Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: Biol Psychol. 2010 Mar 6;84(1):13–25. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.02.005

Cortico-limbic circuitry and the airways: Insights from functional neuroimaging of respiratory afferents and efferents

Karleyton C Evans 1
PMCID: PMC2908728  NIHMSID: NIHMS185494  PMID: 20211221

Abstract

After nearly two decades of active research, functional neuroimaging has demonstrated utility in the identification of cortical, limbic, and paralimbic (cortico-limbic) brain regions involved in respiratory control and respiratory perception. Before the recent boon of human neuroimaging studies, the location of the principal components of respiratory-related cortico-limbic circuitry had been unknown and their function had been poorly understood. Emerging neuroimaging evidence in both healthy and patient populations suggests that cognitive and emotional/affective processing within cortico-limbic circuitry modulates respiratory control and respiratory perception. This paper will review functional neuroimaging studies of respiration with a focus on whole brain investigations of sensorimotor pathways that have identified respiratory-related neural circuitry known to overlap emotional/affective cortico-limbic circuitry. To aid the interpretation of present and future findings, the complexities and challenges underlying neuroimaging methodologies will also be reviewed as applied to the study of respiration physiology.

Keywords: neuroimaging, fMRI, PET, respiration, breathing, dyspnea, motor control, anxiety

1. Introduction

The act of breathing and the perception of breathing impairment are vital homeostatic functions essential to human life. It is widely accepted that brainstem respiratory centers mediate automatic respiratory rhythm (Feldman and Del Negro, 2006); however, the higher motor-cognitive and emotional centers thought to modulate the intrinsic respiratory rhythm as well as mediate the conscious perception of respiratory sensations remain poorly understood. Cortical, limbic and paralimbic sites for respiratory efferent/afferent processing were first identified by pioneering surgical/electrophysiological work in humans and animal models (as reviewed by (Hugelin, 1986)). With an acknowledgement of the ongoing debate over anatomical divisions of limbic vs. paralimbic architectonics (LeDoux, 2000), the integrated brain regions typically involved in motor-cognitive and emotional processing will be collectively referred to as “cortico-limbic” circuitry in this review of respiratory neuroimaging.

The last twenty years have witnessed an amazing acceleration and expansion of positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) technologies. These technological advances have facilitated the non-invasive, in vivo investigation of respiratory–related regional neural activity within cortico-limbic circuitry with spatial resolution on the order of a few millimeters and temporal resolution within fractions of a second. Advanced electroencephalographic event-related potential studies have also contributed to our understanding of cortico-limbic respiratory circuitry but are reviewed elsewhere (Davenport, 2009). Prior to these advances in imaging technology, evidence for respiratory-related activity within cortico-limbic circuitry was largely derived from invasive electrophysiological studies of animals and surgical patients (Foerster, 1936; Kaada and Jasper, 1952; Penfield and Faulk, 1955; Frysinger and Harper, 1990).

The findings from recent PET and fMRI studies reviewed here provide intriguing support for longstanding hypotheses regarding cortico-limbic modulation of respiratory control and respiratory sensation. This circuitry shares remarkable homology with cortico-limbic elements known to mediate cognitive and emotional/affective processing (Dolan, 2000; Cabeza and Kingstone, 2001) as well as primal alarm/threat processing (LeDoux, 2000; Denton, 2005). Recent studies which employed hypothesis-driven experimental paradigms involving cognitive (Evans et al., 2009a) and affective (von Leupoldt et al., 2008) breathing tasks provide preliminary evidence of cortico-limbic modulation of respiratory control and respiratory sensation. In view of the wide-reaching implications regarding this shared circuitry, an integrated neural systems model for cortico-limbic influences on respiratory control and respiratory perception is considered as supported by the literature reviewed (Figure 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Proposed cortico-limbic model for respiratory sensorimotor integration. Regional color-coding follows the same scheme presented in Table 1: blue, predominant motor functionality; red, predominant sensory functionality; purple, mixed sensorimotor functionality. Abbreviations: ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; KF/PB, Kölliker–Fuse/parabrachial nuclei; LC, locus ceruleus; NTS, nucleus tract of solitarius; PAG, periaqueductal grey; SMA supplementary motor area.

Our understanding of cortico-limbic circuitry as related to respiratory control and perception should be viewed as a work in progress. Although the identification and function of many of the cortico-limbic elements discussed herein have been validated across imaging modalities and by various experimental designs, several of the recent findings reviewed require replication and should be considered with caution. Scholars within and outside of the field of neuroimaging, as well as the lay press, have voiced growing concern over the persuasive impact of neuroimaging data given its intrinsic vulnerability to Type I Error (Check, 2005; Dolan, 2008; McCabe and Castel, 2008; Poldrack, 2008; Hamilton, 2009). In keeping with this concern, this author is humbled to state that the discipline of respiratory neuroimaging is still in its infancy. The framework provided here is anticipated to serve as a primer on the current knowledge regarding the role of cortico-limbic circuitry in respiratory control and respiratory sensation and also serve as a guide for the interpretation of future findings given the complexities and challenges underlying neuroimaging studies of respiration.

2. Design and analytic considerations for neuroimaging studies of respiration

A. Neuroimaging methods

The majority of neuroimaging studies of respiration have employed blood flow sensitive techniques such as oxygen15 PET (O-15 PET) and blood oxygenation level dependent fMRI (BOLD-fMRI) to provide indirect measures of neural activity. Arterial spin labeling (ASL) is a relatively new fMRI approach which has only been used in a handful of published respiration studies (e.g., MacIntosh et al., 2008; Pattinson et al., 2009a; Pattinson, 2009b). These techniques are based on the assumption that the magnitude of regional brain activity at any given time is proportional to the regional blood flow (O-15 PET, ASL-fMRI) or oxygen content (BOLD-fMRI) and each method yields statistical maps that reflect regional brain activity.

The time-course for the respiratory process under study should guide the choice of imaging modality as each method confers an optimized temporal resolution. Typical BOLD-fMRI block designs require rapidly alternating conditions (in the order of seconds) for adequate statistical power with standard analytic approaches. Processes with relatively short inter-trial latencies (e.g., within breath, or breath-by-breath time course) may be best addressed with event-related BOLD-fMRI (Buckner, 1998), although this approach requires further validation and replication (Buckner, 1998; Evans et al., 2009a; Evans et al., 2009b). Processes with relatively long inter-trial latencies, may be best addressed with O-15 PET in which the inherent inter-scan latency is 1.5–2 minutes (secondary to oxygen15 half-life). However, innovative, non-standard analytic approaches to BOLD-fMRI block designs have been shown to confer adequate power at inter-trial latencies up to 2 minutes (Evans, 2009c). The PET isotope fluorine18 deoxyglucose (FDG) has a substantially long half-life, which permits the examination of neural activity over a 30 minute period preceding actual image acquisition. Thus, FDG-PET could be used in protocols that involve ambulatory respiratory tasks of long duration, yet few respiratory neuroimaging studies have used FDG-PET (Garakani et al., 2007; Tashiro et al., 2008). ASL-fMRI has also been found to support protocols with significant inter-trial latencies (i.e., minutes to hours) (Wang et al., 2003).

In addition to temporal resolution, there are further considerations regarding advantages and limitations for each neuroimaging method. O-15 PET can provide absolute measures of rCBF and had been considered the “gold standard” method for many years. This quantitative feature can be viewed as a significant advantage over BOLD-fMRI which relies on relative between condition (or group by condition interaction) comparisons. However, with recent advances in MRI technology, ASL-fMRI sequences can now also provide absolute measures of rCBF. Contemporary criticisms of O-15 PET include the radiation risk to subjects, scarcity of research institutions with O-15 capability, and cost (hourly rates are 2–3 times those for fMRI). Furthermore, compared to fMRI, PET confers inferior spatial and temporal resolution. Landmark findings established with O-15 PET for both motor (Ramsay et al., 1993) and sensory (Banzett et al., 2000) respiratory circuits have been replicated and extended with fMRI (Evans et al., 1999; Evans et al., 2002; McKay et al., 2003). Nonetheless, fMRI has limitations, such as physical constraints related to the exclusion of metallic instrumentation and small diameter scanner bores, which can introduce claustrophobic/affective confounds. In addition, fMRI is vulnerable to susceptibility artifacts, common to ventral frontal and medial temporal regions (as reviewed by (Jezzard and Clare, 1999; Merboldt et al., 2001)). fMRI is also particularly vulnerable to confounds related to the respiration itself (considered in detail below).

The focus of this paper is on functional neuroimaging studies of respiratory afferents/efferents within cortico-limbic circuitry, yet it is important to also acknowledge structural neuroimaging methods that have provided complementary findings. For instance, diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) provides an index of the number and composition of axons within white matter tracts and voxel-base morphometry (VBM) enables the quantitative assessment of the volume of specific brain structures of interest. Few respiratory studies have used these methods, yet the published structural findings provide complementary support for the integrated cortico-limbic model we consider herein (Kumar et al., 2008; Macey et al., 2008; Pattinson et al., 2009a).

B. Breathing tasks and stimuli

An overarching aim in respiratory neuroimaging is to address the questions of “which” brain regions are involved and “how” these structures mediate function within afferent and efferent respiratory pathways. The isolation of a particular pathway under study serves as a significant challenge in the design of experimental protocols as the existing evidence suggests that these pathways are bidirectional and considerably interconnected. As reviewed below, attempts to isolate predominantly motor respiratory circuitry have been fairly successful yet not without caveats (e.g., breathing to targets engage neural circuits involved in attention, monitoring, and motivation). Attempts to isolate afferent respiratory circuitry on the other hand have been difficult as the integration of afferent (respiratory sensation) and efferent (motor command) signals together with cognitions and affect have been proposed to jointly contribute to the neural processing underlying respiratory perception (Lansing et al., 2009).

Most neuroimaging studies of respiratory sensation have employed stimuli that evoke dyspnea (breathlessness). It is important to appreciate that the general term “dyspnea” subsumes several sensations such as the sense of respiratory work/effort, chest tightness, and air hunger (an uncomfortable urge to breathe) (Lansing et al., 2009). Air hunger has been suggested as the dominant component of dyspnea (Smith et al., 2009) in pulmonary patients, however, air hunger is often experienced by patients in combination with sensations of increased work/effort or chest tightness (Manning and Schwartstein, 1995). Air hunger is one of the few stimuli used in respiratory neuroimaging studies to actually target one of the primary component sensations of dyspnea (Banzett et al., 2000). Even though respiratory loads have been used (Peiffer et al., 2001; von Leupoldt et al., 2008), a work/effort stimulus of substantial duration has yet to be investigated during imaging. Other stimuli used in respiratory neuroimaging studies have included: hypercapnia (Corfield et al., 1995; Harper et al., 2005), hypoxia (Macey et al., 2005), and breath-hold (Macefield et al., 2006; McKay et al., 2008; Pattinson et al., 2009a; Pattinson, 2009b). Few studies have imposed these stimuli for a sustained duration. Compared to the 3.5 minute air hunger epochs of Banzett et al. (2000), the 15 second breath-holds of McKay et al. (2008) and 24 second epochs of inspiratory load (work/effort) of von Leupoldt et al. (2008) could be viewed as relatively short. Given the variability in subjective reporting of respiratory sensations, the stimuli intended to evoke a given sensation should be well characterized and validated prior to neuroimaging. For example, Banzett and colleagues conducted a series of studies which characterized stimuli that evoke air hunger (Banzett et al., 1989; Banzett et al., 1990; Manning et al., 1992; Banzett et al., 1996; Harty et al., 1996) prior to conducting PET and fMRI studies of air hunger (Banzett et al., 2000; Evans et al., 2002).

C. Considerations for respiratory-related confounds

In contrast to the typical stimuli used in cognitive neuroscience with inert properties relative to the imaging modality (e.g., tactile or visual stimuli), the stimuli typically used in studies of human respiration can serve as confounds that pose significant challenges to neuroimaging (e.g., respiratory loads, hyperpnea, hypercapnia and hypoxia). Respiratory stimuli and associated confounds may not be fully orthogonal to the experimental design and thereby bias or obscure the detection of between-condition differences in regional brain activity.

i. Confounds related to changes in gas tensions

Perhaps the most common and concerning cause of respiratory-related confounds in neuroimaging studies of respiration is task-related changes in arterial gas tensions. For example, hypocapnia causes cerebral vasoconstriction and hypercapnia causes cerebral vasodilatation, both of which serve as significant confounds to blood flow sensitive techniques such as O-15 PET, BOLD-fMRI, and ASL-fMRI (Ramsay et al., 1993; Posse et al., 2001; MacIntosh et al., 2008).

Experimental protocols designed to avoid task-related changes in arterial gases can minimize the associated confounds. Some studies of respiratory motor control used mechanical ventilation as a passive condition and volitional breathing (targeted to the minute ventilation of the passive condition) as an active, comparison condition and thereby avoided between-condition differences in partial pressure of carbon dioxide (Pco2) (Colebatch et al., 1991; Ramsay et al., 1993; Evans et al., 1999).

Another strategy is to use a specialized breathing apparatus that minimizes condition-related changes in Pco2. The contrivance developed by Banzett et al. (2000) employs a dynamic dead-space that maintains nearly constant Pco2 and Po2 within ±1 mmHg. McKay et al. (2003) used this apparatus to minimize changes in end-tidal gases between conditions of volitional hyperpnea (average tidal volume = 1.3 liters) and resting spontaneous breathing (average tidal volume = 0.5 liters). More recently, computer controlled devices have been designed that utilize either feed-forward (Prisman et al., 2008) or feed-back mechanisms. One rather elaborate computer controlled feed-back apparatus enables independent control of the fraction of inspired O2 and CO2 every breath, a process that has been called dynamic end-tidal forcing (DEF) (Wise et al., 2007).

An alternative strategy to contend with changes in gas tensions is to model the anticipated neuroimaging signal change (associated with changes in gas tensions) independent from the condition of interest. Breath-hold studies have employed this strategy, where the non-specific BOLD-fMRI signal changes associated with the rise in Pco2 during the breath-hold were determined from a separate hypercapnic condition of equal magnitude of change in Pco2 to that observed during the breath-hold (McKay et al., 2008; Pattinson et al., 2009a; Pattinson, 2009b). Thus, regional BOLD signal changes reflecting sensorimotor neural activity could then be dissociated from non-specific effects related to hypercapnia during breath-hold.

Despite best efforts to keep gas tensions constant, small breath to breath oscillations in gas tensions are unavoidable in spontaneously breathing subjects even with the most advanced breathing apparatus (e.g., DEF discussed above). Indeed, BOLD-fMRI signal can be modulated by end-tidal Pco2 fluctuations as small as 1 mmHg (Wise et al., 2004). A number of analytic strategies are currently available to correct for these fluctuations. One approach is to model end-tidal Pco2 (acquired during scanning) as a null regressor in BOLD-fMRI analyses (see von Leupoldt et al. (2008), Evans et al. (2009a), Pattinson et al. (2009b)). Another approach is to model the whole brain global signal as a null regressor (Corfield et al., 2001) since the global signal is tightly correlated to Pco2 (Wise et al., 2004; Chang, 2009). This approach has been widely used in BOLD-fMRI studies of respiration (Evans et al., 2002; McKay et al., 2003; McKay et al., 2008; von Leupoldt et al., 2008; Evans et al., 2009a; von Leupoldt et al., 2009a; von Leupoldt et al., 2009b). The global regressor approach is limited by its reliance on the assumption of homogeneity in global signal across the whole brain, though Macey et al. (2004) have refined the global regressor approach by employing voxel-wise time-course models to account for regional variability in global effects. Another caveat to the global regressor approach surrounds its susceptibility to induce Type I Error. Recent empiric studies of connectivity across brain networks have shown the global regressor approach to introduce spurious anti-correlations (de-activations) (Murphy et al., 2009; Van Dijk et al., 2010).

In addition to these regressor methods, recent studies have acquired both BOLD- and ALS-fMRI images during conditions that change arterial gas tensions. Thus condition-related changes in both global and regional CBF can be quantified and incorporated into analytic models to account for confounding effects of CBF changes on the BOLD signal (MacIntosh, 2008; Pattinson, 2009b).

ii. Confounds related to respiratory movement

All neuroimaging techniques are vulnerable to confounds related to gross head movement. Respiratory stimuli (e.g., resistive loads) or changes in respiration (e.g., hyperpnea) can result in head movement within and/or movement in/out of the imaging plane. Both Type I and Type II errors may be encountered with task-related movement. BOLD-fMRI is exquisitely sensitive to movement related artifacts (Birn et al., 1998; Andersson et al., 2001). Notably, subtle movement of the lung and chest-wall during the respiratory cycle can induce shifts in the magnetic field (Birn et al., 2006; Harvey, 2008; Birn et al., 2009). Within-subject image realignment is a standard pre-processing step that aligns the series of images acquired from a given subject (Friston et al., 1995; Friston et al., 2007), thereby correcting for gross head movement prior to performing statistical tests on the data. To further correct for the effects of movement-related variance that persist after pre-processing, the realignment parameters (calculated during the realignment procedure) can be included as null regressors in BOLD-fMRI analyses (Evans et al., 2009a). This approach has been shown to enhance neural signal detection (Lund et al., 2005), particularly in subcortical structures (Friston et al., 1996). However, this approach is highly conservative, given its vulnerability to the Type II error (e.g., may exclude regional imaging signal driven by respiratory-related neural activity that correlate with movement). Other methods employ peripheral measures (e.g., chest expansion via “respiratory belt” systems) to model respiratory movement such as retrospective correction of physiological motion effects (RETROICOR) (Glover et al., 2000; Harvey, 2008) and respiration volume per time (RVT) (Birn et al., 2006). Similar to the global regressor method, these methods incorporate the confounding signals into image analyses as null regressors.

Given this background, the question arises regarding which method for addressing respiratory confounds may be superior. This question may be best addressed by considering the specific respiratory conditions an investigator uses to test a given hypothesis. Examples of these methods are provided within the discussion of empirical findings in the following sections of this paper. Few studies have systematically evaluated these methods in a quantitative manner (see Van Dijk et al., 2010). There is some evidence to suggest RVT could potentially be used as a proxy for Pco2 to model the variance in BOLD signal due to breathing. Chang et al. (2009) demonstrated respiratory motion (as modeled by RVT) to be well correlated with breath-by-breath fluctuations in Pco2 in the eucapnic range. Moreover, spatio-temporal variance in the BOLD signal has been well characterized by both RVT and Pco2 when modeled separately (Chang, 2009). On the other hand, there is substantial support for using the global regressor to account for variance in BOLD signal under various respiratory conditions (i.e., rest, volitional breathing maneuvers, hypercapnia, hypoxia, etc.) (Corfield et al., 2001; Birn et al., 2006; Van Dijk et al., 2010; McKay et al., 2010). However, as noted above, findings of anti-correlation (de-activation) must be interpreted with caution in analyses that use the global regressor (Murphy et al., 2009; Van Dijk et al., 2010).

D. Neuroimaging Analyses

Great care and strong a priori hypotheses are required in performing neuroimaging analyses as neuroimaging statistics are complex, based on many assumptions, and have several different approaches with no one optimal approach (Marchini and Presanis, 2004; Friston et al., 2007). Two general approaches to data analysis have been used in neuroimaging studies of respiration: (1) voxel-wise searches (e.g., statistical parametric mapping (SPM)) and (2) region of interest (ROI) based searches, sometimes referred to as small volume correction (SVC). Voxel-wise SPM approaches implement statistical comparisons across the whole brain. SPM approaches convey several advantages over ROI-based approaches, particularly when combined with a priori hypotheses and appropriately conservative significance thresholds (thereby reducing Type I errors). ROI approaches involve testing one or more specific brain regions of interest and analyzing data in only those regions. ROI approaches are particularly vulnerable to Type II errors, since effects in regions other than those driven by a priori hypotheses may be neglected, and effects within sub-territories of a given ROI may be overlooked due to dilution that occurs from averaging across the entire ROI.

ROI approaches have enjoyed increasing use in respiratory neuroimaging studies as investigators have begun to attribute a priori confidence to certain regions within the cortico-limbic circuitry (e.g., Maldjian et al., 2003; Eickhoff et al., 2006; von Leupoldt et al., 2008; von Leupoldt et al., 2009b). ROIs may be defined by coordinates from prior studies or by probabilistic atlases (Tzourio-Mazonyer et al., 2002; Maldjian et al., 2003; Eickhoff et al., 2006) or by the structural or functional data from the subjects under study (Nieto-Castanon et al., 2003). In addition to the SVC capabilities mentioned above, ROIs can also be used to ascertain functional specificity for a given region. Specifically, raw data may be extracted from a ROI to demonstrate the time course and/or magnitude of neuroimaging signals for a given condition (see http://marsbar.sourceforge.net/) (e.g., Harper et al., 2005; Macey et al., 2005). Like all approaches to neuroimaging analysis, ROI approaches have their caveats, criticisms, and cautions (Poldrack, 2007; Mitsis et al., 2008). Importantly, in contrast to voxel-wise SPM approaches, ROI approaches have not yet undergone systematic, rigorous validity testing. Another important consideration is the risk for reification that can occur when the data extracted from a ROI in a particular data set is used to make further inferences about the same data set, leading to over-interpretation of extended findings (a process aptly referred to as “double-dipping”) (Poldrack, 2007; Kriegeskorte et al., 2009).

Further considerations are required when performing group and between group analyses. The majority of PET and early fMRI respiratory studies used “fixed-effect” analytic models that are based on the assumption that the condition-related effects for each individual make an equivalent or fixed contribution to observed group effects. More recent respiratory neuroimaging studies have used “random-effect” analytic models that account for random within-subject and between-subject (intersession) effects. This is particularly important in fMRI studies where inter-session inconsistencies of BOLD signal are common (Friston et al., 2007). Random-effect analytic models permit formal inferences to be made about the population from which the study sample was drawn. However, compared to fixed effect models, random effect models confer reduced sensitivity due to fewer degrees of freedom (Lazar et al., 2002).

Another approach to group data is the “conjunction” or “main effects of condition” analytic model. Conjunction analyses are statistical tests that determine the probability of regional effects being in common among two (or more) different conditions (e.g., regional effect X occurs in condition A and condition B). Interpretive caution is required particularly when a conjunction analysis is found to be non-significant. Notably, an erroneous conjunction inference can be made in the case where the magnitude of regional effect X is observed to significantly differ between conditions A and B, yet regional effect X would be considered as statistically significant in independent analyses of condition A and condition B (Nichols et al., 2005). It is therefore informative to report such independent findings as they may strengthen or weaken the overall interpretation of the findings. Each of the approaches to neuroimaging analysis has strengths and weaknesses, which should be considered in interpreting the findings in the respiratory neuroimaging literature.

3. Neuroimaging studies of respiratory efferents and volitional motor control

Pioneering O-15 PET studies by researchers at the Charing Cross Hospital, London UK, heralded the first maps of respiratory efferents localized to the motor cortex (Colebatch et al., 1991; Ramsay et al., 1993). In their first study, Colebatch et al. (1991) measured rCBF in healthy subjects during conditions of passive mechanical ventilation and active voluntary targeted hyperpnea at similar minute ventilation, minimizing potential Pco2 confounds. The active condition of hyperpnea was associated with increased rCBF bilaterally in the primary motor cortex and supplementary motor area (SMA) near midline. Increased rCBF in premotor and cerebellar regions was also observed during the active inspiratory condition. Using a similar O-15 PET protocol, a separate cohort of subjects was studied by Ramsay et al. (1993) during conditions of active inspiration and active expiration compared to a condition of mechanical ventilation. The findings of respiratory-related increased rCBF within motor cortical, SMA, pre-motor and cerebellar regions were replicated. As an extension to the previous study, increased rCBF was also observed within the thalamus for both active breathing conditions.

In an additional O-15 neuroimaging study, the Charing Cross Hospital investigators tested the hypothesis that increased respiratory force would evoke increased neural activity and corresponding increased rCBF within the previously identified elements of respiratory motor circuitry (Fink et al., 1996). Scans acquired during graded conditions of inspiratory load were contrasted with conditions of passive mechanical ventilation and unloaded spontaneous ventilation. This study further extended the previous findings observed during hyperpnea as significant increases in rCBF were reported with increasing respiratory force in the superior motor cortex, SMA, and premotor area. Moreover, additional brain regions were detected with incremental force, in particular: the prefrontal cortex, inferolateral sensorimotor cortex, basal ganglia, midbrain, and cerebellar vermis. The authors concluded that increases in inspiratory muscle force indeed results in greater motor cortical activity as well as the additional recruitment of subcortical regions associated with motor control.

The O-15 PET studies of Colebatch et al. (1991), Ramsay et al. (1993), and Fink et al. (1996) were all carefully designed to optimize visualization of regional volitional respiratory-motor neural activity (via changes in rCBF). Taken together, the birth of “respiratory neuroimaging” as a field may be attributed to these nascent O-15 PET studies. However, the findings from these studies should be considered in balance with a few limitations. Although the use of passive mechanical ventilation as a control condition in these studies was critical to minimizing cerebrovascular imaging confounds due to task-related changes in Pco2, mechanical ventilation induces confounds related to (1) upper airway positive pressure and (2) the cognitive requirement for subjects to relax their airway, chest wall, and diaphragm. Lastly, these studies were constrained to the spatio-temporal limitations of O-15 PET noted above (Section 2A, “Neuroimaging Methods”).

In an effort to further replicate and extend the early O-15 PET findings, Evans et al. (1999) conducted the first three-dimensional BOLD-fMRI study of volitional breathing at 1.5 Tesla. BOLD signal was acquired during conditions of passive mechanical ventilation and active voluntary targeted hyperpnea, where minute ventilation and Pco2 were similar between conditions. Compared to the passive condition, BOLD signal in the active hyperpneic condition was greater in the sensorimotor cortex, premotor cortex, and SMA with stereotactic coordinates comparable to the early O-15 PET findings. Additional loci were identified within the prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia. The fMRI modality provided enhanced temporal and spatial resolution compared to the earlier O-15 PET studies. However, compared to more recent BOLD-fMRI studies conducted with higher magnetic field strength and expanded field of view, this study was limited in its sensitivity and brain coverage (i.e., limited field of view precluded visualization of the cerebellum and brainstem).

A subsequent 2 Tesla BOLD fMRI study by McKay et al. (2003) provided the first in vivo evidence of simultaneous cortical and brainstem activity during volitional hyperpnea in humans. In contrast to previous studies of hyperpnea, resting spontaneous breathing was used as the passive condition in place of mechanical ventilation. The specialized breathing contrivance discussed earlier (Banzett et al., 2000) maintained nearly constant Pco2 (±2 mmHg) despite dynamic, threefold increases in ventilation from resting spontaneous breathing to active hyperpnea. This study design also minimized the potential confounds noted above related to mechanical ventilation. During hyperpneic periods, BOLD-fMRI signal increases were observed within the sensorimotor cortex, SMA basal ganglia, thalamus, cerebellum, premotor and prefrontal cortices. Upon small volume correction, an additional loci was identified in midline superior dorsal medulla, likely representing task-related neural activity within the nucleus of the solitary tract (NTS) or the nucleus ambiguous. The medullary finding was thought to represent excitatory or inhibitory activity from either motor cortical efferents or from pulmonary vagal afferents. Despite the uncertainty surrounding functional significance, this study emphasized the potential for in vivo fMRI assessments of respiratory-related neural activity in the human brainstem. Indeed, subsequent methodological advances (see Harvey et al., 2008) have facilitated enhanced sensitivity and specificity in brainstem imaging (e.g., Pattinson et al., 2009a)

In summary, the findings from neuroimaging studies of volitional motor control of breathing converge to define a cortico-striatal-bulbar-cerebellar circuitry (Table 1, Figure 1). Despite the noted caveats for individual studies, findings localized to the sensorimotor cortex, cerebellum, supplementary motor and premotor areas have been common across most neuroimaging studies of motor control. Those neuroimaging studies with increased sensitivity (e.g., fMRI, increased force during PET) identified additional respiratory motor activity within the basal ganglia, thalamus, and prefrontal cortices.

Table 1.

Summary of regional findings from whole brain neuroimaging studies of respiratory efferent/afferent pathways published in the period 1991 to 2010. Regional color-coding follows the same scheme presented in Figure 1: blue, predominant motor functionality; red, predominant sensory functionality; purple, mixed sensorimotor functionality. Studies are listed in descending order of publication for each functional category.

Study Contrast superior
motor
inferior-lateral
motor
pre-motor pons medulla SMA thalamus basal
ganglia
cerebellar
hemispheres
cerebellar
vermis
DLPFC insula frontal
operculum
ACC amygdala hippocamups midbrain

Studies of volitional motor control
Colebatch et al., 1991 hyperpnea vs. mechanical ventilaiton
Ramsay et al., 1993a hyperpnea vs. mechanical ventilaiton
Fink et al., 1996 inspiratory load vs. mechanical ventilation
Evans et al., 1999 hyperpnea vs. mechanical ventilaiton
McKay et al., 2003 hyperpnea vs. spontaneous ventilation

Studies of sensory perception (dyspnea) dyspnea intensity
Corfield et al., 1995 hypercapnia NR
Banzett et al., 2000a air hunger 65%
Peiffer et al., 2001 inspiratory/expiratory load 52%
Brannan et al., 2001** hypercapnia 73%
Parsons et al., 2001** hypercapnia 73%
Evans et al., 2002 air hunger 50%
von Leupoldt et al., 2008 inspiratory load + affective visual stimuli 52%
von Leupoldt et al., 2009a inspiratory load vs. thermal pain NR

Studies of combined sensorimotor function
Macefield et al., 2006 breath-hold
Mazzone et al., 2007 urge to cough
McKay et al., 2008 breath-hold
Evans et al., 2009a spontaneous breathing
Pattinson et al., 2009b breath-hold * * * * *
McKay et al., 2010 hypercapnia + hypoxia

Studies of patients vs. healthy controls
Macey et al., 2004b forced expiratory load, CCHS vs. HC > < < < < < > > >
Harper et al., 2005 hypercapnia, CCHS vs. HC > < > > < < < > >
Macey et al., 2005 hypoxia, CCHS vs. HC > > < > > < > <
Macey et al., 2006 inspiratory load, OSA vs. HC < > < < < < < <
Garakani et al., 2007 doxapram PD vs. HC > > < > > > >
von Leupoldt et al., 2009b inspiratory load, AST vs. HC <

For clarity, only the most significant findings of regional brain activation (n.b., some studies reported de-activations) are presented for each study, indicated by a check-mark (✓). Only primary between-group findings are presented for studies that investigated patient and control cohorts. Greater than (>) or less than (<) symbols indicate significant regional differences using the order convention of patient group, followed by symbol, followed by control group. The reader is referred to the text and original publications for details related to laterality and stereotactic localization.

*

Asterisk indicates regions down regulated by remifentanil (see (Pattinson, 2009b).

**

Double asterisk indicates that the study findings are distributed across several publications; reported findings in the table reflect the cited study.

Abbreviations: ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; AST, asthma; CCHS, congenital central hypoventilation syndrome; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; HC, healthy control; NR, not reported; OSA, obstructive sleep apnea; PD, panic disorder; SMA supplementary motor area.

4. Neuroimaging studies of respiratory afferents and sensory perception (dyspnea)

Until very recently, the principal cortico-limbic components of human respiratory afferent pathways had been largely unknown, mainly due to the lack of useful animal models or relevant clinical lesion studies. The majority of neuroimaging studies that have formally investigated respiratory perception have focused on dyspnea and accordingly, these studies will serve as the focus here. As noted above in Section 2B “Breathing tasks and stimuli,” the general term “dyspnea” subsumes several distinct sensations such as the sense of respiratory work/effort, chest tightness, and air hunger (Lansing et al., 2009). It is quite likely that these different dyspnea sensations may have distinct cortico-limbic representation; however, studies designed to explore this possibility have yet to be published. Thus far, there have been few hypothesis-driven neuroimaging studies specifically designed to investigate dyspnea circuitry. Most dyspnea imaging studies have used either air hunger, respiratory loads (work/effort) or hypercapnic stimuli.

Early evidence for cortico-limbic involvement in respiratory perception was provided by an O-15 PET study of hypercapnic-stimulated breathing versus eucapnic passive mechanical ventilation (Corfield et al., 1995). After correction for between-condition differences in global CBF (modeled as a null regressor via ANCOVA), the hypercapnic condition was associated with increased rCBF localized to the midbrain, cerebellar vermis, and cortico-limbic regions, namely the hypothalamus, thalamus, hippocampal/parahippocampal, fusiform gyri, anterior cingulate, insular and prefrontal cortices. Interestingly, there was no evidence of increased rCBF in the primary motor cortex or SMA, suggesting a lack of motor cortical involvement during the increased ventilatory drive associated with hypercapnia. It should be underscored that this study was not explicitly designed to dissociate sensory from motor neural processes related to the hypercapnic condition, nor to examine dyspnea in isolation. Notably, respiratory sensations for the hypercapnic condition were characterized (e.g., “urge to breathe” and “work/effort”) retrospectively during a supplementary study conducted outside the scanner. The interpretation of these findings can be held with one final caution related to the use of the global null regressor. The temporal resolution of O-15 PET (combined with the study design) precluded the dissociation of inhomogeneous regional vascular responses to hypercapnia from neural responses related to respiratory sensation and ventilatory drive.

Subsequently, three O-15 PET studies of dyspnea were published in five reports from the period 2000–2001 (Banzett et al., 2000; Brannan et al., 2001; Liotti et al., 2001; Parsons et al., 2001; Peiffer et al., 2001). Using mechanical ventilation under a constant hypercapnic background, Banzett et al. (2000a) provoked air hunger (>60% of scale) in healthy subjects during 3.5 minute epochs of low tidal volume (~0.6 liters), alternating with relief epochs of high tidal volume (~1.2 liters). Increased rCBF during air hunger epochs was localized to the anterior insula/operculum (bilaterally), SMA, thalamus and basal ganglia. Peiffer et al. (2001) studied healthy subjects during loaded breathing. Comparison of the high load condition (~50% of scale) to the unloaded condition demonstrated increased rCBF in the right anterior insula/operculum, SMA, left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), pons, cerebellar vermis and cerebellar hemispheres. In a three-paper series Brannan et al. (2001), Liotti et al. (2001), and Parsons et al. (2001) reported on differential rCBF effects observed in healthy subjects during various breathing challenges. Most notably, widespread increases in cerebellar and cortico-limbic rCBF was reported in a contrast of hyperoxic-hypercapnia (Pco2 > 60 mmHg, ~90% inspired O2) versus hyperoxic resting breathing (Table 1). Taken together, these early O-15 PET studies of dyspnea used experimental paradigms that varied significantly, yet the findings of the studies converge to reveal a common limbic/paralimbic circuitry during laboratory induced dyspnea.

More recently, fMRI has been used in studies of respiratory perception. Using a constant Pco2 air hunger protocol similar to the one used by Banzett et al. (2000a), but with shorter, 42-second air hunger epochs, Evans et al. (2002) studied healthy subjects with BOLD-fMRI at 2 Tesla. Voxel-wise SPM analyses were performed in which subjective air hunger ratings, acquired every breath, served as the primary independent regressor and the global BOLD signal served as a null regressor. The findings from this first fMRI study of air hunger replicated and extended those reported in the preceding PET study of Banzett et al. (2000a). Specifically, air hunger was correlated with BOLD signal increases in the anterior insula/operculum (bilaterally), SMA, thalamus, and basal ganglia. Additional findings were localized to the bilateral DLPFC, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), amygdala, cerebellar hemispheres and cerebellar vermis. Limitations of this study include a transient air hunger stimulus (i.e., not steady-state, compared to Banzett et al. (2000a)), and an analytic method that was incapable of dissociating the neural activity related to dyspnea perception from the neural activity related to subjects’ rating their dyspnea.

Evans (2009c) recently presented work in progress in which BOLD-fMRI time-courses were extracted from cortico-limbic functional ROIs (amygdala, anterior insula and ACC) in a separate cohort of healthy subjects during 90 second steady-state epochs of air hunger alternating with epochs of relief. The BOLD-fMRI time-course data were observed to mirror the time-course of subjective reports of air hunger and relief (maximal response within 30 seconds). The findings suggest that recent O-15 PET studies of dyspnea relief (e.g., Peiffer et al. (2008) may not have adequate temporal resolution to fully characterize the neural dynamics underlying changes in respiratory sensation.

Von Leupoldt and colleagues have conducted several fMRI studies (3 Tesla) using brief inspiratory loads (von Leupoldt et al., 2008, 2009a; von Leupoldt et al., 2009b) (Table 1). In one study, von Leupoldt et al. (2008), employed 24-second inspiratory loads (dyspnea reported as 52% of scale) during simultaneous presentation of various affective visual stimuli (scenes previously validated to evoke either positive or negative emotions). Subjective intensity ratings of “increased work and effort of breathing” were similar between different visual stimuli conditions. In contrast to intensity ratings, dyspnea unpleasantness ratings were reported as significantly greater during negative than positive visual stimuli. Differential BOLD-fMRI signal was reported in the amygdala and the anterior insula upon a condition interaction analysis (loaded-negative > unloaded-negative × loaded-positive > unloaded-positive) after statistical correction for small volume. The authors concluded that the amygdala and insula modulated unpleasantness of perceived dyspnea. This conclusion draws on translations from the field of affective neuroscience (LeDoux, 2000; Lang and Davis, 2006; Etkin and Wager, 2007) yet is incompletely supported by the data. The effect seen on dyspnea unpleasantness ratings is non-specific as are the effects observed in the insula and amygdala which could simply be attributed to attention bias toward the negative visual stimuli irrespective of dyspnea perception. Notably, the amygdala is known to respond to overt as well as subliminal visual threats (Morris et al., 1998; Whalen et al., 1998). The findings of von Leupoldt et al. (2008) are nonetheless intriguing and beckon studies with definitive, falsifiable hypotheses to further define the intersection of affective processing and dyspnea perception.

Overall, the insular cortex has been the most consistently reported structure across all the reviewed studies of sensory perception (Table 1). The amygdala, ACC, SMA, DLPFC, thalamus, cerebellar hemispheres, and cerebellar vermis have also been frequently implicated in these studies suggesting a distributed cortico-limbic-cerebellar circuitry to mediate respiratory sensation. The inferior-lateral motor cortex was uniquely activated during inspiratory load stimuli, presumably due to recruitment of upper airway musculature during loads. However, the superior motor cortex, known to drive volitional breathing, failed to demonstrate significant activity during any studies of provocative sensory stimuli.

5. Complementary neuroimaging studies of sensorimotor respiratory circuitry

A. Studies of combined sensorimotor networks

Neural activity across multimodal networks is likely to influence even resting spontaneous breathing as cognitive and emotional demands are thought to modulate the intrinsic brainstem respiratory rhythm (Shea et al., 1987; Shea, 1996; Boiten, 1998). Using a novel event-related BOLD-fMRI approach at 1.5 Tesla, Evans et al. (2009a) demonstrated synchronized respiratory related neural activity with each breath across a distributed cortico-limbic-bulbar circuit during resting spontaneous breathing. Notably, breath-by-breath neural activity was observed at every level of the brainstem, midbrain, dorsal rostral pons (Kölliker-Fuse/parabrachial nuclei/locus ceruleus (KF/PB/LC)) and medulla (NTS or nucleus ambiguous). In addition, the imposition of a cognitive task (random number generation) produced an expected increased breathing rate (8% greater than baseline) accompanied by significant modulation of neuronal activity within the cortico-limbic-bulbar circuitry, localized to the inferior ventral pontine raphe, amygdala and anterior cingulate cortex. The reported findings are quite extraordinary; however, they should be considered balanced by certain caveats. Although several of the identified regions met stringent statistical criteria, some regions (e.g., brainstem loci) only met statistical significance after small volume correction. The event-related, breath-by-breath analytic method used strikes a fine balance between dissociating BOLD signal representing respiratory neural activity from BOLD signal related to artifacts induced by respiration itself (see above, Section 2C “Considerations for respiratory-related confounds”). Nevertheless, the reported findings garnered confirmatory support by two independent analyses; one that used a global null regressor and one that used a Pco2 null regressor. Interestingly, the same data set was subjected to additional analyses that demonstrated the global regressor method to be as effective as other methods (i.e. RVT described by Birn et al., (2006)) in removing respiratory related BOLD signal artifacts (Van Dijk et al., 2010). Further validity of the event-related, breath-by-breath analytic method has been conferred by comparable findings obtained when the method has been performed on other BOLD-fMRI data sets (e.g., Evans et al. (2009b)).

As noted earlier in Section 2B “Breathing task and stimuli,” and highlighted in the preceding example, certain respiratory stimuli may evoke sensory, motor, cognitive and also affective neural activity. Recent studies of cough, breath-hold and hypercapnia have been faced with the challenge of dissociating multi-modal neural activity. For example, Mazzone et al. (2007) administered aerosolized capcasin at doses below cough threshold, to induce an “urge to cough” during BOLD-fMRI (3 Tesla). Compared to placebo (saline) trials, capcasin trials were associated with robust bilateral activation of the insular cortex and widespread activation of sensorimotor regions within cortico-limbic-cerebellar circuitry. Interestingly, the motor cortex, SMA and ACC were the only regions where the magnitude of fMRI-BOLD signal was significantly correlated with subjective urge to cough ratings (based on BOLD-fMRI signal extracted from functionally defined ROIs for the contrast of capsaicin > saline). The authors acknowledge the inability to dissociate sensory, motor and cognitive processes and speculated the observed SMA and ACC activity reflected inhibition of the cough reflex.

McKay et al. (2008) used BOLD-fMRI at 2 Tesla to identify the neural circuits underlying respiratory motor inhibition during 15 second breath-holds at resting expiratory lung volume. In line with the findings of Mazzone et al. (2007), motor cortical, SMA and ACC activation was observed during the brief breath-holds. However, activation was also reported in other regions likely involved in motor inhibition: dorsal rostral pons (KF/PB/LC), inferior ventral pons, thalamus and basal ganglia. In addition, breath-hold related activation of the bilateral insular cortex, amygdala and prefrontal cortex was also noted and suggested to mediate the subjects’ sensory experience, as all subjects reported “a need for more air.” However, this implication is somewhat speculative since subjective reports were only acquired in debriefings after the scans (not simultaneously during the scans). The interpretation of this study is also limited by the use of small volume correction in the brainstem.

Pattinson et al. (2009b) adapted the protocol of McKay et al. (2008) to include breath-hold conditions with and without the opioid drug, remifentanil, in order to test the hypothesis that low-dose remifentanil (1.0 ng/ml infusion) would preferentially down-regulate sensory affective regions but have no significant effect on volitional motor regions. Both BOLD and ASL fMRI data were acquired at 3 Tesla. Voxel-wise SPM image analyses incorporated measures of CBF (via ASL data) as null regressors to account for potential vascular confounds associated with remifentanil infusion. The findings were consistent with the stated hypotheses, as remifentanil during breath-hold resulted in attenuated fMRI-BOLD signal within sensory/affective (inhibitory) circuitry (the insula, operculum, DLPFC, ACC, cerebellum). Low-dose remifentanil had no significant effect on breath-hold induced activation in motor circuitry (the motor cortex, basal ganglia).

Lastly, hypercapnia and hypoxia pose a special challenge to isolating efferent from afferent neural responses. For example, hypercapnia, as discussed earlier, can evoke sensations of dyspnea (urge to breathe, work and effort) and has been associated with widespread limbic/paralimbic and thalamic activity (in the absence of motor cortical activity) (Corfield et al., 1995; Brannan et al., 2001). McKay et al. (2010) recently employed a novel BOLD-fMRI (2 Tesla) paradigm consisting of brief (45 second) periods of isooxic-hypercapnia (mean Pco2 = 48mm Hg), hypoxic-isocapnia (mean Po2 = 54mm Hg) and hypoxic-hypercapnia (mean Po2 = 56mm Hg, mean Pco2 = 47mm Hg) in an attempt to isolate the neural circuits driving chemo-stimulated increases in ventilation without intentionally inducing dyspnea. Here the global regressor method was used to control for respiratory confounds related to task-related changes in arterial gases. Post-scan debriefing revealed that nearly all subjects (8 out of 9) experienced an increased awareness of their breathing but not strong dyspnea (contrast to the studies of (Banzett et al., 2000; Liotti et al., 2001; Evans et al., 2002)). Chemo-stimulated increases in ventilation were associated with bilateral BOLD signal increases within the medulla (NTS, or nucleus ambiguous), pons (KF/PB/LC) and midbrain (red nuclei) as well as the thalamus, basal ganglia, cerebellum, cingulate, frontal operculum and precuneus. The authors concluded that spontaneous chemo-stimulated increases in ventilation are mediated by a network of brainstem respiratory nuclei in coordination with subcortical and higher centers (specifically, the cingulate, operculum and precuneus). Interestingly, cortico-limbic regions typically active during dyspnea (e.g., DLPFC, anterior insula and amygdala) failed to reach significance in this study designed to produce chemo-stimulated increases in ventilation in the absence of significant dyspnea. It is important to note that the reported imaging findings are the result of a main effects of condition analysis (i.e., conjunction, see Section D “Neuroimaging Analyses”) and correspond to only those regions with common activity across the three different conditions (isooxic-hypercapnia, hypoxic-isocapnia, and hypoxic-hypercapnia). Since the study design and analyses did not permit independent or between-condition comparisons of the individual conditions of chemo-stimulated ventilatory drive, the interpretation of the findings is duly limited.

Pattinson et al. (2009a) examined the brainstem and thalamus during brief (11–120 second) periods of very mild hypercapnia (2–4 mmHg above each subject’s baseline) with an optimized BOLD-fMRI approach (i.e., 2.5mm thin coronal-oblique slices at 3 Tesla). A volume corrected, voxel-wise search restricted to brainstem and thalamus revealed hypercapnic induced ventilatory changes associated with regional foci of increased BOLD signal. Specifically, increased BOLD signal was localized within the anteroventral, ventral posterolateral and ventrolateral thalamic nuclei, as well as the dorsal medulla (NTS or nucleus ambiguous), inferior ventral pontine raphe, dorsal rostral pons (KF/PB/LC).

This overview of studies of combined sensorimotor networks highlights the difficulty in dissociating motor from sensory functionality. However, the consistent localization of NTS, KF/PB/LC, and ventral pontine raphe in recent fMRI studies of breathing (McKay et al., 2003; Macefield et al., 2006; McKay et al., 2008; Evans et al., 2009a; Pattinson et al., 2009a; Pattinson, 2009b) is intriguing and encourages future studies to further elucidate the sensorimotor functions of the human brainstem.

B. Functional studies in patient populations

Functional abnormities within essential elements of cortico-limbic and bulbar-cerebellar circuitry have been reported in patients with congenital central hypoventilation syndrome (CCHS), obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) and asthma (Macey et al., 2004; Harper et al., 2005; Macey et al., 2005; von Leupoldt et al., 2009b). Structural abnormalities have also been reported in CCHS and OSA (Kumar et al., 2008; Macey et al., 2008). The neuroimaging studies of CCHS patients are of particular interest as these individuals have diminished perception and ventilatory response to hypercapnia and hypoxia, yet preserved volitional respiratory motor output (Shea et al., 1993; American, 1999). Harper et al. (2005) and Macey et al. (2004b, 2005) used hypercapnic (5% inspired CO2), hypoxic (15% inspired O2) and volitional forced expiratory challenges to differentiate neural responses between CCHS and healthy control (HC) cohorts in a series of BOLD-fMRI studies. These studies employed voxel-wise SPM and ROI time-course analyses. A global regressor method (Macey et al., 2004a) was used to account for task-related changes in arterial gases. Across the studies, between-group responses were quite varied with respect to brain region, laterality and relative BOLD signal magnitude and time-course. However, differential BOLD signal responses were localized to regions previously implicated in respiratory motor control (e.g., midbrain, pons, medulla, thalamus, basal ganglia and cerebellum) as well as limbic regions (e.g., amygdala, hippocampus). Notably, the BOLD-fMRI signal in the insular cortex of the CCHS patients was significantly less during respiratory challenges compared to the control subjects.

Patients with panic disorder (PD) are another interesting population to consider in regard to sensorimotor and affective processing of respiratory stimuli. Despite findings of normal ventilatory response to hypercapnia and hypoxia (Papp et al., 1997; Katzman et al., 2002), PD patients have been shown to consistently demonstrate exaggerated anxiety responses to provocative respiratory stimuli (Abelson et al., 1996; Rassovsky and Kushner, 2003). Garakani et al. (2007) measured regional glucose uptake via FDG-PET in PD patients and healthy control subjects during challenge with doxapram, a potent respiratory stimulant with demonstrated efficacy to provoke panic attacks in PD patients. Compared to healthy controls, PD patients demonstrated exaggerated regional glucose uptake in the amygdala, insula, ACC and basal ganglia, but decreased glucose uptake in the DLPFC during doxapram challenge.

Taken together, the neuroimaging findings in patients with impaired chemosensitivity (CCHS) and exaggerated emotional responses to respiratory stimuli (PD) inform the model of distributed cortico-limbic and bulbar-cerebellar circuitry presented below (Figure 1).

6. Working model for respiratory sensorimotor neural circuitry

Even though the field of respiratory neuroimaging may be viewed as in its early stages, the convergence of findings to date prompts the consideration of a working cortico-limbic model for respiratory sensorimotor circuitry. The model as depicted in Figure 1 should be taken as a preliminary gross graphical summary of human respiratory neural circuitry, based exclusively on consistent cortico-limbic findings across functional neuroimaging studies of respiration. The model considers a Motor Division that carries respiratory efferents responsible for volitional motor control, a Sensory Division that carries respiratory afferents responsible for sensory perception, and regions that share overlap between the two distinct divisions.

A. Motor Division

The motor division of the model proposed here is simplified, focusing on (a) the cortico-spinal circuitry identified by neuroimaging studies of volitional control of breathing, and (b) the bulbo-spinal circuitry identified by neuroimaging studies of chemo-reflex control of breathing. The cortico-limbic circuitry thought to mediate emotional and behavioral modulation of breathing (Garakani et al., 2007; Evans et al., 2009a), requires replication and further evidence prior to its formal integration into the model.

Converging lines of evidence strongly suggest the orchestration of voluntary breathing to occur within the cortico-spinal circuitry. Motor cortical and SMA findings have been common to all neuroimaging studies with hypotheses related to the volitional control of breathing (Ramsay et al., 1993; Evans et al., 1999; McKay et al., 2003) (color coded in blue and purple; Figure 1, Table 1), and these findings are in line with an established motor control literature (Lemon, 1993; Passingham, 1993). Notably, the motor cortical neuroimaging findings are consistent with locations previously associated with diaphragmatic contraction during electrical stimulation (Foerster, 1936) and transcranial magnetic stimulation (Maskill et al., 1991). The SMA is well situated to mediate its established role in coordinating self initiated movement as it lies immediately anterior to the motor cortex, with strong motor cortical, pre-motor, basal ganglia and brainstem connections (Frackowiak et al., 1997; Schmahmann et al., 2004; Nachev et al., 2008; Passingham, 1993; Hanakawa et al., 2008; Nachev et al., 2008).

The basal ganglia, thalamus and cerebellum have also been common findings in neuroimaging studies of volitional breathing (Ramsay et al., 1993; Evans et al., 1999; McKay et al., 2003; Macefield et al., 2006; Evans et al., 2009a; Pattinson et al., 2009a; Pattinson, 2009b) (color coded in purple; Figure 1, Table 1), and are well known to be strongly interconnected motor structures (Houk and Wise, 1995; Ramnani, 2006). Each of these regions is known to have direct projections to the brainstem and each has been implicated in respiratory control (Radna and MacLean, 1981; Hugelin, 1986; Cechetto, 1987; Chen et al., 1992; Harper, 2002; Xu and Frazier, 2002; Ito and Craig, 2008; Lois et al., 2009). It is important to also consider that the basal ganglia, thalamus and cerebellum have known sensory function and have been implicated in studies of respiratory sensation (reviewed above and discussed in the next section).

The bulbo-spinal circuitry has been established as essential for the homeostatic, chemo-reflex control of breathing in the service of maintaining arterial gases to meet metabolic requirements (Feldman and Del Negro, 2006; Nattie and Li, 2009). The findings of Pattinson et al. (2009a) and McKay et al. (2010) point to coordinated ponto-medullary reflex responses to hypercapnia and hypoxia. The bulbo-spinal circuitry likely also facilitates volitional control and emotional modulation of breathing via cortico-spinal and cortico-limbic interactions (McKay et al., 2003; Evans et al., 2009a). Bulbo-spinal lesions that spare cortico-spinal circuitry impair chemo-reflex breathing yet leave the voluntary control of breathing intact, suggesting these circuits to be functionally distinct (Severinghaus and Mitchell, 1962; Nathan, 1963). And aberrant bulbar activity in CCHS patients during hypercapnic and hypoxic challenges (Harper et al., 2005; Macey et al., 2005) provide further in vivo support for the importance of bulbo-spinal circuitry in the chemo-reflex control of breathing.

Respiratory neuroimaging findings within the brainstem have been limited by the challenges surroundings spatial resolution and field of view. Common respiratory-motor brainstem findings have included: the dorsal medulla likely representing the NTS, the ventral pons in proximity of the reticular and raphe nuclei, the dorsolateral pons in proximity to KF/PB nuclei and LC and the midbrain (McKay et al., 2003; Macefield et al., 2006; Evans et al., 2009a; Pattinson et al., 2009a; Pattinson, 2009b). These regions share anatomical and functional connectivity with each other as well as other important brainstem respiratory centers that have yet to be consistently identified by functional imaging (e.g. ventral respiratory group, pre-Bötzinger complex) (Rybak et al., 2004; Song and Poon, 2004; Kubin et al., 2006).

In summary, the motor division of the proposed model for respiratory sensorimotor neural circuitry comprises the motor cortex, SMA, basal ganglia, thalamus, cerebellum and brainstem (Table 1, Figure 1). Given the established motor functionality of these structures it follows that the overt hyperpneic tasks employed by the reviewed neuroimaging studies of volitional breathing would necessarily recruit distributed activity across the cortico-spinal circuitry. As only limited neuroimaging findings of respiratory motor processes related to bulbo-spinal and cortico-limbic circuitry are available, the full characterization of these circuits await future studies.

B. Sensory Division

Given the scope of this paper, the sensory division of the model will be addressed in the context of dyspnea, appreciating the fact that there are numerous respiratory sensations that may not evoke dyspnea (Table 1, Figure 1). The model conceptualizes the perception of dyspnea sensation to arise from the integration of a complex array of neural activity across respiratory sensorimotor networks (e.g., shared elements of the motor division, brainstem chemoreceptors, peripheral chemoreceptors, pulmonary stretch receptors, chest wall mechanoreceptors, etc).

The primary elements of the model’s sensory division include the primary and secondary sensory cortices, insular cortex and associated operculum, ACC, amygdala, DLPFC, and cerebellar vermis. These regions have been consistently implicated in neuroimaging studies of respiratory sensation (color coded in red; Figure 1, Table 1) and found noncontributory to studies of volitional motor control. The insula and associated operculum are posited as the central elements in the sensory division responsible for respiratory interoception given the insula’s known role in sensing of internal state/physiologic homeostasis (Craig, 2002; Denton, 2005). There are heavy reciprocal connections between the insula, amygdala, ACC and brainstem respiratory centers (Mesulam and Mufson, 1982; Mufson and Mesulam, 1982; Augustine, 1996; Gaytan and Pasaro, 1998; Hanamori et al., 1998; Shi and Cassell, 1998; Vertes, 2004; Tsumori et al., 2006). Noted as important to breath awareness in meditative practices (Lazar et al., 2005; Evans et al., 2009b) the insula has also been the most consistently implicated structure in patient populations known to have diminished respiratory sensations (e.g., CCHS, OSA, asthmatics) (Harper et al., 2005; Macey et al., 2005; Macey et al., 2006; von Leupoldt et al., 2009b). The other primary structures in the sensory division (i.e., amygdala, ACC) are suggested to coordinate with the insula in the modulation of attentional and affective experience of respiratory stimuli based on their arousal and valence characteristics. This proposal is supported by the repeated identification of the amygdala and ACC in studies of fear, anxiety, emotional reactivity and general arousal (LeDoux, 2000; Critchley et al., 2002; Mayberg, 2003; Phan et al., 2004; Lang and Davis, 2006; Paulus and Stein, 2006; Etkin and Wager, 2007; Milad, 2007).

Several secondary elements of the model’s sensory division share significant overlap with elements of the motor division, namely the SMA, basal ganglia, thalamus and cerebellar hemispheres (color coded in purple; Figure 1, Table 1). The coordinated recruitment of these elements together with the DLFPC is suggested to mediate inhibition or suppression of desired behaviors (e.g., taking a breath to end a breath-hold) during respiratory challenges that require subjects’ task compliance (McKay et al., (2008); Pattinson et al., (2009b).

To sum up, the sensory division of the proposed model for respiratory sensorimotor neural circuitry is comprised of primary and secondary elements. The insula and supporting primary structures (i.e., ACC, amygdala) mediate the integration of interoceptive, valence, and arousal processing to dyspnea stimuli. Interaction between the DLPFC and secondary sensory structures, namely the SMA, cerebellum, facilitate inhibitory processing to aversive respiratory stimuli/tasks.

7. Conclusion

Prior to neuroimaging, the knowledge regarding respiratory control and respiratory sensation within cortico-limbic circuitry was poorly understood. Now the convergence of PET and fMRI data strongly suggests the volitional control of breathing to be mediated by coordinated network activity within motor cortical, SMA, cerebellar and subcortical regions. A similar convergence of data provides strong support for the sensation of dyspnea to be mediated by network activity within cortico-limbic structures, specifically the anterior insula, ACC, and amygdala. The replication of findings across imaging modalities and experimental paradigms is rather impressive and calls for consideration of the integrated sensorimotor neural circuit model proposed here. As we approach the twenty year anniversary of the first neuroimaging study of respiratory neural circuitry (Colebatch et al., 1991), much has been learned. Indeed, our understanding of respiratory control and sensory perception within cortico-limbic circuitry has been enhanced by the recent evidence for distributed brainstem and cortico-limbic responses to modest changes in arterial gases (Pattinson et al., 2009a; McKay et al., 2010) and short breath-holds (McKay et al., 2008; Pattinson, 2009b), together with preliminary evidence for respiratory-related cortico-limbic modulation during cognitive/emotional tasks (von Leupoldt et al., 2008; Evans et al., 2009a) and during opioid drug administration (Pattinson, 2009b). Nevertheless, many questions remain to be addressed. Emotional, cognitive and pharmacologic influences over respiratory motor output and respiratory sensation remain quite vague despite the level of sophistication employed in the recent respiratory neuroimaging studies. If the field of respiratory neuroimaging continues along its current trajectory it is quite possible that many remaining questions surrounding emotional and cognitive influences over respiratory control and sensation may be addressed in the next twenty years of neuroimaging research! For now, it is hoped that the highlighted empirical findings and presentation of the sensorimotor model will provide an essential framework for basic understanding of the afferent and efferent neural elements within cortico-limbic circuitry.

Acknowledgments

This work was primarily supported by K23 MH086619 (NIMH). Within the last year Dr. Evans has received support from R21 AT003425-01A2S1 (NCCAM), an Investigator Initiated Research Agreement from Pfizer Ltd, and a Faculty Development Award from the Massachusetts General Hospital Executive Committee on Research. Tina Chou and Annette M. Schmid are acknowledged for their editorial assistance. Special thanks are expressed to Kenneth Townsend and Randy Edgington for their artistic contribution to Figure 1.

Footnotes

In press: Biological Psychology as an invited review for a special issue entitled “Psychobiology of Respiration and the Airways”

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Abelson JL, Nesse RM, Weg JG, Curtis GC. Respiratory psychophysiology and anxiety: cognitive intervention in the doxapram model of panic. Psychosom Med. 1996;58(4):302–13. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199607000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. American ST. Idiopathic congenital central hypoventilation syndrome: diagnosis and management. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 1999;160:368–373. doi: 10.1164/ajrccm.160.1.16010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andersson JLR, Hutton C, Ashburner J, Turner R, Friston KJ. Modeling Geometric Deformations in EPI Time Series. Neuroimage. 2001;13:903–919. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Augustine JR. Circuitry and functional aspects of the insular lobe in primates including humans. Brain Research Reviews. 1996;22:229–244. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(96)00011-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Banzett RB, Garcia RT, Moosavi SH. Simple contrivance “clamps” end-tidal PCO(2) and PO(2) despite rapid changes in ventilation. J Appl Physiol. 2000;88(5):1597–600. doi: 10.1152/jappl.2000.88.5.1597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Banzett RB, Lansing RW, Brown R, Topulos GP, Yager SM, Steele B, Londono B, et al. ‘Air hunger’ from increased PCO2 persists after complete neuromuscular block in humans. Respiration Physiology. 1990;81:1–17. doi: 10.1016/0034-5687(90)90065-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Banzett RB, Lansing RW, Evans KC, Shea SA. Stimulus-response characteristics of CO2-induced air hunger in normal subjects. Respir Physiol. 1996;103(1):19–31. doi: 10.1016/0034-5687(95)00050-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Banzett RB, Lansing RW, Reid MB, Adams L, Brown R. ‘Air hunger’ arising from increased PCO2 in mechanically ventilated quadriplegics. Respiration Physiology. 1989;76:53–67. doi: 10.1016/0034-5687(89)90017-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Banzett RB, Mulnier HE, Murphy K, Rosen SD, Wise RJ, Adams L. Breathlessness in humans activates insular cortex. Neuroreport. 2000;11(10):2117–20. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200007140-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Birn RM, Bandettini PA, Cox RW, Jesmanowicz A, Shaker R. Magnetic field changes in the human brain due to swallowing or speaking. Magn Reson Med. 1998;40(1):55–60. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910400108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Birn RM, Diamond JB, Smith MA, Bandettini PA. Separating respiratory-variation-related fluctuations from neuronal-activity-related fluctuations in fMRI. Neuroimage. 2006;31(4):1536–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Birn RM, Murphy K, Handwerker DA, Bandettini PA. fMRI in the presence of task-correlated breathing variations. Neuroimage. 2009;47(3):1092–104. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Boiten FA. The effects of emotional behaviour on components of the respiratory cycle. Biol Psychol. 1998;49(1–2):29–51. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(98)00025-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brannan S, Liotti M, Egan G, Shade R, Madden L, Robillard R, Abplanalp B, et al. Neuroimaging of cerebral activations and deactivations associated with hypercapnia and hunger for air. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98(4):2029–34. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.4.2029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Buckner RL. Event-related fMRI and the hemodynamic response. Hum Brain Mapp. 1998;6:373–7. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1998)6:5/6&#x0003c;373::AID-HBM8&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-P. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cabeza R, Kingstone A. Handbook of functional neuroimaging of cognition. MIT Press; Cambridge, Massachusetts: 2001. [Google Scholar]
  17. Cechetto DF. Central representation of visceral function. Fed Proc. 1987;46(1):17–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Chang C, Glover GH. Relationship between respiration, end-tidal CO2, and BOLD signals in resting-state fMRI. Neuroimage. 2009;47(4):1381–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Check E. Ethicists urge caution over emotive power of brain scans. Nature. 2005;435(7040):254–5. doi: 10.1038/435254a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Chen Z, Eldridge FL, Wagner PG. Respiratory-associated thalamic activity is related to level of respiratory drive. Respiration Physiology. 1992;90(1):99–113. doi: 10.1016/0034-5687(92)90137-l. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Colebatch JG, Adams L, Murphy K, Martin AJ, Lammertsma AA, Tochon-Danguy HJ, Clark JC, et al. Regional cerebral blood flow during volitional breathing in man. J Physiol. 1991;443:91–103. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1991.sp018824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Corfield DR, Fink GR, Ramsay SC, Murphy K, Harty HR, Watson JDG, Adams L, et al. Evidence for limbic system activation during CO2-stimulated breathing in man. Journal of Physiology. 1995;488(1):77–84. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1995.sp020947. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Corfield DR, Murphy K, Josephs O, Adams L, Turner R. Does hypercapnia-induced cerebral vasodilation modulate the hemodynamic response to neural activation? Neuroimage. 2001;13(6 Pt 1):1207–11. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Craig AD. How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nature Neurosci. 2002;3:655–66. doi: 10.1038/nrn894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Critchley HD, Mathias CJ, Dolan RJ. Fear conditioning in humans: the influence of awareness and autonomic arousal on functional neuroanatomy. Neuron. 2002;33(4):653–63. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00588-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Davenport PW, Vovk A. Cortical, Subcortical Central Neural Pathways in Respiratory Sensations. Resp Physiol & Neurobiol. 2009;167(1):72–86. doi: 10.1016/j.resp.2008.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Denton D. The Primordial Emotions: The dawning of consciousness. Oxford University Press; New York: 2005. [Google Scholar]
  28. Dolan RJ. Emotional processing in the human brain revealed through functional neuroimaging. In: Gazzaniga MS, editor. The new cognitive neurosciences. MIT Press; Cambridge, Massachusetts: 2000. pp. 1115–1129. [Google Scholar]
  29. Dolan RJ. Neuroimaging of cognition: past, present, and future. Neuron. 2008;60(3):496–502. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Eickhoff SB, Heim S, Zilles K, Amunts K. Testing anatomically specified hypotheses in functional imaging using cytoarchitectonic maps. NeuroImage. 2006;32(2):570–82. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.04.204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Etkin A, Wager TD. Functional Neuroimaging of Anxiety: A Meta-Analysis of Emotional Processing in PTSD, Social Anxiety Disorder, and Specific Phobia. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(10):1476–88. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07030504. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Evans KC. The cortico-limbic neural response to air hunger shares temporal dynamics with subjective interoception: A proof of concept study for panic disorder. Biol Psych. 2009c;65 (8S):31S. [Google Scholar]
  33. Evans KC, Banzett RB, Adams L, McKay L, Frackowiak RS, Corfield DR. BOLD fMRI identifies limbic, paralimbic, and cerebellar activation during air hunger. J Neurophysiol. 2002;88(3):1500–11. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.3.1500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Evans KC, Dougherty DD, Schmid AM, Scannell E, McCallister A, Benson H, Dusek JA, et al. Modulation of spontaneous breathing via limbic/paralimbic-bulbar circuitry: An event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2009a;47(3):961–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Evans KC, McCallister A, Schmid AM, Scannell E, Dougherty DD, Benson H, Dusek JA, et al. Event-related fMRI identifies respiratory related cortico-limbic and brainstem modulation during meditation. North American Reseach Conference on Complimentary and Integrative Medicine; Minneapolis. 2009b. [Google Scholar]
  36. Evans KC, Shea SA, Saykin AJ. Functional MRI localisation of central nervous system regions associated with volitional inspiration in humans. J Physiol. 1999;520(Pt 2):383–92. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1999.00383.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Feldman JL, Del Negro CA. Looking for inspiration: new perspectives on respiratory rhythm. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7(3):232–42. doi: 10.1038/nrn1871. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Fink GR, Corfield DR, Murphy K, Kobayashi I, Dettmers C, Adams L, Frackowiak RS, et al. Human cerebral activity with increasing inspiratory force: a study using positron emission tomography. J Appl Physiol. 1996;81(3):1295–305. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1996.81.3.1295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Foerster O. Handbuch der Neurologie. Springer; Berlin, Germany: 1936. [Google Scholar]
  40. Frackowiak RSJ, Friston KJ, Frith CD, Dolan RJ, Mazziotta JC. Functional organisation of the motor system. In: Frackowiak RSJ, editor. Human Brain Function. Academic Press; London: 1997. pp. 243–274. [Google Scholar]
  41. Friston KJ, Ashburner J, Frith CD, Pauline JB, Heather JD, Frackowiak RSJ. Spatial registration and normalisation of images. Human Brain Mapping. 1995;2:165–189. [Google Scholar]
  42. Friston KJ, Ashburner J, Kiebel SJ, Nichols TE, Penny WD. Statistical parametric mapping the analysis of functional brain images. Academic Press; London: 2007. [Google Scholar]
  43. Friston KJ, Williams S, Howard R, Frackowiak RS, Turner R. Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series. Magn Reson Med. 1996;35(3):346–55. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910350312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Frysinger RC, Harper RM. Cardiac and respiratory correlations with unit discharge in epileptic human temporal lobe. Epilepsia. 1990;31(2):162–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.1990.tb06301.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Garakani A, Buchsbaum MS, Newmark RE, Goodman C, Aaronson CJ, Martinez JM, Torosjan Y, et al. The effect of doxapram on brain imaging in patients with panic disorder. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2007;17(10):672–86. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2007.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Gaytan SP, Pasaro R. Connections of the rostral ventral respiratory neuronal cell group: an anterograde and retrograde tracing study in the rat. Brain Res Bull. 1998;47(6):625–42. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(98)00125-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Glover GH, Li TQ, Ress D. Image-based method for retrospective correction of physiological motion effects in fMRI: RETROICOR. Magn Reson Med. 2000;44(1):162–7. doi: 10.1002/1522-2594(200007)44:1<162::aid-mrm23>3.0.co;2-e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hamilton J. False signals cause misleading brain scans. National Public Radio All Things Considered. 2009 http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=106235924.
  49. Hanakawa T, Dimyan MA, Hallett M. Motor Planning, Imagery, and Execution in the Distributed Motor Network: A Time-Course Study with Functional MRI. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18(12):2775–88. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hanamori T, Kunitake T, Kato K, Kannan H. Neurons in the posterior insular cortex are responsive to gustatory stimulation of the pharyngolarynx, baroreceptor and chemoreceptor stimulation, and tail pinch in rats. Brain Res. 1998;785(1):97–106. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(97)01392-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Harper RM. Editorial: A Journal for the Neuroscience of the Cerebellum. The Cerebellum. 2002;1:1–2. [Google Scholar]
  52. Harper RM, Macey PM, Woo MA, Macey KE, Keens TG, Gozal D, Alger JR. Hypercapnic exposure in congenital central hypoventilation syndrome reveals CNS respiratory control mechanisms. J Neurophysiol. 2005;93(3):1647–58. doi: 10.1152/jn.00863.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Harty HR, Mummery CJ, Adams L, Banzett RB, Wright IG, Banner NR, Yacoub MH, et al. Ventilatory relief of the sensation of the urge to breathe in humans: are pulmonary receptors important? J Physiol. 1996;490(Pt 3):805–815. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1996.sp021188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Harvey AK, Pattinson KTS, Brooks JCW, Mayhew SD, Jenkinson M, Wise RG. Brainstem Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Disentangling Signal From Physiological Noise. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2008:28. doi: 10.1002/jmri.21623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Houk JC, Wise SP. Distributed modular architectures linking basal ganglia, cerebellum and cerebral cortex: Their role in planning and controlling action. Cerebral Cortex. 1995;2:95–110. doi: 10.1093/cercor/5.2.95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Hugelin A. Forebrain and midbrain influence on respiration. In: Cherniack NS, Widdicombe JG, editors. Handbook of Physiology, Sect. 3: The respiratory System. American Physiological Society; Bethesda: 1986. [Google Scholar]
  57. Ito S, Craig AD. Striatal projections of the vagal-responsive region of the thalamic parafascicular nucleus in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol. 2008;506(2):301–27. doi: 10.1002/cne.21513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Jezzard P, Clare S. Sources of distortion in functional MRI data. Hum Brain Mapp. 1999;8(2–3):80–5. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:2/3&#x0003c;80::AID-HBM2&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-C. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Kaada BR, Jasper H. Respiratory responses to stimulation of temporal pole, insula, and hippocampal and limbic gyri in man. AMA Arch Neurol Psychiatry. 1952;68(5):609–19. doi: 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1952.02320230035004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Katzman MA, Struzik L, Vijay N, Coonerty-Femiano A, Mahamed S, Duffin J. Central and peripheral chemoreflexes in panic disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2002;113(1–2):181–92. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(02)00238-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Kriegeskorte N, Simmons WK, Bellgowan PS, Baker CI. Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12(5):535–40. doi: 10.1038/nn.2303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Kubin L, Alheid GF, Zuperku EJ, McCrimmon DR. Central pathways of pulmonary and lower airway vagal afferents. J Appl Physiol. 2006;101(2):618–27. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00252.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Kumar R, Macey PM, Woo MA, Alger JR, Harper RM. Diffusion tensor imaging demonstrates brainstem and cerebellar abnormalities in congenital central hypoventilation syndrome. Pediatr Res. 2008;64(3):275–80. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e31817da10a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Lang PJ, Davis M. Emotion, motivation, and the brain: reflex foundations in animal and human research. Prog Brain Res. 2006;156:3–29. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)56001-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Lansing RW, Gracely RH, Banzett RB. The multiple dimensions of dyspnea: Review and hypotheses. Respir Physiol Neurobiol. 2009;167(1):53–60. doi: 10.1016/j.resp.2008.07.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Lazar NA, Luna B, Sweeney JA, Eddy WF. Combining brains: a survey of methods for statistical pooling of information. NeuroImage. 2002;16(2):538–50. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Lazar SW, Kerr CE, Wasserman RH, Gray JR, Greve DN, Treadway MT, McGarvey M, et al. Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness. Neuroreport. 2005;16(17):1893–7. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000186598.66243.19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. LeDoux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:155–84. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Lemon RN. Cortical control of the primate hand. Exp Physiol. 1993;78:263–301. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.1993.sp003686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Liotti M, Brannan S, Egan G, Shade R, Madden L, Abplanalp B, Robillard R, et al. Brain responses associated with consciousness of breathlessness (air hunger) Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98(4):2035–40. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.4.2035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Lois JH, Rice CD, Yates BJ. Neural circuits controlling diaphragm function in the cat revealed by transneuronal tracing. J Appl Physiol. 2009;106(1):138–52. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.91125.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Lund TE, Norgaard MD, Rostrup E, Rowe JB, Paulson OB. Motion or activity: their role in intra- and inter-subject variation in fMRI. Neuroimage. 2005;26(3):960–4. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Macefield VG, Gandevia SC, Henderson LA. Neural sites involved in the sustained increase in muscle sympathetic nerve activity induced by inspiratory capacity apnea: a fMRI study. J Appl Physiol. 2006;100(1):266–73. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00588.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Macey KE, Macey PM, Woo MA, Harper RK, Alger JR, Keens TG, Harper RM. fMRI signal changes in response to forced expiratory loading in congenital central hypoventilation syndrome. J Appl Physiol. 2004;97(5):1897–907. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00359.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Macey KE, Macey PM, Woo MA, Henderson LA, Frysinger RC, Harper RK, Alger JR, et al. Inspiratory loading elicits aberrant fMRI signal changes in obstructive sleep apnea. Respir Physiol Neurobiol. 2006;151(1):44–60. doi: 10.1016/j.resp.2005.05.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Macey PM, Kumar R, Woo MA, Valladares EM, Yan-Go FL, Harper RM. Brain structural changes in obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep. 2008;31(7):967–77. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Macey PM, Macey KE, Kumar R, Harper RM. A method for removal of global effects from fMRI time series. Neuroimage. 2004;22(1):360–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Macey PM, Woo MA, Macey KE, Keens TG, Saeed MM, Alger JR, Harper RM. Hypoxia reveals posterior thalamic, cerebellar, midbrain, and limbic deficits in congenital central hypoventilation syndrome. J Appl Physiol. 2005;98(3):958–69. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00969.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. MacIntosh BJ, Pattinson KT, Gallichan D, Ahmad I, Miller KL, Feinberg DA, Wise RG, et al. Measuring the effects of remifentanil on cerebral blood flow and arterial arrival time using 3D GRASE MRI with pulsed arterial spin labelling. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2008;28(8):1514–22. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2008.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Maldjian JA, Laurienti PJ, Kraft RA, Burdette JH. An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage. 2003;19(3):1233–9. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00169-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Manning HL, Schwartstein RM. Pathophysiology of Dyspnea. New England Journal of Medicine. 1995;333(23):1547–53. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199512073332307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Manning HL, Shea SA, Schwartzstein RM, Lansing RW, Brown R, Banzett RB. Reduced tidal volume increases ‘air hunger’ at fixed PCO2 in ventilated quadriplegics. Respiration Physiology. 1992;90(1):19–30. doi: 10.1016/0034-5687(92)90131-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Marchini J, Presanis A. Comparing methods of analyzing fMRI statistical parametric maps. NeuroImage. 2004;22(3):1203–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Maskill D, Murphy K, Mier A, Owen M, Guz A. Motor cortical representation of the diaphragm in man. J Physiol. 1991;443:105–21. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1991.sp018825. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Mayberg HS. Modulating dysfunctional limbic-cortical circuits in depression: towards development of brain-based algorithms for diagnosis and optimised treatment. Br Med Bull. 2003;65:193–207. doi: 10.1093/bmb/65.1.193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Mazzone SB, McLennan L, McGovern AE, Egan GF, Farrell MJ. Representation of capsaicin-evoked urge-to-cough in the human brain using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2007;176(4):327–32. doi: 10.1164/rccm.200612-1856OC. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. McCabe DP, Castel AD. Seeing is believing: the effect of brain images on judgments of scientific reasoning. Cognition. 2008;107:343–352. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. McKay LC, Adams L, Frackowiak RS, Corfield DR. A bilateral cortico-bulbar network associated with breath holding in humans, determined by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage. 2008;40(4):1824–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. McKay LC, Critchley HD, Murphy K, Frackowiak RSJ, Corfield DR. Subcortical and brainstem sites associated with chemo-stimulated increases in ventilation in humans. Neuroimage. 2010;49(3):2526–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. McKay LC, Evans KC, Frackowiak RS, Corfield DR. Neural correlates of voluntary breathing in humans. J Appl Physiol. 2003;95(3):1170–8. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00641.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Merboldt KD, Fransson P, Bruhn H, Frahm J. Functional MRI of the human amygdala? Neuroimage. 2001;14(2):253–7. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Mesulam MM, Mufson EJ. Insula of the old world monkey. III: Efferent cortical output and comments on function. J Comp Neurol. 1982;212(1):38–52. doi: 10.1002/cne.902120104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Milad MR, Quirk GJ, Pitman RK, Orr SP, Fischl B, Rauch SL. A role for the human dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in fear expression. Biol Psychiatry. 2007 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Mitsis GD, Iannetti GD, Smart TS, Tracey I, Wise RG. Regions of interest analysis in pharmacological fMRI: how do the definition criteria influence the inferred result? NeuroImage. 2008;40(1):121–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Morris JS, Ohman A, Dolan RJ. Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala. Nature. 1998;393(6684):467–470. doi: 10.1038/30976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Mufson EJ, Mesulam MM. Insula of the old world monkey. II: Afferent cortical input and comments on the claustrum. J Comp Neurol. 1982;212(1):23–37. doi: 10.1002/cne.902120103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Murphy K, Birn RM, Handwerker DA, Jones TB, Bandettini PA. The impact of global signal regression on resting state correlations: Are anti-correlated networks introduced? NeuroImage. 2009;44:893–905. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Nachev P, Kennard C, Husain M. Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(11):856–69. doi: 10.1038/nrn2478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Nathan PW. The descending respiratory pathway in man. J Neurol Neurosurg Psych. 1963;26:487–99. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.26.6.487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Nattie E, Li A. Central chemoreception is a complex system function that involves multiple brain stem sites. J Appl Physiol. 2009;106(4):1464–6. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00112.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Nichols T, Brett M, Andersson J, Wager T, Poline JB. Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic. Neuroimage. 2005;25(3):653–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Nieto-Castanon A, Ghosh SST, JA, Guenther FH. Region of interest based analysis of functional imaging data. Neuroimage. 2003;19:1303–16. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00188-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Papp LA, Martinez JM, Klein DF, Coplan JD, Norman RG, Cole R, de Jesus MJ, et al. Respiratory psychophysiology of panic disorder: three respiratory challenges in 98 subjects. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154(11):1557–65. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.11.1557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Parsons LM, Egan G, Liotti M, Brannan S, Denton D, Shade R, Robillard R, et al. Neuroimaging evidence implicating cerebellum in the experience of hypercapnia and hunger for air. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98(4):2041–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.4.2041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Passingham RE. The frontal lobes and voluntary action. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 1993. [Google Scholar]
  106. Pattinson KT, Governo RJ, Russell EC, MacIntosh BJ, Russell EC, Corfield DR, Tracey I, Wise RG. Opioids depress cortical centers responsible for the volitional control of respiration. J Neurosci. 2009b;29(25):8177–86. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1375-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Pattinson KT, Mitsis GD, Harvey AK, Jbabdi S, Dirckx S, Mayhew SD, Rogers R, et al. Determination of the human brainstem respiratory control network and its cortical connections in vivo using functional and structural imaging. Neuroimage. 2009a;44(2):295–305. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Paulus MP, Stein MB. An insular view of anxiety. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(4):383–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Peiffer C, Costes N, Herve P, Garcia-Larrea L. Relief of dyspnea involves a characteristic brain activation and a specific quality of sensation. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2008;177:440–449. doi: 10.1164/rccm.200612-1774OC. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Peiffer C, Poline JB, Thivard L, Aubier M, Samson Y. Neural substrates for the perception of acutely induced dyspnea. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2001;163(4):951–7. doi: 10.1164/ajrccm.163.4.2005057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Penfield W, Faulk ME., Jr The insula; further observations on its function. Brain. 1955;78(4):445–70. doi: 10.1093/brain/78.4.445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Phan KL, Wager TD, Taylor SF, Liberzon I. Functional neuroimaging studies of human emotions. CNS Spectr. 2004;9(4):258–66. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900009196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Poldrack RA. Tools of the trade: Region of interest analysis for fMRI. SCAN. 2007;2:67–70. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Poldrack RA. The role of fMRI in cognitive neuroscience: where do we stand? curr opin neurobiol. 2008;18(2):223–7. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Posse S, Kemna LJ, Elghahwagi B, Wiese S, Kiselev VG. Effect of graded hypo- and hypercapnia on fMRI contrast in visual cortex: quantification of T(*)(2) changes by multiecho EPI. Magn Reson Med. 2001;46(2):264–71. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Prisman E, Slessarev M, Han J, Poublanc J, Mardimae A, Crawley A, Fisher J, et al. Comparison of the effects of independently-controlled end-tidal PCO(2) and PO(2) on blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) MRI. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2008;27(1):185–91. doi: 10.1002/jmri.21102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Radna RJ, MacLean PD. Vagal elicitation of respiratory-type and other unit responses in striopallidum of squirrel monkeys. Brain Res. 1981;213(1):29–44. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(81)91246-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Ramnani N. The primate cortico-cerebellar system: anatomy and function. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7(7):511–22. doi: 10.1038/nrn1953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Ramsay SC, Adams L, Murphy K, Corfield DR, Grootoonk S, Bailey DL, Frackowiak RS, et al. Regional cerebral blood flow during volitional expiration in man: a comparison with volitional inspiration. J Physiol. 1993;461:85–101. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1993.sp019503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Ramsay SC, Murphy K, Shea SA, Friston KJ, Lammertsma AA, Clark JC, Adams L, et al. Changes in global cerebral blood flow in humans: effect on regional cerebral blood flow during a neural activation task. J Physiol. 1993;471:521–34. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1993.sp019913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Rassovsky Y, Kushner MG. Carbon dioxide in the study of panic disorder: issues of definition, methodology, and outcome. J Anxiety Disord. 2003;17(1):1–32. doi: 10.1016/s0887-6185(02)00181-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Rybak IA, Shevtsova NA, Paton JF, Dick TE, St-John WM, Morschel M, Dutschmann M. Modeling the ponto-medullary respiratory network. Respir Physiol Neurobiol. 2004;143(2–3):307–19. doi: 10.1016/j.resp.2004.03.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Schmahmann JD, Rosene DL, Pandya DN. Motor projections to the basis pontis in rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol. 2004;478(3):248–68. doi: 10.1002/cne.20286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Severinghaus JW, Mitchell RA. Ondine’s curse: failure of respiratory automaticity whilst asleep. Clin Res. 1962;10:122. [Google Scholar]
  125. Shea SA. Behavioural and arousal-related influences on breathing in humans. Exp Physiol. 1996;81(1):1–26. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.1996.sp003911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Shea SA, Andres LP, Shannon DC, Guz A, Banzett RB. Respiratory sensations in subjects who lack a ventilatory response to CO2. Respir Physiol. 1993;93(2):203–19. doi: 10.1016/0034-5687(93)90006-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Shea SA, Walter J, Pelley C, Murphy K, Guz A. The effect of visual and auditory stimuli upon resting ventilation in man. Respir Physiol. 1987;68(3):345–57. doi: 10.1016/s0034-5687(87)80019-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Shi CJ, Cassell MD. Cortical, thalamic, and amygdaloid connections of the anterior and posterior insular cortices. J Comp Neurol. 1998;399(4):440–68. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19981005)399:4<440::aid-cne2>3.0.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Song G, Poon CS. Functional and structural models of pontine modulation of mechanoreceptor and chemoreceptor reflexes. Respir Physiol Neurobiol. 2004;143(2–3):281–92. doi: 10.1016/j.resp.2004.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Tashiro M, Itoh M, Fujimoto T, Masud MM, Watanuki S, Yanai K. Application of positron emission tomography to neuroimaging in sports sciences. Methods. 2008;45(4):300–6. doi: 10.1016/j.ymeth.2008.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Tsumori T, Yokota S, Kishi T, Qin Y, Oka T, Yasui Y. Insular cortical and amygdaloid fibers are in contact with posterolateral hypothalamic neurons projecting to the nucleus of the solitary tract in the rat. Brain Res. 2006;1070(1):139–44. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, Mazoyer B, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002;15(1):273–89. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Van Dijk KRA, Hedden T, Venkataraman A, Evans KC, Lazar SW, Buckner RL. Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: Theory, properties, and optimization. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103(1):297–321. doi: 10.1152/jn.00783.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Vertes RP. Differential projections of the infralimbic and prelimbic cortex in the rat. Synapse. 2004;51(1):32–58. doi: 10.1002/syn.10279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. von Leupoldt A, Sommer T, Kegat S, Baumann HJ, Klose H, Dahme B, Buchel C. The unpleasantness of perceived dyspnea is processed in the anterior insula and amygdala. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2008;177(9):1026–32. doi: 10.1164/rccm.200712-1821OC. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. von Leupoldt A, Sommer T, Kegat S, Baumann HJ, Klose H, Dahme B, Buchel C. Dyspnea and pain share emotion-related brain network. NeuroImage. 2009a;48(1):200–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. von Leupoldt A, Sommer T, Kegat S, Eippert F, Baumann HJ, Klose H, Dahme B, et al. Down regulation of insular cortex responses to dyspnea and pain in asthma. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2009b;180(3):232–8. doi: 10.1164/rccm.200902-0300OC. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Wang J, Aguirre GK, Kimberg DY, Roc AC, Li L, Detre JA. Arterial spin labeling perfusion fMRI with very low task frequency. Magn Reson Med. 2003;49(5):796–802. doi: 10.1002/mrm.10437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Whalen PJ, Rauch SL, Etcoff NL, McInerney SC, Lee MB, Jenike MA. Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. J Neurosci. 1998;18(1):411–418. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-01-00411.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Wise RG, Ide K, Poulin MJ, Tracey I. Resting fluctuations in arterial carbon dioxide induce significant low frequency variations in BOLD signal. Neuroimage. 2004;21(4):1652–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Wise RG, Pattinson KT, Bulte DP, Chiarelli PA, Mayhew SD, Balanos GM, O’Connor DF, et al. Dynamic forcing of end-tidal carbon dioxide and oxygen applied to functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2007;27(8):1521–32. doi: 10.1038/sj.jcbfm.9600465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Xu F, Frazier DT. Role of the cerebellar deep nuclei in respiratory modulation. Cerebellum. 2002;1(1):35–40. doi: 10.1080/147342202753203078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES