Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Neurobiol Aging. 2010 Jun 11;31(8):1312–1325. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.05.002

3D maps localize caudate nucleus atrophy in 400 AD, MCI, and healthy elderly subjects

Sarah K Madsen a, April J Ho a, Xue Hua a, Priya S Saharan a, Arthur W Toga a, Clifford R Jack Jr c, Michael W Weiner d,e, Paul M Thompson a; the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiativeb,*
PMCID: PMC2903198  NIHMSID: NIHMS203917  PMID: 20538376

Abstract

MRI research examining structural brain atrophy in Alzheimer's disease (AD) generally focuses on medial temporal and cortical structures, but amyloid and tau deposits also accumulate in the caudate. Here we mapped the 3D profile of caudate atrophy using a surface mapping approach in subjects with AD and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) to identify potential clinical and pathological correlates. 3D surface models of the caudate were automatically extracted from 400 baseline MRI scans (100 AD, 200 MCI, 100 healthy elderly). Compared to controls, caudate volumes were lower in MCI (2.64% left, 4.43% right) and AD (4.74% left, 8.47% right). Caudate atrophy was associated with age, sum-of-boxes and global Clinical Dementia Ratings, Delayed Logical Memory scores, MMSE decline 1 year later, and body mass index. Reduced right (but not left) volume was associated with MCI-to-AD conversion, and CSF tau levels. Normal caudate asymmetry (with the right 3.9% larger than left) was lost in AD, suggesting preferential right caudate atrophy. Automated caudate maps may complement other MRI-derived measures of disease burden in AD.

Keywords: Alzheimer's disease (AD), mild cognitive impairment (MCI), normal aging, caudate nucleus, brain mapping, surface mapping, MRI, atrophy, ADNI

1. Introduction

Alzheimer's disease (AD) is the most prevalent form of dementia, affecting over 24 million people worldwide and over 5 million in the U.S. alone (Jorm et al., 1987). Memory systems of the hippocampus and medial temporal lobes are among the first to be affected by plaque and tangle pathology, with subsequent cortical pathology and widespread neuronal loss leading to impairments in language, attention, orientation, visuospatial, and executive functions. Traditionally AD research has focused on regions that degenerate earliest, and less attention has been paid to other subcortical brain regions involved in aging and AD (Rubin, 1999). Preferential caudate atrophy is more typical of motor disorders such as Parkinson's disease (Nakamura et al., 2001), but beta-amyloid and tau pathology also accumulate in the caudate in AD (Braak and Braak, 1990). Many earlier MRI studies of aging (Jernigan et al., 2001; Krishnan et al., 1990; Raz et al., 2003) and AD (Good et al., 2002; Rombouts et al., 2000) show progressive atrophy of the caudate. The caudate plays a vital functional role in forming new associations to acquire explicit memories, and in motor learning (Knowlton et al., 1996; Nakamura et al., 2001). Atrophy of the caudate nucleus may therefore be a valid target, in conjunction with others, in the search for AD biomarkers in the brain.

Despite some prior volumetric MRI studies, no study has mapped caudate atrophy and evaluated its clinical and pathological correlates in a large population of AD subjects. Here we extracted 3D models of caudate nucleus shape in 400 subjects from the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI), to test whether caudate atrophy was related to: (1) AD or MCI diagnosis at time of scanning; (2) baseline cognition assessed using the mini-mental state exam (MMSE), clinical dementia rating (CDR), and immediate and delayed recall Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS); (3) future clinical decline (in MMSE, CDR) one year after scanning; (4) conversion from MCI to AD in the subsequent 1 or 2 years; (5) CSF biomarkers of AD (amyloid beta, tau, p-tau); and (6) other influential factors such as age and sex, Geriatric Depression (GD) scale, presence of tremor, abnormal gait, AD- and obesity-related ApoE and FTO genotypes, and body mass index (BMI). We also ranked the relative strength of factors associated with caudate atrophy, expecting strongest associations with global cognition, then with CSF markers, and then with risk-modifying genotypes. We also wanted to determine how AD interacts with the naturally occurring asymmetry in caudate anatomy (where the right caudate tends to be about 4% larger than the left), to see if there was asymmetry in the disease process.

2. Methods

2.1 Subjects

We analyzed 400 baseline T1-weighted structural MRI scans from the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) public database (http://www.loni.ucla.edu/ADNI/Data/), along with associated demographic information, ApoE and FTO genotypes, CSF biomarker measures (for amyloid beta, tau, p-tau), and clinical and cognitive assessments (detailed below). Data was downloaded on or before June 1, 2009, and reflects the status of the database at that point. ADNI is a five-year study launched in 2004 by the National Institute on Aging (NIA), the National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering (NIBIB), the Food and Drug Administration (FDA), private pharmaceutical companies and non-profit organizations, as a $60 million public-private partnership. The primary goal of ADNI has been to test whether serial MRI, PET, biological markers, and clinical and neuropsychological assessments acquired at multiple sites (as in a typical clinical trial), can replicate results from smaller single site studies measuring the progression of MCI and early AD. A major focus of AD biomarker research is the pre-disease state known as mild cognitive impairment or MCI. Individuals with MCI have a 4-6-fold elevated risk for developing AD in the future, with 15% of MCI individuals developing AD within one year (Petersen et al., 2010). Determination of sensitive and specific markers for very early AD progression will aid researchers and clinicians in monitoring the effectiveness of new treatments, and lessen the time and cost of clinical trials. The Principal Investigator of this initiative is Michael W. Weiner, M.D., VA Medical Center and University of California, San Francisco.

ADNI collects a thorough battery of clinical and cognitive measures. We analyzed several scores that reflect cognitive decline in MCI and AD (Table 1). 100 individuals diagnosed with probable AD (mean age: 75.86±7.25 years), 200 with MCI (75.45±7.03), and 100 healthy elderly controls (76.62±4.83) were studied. The subject pool was age- and sex-matched within each diagnostic group. Data for all 400 subjects was not available on each of the clinical measures analyzed in this study, leading to differing sample sizes for some of the following analyses.

Table 1. Demographic and clinical data.

Mean demographic and clinical data are shown by diagnostic group for all subjects at the time of scanning (baseline); standard deviations are in parentheses. Changes in MMSE scores were measured over a 1-year follow-up interval. Changes in diagnosis were determined after 1- and 2-year follow-up intervals; the number of MCI and control subjects who converted to AD is also shown.

N Women/Men Age (years) Education (years) MMSE CDR CDR-SB
Control 100 53/47 76.62 (4.83) 15.87 (1.84) 29.14 (0.86) 0.47 (0.30) 0.02 (0.09)
MCI 200 100/100 75.45 (7.03) 15.61 (3.16) 26.94 (1.86) 0.43 (0.29) 1.48 (0.84)
AD 100 50/50 75.86 (7.25) 14.96 (3.31) 23.41 (1.86) 0.42 (0.33) 4.48 (1.56)
Delayed Logical Memory Immediate Logical Memory MMSE change at 1 year Diagnosis change after 1 year (number of AD conversions) Diagnosis change after 2 years (number of AD conversions) BMI
Control 12.99 (3.23) 14.11 (3.17) -0.14 (1.42) 0 0 24.77 (3.18)
MCI 3.73 (2.79) 6.94 (3.24) -1.21 (2.88) 36 24 25.91 (4.13)
AD 1.11 (1.74) 3.91 (2.82) -2.62 (4.57) 25.49 (4.12)

Cognitive measures examined included: (1) the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) - a global measure of mental status, evaluating five cognitive domains: orientation, registration, attention and calculation, recall, and language (Cockrell and Folstein, 1988) (2) the global and “sum-of-boxes” clinical dementia rating (CDR and CDR-SB respectively), which measures dementia severity by evaluating patients' performance in six domains: memory, orientation, judgment and problem solving, community affairs, home and hobbies, and personal care; CDR scores of 0, 0.5, 1, 2, and 3 represent no dementia, questionable, mild, moderate, and severe dementia, respectively and CDR-SB scores range from 0 to 18 (Morris, 1993); and (3) immediate and delayed Logical Memory (LM) recall, which are subtests of the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS), which is a modified version of the episodic memory assessment from the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R) (Wechsler, 1987); subjects were asked to recall a short story consisting of 25 pieces of information immediately after it was read to the subject (LM-im) and after a 30 minute delay (LM-del). It is a reasonable first step to survey a broad range of standard clinical tests of AD severity and memory impairment, because particular tests may be differentially associated with differences in caudate nucleus structure, and each test measures different aspects of cognitive performance. Also, it is important to compare different clinical measures to determine which tests associate best with atrophy for future clinical trials. This also makes it easier to make a direct comparison with a wide range of studies that have assessed these different measures.

We also examined Geriatric Depression (GD) scores (higher scores indicate greater depression severity), presence or absence of tremor, and normal versus abnormal gait in order to assess some of the emotional and motor functions traditionally associated with the caudate nucleus. Since recent studies have found that cardiovascular factors are associated with faster cognitive decline and more rapid progression of AD (Helzner et al., 2009), we also examined associations between atrophy and cardiovascular risk factors, such as body mass index (BMI), which has been associated with brain atrophy in recent tensor-based morphometry studies of healthy elderly subjects (Raji et al., 2009) and in MCI and AD (Ho et al., 2010a). BMI was calculated as weight (kg) over height (m2) - a simple but widely-used measure for classifying obese and overweight subjects. We also tested for associations between atrophy and the FTO genotype, based on recent findings that a particular allele of the prevalent FTO gene is associated with higher weight and waist circumference. FTO genotype has also been linked to brain atrophy in a tensor-based morphometry study (Ho et al., 2010b).

2.2 MRI acquisition and preprocessing

Scans were acquired on 1.5T MR scanners at 60 sites across the United States and Canada. Different types of scanners (GE, Siemens, or Philips) and various software platforms were used, but a standardized MRI protocol ensured cross-site comparability (Jack et al., 2008). A typical 1.5T MR protocol involved a 3D sagittal MP-RAGE scan with repetition time (TR): 2400 ms, minimum full TE, inversion time (TI): 1000 ms, flip angle: 8°, 24 cm field of view, and a 192×192×166 acquisition matrix in the x-, y-, and z- dimensions, yielding a voxel size of 1.25×1.25×1.2 mm3, later reconstructed to 1 mm3 isotropic voxels.

Image corrections were applied using a processing pipeline at the Mayo Clinic, consisting of: (1) correction of geometric distortion due to gradient non-linearity (Jovicich et al., 2006), i.e. “gradwarp” (2) “B1-correction” to adjust for image intensity inhomogeneity due to B1 nonuniformity (Jack et al., 2008), (3) “N3” bias field correction for reducing residual intensity inhomogeneity (Sled et al., 1998), and (4) geometrical scaling to remove scanner- and session-specific calibration errors using a phantom scan acquired for each subject (Gunter et al., 2006). All original image files as well as all corrected images are available at http://www.loni.ucla.edu/ADNI/Data/.

To adjust for global differences in brain positioning and scale across individuals, all scans were linearly registered to the stereotaxic space defined by the International Consortium for Brain Mapping (ICBM-53) (Mazziotta et al., 2001) with a 9-parameter (9P) transformation (three translations, three rotations, three scales) using the Minctracc algorithm (Collins et al., 1994). This registration is one standard method to account for global differences in brain scale across subjects; alternatives include scaling each subject's caudate nucleus volume for overall head size, using scalp images, or covarying for total intracranial volume in a general linear model. Globally aligned images were resampled in an isotropic space of 220 voxels along each axis (x, y, and z) with a final voxel size of 1 mm3.

2.3 Automated segmentation

We automatically extracted models of the caudate nucleus from each registered MRI scan, using an automated segmentation method based on adaptive boosting, that we recently developed and validated (Morra et al., 2008). Adaboost is a machine learning approach that learns to segment a structure in new images based on a small training set of expertly delineated tracings. As in our hippocampal studies (Morra et al., 2009a, 2009b), we created a small representative training set of 21 MRI scans (7 AD, 7 MCI, and 7 controls) that were expertly hand-labeled according to a validated protocol (Looi et al., 2007). These manual traces were then used to produce automated segmentations of the large (non-overlapping) testing set of 400 MRI scans. Using the image intensities and gradients in the training set scans, and statistical information on the likely position and geometry of the caudate, the algorithm learns a classification rule to designate each voxel as caudate or non-caudate. The automated segmentation incorporates ∼13,000 features including intensity, combinations of x, y, and z coordinates, and gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid tissue classifications. As is standard in machine learning, images used in the training set were excluded from all analyses presented here.

2.4 Volumetric analysis

For each subject, we determined left and right caudate volumes from the automatically generated segmentations. From these, we calculated the mean volume and a measure of asymmetry (L-R)/[2(L+R)] for each diagnostic group and tested for group and hemispheric differences in caudate volume.

2.5 Statistical maps

Parametric surface models were created from the 400 automatically generated caudate segmentations, which were then used to create statistical maps associating local volumetric differences with clinical and cognitive scores (Csernansky et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2007). As shown in Figure 1, surface models were created from each subject's automatically generated binary segmentation (Thompson et al., 2004). A central curve was calculated through the longitudinal axis of each model; the radial distance from this medial line to each surface point was used as a highly localized measure of atrophy. This same approach was used in two prior papers based on manual tracings (Butters et al., 2008) and tracings from another automated segmentation algorithm (Becker et al., 2006). We have used the same radial distance mapping approach to map hippocampal and ventricular shape differences (Chou et al., 2009; Morra et al., 2009a, 2009b).

Figure 1. Mapping caudate atrophy.

Figure 1

After image preprocessing, our Adaboost algorithm automatically segments the caudate nucleus bilaterally. A central curve (top right) was calculated through the longitudinal axis of each caudate and the radial distance to each point in a 3D surface mesh was used as a highly localized measure of atrophy (middle right). P-values are calculated at each surface point (bottom right) showing the significance of differences in radial distances between diagnostic groups or their associations with clinical scores. These maps visualized the profile of local shape differences or their clinical correlates at a point-wise level.

Based on a computed point-wise correspondence of the structure surfaces across subjects, geometric surface averaging was performed across all subjects in each diagnostic group. At each surface point, a correlation was run to compare diagnostic groups and determine the association of diagnosis or clinical scores with atrophy, as measured by differences in radial distance. In all maps shown, we used a multiple regression adjusted for age and sex. In these regressions, adjustments were made for age and sex by including them as covariates in the model. We did not adjust our maps for effects of ApoE genotype because we did not detect any significant effect of ApoE genotype on caudate nucleus shape in either the pooled sample or in any diagnostic group, considered separately. All maps presented in this manuscript refer to one-sided tests in the direction expected for the values of each clinical measure. For example, a lower MMSE score indicates poorer cognitive performance, which is expected to correlate with caudate nucleus atrophy rather than expansion. For a measure such as MMSE, we used a positive one-sided test, corresponding to the hypothesis that a lower MMSE score (poorer performance) would be associated with volume reductions in the caudate nucleus. Negative one-sided tests are used for measures in which higher scores are expected with greater atrophy in the caudate, such as BMI in which a lower score is generally considered to be indicative of better health.

Color coded p-values were mapped onto the average left and right caudate models. To correct for multiple comparisons across surface points, permutation tests provided an overall significance value for each statistical map. Specifically, the supra-threshold area (points with p < 0.01) in the map was compared to a null distribution for this same area, estimated from 100,000 random permutations of the covariates (Thompson et al., 2003).

3. Results

3.1 Volumetric results

First, we performed a volumetric analysis of the left and right caudate nucleus to demonstrate volumetric differences between normal aging and AD. As shown in Figure 2, controls had the highest bilateral caudate nucleus volume (mean: 3777, SD = 1017 mm3), followed by MCI (mean: 3648, SD = 1062 mm3; 3.41% lower), with AD subjects showing the lowest volumes (mean: 3543, SD = 1302 mm3; 6.20% lower); this confirmed the expected trend of AD < MCI < controls.

Figure 2. Volumetric Analysis (N=400).

Figure 2

Mean volumes for left, right, and pooled (left plus right) caudate volumes in the 3 diagnostic groups; error bars denote standard errors of the mean. Compared to normal, caudate volume is lower bilaterally in MCI and still lower in AD. Also, right caudate volume is greater than left in the whole sample (p < 0.001), MCI (p = 0.005), and controls (p < 0.001), but the asymmetry is not detected in AD. Between groups, the right caudate was smaller in AD (-300 mm3, -8.47%, p = 0.001) and MCI (-163 mm3, -4.43%, p = 0.002) than in controls.

We also calculated Cohen's d statistics to tabulate the effect sizes for group differences in pooled (left plus right hemisphere) volumes for the caudate nucleus, hippocampus, and lateral ventricles in an sample of ADNI subjects for whom all these structures were measured (AD: N=93; MCI, N=186; Controls: N=100). While the values listed in Table 2 here were calculated using our current sample, the Cohen's d statistic allows for direct comparison with other past and future results from samples of different sizes. Previous studies have reported effect sizes around d=1.1 for hippocampal volume comparisons of AD subjects versus controls and d=0.9 for MCI versus controls (Shen et al., 2010), and these values are a little higher than those in Table 2. The hippocampal and ventricular volumes come from our previous papers that used validated methods in the ADNI baseline data (Morra et al., 2009b; Chou et al., 2009, 2010); the highest achievable N for these comparisons was 400; all three subcortical measures were available for all of the subjects whose caudates were analyzed in this study.

Table 2. Effect sizes for caudate nucleus, hippocampus, and ventricle volumes. Cohen's d statistics are shown to indicate effect sizes for group differences in the pooled (left plus right) volumes of the caudate nucleus, hippocampus (Morra et al., 2009), and lateral ventricles (Chou et al., 2009, 2010). For the continuous variables (MMSE, CDR, CDR-SB, and BMI scores), partial correlations (r-values) are reported from a multiple regression model that controlled for age and sex. Significant results are highlighted with an asterisk (p < 0.05). The ventricular effects have an opposite sign to the others as the ventricles tend to expand in AD while other subcortical structures become smaller.

As expected, effect sizes are generally largest for the hippocampus, then the lateral ventricles, then the caudate.

Cohen's d Caudate: Hippocampus: Ventricles: N:
AD vs. Controls 0.378* 0.952* -0.619* 193
MCI vs. Controls 0.252* 0.473* -0.205 286
AD vs. MCI 0.148 0.485* -0.401* 279
r-value Caudate: Hippocampus: Ventricles: N:
MMSE 0.175* 0.349* -0.205* 400
CDR -0.062 -0.192* 0.143* 400
CDR-SB -0.209* -0.365* 0.225* 400
BMI -0.177* -0.197* -0.028 373

For continuous variables (MMSE, CDR, CDR-SB, and BMI scores), we computed a partial correlation from a multiple regression model, controlling for sex and age. The results are shown below in Table 2.

A significant rightward asymmetry was found (the right caudate was larger) in controls (p < 0.001, asymmetry = 3.86%) and in MCI (p = 0.01, asymmetry = 2.13%). This asymmetry was no longer detected in AD. This rightward asymmetry is subtle and not always detected in MRI studies (see Discussion). Between groups, right caudate volume was smaller in AD (-300 mm3, -8.47%, p = 0.001) and MCI (-163 mm3, -4.43%, p = 0.002) versus controls, but left caudate volume was not significantly different between diagnostic groups.

We compared MCI subjects who converted to AD with those who remained in the MCI diagnostic group at future follow-ups. At both 1-year and 2-year time points, we found significantly greater baseline atrophy in the right caudate among MCI-to-AD converters compared to non-converting MCI subjects (p = 0.033, 0.99%; p = 0.032, 2.41% respectively for 1- and 2-year follow ups).

Next we examined correlations between caudate nucleus volume and a variety of clinical measures including MMSE, CDR, and Logical Memory scores, with the goal of ranking their strength of association. Poorer MMSE scores at baseline (p < 0.001, N=400) and changes in MMSE scores from baseline to a 1-year follow-up (p = 0.008, N=400) were both significantly correlated with greater right caudate atrophy at baseline, when all diagnostic groups were pooled. Similarly, greater atrophy of the right caudate was also correlated with poorer global CDR scores (p = 0.013, N=400). Both right and left caudate atrophy were associated with worse “sum of boxes” CDR scores (p = 0.010, for the left; p > 0.001, for the right, N=400) and with poorer Delayed Logical Memory recall (p = 0.019, for the left; p = 0.005, for the right, N=395). Poorer Immediate Logical Memory recall was also correlated with right caudate atrophy (p = 0.004, N=395).

3.2 Group difference maps

Next we created 3D maps showing the significance levels (p-values) for local caudate atrophy in AD and MCI versus controls (Figure 3), after controlling for age and sex. Separate maps are shown for the left and right caudate nucleus.

Figure 3. Caudate Atrophy in AD and MCI and baseline clinical scores.

Figure 3

3D maps show significant atrophy in AD and MCI versus controls, with p-values color-coded at each surface point. Red indicates areas where reduced caudate nucleus volume is associated (p < 0.05) with the poorer clinical diagnosis; yellow to blue areas are weakly or not associated. The top set of significance maps are labeled to indicate the orientation of the average caudate nucleus shapes, which is consistent in all figures in this paper (anterior caudate head points upward in this figure, posterior tail points downward, the left pair of maps represents the superior surface, while the right pair represents the inferior surface of the caudate nucleus, as seen from below). Permutation tests showed that the group differences between AD and controls (Left: p = 0.022, corrected; Right: p = 0.001, corrected; N=200) and between MCI and controls (Left: p = 0.023, corrected; Right: p = 0.012, corrected; N=300) are significant bilaterally. Association maps are significant bilaterally for CDR-SB (Left: p = 0.002, corrected; Right: p < 0.001, corrected; N=400), CDR (Left: p = 0.029; Right: p = 0.022, corrected; N=400), and Delayed Logical Memory (Left: p = 0.015, corrected; Right: p = 0.003, corrected; N=395). Associations with Immediate Logical Memory (Right: p = 0.006, corrected; N=395) were detected only for the right caudate nucleus.

Relative to controls, greater atrophy was detected in AD (Left: p = 0.022, corrected; Right: p = 0.001, corrected; N=200) and MCI (Left: p = 0.023, corrected; Right: p = 0.012, corrected; N=300), but not in AD versus MCI (Left: p = 0.383, corrected; Right: p = 0.083, correct; N=300).

3.3 Baseline association maps of clinical scores

Next we created maps of correlations for the clinical and cognitive scores that showed significant associations in our volumetric analyses (Figure 3). These map-based results were largely in line with the analysis of overall volumes. Tests where poorer scores were associated with volume reduction bilaterally (CDR-SB, Delayed Logical Memory) also showed a significant bilateral effect in the maps. Bilateral atrophy was associated with greater impairment measured by CDR (Right: p = 0.029, corrected; N=400), CDR-SB (Left: p = 0.002, corrected; Right: p < 0.001, corrected; N=400), and in Delayed Logical Memory (Left: p = 0.015, corrected; Right: p = 0.003, corrected; N=395). Poorer Immediate Logical Memory scores (Right: p = 0.006, corrected; N=395) were associated with right caudate atrophy only, as in the volumetric analysis.

3.4 Associations with clinical scores one year later

To investigate the predictive value of our caudate maps, we associated baseline caudate anatomy with change in MMSE and CDR scores between baseline and 1-year later (Figure 4). As suggested by our volumetric analysis, right caudate atrophy was highly associated with baseline MMSE scores in the full sample (p = 0.001, corrected, N=400) and with 1-year interval change in MMSE scores in the MCI group (Left: p = 0.037; Right: p = 0.001, corrected, N=200). Associations of 1-year change in MMSE scores were not significant for either AD or control groups; however we cannot rule out that these associations may be found over a longer time interval, greater than one year. Although associations with baseline CDR and CDR-SB were significant (Figure 3), associations with 1-year change in these scores were not significant after multiple comparisons correction.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Maps of associations with MMSE scores at baseline and one year later, MCI-to-AD conversion, and CSF concentrations of Tau pathology. 3D maps show areas of significant associations between local volumetric atrophy in the caudate and MMSE scores at baseline and after a 1-year follow-up interval, with p-values color-coded at each surface voxel. Associations were only detected in the right caudate nucleus for baseline MMSE scores (Right: p = 0.001, corrected). Change in MMSE score after a 1-year follow-up interval was bilaterally associated in the MCI group only (Left: p = 0.037; Right: p = 0.001, corrected; N=200). Those who converted from MCI to AD after one year and two years showed greater right caudate atrophy at baseline than those who did not (1-year conversion: p = 0.008, corrected; 2-year conversion: p = 0.035, corrected). For CSF levels of tau at time of scanning, associations were significant for the right but not left caudate for the tau CSF biomarker (p = 0.036, corrected, N=216)

3.5 Greater atrophy in MCI-to-AD converters

To investigate the predictive value of local caudate nucleus atrophy, we created significance maps showing the association between caudate nucleus atrophy at baseline among MCI subjects at who converted to AD (converters) versus those who remained classified as MCI (non-converters) one or two years after their baseline scan (Figure 4). Right caudate volumes were lower at baseline for those who had converted to AD by one year later (p = 0.008, corrected; N=174) or two years later (p = 0.035, corrected; N=50).

3.6 Associations with CSF biomarkers

Right but not left caudate atrophy (Figure 4) was associated with CSF tau concentrations (Right: p = 0.021, corrected; N=216, AD: N=59, MCI: N=102, controls: N=55). Additionally, correlation analyses within each diagnostic group revealed that both left and right caudate nucleus volume are correlated with CSF tau concentration in the AD subjects (Left: r = 0.239, p = 0.034; Right: r = 0.370, p = 0.002; N=59) but not in the MCI or control groups (r < 0.1, p > 0.05). Correlation maps of associations between radial atrophy and p-tau, amyloid beta concentrations, ratio measures of tau/amyloid beta and p-tau/amyloid beta were also assessed, but those correlation maps did not pass permutation-based corrections for multiple comparisons. It would be interesting to see if the CSF associations hold up on a group-by-group level since other ADNI labs have found that tau concentrations increase progressively from controls to MCI to AD (Shaw et al., 2009); however, our current study did not have enough power to detect associations within each diagnostic group separately (as sample sizes with available CSF data are only around 50 subjects per group).

3.7 Associations with additional clinical variables

Significant associations were also found in maps for age (Left: p = 0.039, corrected; Right: p = 0.001, corrected; N=400) and sex (Left: p < 0.001, corrected; Right, p < 0.001, corrected; N=400), with smaller caudates in men than women (Figure 5), and smaller caudates in older than younger people. Examining emotional domains, we also found significant associations between more impaired Geriatric Depression scores and atrophy of the right caudate nucleus (Right, p = 0.004, corrected; N=395). In our analysis of motor function, the presence of abnormal gait was significantly associated with volume reductions in the right caudate nucleus (Right, p = 0.027, corrected; N=395); however maps of associations between atrophy and the presence of tremor in our subjects were not significant.

Figure 5. Associations between caudate atrophy, body mass index (BMI), age and sex.

Figure 5

3D maps show areas of significant associations between local volumetric atrophy in the caudate nucleus and several additional clinical variables. Significant associations were found for age (Left: p = 0.039, corrected; Right: p = 0.001, corrected; N=400) and sex (Left: p < 0.001, corrected; Right, p < 0.001, corrected; N=400), with smaller caudates in men than women (8.58% bilaterally). Significant associations were also found in the right caudate nucleus for Geriatric Depression scores (Right, p = 0.004, corrected; N=395) and for abnormal versus normal gait (Right, p = 0.027, corrected; N=395). BMI scores at time of scanning for all subjects and for AD subjects only. For BMI, significant associations were found in all subjects (Left: p < 0.001, corrected; Right: p = 0.0021, corrected; N=393) and in AD subjects only (Left: p = 0.015, corrected; Right: p = 0.005, corrected; N=96).

In line with recent tensor-based morphometry findings that higher BMI is associated with brain atrophy in the elderly, caudate atrophy was found here to be associated with higher BMI in our full sample (Left: p < 0.001, corrected; Right: p = 0.002, corrected; N=393) and in the AD subjects considered separately (Left: p = 0.015, corrected; Right: p = 0.005, corrected; N=96). Within-group associations for MCI and control groups were not significant.

Maps of associations with the FTO genotype, which is linked to higher BMI and with brain atrophy in the elderly (Ho et al., 2010b), were not significant after permutation-based correction for multiple comparisons. Maps associating local caudate nucleus volume with APOE genotype were also not significant after multiple comparisons correction.

3.8 Ranking effect sizes for different covariates

Results from our permutation-corrected significance maps agreed with our volumetric results for each statistical test performed, while providing a more detailed spatial profile of regional atrophy. Adjusting for age and sex in these regressions did not affect these associations materially, but slightly boosted effect sizes (data not shown). To rank the covariates in terms of their effect sizes, we ran the maps with randomly selected samples of 400, 300, 200, 96, and 50 subjects, to find the reduced sample size needed to detect each effect, using the results of the permutation test to correct for multiple comparisons. A sample size of 96 was chosen instead of 100 to allow inclusion of the associations for BMI in the AD group only, which consisted of 96 subjects. Only 50 subjects were needed to detect the BMI effect in the AD group (Left: p = 0.024, corrected; Right: p = 0.022, corrected; N=50). 96 subjects were needed to detect effects of BMI in a representative sample of the entire subject pool (Left: p = 0.0031, corrected; Right: p = 0.006, corrected; N=96), associations with baseline CDR-SB scores (Right: p = 0.037, corrected; N=96), and the comparison of AD versus controls (Right: p = 0.038, corrected; N=96). Effects of baseline MMSE scores were first detected at 200 subjects (Right: p < 0.001, corrected; N=200).

4. Discussion

This study demonstrates that 3D maps of the caudate nucleus localize statistically significant regions of volume reduction associated with (1) diagnosis and clinical and cognitive scores, (2) future decline in MMSE scores at 1-year follow-up in the MCI group, (3) conversion from MCI to AD 1 and 2 years after scanning, (4) baseline CSF concentrations of tau, and (5) additional clinical measures including BMI, Geriatric Depression, and abnormal gait. The variables most strongly associated with caudate atrophy were found using a reduced N analysis, namely (in this rank order): BMI in the AD group only, BMI in all subjects, CDR-SB scores, group difference between AD and controls, and MMSE scores at baseline. It is interesting to note that cardiovascular measures and CDR-SB scores, which rely on interviews from both patients and informants, were more powerfully associated with caudate volume reductions in our 3D maps, compared to seemingly more direct measures of AD such as the MMSE. Although some of these measures are highly correlated and may be viewed as redundant, we felt that it was important to take a broad approach initially, in analyzing multiple measures of cognition and health. This has practical significance for the planning of clinical trials, which currently use a variety of similar tests, and also allows direct comparison with other studies that each may use different tests.

Caudate atrophy was generally localized to anterior regions and was specific to the right caudate for several variables assessed. Caudate atrophy in AD (versus controls) was 2-fold greater on the right than the left. Further study is needed to confirm the greater vulnerability of the right caudate. The natural asymmetry of the caudate (right 3.9% larger than the left in controls) has been found in many but not all studies, and large samples are needed to detect it. Most literature supports a rightward (right larger than left) asymmetry in caudate nucleus volume (Giedd et al., 1996; Ifthikharuddin et al., 2000; Larisch et al., 1998; Peterson et al., 1993; Yamashita et al., 2009), but some studies have found leftward, sex-specific, or no asymmetry (Gunning-Dixon et al., 1998; Szabo et al., 2003). Based on our current findings and previous literature, it may be possible to view asymmetry findings in brain structures as a general indicator of poor brain health. Psychosis has been linked to genes involved in the development of cerebral asymmetry (Laval et al., 2004) and differences in laterality have been found to covary with, or predict, individual differences in susceptibility to stress pathology and drug sensitivity (Carlson et al., 1989). The presence or change in direction of asymmetry has been reported (but not always consistently) in disorders ranging from schizophrenia (Crow et al., 1996), ADHD (Castellanos et al., 1996), bipolar (Javadapour et al., 2010) and affective mood disorders (Baumann et al., 1999), and Tourette's syndrome (Klieger et al., 1997).

There may be some skepticism that the caudate is a natural place to look for clinical correlations in AD, but it may give additional power to detect associations with cognition and future decline, if used along with structures that are more commonly studied. For example, it is widely accepted that the hippocampus - the target of the most published AD morphometry studies - is challenging to segment reliably, with a notorious 2-fold difference in mean hippocampal volumes across studies and imaging centers (Frisoni et al., 2010). Prior studies using the same radial distance mapping technique to other brain structures in ADNI have also found correlations between hippocampal atrophy (Morra et al., 2009), ventricular expansion (Chou et al., 2009, 2010) and clinical variables such as MMSE, CDR, and CDR-SB scores. There are also differences between diagnostic groups for the volumes of all of these structures (AD versus controls, MCI versus controls). As shown in Table 2, and as might be expected, hippocampal volumes gave greatest effect sizes for detecting group differences, within ranges supported by previous studies (Shen at al., 2010), and for detecting correlations with clinical scores. Effect sizes for lateral ventricle volumes also gave relatively strong effect sizes, although they were generally weaker than the hippocampal effect sizes. Caudate effect sizes, while significant, were generally lower than those for the ventricles and hippocampus. As one exception to this rank order, the caudate gave numerically marginally better effect sizes than ventricular volume for distinguishing MCI from controls. Even so, we did not performed statistical tests for differences in effect sizes across structures, as so many measures were made that a stringent multiple comparisons correction would be needed. Despite these generally weaker effects, the current study of the caudate also found significant associations between reduced volume and MCI-to-AD conversion, and with decline in MMSE scores after a one-year follow-up in the MCI group, whereas these associations were not detectable in studies of ventricular expansion (Chou et al., 2009) or hippocampal atrophy (Morra et al., 2009a, 2009b), using the same ADNI subject pool (with 240 and 490 subjects, respectively); the latter study also used the same Adaboost machine learning algorithm, applied to hippocampal segmentation. This is an intriguing finding, suggesting caudate volume at baseline may predict later clinical changes relatively well, perhaps because caudate atrophy is only severe when the disease is rapidly progressing.

The caudate has previously been implicated in AD through multiple lines of evidence including histological findings of tau and amyloid accumulations (Braak and Braak, 1990). Volumetric studies find reductions in caudate nucleus volume with normal aging (Jernigan et al., 2001; Krishnan et al., 1990; Raz et al., 2003) in AD versus controls (Rombouts et al., 2000), and in many other or neurological, psychiatric or neurodevelopmental conditions including autism (Turner et al., 2006), Fragile X syndrome (Gothelf et al., 2008), elderly depression (Butters et al., 2008), and HIV (Becker et al., 2006). Caudate lesions can produce deficits in executive control and cognitive processing speed (Rubin, 1999). Interestingly, a recent study also found subcortical brain volume reductions, encompassing the caudate nucleus, in elderly individuals with a higher body mass index (BMI, a measure of obesity; Raji et al., 2009).

The anterior caudate head is the most closely involved in traditionally studied cognitive processes and integrates major inputs from the dorsolateral and orbitofrontal cortices, involved in attention and planning (Cummings et al., 1995). Disturbances in the hemispheric asymmetry of the caudate head are associated with ADHD and schizophrenia (Ballmaier et al., 2008; Blanton et al., 1999; Hynd et al., 1993). One study found significant caudate volume reductions in depressed elderly and created 3D maps using the same technique here, except on manually-derived segmentations; however, their maps were not significant after permutation-based correction for multiple comparisons (Butters et al., 2008).

Beyond the caudate and the medial temporal lobes, additional basal ganglia and subcortical gray matter structures are implicated in AD and age-related neurodegeneration. Putamen and thalamus volumes are reduced in AD, and volume decreases correlate with cognitive deficits (de Jong et al., 2008). Putamen volumes are reduced in frontotemporal lobe degeneration, but not in AD (Looi et al., 2009). A recent study of individuals with Down's Syndrome - a group at heightened risk for developing AD - found significant associations in those who developed AD and bilateral volume reductions in hippocampus and caudate and in the right amygdala and putamen (Beacher et al., 2009). In vivo imaging with [11C]-PIB PET, which is thought to map the profile of amyloid deposition in the living brain, has correlated increased amyloid signal with increased atrophy in the hippocampus and amygdala (Frisoni et al., 2009). In addition, post-mortem studies have localized diffuse amyloid plaques in the caudate nucleus in AD (Ikonomovic et al., 2008).

In a PET study of dopamine D1 and D2 receptors, researchers found a 14% reduction in mean signal for D1 receptors for the caudate and putamen in AD, but no difference in D2 receptor signal (Kemppainen et al., 2000). The basal ganglia also have the highest iron content of any brain region, which is interesting because AD patients exhibit disrupted iron metabolism that may relate to oxidative damage and higher iron levels are found in the caudate and putamen in AD patients compared to controls (Bartzokis et al., 2000).

A common limiting factor in neuroimaging studies of AD is the labor-intensive task of manually segmenting subcortical structures on large MRI datasets, requiring long-term effort from expertly trained neuroanatomists. Fortunately, automated methods now exist to efficiently segment many subcortical structures including the hippocampus and caudate nucleus (Fischl et al., 2002; Powell et al., 2008; Yushkevich et al., 2006). Here we used an automated method based on adaptive boosting, that we recently developed and validated (Morra et al., 2008). Using this method, we efficiently created caudate nucleus segmentations in 400 ADNI brain MRI scans. The resulting 3D maps had enough statistical power to detect local volume differences associated with a wide variety of clinical variables in AD. Our maps detected associations between caudate atrophy and CSF levels of tau, which have not always been associated with other MRI-derived measures in the same cohort (Chou et al., 2009). CSF levels of tau are significantly associated with higher rates of temporal lobe atrophy (Leow et at., 2009), but only showed a trend level of association with the rate of hippocampal atrophy (Schuff et al., 2009). In prior amyloid PET studies, amyloid deposition has been noted in the caudate nucleus (Ikonomovic et al., 2008). Here we did not detect a relation between the level of CSF amyloid and the volume of the caudate. In future, a direct PET-based brain measure of amyloid in the caudate may correlate better with atrophy than CSF-based measures of amyloid burden. Limited sample sizes may have also made it hard to detect an association; even so, the sample was large enough for Tau levels to be reliably correlated with in maps of atrophy for the right caudate nucleus in the pooled sample and bilaterally for the volumetric summaries in the AD group.

A possible confounding factor in some voxel-based morphometric studies of the caudate is the potential for mis-registration of anatomy across subjects along the ventricles, especially in elderly subjects with substantial brain degeneration. Since the caudate nucleus is a gray matter structure that lies just below the boundary of the lateral ventricles, some volumetric studies may be confounded by poor or biased registration along the lateral ventricular surface. This effect is unlikely in the current study as our measures of radial atrophy are intrinsic, meaning they are independent of whether the structure is translated in space. They are computed from a centerline traced down the center of the structure, and do not rely on the correct registration of images across subjects, as some voxel-based averaging methods do.

The etiology of caudate atrophy in AD and MCI is of interest. One probable explanation is that we are assessing neuronal atrophy, secondary to the accumulation of amyloid plaque pathology and tau neurofibrillary tangles (Braak and Braak, 1990). The caudate may also experience a loss of afferent projections from other brain regions. As such, a limitation of the study is that the associations between caudate atrophy and cognition may in reality be mediated by atrophy occurring in other brain structures, such as the hippocampus. The intent in this study was to identify correlates of caudate atrophy, but it is likely that the more immediate causes of the clinical differences may be atrophy (or functional compromise) of structures elsewhere in the brain, not necessarily those assessed here.

A further limitation of the current study is that the available sample sizes made it necessary to use a pooled sample including both patients and controls for several analyses. Pooling subjects allowed us to achieve sufficient statistical power to detect important findings, but there is also a risk of recovering group sampling effects due to the differences between diagnostic groups in cognitive measures. Where sample sizes and effect sizes were sufficient for within-group correlations to be detected, we did find statistically significant associations in one-year change in MMSE score in the MCI group and in BMI in the AD group. It is possible that there are additional within-group associations for these and other measures that did not survive the sample size reduction. It can also be argued that AD, MCI, and healthy elderly controls represent stages on a continuum - in that case, binning the subjects into diagnostic groups could limit the power of correlations found across the spectrum and lead to false negative results.

In this study, we report correlations between atrophy and cognitive or CSF-derived measures in a pooled ADNI sample (combining patients with AD, MCI and controls), while also reporting several within-groups correlations split by diagnosis (“disaggregated” analyses of AD subjects only, for example). Both types of analysis are complementary and each has its own limitations. When analyzing a mixed cohort of subjects, our goal is to determine (1) the cognitive correlates of atrophy in the entire sample, and (2) whether the chosen biomarker of disease burden is linked with decline across the full spectrum of controls, MCI, and AD subjects. As the whole cohort is arguably a continuum, it is vital to look beyond diagnostic categories to analyze relationships between atrophy and particular measures of cognitive function. A within-group analysis, of AD subjects only, for example, may not be able to detect these correlations due to a “restricted range” effect seen when looking at a limited range of differences in brain structure and cognitive performance. By running split analyses only, many important correlations may be missed. For instance, brain atrophy correlates well with CSF-derived measures of tau pathology across the continuum from healthy aging to MCI and to AD. However, it is possible that no correlation will be detected if we sub-select a group such as MCI, with a very narrow range of disease burden. Furthermore, it is a fallacy to pre-select groups that are defined partly on cognitive measures, and then later test for correlations with these same cognitive scales. If the selection criterion for the group correlates with the variable of interest, we will find many false negatives due to the truncated range, making the results uninterpretable.

Pooled analyses have limitations to consider, as well. When cognitive measures are correlated with diagnosis, correlations in a pooled cohort may show similar patterns to those found when directly comparing AD subjects with controls. Another consideration when using pooled cohorts such as ADNI, is that correlations may be influenced by the proportion of subjects in each diagnostic group. By design, ADNI oversampled subjects with cognitive decline, including a 1:2:1 ratio of AD:MCI:controls. Correlations in this particular sample may not be detected to the same degree in other population studies with different proportions of subjects or within each diagnostic group. For these reasons, we chose to use a combination of both pooled and split analyses, which have complementary value in understanding the cognitive and pathological correlates of atrophy.

Since it can be difficult to tease apart contributions of each individual brain region to a disease such as AD, studying effects of the disease on specific anatomical structures, beyond those most commonly targeted, may help to piece together relevant circuitry affected by AD pathology. Multiple AD biomarkers can be used in a mutually reinforcing way to identify subjects most likely to decline to AD in a sample (Kohannim et al., unpublished results), so caudate atrophy may inform the classification of AD, MCI or the prediction of future decline. Another line of work involves genome-wide association studies of caudate volume in large subject cohorts such as ADNI. This information will improve our understanding of the mechanistic processes involved in brain aging and AD.

Acknowledgments

Data collection and sharing for this project was funded by the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) (National Institutes of Health Grant U01 AG024904). ADNI is funded by the National Institute on Aging, the National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering, and through generous contributions from the following: Abbott, AstraZeneca AB, Bayer Schering Pharma AG, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eisai Global Clinical Development, Elan Corporation, Genentech, GE Healthcare, GlaxoSmithKline, Innogenetics, Johnson and Johnson, Eli Lilly and Co., Medpace, Inc., Merck and Co., Inc., Novartis AG, Pfizer Inc, F. Hoffman-La Roche, Schering-Plough,

Synarc, Inc., and Wyeth, as well as non-profit partners the Alzheimer's Association and Alzheimer's Drug Discovery Foundation, with participation from the U.S. Food and Drug Administration. Private sector contributions to ADNI are facilitated by the Foundation for the National Institutes of Health (http://www.fnih.org/). The grantee organization is the Northern California Institute for Research and Education, and the study is coordinated by the Alzheimer's Disease Cooperative Study at the University of California, San Diego. ADNI data are disseminated by the Laboratory for Neuro Imaging at the University of California, Los Angeles. This research was also supported by NIH grants P30 AG010129, K01 AG030514, and the Dana Foundation.

Footnotes

Disclosure Statement: The authors have no potential financial or personal conflicts of interest including relationships with other people or organizations within three years of beginning the work submitted that could inappropriately influence this work.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Ballmaier M, Schlagenhauf F, Toga AW, Gallinat J, Koslowski M, Zoli M, Hojatkashani C, Narr KL, Heinz A. Regional patterns and clinical correlates of basal ganglia morphology in non-medicated schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 2008;106(2-3):140–147. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.08.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bartzokis G, Sultzer D, Cummings J, Holt LE, Hance DB, Henderson VW, Mintz J. In vivo evaluation of brain iron in Alzheimer disease using magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57(1):47–53. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.1.47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Baumann B, Danos P, Krell D, Diekmann S, Leschinger A, Stauch R, Wurthmann C, Berstein HG, Bogerts B. Reduced volume of limbic system-affiliated basal ganglia in mood disorders: Preliminary data from a postmortem study. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 1999;11:71–78. doi: 10.1176/jnp.11.1.71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Beacher F, Daly E, Simmons A, Prasher V, Morris R, Robinson C, Lovestone S, Murphy K, Murphy DG. Alzheimer's disease and Down's syndrome, an in vivo MRI study. Psychol Med. 2009;39(4):675–684. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Becker JT, Davis SW, Hayashi KM, Meltzer CC, Toga AW, Lopez OL, Thompson PM. Three-dimensional patterns of hippocampal atrophy in mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2006;63:97–101. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.1.97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Blanton RE, Levitt J, Thompson PM, Badrtalei S, Capetillo-Cunliffe L, Toga AW. Average 3-Dimensional Caudate Surface Representations in a Juvenile-Onset Schizophrenia and Normal Pediatric Population. 5th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain; 1999. Presentation 621. [Google Scholar]
  7. Braak H, Braak E. Alzheimer's disease, Striatal amyloid deposits and neurofibrillary changes. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 1990;49(3):215–224. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Butters MA, Aizenstein HJ, Hayashi KM, Meltzer CC, Seaman J, Reynolds CF, Toga AW, Thompson PM, Becker JT IMAGe Research Group. Three-dimensional mapping of the caudate nucleus in late-life depression. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2008;17(1):4–12. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31816ff72b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Carlson JN, Glick SD. Cerebral lateralization as a source of interindividual differences in behavior. Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences. 1989;45(9):788–798. doi: 10.1007/BF01954054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Castellanos FX, Giedd JN, Marsh WL, Hamburger SD, Vaituzis AC, Dicktein DP, Sarfatti SE, Vauss YC, Snell JW, Lange N, Kaysen D, Krain AL, Ritchie GF, Rajapakse JC, Rapoport JL. Quantitative brain magnetic resonance imaging in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1996;35(7):607–616. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830070053009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chou YY, Lepore N, Avedissian C, Madsen SK, Parikshak N, Hua X, Shaw LM, Trojanowski JQ, Weiner MW, Toga AW, Thompson PM the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Mapping correlations between ventricular expansion and CSF amyloid and tau biomarkers in 240 subjects with Alzheimer's disease, mild cognitive impairment and elderly controls. Neuroimage. 2009;46(2):394–410. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chou YY, Lepore N, Saharan P, Madsen SK, Hua X, Jack CJ, Jr, Shaw L, Trojanowski JQ, Weiner MW, Toga AW, Thomspon PM the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Ranking the Clinical and Pathological Correlates of Ventricular Expansion Mapped in 804 Alzheimer's Disease, MCI, and Normal Elderly Subjects. Neurobiology of Aging, Special Issue on ADNI. 2010 Revision to be submitted 23 April 2010. [Google Scholar]
  13. Cockrell JR, Folstein MF. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) Psychopharmacol Bull. 1988;24(4):689–692. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Collins DL, Neelin P, Peters TM, Evans AC. Automatic 3D intersubject registration of MR volumetric data in standardized Talairach space. J Comput Assist Tomogr. 1994;18:192–205. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Crow TJ, Done DJ, Sacker A. Cerebral lateralization is delayed in children who later develop schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 1996;22(3):181–185. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(96)00068-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Csernansky JG, Wang L, Joshi SC, Ratnanather JT, Miller MI. Computational anatomy and neuropsychiatric disease, Probabilistic assessment of variation and statistical inference of group difference, hemispheric asymmetry, and time-dependent change. Neuroimage. 2004;23(Suppl 1):S56–S68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cummings JL. Anatomic and behavioural aspects of frontal-subcortical circuits. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1995;769:1–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb38127.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. De Jong LW, van der Hiele K, Veer IM, Houwing JJ, Westendorp RG, Bollen EL, de Bruin PW, Middelkoop HA, van Buchem MA, van der Grond J. Strongly reduced volumes of putamen and thalamus in Alzheimer's disease, an MRI study. Brain. 2008;131(12):3277–3285. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, Albert M, Dieterich M, Haselgrove C, van der Kouwe A, Killiany R, Kennedy D, Klaveness S, Montillo A, Makris N, Rosen B, Dale AM. Whole brain segmentation, Automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. 2002;33:341–355. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00569-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Frisoni GB, Lorenzi M, Caroli A, Kemppainen N, Någren K, Rinne JO. In vivo mapping of amyloid toxicity in Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2009;72(17):1504–1511. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181a2e896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Giedd JN, Snell JW, Lange N, Rajapakse JC, Casey BJ, Kozuch PL, Vaituzis AC, Vauss YC, Hamburger SD, Kaysen D, Rapoport JL. Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of human brain development, ages 4-18. Cereb Cortex. 1996;6(4):551–560. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.4.551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Good CD, Scahill RI, Fox NC, Ashburner J, Friston KJ, Chan D, Crum WR, Rossor MN, Frackowiak RSJ. Automatic Differentiation of anatomical patterns in the human brain: validation with studies of degenerative dementias. NeuroImage. 2002;17(1):29–46. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gothelf D, Furfaro JA, Eckert MA, Hall SS, O'Hara R, Erba HW, Ringel J, Hayashi KM, Patnaik S, Golianu B, Kraemer HC, Thompson PM, Piven J, Reiss AL. Neuroanatomy of Fragile X Syndrome is Associated with Aberrant Behavior and FMRP. Annals of Neurology. 2008;63(1):40–51. doi: 10.1002/ana.21243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gunning-Dixon FM, Head D, McQuain J, Acker JD, Raz N. Differential aging of the human striatum, A prospective MR imaging study. American Journal of Neuroradiology. 1998;19(8):1501–1507. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gunter J, Bernstein M, Borowski B, Felmlee J, Blezek D, Mallozzi R. Validation testing of the MRI calibration phantom for the Alzheimer's disease neuroimaging initiative study. ISMRM 14th Scientific Meeting and Exhibition; Seattle, Washington. 2006. [Google Scholar]
  26. Helzner EP, Luchsinger JA, Scarmeas N, Cosentino S, Brickman AM, Glymour MM, Stern Y. Contribution of vascular risk factors to the progression in Alzheimer Disease. Archives of Neurology. 2009;66(3):3434–348. doi: 10.1001/archneur.66.3.343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Ho AJ, Raji CA, Becker JT, Lopez OL, Kuller LW, Hua X, Lee S, Hibar D, Dinov ID, Stein JL, Jack CR, Weiner MW, Toga AW, Thompson PM. Obesity and Brain Structure in 700 AD and MCI Patients. Neurobiology of Aging, Special Issue on ADNI. 2010a April; doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.04.006. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Ho AJ, Stein JL, Hua X, Lee S, Hibar DP, Leow AD, Dinov ID, Toga AW, Saykin AJ, Shen L, Foround T, Pankratz N, Huentelman MJ, Craig DW, Gerber JD, Allen AN, Corneveaux JJ, Stephan DA, Decarli CS, Dechairo BM, Potkin SG, Jack CR, Jr, Weiner MW, Raji CA, Lopez OL, Becker JT, Carmichael OT, Thompson PM the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. A commonly carried allele of the obesity-related FTO gene is associated with reduced brain volume in the healthy elderly. PNAS Epub ahead of print. 2010b doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910878107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hynd GW, Hern KL, Novey ES, Eliopulos D, Marshall R, Gonzalez JJ, Voeller KK. Attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder and asymmetry of the caudate nucleus. J Child Neurol. 1993;8(4):339–347. doi: 10.1177/088307389300800409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Ifthikharuddin SF, Shrier DA, Numaguchi Y, Tang X, Ning R, Shibata DK, Kurlan R. MR volumetric analysis of the human basal ganglia, normative data. Acad Radiol. 2000;7:627–634. doi: 10.1016/s1076-6332(00)80579-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Ikonomovic MD, Klunk WE, Abrahamson EE, Mathis CA, Price JC, Tsopelas ND, Lopresti BJ, Ziolko S, Bi W, Paljug WR, Debnath ML, Hope CE, Isanski BA, Hamilton RL, DeKosky ST. Post-mortem correlates of in vivo PiB-PET amyloid imaging in a typical case of Alzheimer's disease. Brain. 2008;131(6):1630–1645. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Jack CR, Bernstein MA, Fox NC, Thompson P, Alexander G, Harvey D, Borowski B, Britson PJ, Whitwell J, Ward C, Dale AM, Felmlee JP, Gunter JL, Hill DL, Killiany R, Schuff N, Fox-Bosetti S, Lin C, Studholme C, DeCarli CS, Krueger G, Ward HA, Metzger GJ, Scott KT, Mallozzi R, Blezek D, Levy J, Debbins JP, Fleisher AS, Albert M, Green R, Bartzokis G, Glover G, Mugler J, Weiner MW. The Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI), MRI methods. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2008;27:685–691. doi: 10.1002/jmri.21049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Javadapour A, Malhim GS, Ivandovski B, Chen X, Wen W, Sachdev P. Hippocampal volumes in adults with bipolar disorder. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 2010;22:55–62. doi: 10.1176/jnp.2010.22.1.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Jernigan TL, Archibald SL, Fennema-Notestine C, Gamst AC, Stout JC, Bonner J, Hesselink JR. Effects of age on tissues and regions of the cerebrum and cerebellum. Neurobiol Aging. 2001;22:581–94. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(01)00217-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Jorm AF, Korten AE, Henderson AS. The prevalence of dementia, a quantitative integration of the literature. Acta Pscyhiatr Scan. 1987;765:465–479. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1987.tb02906.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Jovicich J, Czanner S, Greve D, Haley E, van der Kouwe A, Gollub R, Kennedy D, Schmitt F, Brown G, Macfall J, Fischl B, Dale A. Reliability in multi-site structural MRI studies, Effects of gradient non-linearity correction on phantom and human data. Neuroimage. 2006;30:436–443. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kemppainen N, Ruottinen H, Nâgren K, Rinne JO. PET shows that striatal dopamine D1 and D2 receptors are differentially affected in AD. Neurology. 2000;55(2):205–209. doi: 10.1212/wnl.55.2.205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Klieger PS, Fett KA, Dimitsopulos T, Kurlan R. Asymmetry of basal ganglia perfusion in Tourette's syndrome shown by technetium-99m-HMPAO SPECT. The Journal of Nuclear Medicine. 1997;38(2):188–191. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Knowlton BJ, Mangels JA, Squire LR. A Neostriatal Habit Learning System in Humans. Science. 1996;273(5280):1399–1402. doi: 10.1126/science.273.5280.1399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kohannim O, Hua X, Hibar DP, Lee S, Chou YY, Toga AW, Jack CR, Jr, Weiner MW, Thompson PM the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Boosting power for AD clinical trials using classifiers based on multiple biomarkers. Neurobiology of Aging, Special Issue on ADNI studies. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.04.022. Unpublished results. PM. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Krishnan KR, Husain MM, McDonald WM, Doraiswamy PM, Figiel GS, Boyko OB, Ellinwood EH, Nemeroff CB. In vivo stereological assessment of caudate volume in man, effect of normal aging. Life Sci. 1990;47:1325–1329. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(90)90196-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Larisch R, Meyer W, Klimke A, Kehren F, Vosberg H, Muller-Gartner HW. Left-right asymmetry of striatal dopamine D2 receptors. Nucl Med Commun. 1998;19(8):781–787. doi: 10.1097/00006231-199808000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Laval SH, Dann JC, Butler RJ, Loftus J, Rue J, Leask SJ, Bass N, Comazzi M, Vita A, Nanko S, Shaw S, Peterson P, Shields G, Smith AB, Stewart J, Delisi LE, Crow TJ. Evidence for linkage to psychosis and cerebral asymmetry (relative hand skill) on the X chromosome. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B. Neuropsychiatric Genetics. 2004;81(5):420–427. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-8628(19980907)81:5<420::aid-ajmg11>3.0.co;2-e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Leow AD, Yanovsky I, Parikshark N, Hua X, Lee S, Toga AW, Jack CR, Jr, Bernstein MA, Britson PJ, Gunter JL, Ward CP, Borowski B, Shaw LM, Trojanowski JQ, Fleisner AS, Harvey D, Kornak J, Schuff N, Alexander GE, Weiner MW, Thompson PM the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative: A 1-year follow up study using tensor-based morphometry correlating degenerative rates, biomarkers, and cognition. Neuroimage. 2009;45(3):645–655. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Looi JCL, Lindberg O, Liberg B, Tatham V, Kumar R, Maller J, Millard E, Sachdev P, Hogberg G, Pagani M, Botes L, Engman EL, Yi Z, Svensson L, Wahlun LO. Volumetrics of the caudate nucleus, Reliability and validity of a new manual tracing protocol. Psychiatry Research. Neuroimaging. 2007;163:279–288. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Looi JC, Svensson L, Lindberg O, Zandbelt BB, Ostberg P, Orndahl E, Wahlund LO. Putaminal volume in frontotemporal lobar degeneration and Alzheimer's disease, differential volumes in dementia subtypes and controls. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2009;30(8):1552–1560. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A1640. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Mazziotta J, Toga A, Evans A, Fox P, Lancaster J, Zilles K, Woods R, Paus T, Simpson G, Pike B, Holmes C, Collins L, Thompson P, MacDonald D, Iacoboni M, Schormann T, Amunts K, Palomero-Gallagher N, Geyer S, Parsons L, Narr K, Kabani N, Le Goualher G, Boomsma D, Cannon T, Kawashima R, Mazoyer B. A probabilistic atlas and reference system for the human brain, International Consortium for Brain Mapping (ICBM) Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2001;356:1293–1322. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2001.0915. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Morra J, Tu Z, Apostolova LG, Green AE, Avedissian C, Madsen SK, Parikshak N, Hua X, Toga AW, Jack CR, Weiner MW, Thompson PM the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Iniative. Validation of a fully automated 3D hippocampal segmentation method using subjects with Alzheimer's disease, mild cognitive impairment, and elderly controls. Neuroimage. 2008;43(1):59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Morra JH, Tu Z, Apostolova LG, Green AE, Avedissian C, Madsen SK, Parikshak N, Hua X, Toga AW, Jack CR, Jr, Schuff N, Weiner MW, Thompson PM Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Automated 3D mapping of hippocampal atrophy and its clinical correlates in 400 subjects with Alzheimer's disease, mild cognitive impairment, and elderly controls. Human Brain Mapping. 2009a;30(90):2766–2788. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Morra JH, Tu Z, Apostolova LG, Green AE, Avedissian C, Madsen SK, Parikshak N, Toga AW, Jack CR, Jr, Schuff N, Weiner MW, Thompson PM, Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Automated mapping of hippocampal atrophy in 1-year repeat MRI data from 490 subjects with Alzheimer's disease, mild cognitive impairment, and elderly controls. Neuroimage. 2009b;45(1S):S3–S15. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Morris JC, Edland S, Clark C, Galasko D, Koss E, Mohs R, van Belle G, Fillenbaum G, Heyman A. The consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer's disease (CERAD). Part IV. Rates of cognitive change in the longitudinal assessment of probable Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1993;43(12):2457–2565. doi: 10.1212/wnl.43.12.2457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Nakamura T, Ghilardi MF, Mentis M, Dhawan V, Fukuda M, Hacking A, Moeller JR, Ghez C, Eidelberg D. Functional networks in motor sequence learning, abnormal topographies in Parkinson's disease. Hum Brain Mapp. 2001;12:42–60. doi: 10.1002/1097-0193(200101)12:1&#x0003c;42::AID-HBM40&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-D. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Peterson BS, Riddle MA, Cohen DJ, Katz LD, Smith JC, Leckman JF. Human basal ganglia volume asymmetries on magnetic resonance images. Magn Reson Imaging. 1993;11(4):493–498. doi: 10.1016/0730-725x(93)90468-s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Powell S, Magnotta VA, Johnson H, Jammalamadaka VK, Pierson R, Andreasen NC. Registration and machine learning-based automated segmentation of subcortical and cerebellar brain structures. Neuroimage. 2008;39:238–247. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Raji CA, Ho AJ, Parikshak NN, Becker JT, Lopez OL, Kuller LH, Hua X, Leow AD, Toga AW, Thompson PM. Brain Structure and Obesity. Human Brain Mapping. 2009 doi: 10.1002/hbm.20870. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Raz N, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Head D, Gunning-Dixon F, Acker JD. Differential aging of the human striatum, longitudinal evidence. American Journal of Neuroradiol. 2003;003(24):1849–56. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Rombouts SA, Barkhof F, Witter MP, Scheltens P. Unbiased whole-brain analysis of gray matter loss in Alzheimer's disease. Neurosci Lett. 2000;285:231–233. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(00)01067-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Rubin DC. Frontal-striatal circuits in cognitive aging, evidence for caudate involvement. Aging Neuropsych Cog. 1999;6:241–259. [Google Scholar]
  59. Schuff N, Woerner N, Boreta L, Kornfield T, Shaw LM, Trojnakowski JQ, Thompson PM, Jack CR, Jr, Weiner MW the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. MRI of hippocampal volume loss in early Alzheimer's disease in relation to ApoE genotype and biomarkers. Brain. 2009;132:1067–1077. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Shaw LM, Vanderstichele H, Knapik-Czajka M, Clark CM, Aisen PS, Petersen RC, Blennow K, Soares H, Simon A, Lewczuk P, Dean R, Siemers E, Potter W, Lee VMY, Trojanowski JQ the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Cerebrospinal fluid biomarker signature in Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative subjects. Annals of Neruology. 2009;65(4):403–413. doi: 10.1002/ana.21610. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Shen L, Saykin AJ, Kim S, Firpi HA, West JD, Risacher SL, McDonald BC, McHugh TL, Wishart HA, Flashman LA. Comparison of Manual and Automated Determination of Hippocampal Volumes in MCI and Early. AD Brain Imaging and Behavior. 2010;4:86–95. doi: 10.1007/s11682-010-9088-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Sled JG, Zijdenbos AP, Evans AC. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. Vol. 17. 1998. A nonparametric method for automatic correction of intensity nonuniformity in MRI data; pp. 87–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Szabo A, Lancaster JL, Xiong J, Cook C, Fox P. MR Imaging Volumetry of Subcortical structures and cerebellar hemispheres in normal persons. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2003;24:644–647. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Thompson PM, Hayashi KM, de Zubicaray G, Janke AL, Rose SE, Semple J, Herman D, Hong MS, Dittmer SS, Doddrell DM, Toga AW. Dynamics of gray matter loss in Alzheimer's disease. J Neurosci. 2003;23:994–1005. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-03-00994.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Thompson PM, Hayashi KM, De Zubicaray GI, Janke AL, Rose SE, Semple J, Hong MS, Herman DH, Gravano D, Doddrell DM, Toga AW. Mapping hippocampal and ventricular change in Alzheimer disease. Neuroimage. 2004;22:1754–1766. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Turner KC, Frost L, Linsenbardt D, McIlroy JR, Müller RA. Atypically diffuse functional connectivity between caudate nuclei and cerebral cortex in autism. Behav Brain Funct. 2006;2:34. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-2-34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Wang L, Beg F, Ratnanather T, Ceritoglu C, Younes L, Morris JC, Csernansky JG, Miller MI. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. Vol. 26. 2007. Large deformation diffeomorphism and momentum based hippocampal shape discrimination in dementia of the Alzheimer type; pp. 462–470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Wechsler D. The Psychological Corporation, Texas. 1987. Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised. [Google Scholar]
  69. Yamashita K, Yoshiura T, Hiwatashi A, Noguchi T, Togao O, Takayama Y, Nagao E, Kamano H, Hatakenaka M, Honda H. Volumetric Asymmetry and Differential Aging Effect of the Human Caudate Nucleus in Normal Individuals, A Prospective MR Imaging Study. J Neuroimaging. 2009 doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6569.2009.00403.x. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Yushkevich PA, Piven J, Hazlett HC, Smith RG, Ho S, Gee JC, Gerig G. User-guided 3D active contour segmentation of anatomical structures, Significantly improved efficiency and reliability. Neuroimage. 2006;31:1116–1128. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES