Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Jul 28.
Published in final edited form as: Complement Health Pract Rev. 2009 Jan;14(1):3–9. doi: 10.1177/1533210109333718

Envisioning a Future Contemplative Science of Mindfulness: Fruitful Methods and New Content for the Next Wave of Research

Eric Garland 1, Susan Gaylord 1
PMCID: PMC2910917  NIHMSID: NIHMS217664  PMID: 20671798

Abstract

Mindfulness is an ancient spiritual practice as well as a unique behavioral technique involving the cultivation of non-judgmental, non-reactive, metacognitive awareness of present-moment experience. Given the growing interest in mindfulness across numerous academic and clinical disciplines, an agenda is needed to guide the next wave of research. Here, we suggest four areas that, in our view, are important for a future contemplative science of mindfulness: performance-based measures of mindfulness, scientific evaluation of Buddhist claims, neurophenomenology of mindfulness, and measuring changes in mindfulness-induced gene expression. By exploring these domains, the wisdom of the meditative traditions may be complemented by leading-edge empirical research methodologies.

Keywords: mindfulness, methods, neurophenomenology, psychosocial genomics, qualitative


Mindfulness is an ancient spiritual practice as well as a unique behavioral technique that is now being studied for its health benefits. Mindfulness involves the effort to attend, nonjudgmentally, to present-moment experience, and to sustain this attention over time, with the aim of cultivating stable, nonreactive, metacognitive awareness. Introduced over 2,500 years ago by Shakyamuni Buddha, mindfulness has been recommended as a technique for calming the mind and generating insight and compassion, all important for improving the human condition. As interest in research on mindfulness is building in fields as disparate as medicine, psychology, social work, and neuroscience, it is timely that we examine the methods and content of exploration, to ensure that the process and product of our research is optimally fruitful. Here, we suggest 4 areas that, in our view, are important for a future contemplative science of mindfulness (Wallace, 2006), an investigative approach that couples the wisdom of the meditative traditions with complementary, leading-edge empirical research methodologies.

Performance-Based Measures of Mindfulness

Increasingly, there is a need to further specify and operationalize the measurement of mindfulness for use in both clinical and basic science research programs. A number of questionnaires are currently used to quantify both state and trait mindfulness in studies, such as the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney, 2006), the Toronto Mindfulness Scale (Lau et al., 2006), and the Mindful Attention and Awareness Scale (Brown & Ryan, 2003). However, self-report scales are notoriously subject to threats to validity, such as inadequate explication of constructs, reactivity to being in an experimental or treatment condition, experimenter expectancies, social desirability biases, and misinterpretation of question items (Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002). Indeed, it should be recognized that questionnaire items serve as proxies for latent variables of interest (DeVellis, 2003). Oftentimes, multiple operationalizations of a given construct are possible, and allow for triangulation, thereby reducing monooperation bias, that is, the tendency for any one operationalization to inadequately represent the construct of interest (Shadish et al., 2002).

Given these concerns, we predict the growing need for performance-based, cognitive-behavioral measures of mindfulness. Methodologies from both cognitive and experimental psychology can serve as exemplars of this type of measurement. Although recent investigations have used cognitive measures such as the Attention Network Test to index mindfulness training-induced cognitive changes (Chambers, Lo, & Allen, 2007; Jha, Krompinger, & Baime, 2007; Tang et al., 2007), to our knowledge, no ecologically valid performance-based measure has been pioneered. As an example of research that might lead to the development of such a measure, Khalsa et al. (2008) recently investigated the ability of experienced meditators (Tibetan Buddhist and Kundalini yoga adepts) to detect their own heartbeats, a capability purported to be enhanced by meditative practice. Although the authors could not confirm their hypothesis that experienced meditators would exhibit heightened interoceptive awareness of the heart, the detection of other physiological or cognitive processes might serve as valid measures of mindfulness. Such a measure might be directly associated with mindfulness practices, such as the ability to focus on the breath as an anchor for one's awareness or the ability to know when one's mind has wandered (i.e., a sustained attention to response task, cf. Smallwood, McSpadden, & Schooler, 2007).

Similarly, behavioral measures of equanimity in response to laboratory stressors could be used as a somewhat more indirect proxy. Reports of mental events (thoughts, memories, imagery, emotional states) prompted by ecological momentary assessment during mindful breathing practice could be used to assess the quality and depth of state levels of mindfulness. Use of such measures would complement self-report scales, and if incorporated along with psychophysiological, biomarker, and brain imaging methodologies, could allow for a rich quantification of mindfulness.

Scientific Evaluation of Buddhist Claims

Across many Buddhist traditions, the intent of mindfulness training was to dispel ignorance (avidya) resulting in attachment and suffering and to realize the impermanence of the self. By cultivating nondiscursive awareness, one may gain insight (vipassana) into one's own essential nature, ultimately coming to the realization that there is no permanent self (anatman), that all objects are ultimately devoid of an independent identity (sunyata) and that all phenomena arise from an interdependent, cooriginating field (pratityasamutpada). These realizations are thought to result in the cessation of personal suffering and the genesis of compassion.

Qualitative interviews could tap the development of these ontological realizations in mindfulness practitioners, while self-report instruments and psychophysiological indices of stress and negative affect could assess their purported effect. Cognitive tasks could be used to objectively assess mindfulness-induced changes related to self-identification; for example, using a dichotic listening task, autobiographically relevant distractor stimuli such as the listener's name could be played in one's ear while he or she attempts to attend to and repeat a narrative played in the other ear. Growth curve analysis could be used to model how the inception of these realizations predicts variation in health trajectories over time.

Qualitative methodology could also be used to further explore changes in spiritually related constructs such as compassion, selflessness, and devotion with increased mindfulness and present-moment awareness. Traditional claims made about mindfulness may be supported by such research; however, it is also possible that training in mindfulness may have other effects among modern Westerners whose culture differs so greatly from the ancient Indo-Sino-Tibetan cultural roots of Buddhism. Such effects might include the facilitation of meaning-making processes like positive reappraisal (Garland, Gaylord, & Park, 2009), an emotion focused coping strategy used to find benefit in adversity for the purpose of healthy adaptation.

Neurophenomenology of Mindfulness

In line with neurophenomenology (Rudrauf, Lutz, Cosmelli, Lachaux, & Le Van Quyen, 2003; Varela, 1996), experimenters could combine first-person self-report data with neuroimaging technologies such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and electroencephalogram (EEG) to study the dynamic psychobiological effects of mindfulness practice on consciousness. With this methodology, one could reveal the structural features (Rudrauf et al., 2003) of the states of consciousness induced by mindfulness training. Such a mixed methods approach would capture the interpenetrating qualitative and quantitative aspects of the phenomena in question.

The neurophenomenological approach involves the active generation of experiential or phenomenological descriptive categories by a participant who is well trained in articulating fine-grained distinctions of mental experience. Long-term adepts of meditation are purportedly able to consistently invoke stable psychological states and describe them in greater and more accurate detail. By modeling associations between these precise phenomenological descriptions and changes in neural activity over time, mindfulness researchers may discover the biobehavioral correlates of specific mental processes relevant to both the experience of meditation and its clinical application.

Measuring Changes in Mindfulness-Induced Gene Expression

Recent evidence that mindfulness training increases antibody titers (Davidson et al., 2003) leads to recovery of natural-killer cell activity after cancer-related reductions (Witek-Janusek et al., 2008) and decreases cytokine production (Carlson, Speca, Faris, & Patel, 2007). These cellular changes may be mediated by experience-dependent gene expression, the process whereby social-environmental signals turn genes “on” and “off,” modulating gene translation and leading to alterations in protein synthesis (Pinaud, 2004). This hypothesis is supported by suggestive evidence that years of meditation experience correlates with increased cortical thickness in the right anterior insula and left superior temporal gyrus, brain areas associated with visceral attention, and self-awareness (Holzel et al., 2007; Lazar et al., 2005). Such mindfulness-induced neuroplasticity is likely mediated by changes in gene expression, the mechanism by which neurons secrete growth factors that activate gene translation in cell nuclei, resulting in the remodeling of synaptic connections (Mundkur, 2005).

Because stress affects neurogenesis through alterations in gene expression and translation (Glaser et al., 1990; Warner-Schmidt & Duman, 2006), ultimately leading to dysregulation of affect (Post, 1992), psychosocial interventions designed to reduce distress and improve mood may affect physiology through this pathway. Maximillian Muenke (2008), Chief of the Medical Genetics Branch at the National Institutes of Health, recently suggested that the therapeutic effects of stress reduction interventions such as clinical hypnosis may be mediated by changes in gene expression. Indeed, many mind–body interventions, including mindfulness, may affect the body through experience-dependent gene expression (Rossi, 2004). In line with this hypothesis, a recent study of Sudarshan Kriya, a meditative breathing practice, found increases in cellular markers of gene expression in practitioners relative to controls (Sharmaa et al., 2008). Although this research supports the experience-dependent gene expression hypothesis of mind–body medicine, its cross-sectional design does not allow for causal inference. However, in light of this potential shortcoming, another study identified alterations in the expression of 1,561 genes involved in the stress response before and after exposure to 8 weeks of meditation training (Dusek et al., 2008). As DNA microarray technology becomes more available, it may be possible to correlate the genomic effects of mindfulness with treatment outcomes in the not-too-distant future (Rossi, 2005).

Conclusion

The four domains of research direction outlined above are but a few of the many potential areas for future research. The positive findings so far on the health benefits of mindfulness (Greeson, 2009, in this issue) justify using the best tools of science to increase the evidence base for this increasingly popular complementary therapy while seeking to elucidate claims proffered by the classical canons of the meditative traditions.

Acknowledgments

ELG was supported by Grant Number T32AT003378 from the National Center for Complementary and Alternative Medicine, a Francisco J. Varela Research Grant from the Mind and Life Institute, and an Armfield-Reeves Innovation Grant from the UNC-Chapel Hill School of Social Work.

Biographies

Eric Garland, MSW, LCSW, is a Pre-doctoral Fellow in Complementary and Alternative Medicine with the University of North Carolina - Chapel Hill (UNC-CH) Program on Integrative Medicine. Mr. Garland currently works as a licensed psychotherapist in an integrative medicine setting while pursuing a PhD from the UNC-CH School of Social Work, where he conducts clinical research on mind-body interventions. He recently received a National Research Service Award from the National Center for Complementary and Alternative Medicine (NCCAM) as well as a Francisco J. Varela Research Grant from the Mind and Life Institute (Boulder, CO) to support his investigations of mindfulness, implicit cognition, stress psychophysiology, and addiction. Mr. Garland has over seven years of clinical experience working with diverse patient populations, and specializes in the use of mindfulness-based psychotherapies and clinical hypnosis to treat a wide range of emotional, behavioral, and psychosomatic disorders. He is a professional member of the American Society of Clinical Hypnosis and the National Association of Social Workers.

Susan A. Gaylord, Ph.D. is Director of the Program on Integrative Medicine and Assistant Professor in the Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of North Carolina School of Medicine, where, since 1998, she has led the development of research, education, and clinical initiatives in complementary, alternative, and integrative medicine. In 1995, she founded the multidisciplinary course “Principles and Practices of Complementary and Alternative Medicine (CAM),” attended in the past twelve years by over 700 health professions students and faculty. She is also course director for two other UNC CAM medical school electives: “Fieldwork in Complementary and Alternative Medicine” and “Introduction to Integrative Medicine.” She was principal investigator on a five-year NIH grant to develop and evaluate innovative CAM educational methodologies for health professionals, and is currently principal investigator on an NIH grant to study Mindfulness for IBS, as well as co-PI (with Doug Mann, PI) on a grant to study Craniosacral Therapies for Migraine. She is also the PI on a Blue-Cross Blue-Shield funded project to develop a pilot integrative diabetes management program in a rural underserved population in NC. She is the founder and director of the UNC Mindfulness-based Stress and Pain Management program (with Douglas Mann, MD as co-director) and led the development of the interdisciplinary Integrative Medicine Consult Service in the Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. She has played a leadership role in implementing the Planetree program (holistic, patient-centered care) at UNC Health Care, with its pilot program in the Rehabilitation Inpatient Unit, and is the chair of the Planetree Taskforce on CAM. Research interests include studies of outcomes and mechanisms of action of specific complementary therapies - including homeopathy, mindfulness, craniosacral therapy and acupuncture; the role of health beliefs in determining care pathways; cancer therapeutics; geriatrics; and women's health issues.

Footnotes

References

  1. Baer RA, Smith GT, Hopkins J, Krietemeyer J, Toney L. Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment. 2006;13:27–45. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Brown KW, Ryan RM. The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;84:822–848. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Carlson LE, Speca M, Faris P, Patel KD. One year pre-post intervention follow-up of psychological, immune, endocrine and blood pressure outcomes of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) in breast and prostate cancer outpatients. Brain Behavior & Immunity. 2007;21:1038–1049. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2007.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Chambers R, Lo BCY, Allen NB. The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2008;32:303–322. [Google Scholar]
  5. Davidson RJ, Kabat-Zinn J, Schumacher J, Rosenkranz M, Muller D, Santorelli SF, et al. Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2003;65:564–570. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000077505.67574.e3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. DeVellis RF. Scale development: Theory and applications. 2nd. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  7. Dusek JA, Otu HH, Wohlhueter AL, Bhasin M, Zerbini LF, et al. Genomic Counter-Stress Changes Induced by the Relaxation Response. PLoS ONE. 2008;3(7):e2576. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Garland EL, Gaylord S, Park J. The role of mindfulness in positive reappraisal. Explore. 2009;5:37–44. doi: 10.1016/j.explore.2008.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Glaser R, Kennedy S, Lafuse WP, Bonneau RH, Speicher C, Hillhouse J, et al. Psychological stress-induced modulation of interleukin 2 receptor gene expression and interleukin 2 production in peripheral blood leukocytes. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1990;47:707–712. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810200015002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Greeson JM. Mindfulness research update: 2008. Complementary Health Practice Review. 2009;14:9–17. doi: 10.1177/1533210108329862. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Holzel BK, Ott U, Gard T, Hempel H, Weygandt M, Morgen K, et al. Investigation of mindfulness meditation practitioners with voxel-based morphometry. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2007;3:55–61. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Jha A, Krompinger J, Baime M. Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience. 2007;7:109–119. doi: 10.3758/cabn.7.2.109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Khalsa SS, Rudrauf D, Damasio AR, Davidson RJ, Lutz A, Tranel D. Interoceptive awareness in experienced meditators. Psychophysiology. 2008;45:671–677. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00666.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Lau MA, Bishop SR, Segal ZV, Buis T, Anderson ND, Carlson L, et al. The Toronto Mindfulness Scale: development and validation. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2006;62:1445–1467. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Lazar SW, Kerr CE, Wasserman RH, Gray JR, Greve DN, Treadway MT, et al. Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness. Neuroreport. 2005;16:1893–1897. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000186598.66243.19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Muenke M. Invited address at the 59th Annual Society for Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis Scientific Program. King of Prussia, PA: 2008. A paradigm shift in genetics: How hypnosis influences our genes. [Google Scholar]
  17. Mundkur N. Neuroplasticity in children. Indian Journal of Pediatrics. 2005;72:855–857. doi: 10.1007/BF02731115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Pinaud R. Experience-dependent immediate early gene expression in the adult central nervous system: Evidence from enriched-environment studies. International Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;114:321–333. doi: 10.1080/00207450490264142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Post RM. Transduction of psychosocial stress into the neurobiology of recurrent affective disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1992;149:999–1010. doi: 10.1176/ajp.149.8.999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Rossi EL. Stress-induced alternative gene splicing in mind-body medicine. Advances in Mind Body Medicine. 2004;20:12–19. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Rossi EL. Prospects for exploring the molecular-genomic foundations of therapeutic hypnosis with DNA microarrays. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis. 2005;48:165–182. doi: 10.1080/00029157.2005.10401514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Rudrauf D, Lutz A, Cosmelli D, Lachaux JP, Le Van Quyen M. From autopoiesis to neurophenomenology: Francisco Varela's exploration of the biophysics of being. Biological Research. 2003;36:27–65. doi: 10.4067/s0716-97602003000100005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Shadish WR, Cook TD, Campbell DT. Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. New York: Houghton Mifflin Company; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  24. Sharmaa H, Dattaa P, Singha A, Sena S, Bhardwajb N, Kochupillaib V, et al. Gene expression profiling in practitioners of Sudarshan Kriya. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2008;64:213–218. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2007.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Smallwood J, McSpadden M, Schooler JW. The lights are on but no one's home: meta-awareness and the decoupling of attention when the mind wanders. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 2007;14:527–533. doi: 10.3758/bf03194102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Tang YY, Ma Y, Wang J, Fan Y, Feng S, Lu Q, et al. Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2007;104:17152–17156. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707678104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Varela FJ. Neurophenomenology: A methodological remedy to the hard problem. Journal of Consciousness Studies. 1996;3:330–350. [Google Scholar]
  28. Wallace BA. Contemplative science: Where Buddhism and neuroscience converge. New York: Columbia University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  29. Warner-Schmidt JL, Duman RS. Hippocampal neurogenesis: opposing effects of stress and antidepressant treatment. Hippocampus. 2006;16:239–249. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Witek-Janusek L, Albuquerque K, Chroniak KR, Chroniak C, Durazo-Arvizu R, Mathews HL. Effect of mindfulness based stress reduction on immune function, quality of life and coping in women newly diagnosed with early stage breast cancer. Brain Behavior & Immunity. 2008;22:969–981. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2008.01.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES