerebroventricular—i.c.v. injections, a 22-gauge stainless steel guide cannula (constructed from hypodermic needle, Becton Dickinson, Brazil, cut at 10 mm in length) was inserted 1.0 mm posterior to bregma, 1.4 mm lateral to midline, and 3.4 mm ventral to dura membrane, within the EEG electrodes. The guide cannula was covered with an easily removable lid adopted from a fine stainless steel wire, which was inserted tightly onto the guide cannula hole. After the surgical procedure, antibiotics (Pentabiótico Fort-Dodge, Brazil) and sodium diclofenac were administered and the animal was allowed to recover from surgery for 15 days. Three days before the beginning of experiments, lateral ventricular cannula placement was verified by assessing the drinking response elicited to up to 5 nmol angiotensin II administration. After the multiple injections schedules, the ventricular cannula placement was confirmed postmortem by injection 3 μL of methylene blue followed by microscope visualization. Animals were habituated to the cables and to the recording environment for 3 days before baseline recording. Baseline sleep was recorded on two consecutive days (2 × 24 hours) and the parameters are represented by the average of these two days. After the baseline recording, in the period that preceded REM sleep deprivation (REMSD), animals were adapted to the sleep deprivation chambers for 30 minutes per day for three consecutive days.

Electrophysiological signals were recorded on a digital polygraph (Neurofax QP 223 A Nihon Kohden Co., Tokyo, Japan). After conventional amplification, the EEG signals were conditioned through analog filters, using cut off frequencies of 1.0 Hz and 35.0 Hz and were then sampled at 200 Hz using a 16 bit A/D converter. Recordings were displayed on 10 s epochs and submitted off-line to visual scoring routine, as described previously [37]. In summary the stages of wake-sleep were defined as follows: (a) waking (low voltage and high frequency EEG, whereas EMG displays high voltage during active waking or low during voltage quiet waking); (b) NREM sleep (EEG high voltage within slow waves and spindles, also classified, separately, in low—between 2.0 and 3.0 μV, and high amplitudes—from 3.0 μV on, and low EMG amplitude); and (3) REM sleep (low EEG voltage with prominent theta rhythm on medial EEG deviation accompanied of the very low EMG activity). Each 10 s epoch was characterized by the predominant wave pattern present in more than half of the epoch. In some periods, interference or noise made it impossible to characterize the behavioral state and a critical evaluation of the previous and subsequent periods was made. The percentage of 10 s periods excluded from sleep scoring was below 9%.

The parameters used for sleep analysis were the following: total sleep time, total NREM time (considering low and high amplitude fractions) REM time and bouts, and total wake time—active and quiet periods—and episodes of waking (2.0 minutes). Fast Fourier Transform (Hanning window) was computed on 256 points for each 10 s epochs (corresponding to each vigilance state) with a resolution of 0.78 Hz (null value was attributed to the remaining time). Nonoverlapping bands were set giving 0.5 Hz bins from 1.0 to 5.0 Hz, and 1.0 Hz bins from 5.1 Hz to 25.0 Hz; those above 25.0 Hz were discarded from the analysis. EEG epochs containing noise or artifacts (those that did not allow doubtful behavioral state classification) were excluded from the analysis by visual inspection and/or spectral tools (e.g., if average power exceeded 2000 μV2 over a 1.0–25.0 Hz frequency range). Slow wave activity was calculated as mean power density on 1.0–4.0 Hz band (delta) and the theta-delta ratio, dividing the power density of the fast theta (6.0–9.0 Hz) band by the mean power density on the delta band. Although two ipsilateral bipolar electrodes were used for sleep scoring, only the lateral parietoparietal deviation was used for spectral data analysis, except for the theta-delta ratio, when the two deviations were employed (the lateral to delta and the medial frontoparietal to theta activity measures).

2.3. REM Sleep Deprivation (REMSD) Procedure and Drug Administration

Sleep deprivation was accomplished by the single platform method, in which each animal was placed onto a narrow cylindrical platform, 6.5 cm in diameter, surrounded by water to about 1 cm below the platform surface. This method is well known to selectively suppress REM sleep; however, it also produces partial NREM deprivation, with 37–50% reduction from baseline levels [38, 39].

CRH (Sigma, USA): 3 μg/animal [22, 40, 41] or αhCRH (Sigma, USA): 20 μg/animal [4244] were diluted in artificial cerebrospinal fluid—ACSF (NaCl 127 mM, KCl 2.5 mM, MgCl2 0.9 mM, Na2HPO4 1.2 mM, CaCl2 1.3 mM, NaHCO3 21 mM, C6H12O6 3.4 mM, and pH 7.3, sterile- and pyrogen- free) and administered twice/day, at 7:00 h and 19:00 h, in addition to a single injection at the end of the REM deprivation period, making a total of nine administrations. Control animals were treated with ACSF under the same scheme. Final volume of each i.c.v. injection was 3-4 μL, delivered at a flow rate of 1.5 μL/minute with an injection cannula, made from another 30-gauge dentist needle (Becton Dickinson, Brazil, cut at 25 mm in length, but inserted into guide cannula up to 10.5 mm limit) connected to a polypropylene tubing (PE 10, Becton Dickinson, USA) which, in turn, was linked on another terminal end to a 10 μL microsyringe (Hamilton, USA), placed onto a automatic microinfusion pump (Insight, Brazil). Prophylactic aseptic techniques were strictly employed during all administrations and less than 20% of animals were not used during the chronic experiments because of signs of infection (e.g., fever, weight loss, apathy, wet fur, and poor physical appearance). After four days under this protocol, rats were returned to sleep freely in their individual home cages (recovery period). Over the subsequent three days, the rats were continuously monitored.

2.4. Plasma Hormone Determination

Trunk blood was obtained by decapitation, approximately 2 hours after the last administration from matched groups, run simultaneously with the sleep study. During this period, the REMSD animals were not allowed to sleep (they were put back into the deprivation chambers). Blood was collected in chilled K2EDTA (0.46 mM, e.g., 7.5% solution at a volume of 0.1 mL diluted in 5 mL of blood)-containing vials, centrifuged at 2300 rpm at 4°C for 20 minutes, and plasma was collected and frozen at −20°C for further analysis. Plasma ACTH was determined by sequential immunometric assay (DPC Immulite, Los Angeles, CA) and the sensitivity of the method is 9 pg/mL, and intra- and interassay variations are 9.4% and 9.6%, respectively. Corticosterone levels were assayed by specific radioimmunoassay (INC Biomedicals, Costa Mesa, CA). The sensitivity of the assay is 1.25 ng/mL and the intra- and inter-assay variations are, respectively 6.5% and 7.1%, as informed by the manufacturer. All samples were assayed in duplicate.

2.5. Statistics

Hormonal data were analyzed by a two-way ANOVA, with main factor Group (CTL—control home cage and REM sleep deprivation—REMSD) and Treatment (ACSF, CRH, and αhCRH). Sleep parameters were analyzed by a two-way ANOVA for repeated measures, with main factors Treatment (ACSF, CRH, and αhCRH) and Day (repeated measure: Baseline and Recovery days 1 [R1], 2 [R2], and 3 [R3]). The spectra power density was analyzed by Student's t tests for independent samples, every 12 hours period, for each behavioral state separately. The theta-delta ratio was analyzed by covariance analyses (ANCOVA) where the baseline index was the predictive factor and treatments, the independent variable. All EEG data were analyzed during the light and dark phases, separately. Posthoc analysis was performed by the Newman-Keuls test. The level of significance was set at P ≤ .05.

3. Results

3.1. HPA Axis Hormones (Figure 1)

Figure 1.

Figure 1

ACTH and CORT Plasma Levels. CTL, Control; ACSF, artificial cerebrospinal fluid; CRH, corticotrophin-releasing hormone; αhCRH, alpha-helical CRH9−41 *- different from CTL+ACSF, #- different from PSD+ACSF, and - different from CTL+CRH, - different from CTL+αhCRH; ANOVA, followed by Newman-Keuls test, P ≤ .05.

ACTH —

There was a significant interaction between Group and Treatment (F 2,54 = 0.67, P ≤ .0005), in which REMSD + ACSF animals showed higher ACTH levels than their CTL counterparts (P ≤ .05), whereas REMSD+ αhCRH animals exhibited lower levels than CTL+ αhCRH ones (P ≤ .05). In addition, in CTL animals, both CRH and αhCRH resulted in higher ACTH levels than ACSF administration (P ≤ .0005).

Corticosterone —

Again, a significant interaction between Group and Treatment (F 2,53 = 19.52, P ≤ .00005) was found. Newman-Keuls analysis of this interaction showed that all groups exhibited higher corticosterone plasma levels than CTL + ACSF animals (CTL+CRH: 853.3%, P ≤ .0005; CTL + αhCRH: 230.5%, P ≤ .05; REMSD + ACSF: 115.4%, P ≤ .05; REMSD + CRH: 575.6%, P ≤ .0005 and REMSD + αhCRH: 747.6%, P ≤ .0005). However, CRH treatment in REMSD rats resulted in lower CORT levels than in CTL rats (−29.1%, P ≤ .01), although they were still higher than REMSD+ACSF animals (213.6%, P ≤ .0005). On the other hand, αhCRH treatment led to higher CORT levels in REMSD than in CTL (156.4%, P ≤ .0005) and REMSD + ACSF rats (293.5%, P ≤ .0005). Finally, CORT concentrations were lower in CTL + αhCRH than in CTL+CRH (−65.33%, P ≤ .0005).

3.2. Sleep Parameters

3.2.1. Total Sleep Time (Figure 2)

Figure 2.

Figure 2

NREM Sleep. Data were obtained in recording periods of approximately 11 hours during the light and dark phases. ACSF, artificial cerebrospinal fluid; CRH, corticotrophin-releasing hormone; αhCRH, alpha helical CRH9−41; REMSD, 96 hours REM sleep deprivation period; Rec. Recovery period. The white back panels indicate the light phase and the shaded ones, the dark phase. *- different from baseline, effects of day are indicated by connecting lines above the symbols. ANOVA, followed by Newman-Keuls, P ≤ .05.

Light Phase —

A main effect of Day (F 3,63 = 2.52, P ≤ .00001) was found. Reduced sleep time was observed on the 2nd(10.2%, P ≤ .05) and 3rd recovery days when compared to baseline (22.9%, P ≤ .0005).

Dark Phase —

Again main effect of Day was observed (F 3,63 = 13.89, P ≤ .00001). Animals showed 24.4% increased sleep time on the first recovery day (P ≤ .001).

3.2.2. Total NREM Time (Figure 2)

Light Phase —

A main effect of Day was detected (F 3,63 = 6.84, P ≤ .0005). Total NREM was reduced (20.3%) on the 3rd recovery day, relative to baseline.

Dark Phase —

An effect of Day emerged (F 3,63 = 9.05, P ≤ .00005) and the rats showed more NREM (16.8%, P ≤ .01) than baseline.

3.2.3. REM Sleep Time (Figure 3)

Figure 3.

Figure 3

REM Sleep. Data were obtained in recording periods of approximately 11 hours during the light and dark phases. ACSF, artificial cerebrospinal fluid; CRH, corticotrophin-releasing hormone; αhCRH, alpha helical CRH9−41; REMSD, 96 hours REM sleep deprivation period; Rec. Recovery period. The white back panels indicate the light phase and the shaded ones, the dark phase. *- different from baseline, - different from ACSF group. Main effects of day are indicated by connecting lines above the symbols. ANOVA, followed by Newman-Keuls, P ≤ .05.

Light Phase —

A main effect of Day was found (F 3,63 = 22.70, P ≤ .00001). Animals showed an increase on this phase (53.3%, P ≤ .0005) during the first recovery day, whereas a reduction of REM was also observed during the last recovery day (32.8%, P ≤ .02).

Dark Phase —

Again, a main effect of Day was found (F 3,63 = 15.32, P ≤ .00001) and the animals showed an increase of 71.2% (P ≤ .0005) during first recovery day, when compared to baseline amounts.

3.2.4. REM Bouts (Figure 3)

Light Phase —

A main effect of Day was found (F 3,63 = 11.29, P ≤ .00001) and a decrease of REM bouts was detected on the 3rd recovery day (39.3%, P ≤ .0005).

Dark Phase —

Again, a main effect of Day was detected (F 3,63 = 7.83, P ≤ .0005). During the first recovery day, animals displayed more bouts than baseline sleep (P ≤ .01).

3.2.5. Mean Length of REM Episodes (Figure 3)

Light Phase —

A two-way interaction between Day and Treatment was found (F 6,63 = 3.59, P ≤ .005). Posthoc analysis showed that REM episodes were longer after ACSF administration than at baseline (124.2%, P ≤ .0005). Administration of CRH (45.5%, P ≤ .005) and αhCRH (40.0%, P ≤ .005) shortened the length of REM episodes during the first recovery day compared to ACSF-treated rats.

Dark Phase —

There was a Day effect (F 3,57 = 4.10, P ≤ .02), in which the animals showed longer REM episodes on the first recovery night (28.7%, P ≤ .05).

3.2.6. Total Wake Time (Figure 4)

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Awake. Data were obtained in recording periods of approximately 11 hours during the light and dark phases. ACSF, artificial cerebrospinal fluid; CRH, corticotrophin-releasing hormone; αhCRH, alpha helical CRH9−41; REMSD, 96 hours REM sleep deprivation period; Rec, Recovery period. The white back panels indicate the light phase and the shaded ones, the dark phase. *- different from baseline. Main effects of day are indicating by connecting lines above the symbols. ANOVA/Newman-Keuls, P ≤ .05.

Light Phase —

A two-way interaction between Day and Treatment was detected (F 6,63 = 3.53, P ≤ .005) and post-hoc tests revealed that CRH-treated animals spent more time awake during the last recovery day than in baseline (41.8%, P ≤ .01).

Dark Phase —

A two-way interaction was found between Day and Treatment (F 6,63 = 2.94, P ≤ .02) and both CRH- and αhCRH-treated rats showed less total wake during the first recovery day than on baseline (19.1%, P ≤ .05 and 21.4%, P ≤ .05, resp.).

3.2.7. Number of Awakenings (Figure 4)

Light Phase —

A two-way interaction was found (F 6,63 = 2.61, P ≤ .05). Post-hoc tests showed that CRH-treated rats displayed more events of awakenings during the last recovery day relative to baseline (62.6%, P ≤ .05).

Dark Phase —

No changes were found.

3.3. Spectral Data

3.3.1. Spectral Power during NREM (Figure 5)

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Spectral Power During Total NREM on the 2nd (a) and 3rd (b) Recovery Dark Phases. Results are expressed as percentage of ACSF-treated group, obtained from the mean power of each spectra band. ACSF, artificial cerebrospinal fluid; CRH, corticotrophin-releasing hormone; αhCRH, alpha helical CRH9−41. Grey line above graphics indicates differences of αhCRH from ACSF and black ones, the difference between the CRH and αhCRH treatments. Student's t tests, P ≤ .05.

Light Phase —

During the third recovery day, CRH-treated animals showed reductions in the 19.01–20.0 Hz (19.2%, P ≤ .05) and 20.1–21.0 Hz bands (18.4%, P ≤ .05) when compared to ACSF-treated rats. Moreover, increased power above ACSF animals was observed in the 1.6–2.0 Hz band (53.3%, P ≤ .02) in CRH- and in the 1.0–1.5 Hz band (34.8%, P ≤ .05) in αhCRH-treated groups.

Dark Phase —

On the second recovery day, αhCRH led to increased power in the 1.0–3.0 Hz bands (average of 4 bins of 0.5 Hz each = 53.7%, P ≤ .05), when compared to ACSF group and increased all bands above 6.0 Hz when compared to CRH-treated rats (average of 19 bins of 1.0 Hz each = 28.5%, P ≤ .05). Finally, during the last recovery day, αhCRH produced an increase in all bands above 7.0 Hz when compared to CRH-treated animals (average of 18 bins of 1.0 Hz each = 27.1%, P ≤ .05).

3.3.2. Theta-Delta Ratio (Figure 6)

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Theta/Delta. Theta/delta ratio is shown as mean of the total power in fast the θ (6.6–9.0 Hz) band divided by total power in fast δ (2.5–4.0 Hz) band, computed throughout ~11 hours period in the light and dark phases of the recovery period. ACSF, artificial cerebrospinal fluid; CRH, corticotrophin-releasing hormone; αhCRH, alpha helical CRH9−41; AW, active wake; QW, quiet wake; L NREM, low amplitude NREM sleep; H NREM, high amplitude NREM sleep; PS, REM sleep; Rec. Recovery period. The white panels indicate the light phase and the gray ones, the dark phase. *- different from baseline, #-different from ACSF group, and - different from αhCRH group. Main effects of sleep parameter are indicated by connecting lines above the bars. ANCOVA, followed by the Newman-Keuls test, P ≤ .05.

Light Phase —

A main effect of Treatment was detected for active wake (AW) (F 2,20 = 4.45, P ≤ .05) and post-hoc analysis showed that CRH animals showed reductions of θ/δ relative to ACSF (12.3%, P ≤ .05) and αhCHR rats (17.6%, P ≤ .01). During Quiet Wake (QW), effect of Treatment was again revealed (F 2,19 = 3.94, P ≤ .05), and CRH-displayed smaller θ/δ ratio than αhCHR-treated animals (16.8%, P ≤ .01). No effects were found in Low-Amplitude NREM (L-NREM) or in High-Amplitude NREM (H-NREM) during the recovery period. During REM sleep, a significant effect of Treatment (F2,20 = 7.14, P ≤ .005) and an interaction between Day and Treatment (F4,40 = 2.90, P ≤ .05) were found. In regards to the Treatment factor, CRH-treated animals showed a smaller θ/δ ratio than ACSF- (15.6%, P ≤ .01) and αhCHR-treated animals (22.1%, P ≤ .005). No post-hoc differences were detected for the interaction.

Dark Phase —

During AW a main effect of Day was observed (F 2,40 = 12.61, P ≤ .00005); however, the post-hoc analysis did not revealed any differences among the days. For QW, main effects of Treatment (F 2,20 = 3.59, P ≤ .05) and Day (F 2,40 = 10.27, P ≤ .0005) were found. The Newman-Keuls test showed that CRH treatment reduced θ/δ ratio when compared to ACSF (9.7%, P ≤ .05) and αhCHR (15.0%, P ≤ .005) treatments. In regards to the effect of Day, there was a reduction of θ/δ on the 3rd compared to the 1st recovery day (6.1%, P ≤ .05). No effects were detected in L-NREM, whereas a main effect of Day was detected in H-NREM (F 2.40 = 3.35, P ≤ .05) and the post-hoc analysis showed that the θ/δ ratio was reduced on the 2nd and 3rd recovery days (9.7%, P ≤ .0001 and 13.7%, P ≤ .0001, resp.). During REM sleep, a main effect of Treatment was detected (F 2,18 = 4.00, P ≤ .05) and CRH treatment reduced the θ/δ ratio compared to ACSF (12.4%, P ≤ .05) and to αhCHR (17.1%, P ≤ .01).

4. Discussion

The main results of the present study can be summarized as follows: (1) both CRH and αhCRH increased ACTH and CORT secretions, although these were lower in REMSD than in control rats; (2) both peptides impaired sleep in later periods of the recovery sleep, but did not interfere with the immediate sleep rebound, except for a reduction in the length of REM sleep episodes; (3) rats treated with αhCRH exhibited more high frequency bands in NREM than rats treated with CRH during the last dark phase of the recovery period; and (4) CRH-treated rats exhibited lower theta/delta ratio, indicating an impairment of the homeostatic sleep rebound.

As expected, REMSD resulted in increased secretion of ACTH and CORT levels, relative to control, nondeprived rats. Repeated administration of CRH, during REMSD, however, led to opposite effects, for example, levels in REMSD were lower than those of control rats. This result can be explained by the well-known stimulating effect that REMSD exerts on the CRH-producing neurons, with increased mRNA [11] and immunoreactivity of CRH [10] in the PVN, which could lead to lower density of CRH receptors. In fact, a previous study showed that REMSD results in lower CRH receptor density in the pituitary and striatum [45]. Despite that, no major effects in the sleep macrostructure were observed during rebound, except for a reduction of the length of REMS episodes.

We found that αhCRH reduced ACTH, but increased CORT release by almost 3-fold in REMSD rats compared to CSF-treated animals. Alpha-helical CRH-induced attenuation of the ACTH response to REMSD resembled those of a previous study with restraint stress [46]. Some studies indicate that in order to achieve an effective blockade of the behavioral stress response, αhCRH dose must be higher than 25 μg (in the present study the dose was 20 μg/animal) [42, 47, 48], even though ACTH and CORT secretions may still not be completely suppressed [42]. Some studies even indicate that αhCRH, in doses higher than 25 μg, acts as an agonist of the type 1 CRH receptor (CRH-R1), leading to behavioral and hormonal responses similar to those elicited by CRH [4952]. αhCRH antagonist action is evident in stressful, but not under basal conditions [5355], possibly due to the fact that it binds more efficiently to CRH-R2 receptors [56], whereas CRH-R1 is the predominant type in the pituitary [5759]. Interestingly, the distribution of CRH receptors in sleep-related areas indicates that CRH-R1 is densely located in the laterodorsal tegmental nucleus and CRH-R2, in the dorsal raphe, without any overlapping [59], suggesting that both receptors types may be involved in sleep regulation.

Increased NREMS is seen with higher doses of αhCRH (i.c.v., 25 μg/rat) within two hours of drug administration [43], whereas a higher dose (i.c.v., 100 μg/rat) prevents immobilization stress- and sleep deprivation-induced sleep rebound [32, 60]. It is possible that the dose of αhCRH used in the present study, also infused i.c.v., was not high enough to produce the same sleep changes as reported by Gonzalez and Valatx's [60] paper, however, in their study, the compound was administered every two hours throughout the deprivation period, mounting to a much larger dose in a much shorter period of sleep deprivation. Moreover, this schedule of administration is likely to maintain receptors blocked throughout the entire sleep deprivation period.

The most remarkable effect of CRH and αhCRH treatments in sleep macrostructure was a shortening of the length of REMS episodes, compared with vehicle-infused rats, during the first light period of recovery sleep, indicating and impairment of REM sleep regulation. The influence of CRH on REM sleep appears to be bimodal. On the one hand, intra-hippocampal CRH infusion reduces theta rhythm by acting on both CRH receptors, present in hippocampal CA1 field and dentate gyrus [61]. On the other hand, overexpression of CRH leads to more spontaneous and sleep deprivation-induced REM sleep [62]. At present, it is not possible to determine how both drugs, acting predominantly through different CRH receptors, could lead to similar results in REM sleep regulation. One possibility may involve changes in serotonergic transmission, since it has been shown that the raphe nucleus projects heavily to the hippocampus and medial septum [63, 64] and that stimulation of this region suppresses theta rhythm in the EEG, regardless of the activity in the septal area [65, 66]. Lesions of the raphe may result in permanent hippocampal theta rhythm [67] and infusion of 5-HT1A agonist in the dorsomedial raphe impairs hippocampal and cortical theta rhythm [68]. Considering that the predominant CRH receptor in the raphe is the low affinity CRH-R2 [61] and that activation of these receptors with high or repeated αhCRH administrations lead to serotonin release in this area [69], there is a possibility that the treatment used in the present study might have caused an increase in serotonergic activity, which impaired theta rhythm and, consequently, REM sleep.

Regarding sleep microstructure, during NREM sleep, αhCRH produced an increase in the high frequency bands during the last two dark phases, compared to CRH, suggesting opposite homeostatic and late circadian responses exerted by these peptides. Thus, αhCRH-treated rats appeared to exhibit shallower NREM sleep than CRH-treated rats, considering that high frequency bands are predominant during waking. However, shallower NREM sleep during the active period of rats indicates normal circadian rhythm and, therefore, a return to homeostasis. CRH also reduces low frequency (1.0–6.0 Hz) spectral potency in rats [25] and in humans, there is an increase in waking and delta sleep EEG sigma band (11.0–15.0 Hz) [70]. CRH receptors are present in several thalamic nuclei [71, 72], although they inhibits spontaneous activity of these neurons [73]. Activation of these receptors might result in inactivity of reticular cells, responsible for the generation of synchronization of low frequency waves in the cortical EEG [74] and reduction of the low frequency power spectrum [25]. CRH deleterious effects on sleep appear to be mediated by CRH-R1, since R129919, a specific CRH-R1 antagonist increases slow wave sleep in depressed patients [75]. This may explain why αhCRH did not affect low frequency power spectrum, since this substance blocks preferentially the type 2 CRH receptor. Moreover, CRH mRNA expression on the posterior nucleus of the thalamus is augmented during the rat resting period [76, 77]. Activation of this nucleus is related to the generation of high frequency β waves and suppression of δ and spindle activity during slow wave sleep [78, 79]. Collectively, these data could explain the increased potency of EEG high frequency bands.

A relatively recent index is the theta/delta (θ/δ) ratio, which represents a marker of homeostatic sleep compensation and is known to be increased after sleep deprivation [80, 81], being characteristic of each sleep phase [82]. In humans, for instance, this increase takes place during the dark period, which corresponds to the resting period [83]. The reduction of θ/δ ratio in CRH-treated rats during waking and REM sleep suggests that the homeostatic compensation is flawed, because during REM sleep decreased delta and/or increased theta activity is supposed to occur.

Theta rhythm is one of the most prominent features of REM sleep in the rat [35, 84], which is generated by cell populations that flow to CA1 stratum oriens and dentage gyrus stratum molecular [85, 86]. The reduction of theta/delta rhythm in CRH-treated rats might have occurred due to a reduction of theta potency, rather than an increase in delta power. The reason for this conclusion is threefold: (1) the reduction of the index took place during waking and REM sleep, when theta predominates; (2) there were no changes during low and high NREM sleep, when delta predominates; and (3) as a general rule, low frequencies (1.0–5.0 Hz) were unchanged during NREM sleep.

In conclusion, chronic CRH administration during REM sleep deprivation impaired the homeostatic compensation phenomenon, likely due to its excitatory action on neuronal tissue. This impairment occurred in the sleep macrostructure, with shortening of REM sleep episodes, as well as in the microstructure, in later phases of the recovery period. Because αhCRH acts at the CRH-R2 and may have agonistic properties when repeatedly administered in high doses, it produced paradoxical changes on hormone secretion and sleep homeostasis, being, sometimes, similar to CRH.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Dr. Teunis van Beelen for EDF converter software. This work was supported by Associação Fundo de Incentivo à Psicofarmacologia (AFIP) and Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo (FAPESP/CEPID# 98/14303-3). Ricardo Borges Machado is the recipient of PhD fellowship from Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo (FAPESP# 04/02213-2). Deborah Suchecki is the recipient of a scholarship from the National Research Council (CNPq).

References

  • 1.Rechtschaffen A, Bergmann BM, Gilliland MA, Bauer K. Effects of method, duration, and sleep stage on rebounds from sleep deprivation in the rat. Sleep. 1999;22(1):11–31. doi: 10.1093/sleep/22.1.11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Dement W. The effect of dream deprivation. Science. 1960;131:1705–1707. doi: 10.1126/science.131.3415.1705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Knutson KL, Spiegel K, Penev P, Van Cauter E. The metabolic consequences of sleep deprivation. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2007;11(3):163–178. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Meerlo P, Sgoifo A, Suchecki D. Restricted and disrupted sleep: effects on autonomic function, neuroendocrine stress systems and stress responsivity. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2008;12(3):197–210. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Rechtschaffen A, Bergmann BM. Sleep deprivation in the rat: an update of the 1989 paper. Sleep. 2002;25(1):18–24. doi: 10.1093/sleep/25.1.18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Tobler I, Murison R, Ursin R, Ursin H, Borbely AA. The effect of sleep deprivation and recovery sleep on plasma corticosterone in the rat. Neuroscience Letters. 1983;35:297–300. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(83)90333-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Coenen AML, Van Luijtelaar ELJM. Stress induced by three procedures of deprivation of paradoxical sheep. Physiology and Behavior. 1985;35(4):501–504. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(85)90130-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Suchecki D, Tufik S. Social stability attenuates the stress in the modified multiple platform method for paradoxical sleep deprivation in the rat. Physiology and Behavior. 2000;68(3):309–316. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(99)00181-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Suchecki D, Lobo LL, Hipolide DC, Tufik S. Increased ACTH and corticosterone secretion induced by different methods of paradoxical sleep deprivation. Journal of Sleep Research. 1998;7(4):276–281. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.1998.00122.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Galvao MDOL, Sinigaglia-Coimbra R, Kawakami SE, Tufik S, Suchecki D. Paradoxical sleep deprivation activates hypothalamic nuclei that regulate food intake and stress response. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(8):1176–1183. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Koban M, Wei WL, Hoffman GE. Changes in hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing hormone, neuropeptide Y, and proopiomelanocortin gene expression during chronic rapid eye movement sleep deprivation of rats. Endocrinology. 2006;147(1):421–431. doi: 10.1210/en.2005-0695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Rampin C, Cespuglio R, Chastrette N, Jouvet M. Immobilisation stress induces a paradoxical sleep rebound in rat. Neuroscience Letters. 1991;126(2):113–118. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(91)90532-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Meerlo P, Pragt BJ, Daan S. Social stress induces high intensity sleep in rats. Neuroscience Letters. 1997;225(1):41–44. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(97)00180-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Pawlyk AC, Jha SK, Brennan FX, Morrison AR, Ross RJ. A rodent model of sleep disturbances in posttraumatic stress disorder: the role of context after fear conditioning. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57(3):268–277. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Palma BD, Suchecki D, Tufik S. Differential effects of acute cold and footshock on the sleep of rats. Brain Research. 2000;861(1):97–104. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(00)02024-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Sanford LD, Tang X, Ross RJ, Morrison AR. Influence of shock training and explicit fear-conditioned cues on sleep architecture in mice: strain comparison. Behavior Genetics. 2003;33(1):43–58. doi: 10.1023/a:1021051516829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Altman JL, Whitehead WE, Rechtschaffen A. Effects of 5 hours of restraint stress on subsequent sleep in rat. Psychonomic Science. 1972;26:152–154. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Machado RB, Tufik S, Suchecki D. Chronic stress during paradoxical sleep deprivation increases paradoxical sleep rebound: association with prolactin plasma levels and brain serotonin content. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008;33:1211–1224. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Vazquez-Palacios G, Retana-Marquez S, Bonilla-Jaime H, Velazquez-Moctezuma J. Further definition of the effect of corticosterone on the sleep-wake pattern in the male rat. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 2001;70(2-3):305–310. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(01)00620-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bradbury MJ, Dement WC, Edgar DM. Effects of adrenalectomy and subsequent corticosterone replacement on rat sleep state and EEG power spectra. American Journal of Physiology. 1998;275:R555–R565. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1998.275.2.R555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Marinesco S, Bonnet C, Cespuglio R. Influence of stress duration on the sleep rebound induced by immobilization in the rat: a possible role for corticosterone. Neuroscience. 1999;92(3):921–933. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(99)00045-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Rivier C, Brownstein M, Spiess J. In vivo corticotropin releasing factor-induced secretion of adrenocorticotropin, β-endorphin, and corticosterone. Endocrinology. 1982;110(1):272–278. doi: 10.1210/endo-110-1-272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Opp MR. Rat strain differences suggest a role for corticotropin-releasing hormone in modulating sleep. Physiology and Behavior. 1997;63(1):67–74. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(97)00390-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Chang F-C, Opp MR. Corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) as a regulator of waking. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2001;25(5):445–453. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(01)00024-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ehlers CL, Reed TK, Henriksen SJ. Effects of corticotropin-releasing factor and growth hormone-releasing factor on sleep and activity in rats. Neuroendocrinology. 1986;42:467–474. doi: 10.1159/000124489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ehlers CL, Henriksen SJ, Wang M, Rivier J, Vale W, Bloom FE. Corticotropin releasing factor produces increases in brain excitability and convulsive seizures in rats. Brain Research. 1983;278:332–336. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90266-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Valentino RJ, Foote SL, Aston-Jones G. Corticotropin-releasing factor activates noradrenergic neurons of the locus coeruleus. Brain Research. 1983;270(2):363–367. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90615-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.De Souza EB. Corticotropin-releasing factor receptors in the rat central nervous systemml: characterization and regional distribution. Journal of Neuroscience. 1987;7(1):88–100. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.07-01-00088.1987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Jones BE. Brain mechanisms of sleep-wake states. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC, editors. Principles and Pratice of Sleep Medicine. Philadelphia, Pa, USA: WB Saunders; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Born J, Spath-Schwalbe E, Schwakenhofer H, Kern W, Fehm HL. Influences of corticotropin-releasing hormone, adrenocorticotropin, and cortisol on sleep in normal man. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. 1989;68(5):904–911. doi: 10.1210/jcem-68-5-904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Marrosu F, Gessa GL, Giagheddu M, Fratta W. Corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) increases paradoxical sleep (PS) rebound in PS-deprived rats. Brain Research. 1990;515(1-2):315–318. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90614-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Gonzalez MM, Valatx JL. Effect of intracerebroventricular administration of α-helical CRH9−41 on the sleep/waking cycle in rats under normal conditions or after subjection to an acute stressful stimulus. Journal of Sleep Research. 1997;6(3):164–170. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.1997.00042.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Machado RB, Tufik S, Suchecki D. Metyrapone and corticosterone decrease slow wave sleep in paradoxical sleep-deprived rats. Sleep. 2008;31:A109–A109. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Rotllant D, Armario A. A single dose of metyrapone caused long-term dysregulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in the rat. Neuroscience. 2005;130(2):427–434. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Timo-Iaria C, Negrao N, Schmidek WR, Hoshino K, de Menezes CEL, da Rocha TL. Phases and states of sleep in the rat. Physiology and Behavior. 1970;5(9):1057–1062. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(70)90162-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Rosenberg RS, Bergmann BM, Rechtschaffen A. Variations in slow wave activity during sleep in the rat. Physiology and Behavior. 1976;17(6):931–938. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(76)90011-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Machado RB, Suchecki D, Tufik S. Sleep homeostasis in rats assessed by a long-term intermittent paradoxical sleep deprivation protocol. Behavioural Brain Research. 2005;160(2):356–364. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Grahnstedt S, Ursin R. Platform sleep deprivation affects deep slow wave sleep in addition to REM sleep. Behavioural Brain Research. 1985;18(3):233–239. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(85)90031-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Machado RB, Hipolide DC, Benedito-Silva AA, Tufik S. Sleep deprivation induced by the modified multiple platform technique: quantification of sleep loss and recovery. Brain Research. 2004;1004(1-2):45–51. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.01.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Opp MR. Corticotropin-releasing hormone involvement in stressor-induced alterations in sleep and in the regulation of waking. Advances in Neuroimmunology. 1995;5(2):127–143. doi: 10.1016/0960-5428(95)00004-l. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Price ML, Lucki I. Regulation of serotonin release in the lateral septum and striatum by corticotropin-releasing factor. Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21(8):2833–2841. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-08-02833.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Korte SM, Korte-Bouws GAH, Bohus B, Koob GF. Effect of corticotropin-releasing factor antagonist on behavioral and neuroendocrine responses during exposure to defensive burying paradigm in rats. Physiology and Behavior. 1994;56(1):115–120. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90268-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Chang F-C, Opp MR. Blockade of corticotropin-releasing hormone receptors reduces spontaneous waking in the rat. American Journal of Physiology. 1998;275:R793–R802. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1998.275.3.R793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Chang F-C, Opp MR. Pituitary CRH receptor blockade reduces waking in the rat. Physiology and Behavior. 1999;67(5):691–696. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(99)00139-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Fadda P, Fratta W. Stress-induced sleep deprivation modifies corticotropin releasing factor (CRF) levels and CRP binding in rat brain and pituitary. Pharmacological Research. 1997;35(5):443–446. doi: 10.1006/phrs.1997.0155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Maruyama H, Makino S, Noguchi T, Nishioka T, Hashimoto K. Central type 2 corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor mediates hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis activation in the rat. Neuroendocrinology. 2007;86(1):1–16. doi: 10.1159/000103556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Adamec RE, McKay D. Amygdala kindling, anxiety, and corticotrophin releasing factor (CRF) Physiology and Behavior. 1993;54(3):423–431. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(93)90230-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Conti LH, Costello DG, Martin LA, White MF, Abreu ME. Mouse strain differences in the behavioral effects of corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) and the CRF antagonist α-helical CRF9−41 . Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1994;48(2):497–503. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(94)90559-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Baldwin HA, Rassnick S, Rivier J, Koob GF, Britton KT. CRF antagonist reverses the “anxiogenic” response to ethanol withdrawal in the rat. Psychopharmacology. 1991;103(2):227–232. doi: 10.1007/BF02244208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Menzaghi F, Rassnick S, Heinrichs S. The role of corticotropin-releasing factor in the anxiogenic effects of ethanol withdrawal. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1994;739:176–184. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1994.tb19819.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Fisher L, Rivier C, Rivier J, Brown M. Differential antagonist activity of alpha-helical corticotropin-releasing factorfactor in three bioassay systems. Endocrinology. 1991;129:1312–1316. doi: 10.1210/endo-129-3-1312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Winslow JT, Newman JD, Insel TR. CRH and α-helical-CRH modulate behavioral measures of arousal in monkeys. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1989;32(4):919–926. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(89)90059-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Monnikes H, Heymann-Monnikes I, Tache Y. CRF in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus induces dose-related behavioral profile in rats. Brain Research. 1992;574(1-2):70–76. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)90801-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Melia KR, Duman RS. Involvement of corticotropin-releasing factor in chronic stress regulation of the brain noradrenergic system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1991;88(19):8382–8386. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.19.8382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Singewald N, Zhou G-Y, Chen F, Philippu A. Corticotropin-releasing factor modulates basal and stress-induced excitatory amino acid release in the locus coeruleus of conscious rats. Neuroscience Letters. 1996;204(1-2):45–48. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(96)12312-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Kishimoto T, Pearse RV, II, Lin CR, Rosenfeld MG. A sauvagine/corticotropin-releasing factor receptor expressed in heart and skeletal muscle. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1995;92(4):1108–1112. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.4.1108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Dieterich KD, Lehnert H, De Souza EB. Cotricotropin-releasing factor receptors: an overview. Experimental and Clinical Endocrinology and Diabetes. 1997;105(2):65–82. doi: 10.1055/s-0029-1211730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Turnbull AV, Rivier C. Corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) and endocrine responses to stress: CRF receptors, binding protein, and related peptides. Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine. 1997;215(1):1–10. doi: 10.3181/00379727-215-44108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Van Pett K, Viau V, Bittencourt JC, et al. Distribution of mRNAs encoding CRF receptors in brain and pituitary of rat and mouse. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2000;428(2):191–212. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20001211)428:2<191::aid-cne1>3.0.co;2-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Gonzalez MM, Valatx J-L. Involvement of stress in the sleep rebound mechanism induced by sleep deprivation in the rat: use of alpha-helical CRH9−41 . Behavioural Pharmacology. 1998;9(8):655–662. doi: 10.1097/00008877-199812000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Chalmers DT, Lovenberg TW, De Souza EB. Localization of novel corticotropin-releasing factor receptor (CRF2) mRNA expression to specific subcortical nuclei in rat brain: comparison with CRF1 receptor mRNA expression. Journal of Neuroscience. 1995;15(10):6340–6350. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-10-06340.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Kimura M, Muller-Preuss P, Lu A, et al. Conditional corticotropin-releasing hormone overexpression in the mouse forebrain enhances rapid eye movement sleep. Molecular Psychiatry. 2010;15(2):154–165. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Bonvento G, Scatton B, Claustre Y, Rouquier L. Effect of local injection of 8-OH-DPAT into the dorsal or median raphe nuclei on extracellular levels of serotonin in serotonergic projection areas in the brain. Neuroscience Letters. 1992;137(1):101–104. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(92)90308-t. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Kohler C, Chan-Palay V, Steinbusch H. The distribution and origin of serotonin-containing fibers in the septal area: a combined immunohistochemical and fluorescent retrograde tracing study in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1982;209(1):91–111. doi: 10.1002/cne.902090109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Assaf SY, Miller JJ. The role of a raphe serotonin system in the control of septal unit activity and hippocampal desynchronization. Neuroscience. 1978;3(6):539–550. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(78)90018-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Vertes RP, Kocsis B. Brainstem-diencephalo-septohippocampal systems controlling the theta rhythm of the hippocampus. Neuroscience. 1997;81(4):893–926. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(97)00239-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Maru E, Takahashi LK, Iwahara S. Effects of median raphe nucleus lesions on hippocampal EEG in the freely moving rat. Brain Research. 1979;163(2):223–234. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90351-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Fonoff ET, Silva CPC, Ballester G, Timo-Laria C. Electro-oscillographic correlation between dorsal raphe nucleus, neocortex and hippocampus during wakefulness before and after serotoninergic inactivation. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research. 1999;32(4):469–672. doi: 10.1590/s0100-879x1999000400015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Price ML, Curtis AL, Kirby LG, Valentino RJ, Lucki I. Effects of corticotropin-releasing factor on brain serotonergic activity. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1998;18(6):492–502. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(97)00197-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Antonijevic IA, Murck H, Frieboes R-M, Holsboer F, Steiger A. Hyporesponsiveness of the pituitary to CRH during slow wave sleep is not mimicked by systemic GHRH. Neuroendocrinology. 1999;69(2):88–96. doi: 10.1159/000054406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.De Souza EB. Corticotropin-releasing factor receptors: physiology, pharmacology, biochemistry and role in central nervous system and immune disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1995;20(8):789–819. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(95)00011-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.De Souza EB, Insel TR, Perrin MH. Corticotropin-releasing factor receptors are widely distributed within the rat central nervous system: an autoradiographic study. Journal of Neuroscience. 1985;5(12):3189–3203. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.05-12-03189.1985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Eberly LB, Dudley CA, Moss RL. Iontophoretic mapping of corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) sensitive neurons in the rat forebrain. Peptides. 1983;4(6):837–841. doi: 10.1016/0196-9781(83)90077-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Steriade M, McCormick DA, Sejnowski TJ. Thalamocortical oscillations in the sleeping and aroused brain. Science. 1993;262(5134):679–685. doi: 10.1126/science.8235588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Held K, Kunzel H, Ising M, et al. Treatment with the CRH1-receptor-antagonist R121919 improves sleep-EEG in patients with depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2004;38(2):129–136. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3956(03)00076-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Hsu DT, Lombardo KA, Bakshi VP, Balachandran JS, Roseboom PH, Kalin NH. Acute stress-induced increases in thalamic CRH mRNA are blocked by repeated stress exposure. Brain Research. 2001;915(1):18–24. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(01)02807-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Hsu DT, Bakshi VP, Roseboom PH, Kalin NH. Diurnal changes in corticotropin-releasing hormone messenger RNA in the rat thalamus. Neuroscience Letters. 2003;338(1):33–36. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(02)01365-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Canu M-H, Buser P, Rougeul A. Relationship between posterior thalamic nucleus unit activity and parietal cortical rhythms (Beta) in the waking cat. Neuroscience. 1994;60(3):679–688. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(94)90496-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Canu M-H, Rougeul A. Nucleus reticularis thalami participates in sleep spindles, not in β-rhythms concomitant with attention in cat. Comptes Rendus de l’Academie des Sciences. Series III. 1992;315(12):513–520. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Maloney KJ, Cape EG, Gotman J, Jones BE. High-frequency gamma electroencephalogram activity in association with sleep-wake states and spontaneous behaviors in the rat. Neuroscience. 1997;76:541–555. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(96)00298-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Vyazovskiy VV, Tobler I. Theta activity in the waking EEG is a marker of sleep propensity in the rat. Brain Research. 2005;1050(1-2):64–71. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.05.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Luijtelaar ELJM, Coenen AML. An EEG averaging technique for automated sleep-wake stage identification in the rat. Physiology and Behavior. 1984;33(5):837–841. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(84)90056-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Finelli LA, Baumann H, Berbely AA, Achermann P. Dual electroencephalogram markers of human sleep homeostasis: correlation between theta activity in waking and slow-wave activity in sleep. Neuroscience. 2000;101:523–529. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00409-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Gerbrandt LK, Lawrence JC, Eckardt MJ, Lloyd RL. Origin of the neocortically monitored theta rhythm in the curarized rat. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1978;45(4):454–467. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(78)90290-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Bland BH, Konopacki J, Kirk IJ, Oddie SD, Dickson CT. Discharge patterns of hippocampal theta-related cells in the caudal diencephalon of the urethan-anesthetized rat. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1995;74(1):322–333. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.74.1.322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Vinogradova OS, Brazhnik ES, Stafekhina VS, Kichigina VF. Modulation of septal influences on hippocampal neurons by cholinergic substances. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology. 1995;25(6):453–461. doi: 10.1007/BF02359273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from International Journal of Endocrinology are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES