Abstract
Objective This study examined the longitudinal effects of a school-based program on kindergarten and first grade children's fruit and vegetable (F&V) consumption. Methods The program included lunchroom, classroom, school-wide, and family components. The primary dependent variable, F&V consumed at lunch, was assessed using weighed plate waste. Hierarchical linear models were used to analyze the differences between intervention and control groups and to account for repeated measurements. Results Children in the experimental group consumed more F&V (F = 29 g; V = 6 g; 0.43 portions/lunch; 0.28 servings/lunch) at the end of Year 1 compared with children in the control group. At the end of Year 2, children in the experimental group consumed more fruit (21 g; 0.23 portions/lunch; 0.15 servings/lunch), but not more vegetables compared with children in the control group. Conclusions The intervention resulted in increased F&V consumption, with more pronounced and enduring effects for fruits than vegetables.
Fruits and vegetables (F&V) are important components of healthful diets. Despite calls for increased F&V consumption (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services & U.S. Department of Agriculture, 2005), one study found that over 95% of black and white girls failed to meet the Healthy People 2010 recommendations for fruit and vegetable intake on even a single day (Striegel-Moore et al., 2006). Schools can play an important role in the promotion of healthy eating (IOM, 2005, 2007), with ∼28 million children participating in the National School Lunch Program (IOM, 2004). Regulations governing this program require that schools serve at least two fruits or vegetables each day in the school lunch (Code of Federal Regulations, 2007).
A number of researchers have focused on school-based promotion of F&V consumption. Knai, Pomerleau, Lock, and McKee (2006) conducted an international research review of interventions designed to promote F&V consumption among school-aged children. Of the 15 studies designed to encourage children to eat more F&V, 10 had significant positive effects on students’ consumption. Given the importance of promoting F&V with young children (Kelder, Perry, Klepp, & Lytle, 1994) surprisingly, only three studies included children under third grade (i.e., Auld, Romaniello, Heimendinger, Hambidge & Hambidge, 1998; Horne, Tapper, Lowe, Hardman, Jackson, & Wooner, 2004; Perry et al., 2004). All of these studies with young children found improvements in their F&V consumption relative to a control group (i.e., between 0.14 and 0.58 servings), Auld et al. (1998) used non-school personnel (i.e., special resource teachers and parents) to implement their program, Perry et al. (2004) made changes to the F&V that were provided to students in the cafeteria, and Horne et al. (2004) implemented a token economy where lunch aides gave students hand stamps when they ate some of their F&V and then teachers gave students with hand stamps small prizes after lunch. Hendy, Williams, and Carnise (2005) also used a token economy system with older students, but their intervention was implemented by study researchers, who provided prizes contingent on F&V consumption.
The use of rewards contingent upon food consumption is controversial, as it has been shown to decrease children's food preferences (Birch, Marlin, & Rotter, 1984). To avoid “overjustification effects,” whereby children's intrinsic motivation to eat F&V would be reduced as a result of external reinforcement (Lepper, Greene, & Nisbett, 1973), Hendy et al. (2005) used small and delayed reinforcement, provided reinforcement contingent upon a few bites of the target food, and incorporated peer participation and modeling. They demonstrated that children did not report decreased preferences for F&V at follow-up. Whereas the use of token economy systems appeared effective in the Horne et al. (2004) and Hendy et al. (2005) studies, the extent to which this type of intervention can be sustained by school staff over time is questionable. Therefore, it is important to investigate the effectiveness, fidelity, and acceptability of reinforcement-based interventions that are simpler to use, require fewer resources, and are implemented over time by school staff, rather than research personnel. Given the potential for schools to help children increase F&V consumption, there is a need to design effective programs that are affordable, acceptable, feasible, and sustainable.
The purpose of this study was to examine the impact of a multi-year, multi-component, school-based F&V promotion program on students’ F&V consumption during school lunch. Unlike previous studies, this study did not modify the type of food served in the cafeteria, but instead used a reinforcement-based intervention that was contained in the cafeteria and implemented by school staff. This study is one of the very few school-based efforts to include children as young as kindergarten. It was anticipated that our program would be perceived as acceptable and feasible, and it was hypothesized that students participating in the program would demonstrate greater F&V consumption relative to students in a control group. Second, it was hypothesized that students in the experimental group would demonstrate sustained increases in F&V consumption over time.
Method
Participants and Setting
This study was conducted in a large, urban school district in the Northeastern part of the United States. All kindergarten and first grade children attending four public schools were invited to participate in the spring and fall 2005. Parents provided written consent (56% experimental group; 45% control group; N = 297) for their child to participate. Demographic information about the sample is summarized in Table I. Participating school staff who served as interventionists included: (a) lunch aides (n = 12; 6/cafeteria); (b) classroom and computer teachers (n = 24); (c) principals (n = 2); and (d) athletic coaches (n = 4; full-time staff who promoted physical activity). The ratio of lunch aides to students in the cafeteria was 1:30. There were approximately 90 students in each lunch period. With the exception of the athletic coaches, all interventionists were employed by the school district.
Table I.
Participant Demographic Information by Group
Group |
||
---|---|---|
Experimental | Control | |
(n = 149) | (n = 148) | |
Participation rate (%) | 56 | 45 |
Percentage of attrition | ||
Pre-intervention to Year 1a | 3 | 5 |
Year 1 to Year 2a | 20 | 29 |
Mean age in years (SD) | 6.2 (0.53) | 6.2 (0.60) |
Gender (% boys) | 51 | 51 |
Race/ethnicity (%) | ||
African American | 29 | 36 |
Latino | 41 | 51 |
Asianb | 24 | 0 |
White | 3 | 4 |
Other | 2 | 9 |
Parent born outside United States (%)b | 74 | 55 |
Non-English spoken at home (%)b | 70 | 40 |
Children eligible for free or reduced price lunch (%) | 94 | 88 |
BMI-for-age z-score at pre-intervention | 0.80 (1.13) | 0.93 (1.07) |
aAttrition was due to students moving to a new school.
bGroups are significantly different.
School-Based Fruit and Vegetable Promotion Program (F&VPP)
Based in social learning theory (Bandura, 1977), the program included school-wide, classroom, lunchroom, and family components to promote F&V consumption with an emphasis on F&V in the school lunch (Blom-Hoffman, 2008). Program components were designed to capture students’ attention and to increase retention of F&V nutrition information by having influential role models (i.e., cartoon characters, videos with same age peers, the school principal, coaches) deliver consistent information across multiple settings. The program was designed: (a) to include repetition of messaging across settings, modalities, and messengers; (b) to use symbolic and live role modeling; (c) to incorporate vicarious and direct reinforcement; and (d) to be implemented entirely by school staff. Given the heavy demands placed on school staff, the program was designed to have many school staff members take on very minor roles related to program delivery. In this manner, staff members were not burdened by their roles related to program implementation and children were surrounded by the program across multiple settings at school.
The school-wide component included loudspeaker announcements providing an interesting fact about the “F&V of the day.” The announcement was made by a high profile, respected adult in the school (i.e., the school principal or coach) and was designed to increase students’ attention to the messages. There was no cost associated with this component.
The classroom component included the 5-A-Day Adventures CD-ROM (Dole Food Company, 2000) used during computer classes. The computer program delivered health information to students via attractive, engaging cartoon characters and videos with same age peers. The characters and child actors served as symbolic role models for children in the study. There was no cost associated with this component as the CD-ROM was available to schools free of charge.
The lunchtime component involved hanging cafeteria posters reflecting the F&V of the day, and lunch aides “catching” students eating F&V by giving them verbal praise and a sticker contingent on one bite of these foods. The cafeteria posters, which changed daily to reflect the F&V being served that day in the school lunch, incorporated the cartoon characters from the classroom component and were placed as close to the cafeteria line as possible, so as to be visible as students were making decisions about whether or not to take a serving of fruit or vegetables. The attractive, engaging characters were intended to serve as symbolic role models to students and to capture their attention while they were standing on the cafeteria line. The “caught eating F&V” component was a reinforcement-based intervention that was designed to capitalize on the effects of peer role modeling and vicarious learning. Students observed peers being reinforced for taking a bite of a fruit or vegetable with stickers that incorporated the cartoon characters from the CD-ROM or F&V drawings designed by the students. Costs associated with this component were $0.04/sticker and a one-time cost of approximately $100 to print the posters.
The family component involved a series of interactive children's books assigned as homework, the 5-A-Day Kids Cookbook (Dole Food Company, 2004), and a school cookbook developed by children, parents and teachers, which was used as a fundraiser (Blom-Hoffman, Wilcox, Dunn, Leff, & Power, 2008). The five interactive children's books were designed to: (a) reinforce a simple health message that students learned in school; (b) communicate consistent health information to caregivers; and (c) provide a context for caregivers and children to discuss health information together. Unlike other modes of delivering nutrition information to families (e.g., newsletters or workshops), the interactive children's books, which were assigned as homework, provided a cost effective, time efficient mechanism to deliver health information to large numbers of families in the comfort of their homes. Activities in the books included goal setting, self-monitoring, and symbolic role modeling with the cartoon characters, and each book cost approximately $3.38 (Blom-Hoffman et al., 2008).
Measures
The study design was longitudinal with measurements collected in the winter 2006 (Pre-intervention), spring 2006 (“Year 1”) and spring 2007 (“Year 2”), except where otherwise noted. Table II summarizes the data collected from this study.
Table II.
Data Collection Measures, Source, Wave, and Sample Size
Measure | Data source | Data collection wave | Sample size |
|
---|---|---|---|---|
Experimental | Control | |||
Fruit intake | Students | Pre-intervention | 128 | 123 |
Year 1 | 127 | 123 | ||
Year 2 | 113 | 97 | ||
Vegetable intake | Students | Pre-intervention | 131 | 115 |
Year 1 | 128 | 120 | ||
Year 2 | 113 | 96 | ||
Adapted Fruit and Vegetable | Students | Pre-intervention | 135 | 130 |
Preference Questionnaire (A-FVPQ) | Year 1 | 131 | 105 | |
Year 2 | 113 | 99 | ||
BMI-for-age z-scores | Students | Pre-intervention | 148 | 145 |
Gender | Students | Pre-intervention | 149 | 148 |
Race/ethnicity | Parents | Pre-intervention | 99 | 91 |
School | Design | Pre-intervention | 149 | 148 |
Parent questionnaire | Parents | Pre-intervention | 101 | 95 |
Student acceptability | Students | Year 1 | 113 | N/A |
Year 2 | 95 | |||
Teacher acceptability | Teachers | Year 1 | 15 | N/A |
Year 2 | 14 | |||
Lunch aide acceptability | Lunch aides | Year 1 | 12 | N/A |
Year 2 | 9 |
Intervention Monitoring and Fidelity
Unannounced intervention fidelity checks were conducted by a research assistant (RA) to assess the extent to which lunch monitors implemented the lunchtime components of the intervention (i.e., providing stickers contingent on a bite of fruit or vegetable and hanging up the correct F&V of the day). Direct observations were recorded on a fidelity checklist. Fidelity of the lunchtime observations for each school was expressed as a percent of days the lunchtime components were observed to be implemented. Morning announcements were recorded in a log by the school staff member who made them, and the percent of school days that morning announcements were made was calculated.
Acceptability Questionnaires
Intervention acceptability refers to the degree to which interventionists find an intervention appropriate, fair and reasonable (Witt & Elliott, 1985). Lunch aides and classroom teachers completed acceptability questionnaires (Blom-Hoffman, 2008). At the end of both years participating lunch aides completed a 10-item acceptability questionnaire using a six-point scale (1 = strongly disagree; 6 = strongly agree; α = .85) to report their perceptions of the lunchtime component. A sample item included: “Giving stickers to students when they eat their fruits and vegetables during lunch is a good way to help them eat more fruit and vegetables.” Teachers completed a 17-item questionnaire using the same six-point scale described above (α = .89) at the end of both intervention years. A sample item included: “The fruit and vegetable stickers are an acceptable way to encourage students to eat more fruit and vegetables during school lunch.”
Child Questionnaire
Children also completed a brief questionnaire containing both open and closed-ended responses. Children used a three-point pictorial rating scale to report responses to four closed ended questions that assessed their perceptions of the program. Sample items included: “How much do you think the stickers help you eat your fruit and vegetables at lunch?” and “How much did you like getting stickers?” Open-ended questions were designed to examine children's awareness of the intervention. For example, children who indicated they received stickers at lunch were asked “What did you have to do to get the lunch stickers?” This information was elicited to provide additional evidence above and beyond the fidelity checks that the intervention was being implemented as intended.
Child F&V Preferences
Child-reported F&V preferences were assessed using an adapted version of the Fruit and Vegetable Preference Questionnaire (A-FVPQ). The original questionnaire was developed by Domel, Baranowski, Davis, Leonard, Riley, & Baranowski (1993) to assess older students. Blom-Hoffman, Dai, & Franko (2004) adapted this questionnaire by including food photographs and a three-point (0 = does not like, 1 = likes a little; 2 = likes a lot) pictorial response card similar to that described by Birch and Sullivan (1991), and examined the psychometric properties of the adapted questionnaire with a separate sample of 5 to 7-year-old children. The A-FVPQ consisted of 20 items (11 fruits; nine vegetables). Students were shown a picture of each food item and were asked to report how much they liked the food using the pictorial scale. Scores on the fruit preference scale ranged from 0 to 22, and scores on the vegetable preference scale ranged from 0 to 18. The A-FVPQ had acceptable internal consistency (vegetable scale α = .66; fruit scale α = .68), test-retest reliability (vegetable ICC = .79; fruit ICC = .72), and convergent validity (assessed by having students sort fresh fruits and vegetables from the questionnaire according to their preferences: total fruit preference ICC = .72; total vegetable preference ICC = .80).
Weighed Plate Waste
Direct measurements of student lunchtime F&V intake were used as the primary dependent variable to assess F&V consumption. Assessments were conducted in the four school cafeterias on three days within each data collection phase, so that a total of 36 days of data were collected. Across each school and each data collection phase assessments were scheduled to sample a range of consistent, similar foods (i.e., cooked green vegetable, raw vegetable, cooked corn, fresh and canned fruit). Prior to each data collection lunch period, three school lunches were purchased to determine the average portion sizes served. In every instance processed fruit was purchased from foods already portioned out in containers for students and whole, fresh fruits were randomly selected from serving bins. In most cases vegetables were purchased from trays on the lunch line that were already portioned out for students. In some cases when vegetables were not pre-portioned, cafeteria staff, blind to study purposes, group assignments, and hypotheses, was asked to provide the portion of the vegetable to the research staff that students would receive on a lunch tray. In these cases, the same serving utensil that was used to serve children was used to portion the sample servings.
The average fruit portion served in the schools was 92 g (range = 88–95) and the average vegetable portion served was 55 g (range = 54–56). A one-way analysis of variance was conducted to explore differences in fruit and vegetable portion size across the three time points. There was no statistically significant difference in the portion sizes served to children for fruit [F(2,45) = 0.45, p = ns] or vegetables [F(2,32) = 0.46, p = ns] across the three time points. According to the FDA, the average fruit serving size is 140 g and vegetable serving size is 85 g (Eldridge, Smith-Warner, Lytle, & Murray, 1998). These figures were used to calculate changes in consumption in terms of portion and serving size.
Trained RAs, blind to study purposes, hypotheses and group assignments, observed children throughout the lunch period and collected trays. All children wore nametags so RAs were able to identify them. When students were finished eating they were instructed to place their nametag on their tray. While observing children in small groups, RAs recorded each of the foods on the trays. To assess reliability of direct observations, two independent RAs simultaneously observed 23% of students across the study. Kappa coefficients for fruits (range = .69–.86) and vegetables (range = .86–.93) ranged from “moderate” to “nearly perfect” agreement.
At the end of the lunch periods, trays of students who took the school lunch (Pre-intervention = 97%; Year 1 = 97%; Year 2 = 98%) were weighed on a food scale that was accurate to 1 g (Salter Model 2006, Kent, England). Only foods served in the school lunch were measured. The scale was checked for accuracy using calibration weights. Each fruit and vegetable was weighed separately. Food intake was calculated as follows: Initial mean portion served in grams minus amount of food left on the tray in grams.
Caregiver Questionnaire
At Pre-intervention, primary caregivers (n = 196; 66%) were interviewed by phone in their native language (English, Spanish, Vietnamese) by a native speaker, who used a structured questionnaire to obtain demographic information, including child race and ethnicity.
Child BMI
The lead author weighed and measured all of the children, who removed their shoes and sweaters. Anthropometric techniques followed those described by Lohman, Roche, and Martorell (1988). Weight was measured to 0.1 kg (Seca digital electronic scale, Creative Health Products, Plymouth, MI). Standing height was measured to 0.1 cm with a portable stadiometer (Shorr Productions, Olney, MD). BMI-for-age z-scores were calculated using EpiInfo (CDC, 2004), which used normative data from the CDC 2000 reference growth charts (Ogden et al., 2002).
Procedures
The four elementary schools were selected to participate in this study because they were already collaborating with the university on a physical activity promotion initiative for older grades. The schools were randomly assigned to condition (i.e., experimental or control). The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Northeastern University and by the school district's research office. Multiple participant recruitment procedures were used in this study (Blom-Hoffman, Leff, Franko, Weinstein, Beakley, & Power, 2009). These procedures included increasing communication about the study between the principal investigator and the school staff and students, sending consent forms home for parents to sign, and setting up a class-wide reinforcement system designed to reward returned consent forms regardless of parents’ willingness to have their child participate in the study. Although parental consent was required for students’ to participate in the outcome evaluation, all students received the program. School staff was not aware of which students had obtained parental consent and which had not.
Following recruitment, Pre-intervention data were collected. Students’ height and weight were measured, the A-FVPQ was administered, and BMI and plate waste data were collected. The program was implemented continuously in the two experimental group schools between the winter 2006 and spring 2007. Data collection procedures were repeated in the spring 2006 (Year 1) and the spring 2007 (Year 2). All measures used in this study along with the source of information, administration period, and sample size associated with each measure are described in Table II.
Interventionist training was conducted by the first author, who met with teachers individually or in small groups to demonstrate the use of the CD-ROM and with lunch monitors as a group prior to implementation of the “caught eating F&V” intervention. Lunch monitors and first author discussed the importance of helping children eat F&V in the school lunch and the lunch monitors’ desire to help students improve their eating. The “caught eating F&V” component was described, sample stickers were shared, and lunch monitors determined how it would be best implemented in their cafeteria. Following this initial meeting, the first author observed the initial day of implementation and provided feedback to the lunch monitors. The RA who conducted fidelity checks provided additional stickers when needed. At the end of each year, a wrap-up meeting with the lunch monitors was held.
Statistical Analyses
In this study, students were nested within schools and data were collected for each student at multiple time points. Analyses were conducted at the time point (within-student) level, taking into account the repeated measurements within students. School was treated as a fixed effect (i.e., differences among school means were modeled) rather than as a random effect (modeling the correlation among students within schools), due to the small number of schools, making it unlikely that random school effects could be accurately estimated (Brown & Prescott, 1999). Hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) was used to analyze the data, due to the ability of HLM to accommodate repeated measures and to handle missing data that are inevitable in longitudinal analyses (Beunckens, Molenberghs, & Kenward, 2005; Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992; Verbeke & Molenberghs, 2000). A separate model was constructed for the amount of fruit consumed (intake in grams) and the amount of vegetables consumed (intake in grams). Group differences in each measure Yij (i denotes yearly measures within students and j denotes students) after the intervention (Years 1 and 2) were modeled as follows:
![]() |
where β0j = γ00 + u0j (a student-specific intercept term); Var(rij) = σ2 (the estimated variance within students); Var(u0j) = τ00 (the estimated variance between students).
In the model equation, β0j is an intercept term, β1 is the estimated effect of group and β2 through βn represent the estimated effects of a set of adjustment variables, specifically the dependent variable measured at pre-intervention and sex, race/ethnicity, BMI z-score, F&V preference, and school. Model estimates are interpreted as estimated fruit/vegetable consumption in Years 1 and 2 after statistically controlling for differences in the amount consumed at pre-intervention and the other adjustment variables. A sustained intervention effect is indicated by a significant group difference in both Years 1 and 2; an unsustained or temporary intervention effect is indicated by a significant group difference in Year 1 but not in Year 2. Hierarchical linear models (PROC MIXED in SAS v.9.1.3, SAS Institute, Cary, NC) were used to account for the repeated measurements (Verbeke & Molenberghs, 2000). A random intercept was modeled and all other effects were fixed.
There were missing data at pre-intervention and follow-up time points. Preliminary analyses showed that the data were not missing completely at random (Rubin, 1987), therefore handling the missing data by excluding observations with missing values would not be a valid approach. Simple forms of imputation (e.g., median imputation, last-observation-carried-forward) were ruled out because they can lead to biased estimates (Allison, 2002). Multiple imputation (PROC MI and MIANALYZE in SAS) was selected to handle missing data because in longitudinal clinical trials involving missing observations at baseline as well as at follow-up, multiple imputation produces more accurate estimates than standard methods for handling missing data (Tang, Song, Belin, & Unutzer, 2005). Briefly, multiple imputations involve replacing each missing data point with several plausible values that are estimated based on regression using the non-missing data points, with an element of randomness added to the imputation. This enables efficient use of data from participants who failed to complete some measurements (as opposed to excluding such participants from the analysis), while at the same time adjusting for the uncertainty arising from the fact that some data are being imputed.
Diagnostics indicated that all models were reasonably consistent with statistical assumptions. Additional models were estimated using F&V preference as the dependent variable, utilizing the same analytic techniques as in the intake models. The amount of fruit consumed was compared with the amount of vegetables consumed using paired t-tests, a separate test for each study year. All tests of significance were two-sided and performed at the α = .05 level. The intra-class correlation (ICC) is reported as an estimate of the proportion of the variance in F&V consumption that was between as opposed to within students [ICC = τ00/(τ00 + σ2); Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992], after taking the adjustment variables into account. Cohen's d, interpreted as the standardized difference between groups, was used as an indicator of effect sizes; as a rule of thumb, d values around .2 have been considered “small,”.5 are “medium,” and .8 are “large” (Cohen, 1988).
Results
Sample and Attrition
The study sample was racially and ethnically diverse (Table I). In both the experimental and control groups many children had a parent born outside the United States and non-English languages were spoken in the homes. Children's BMI-for-age z-scores, age in years and gender were similar between the two groups. The experimental group had more Asian children, more children with a parent born outside the United States, and more non-English spoken at home. There was significant attrition between Year 1 and Year 2 data collection waves because many students transferred to other non-study schools.
Process Evaluation
Fidelity checks for the lunchtime components were conducted on 24% and 16% of school days in Years 1 and 2, respectively. In both years, integrity for the “caught eating F&V” lunchtime procedures was high (88% of days checked in Year 1 and 94% of days checked in Year 2). The correct F&V of the day poster was hung on 62% and 59% of days checked in Years 1 and 2, respectively. According to school logs, morning announcements were made on 91% and 65% of school days in Years 1 and 2, respectively.
Children's perceptions of the F&V program were positive. Almost all children (98%) reported hearing the morning announcements. Most reported liking the announcements “a lot” (78%). Nearly all students reported receiving the lunchtime stickers (94%). Almost all students reported either that they need to eat F&V (75%) or “eat healthy” at lunch (19%) to receive a sticker. Many students reported that the stickers helped them eat their F&V “a lot” (87%), and they liked receiving the stickers “a lot” (73%).
Most lunch aides completed the acceptability questionnaire at the end of Years 1 (100%) and 2 (75%). On the six-point scale, lunch aides rated the program as highly acceptable (Year 1: M = 5.62; SD = 0.65; Year 2: M = 5.67; SD = 0.39). Moreover, in both years 100% of the lunch aides reported that they “strongly agreed” that giving stickers helped the children to eat more F&V in the school lunch.
Most teachers completed the acceptability questionnaire in Years 1 (100%) and 2 (88%). Using the six-point scale, teachers rated the program as highly acceptable (Year 1: M = 5.51; SD = 0.38; Year 2: M = 5.34; SD = 0.50). Only one teacher reported a negative side effect from the program. She was concerned that the program's emphasis on increasing F&V consumption may have inadvertently communicated that the other foods were less important.
Student F&V Preferences
Students’ fruit preferences were higher than their vegetable preferences across all three time points, and preferences were remarkably stable across time (fruit preference range M = 18.54–18.78; SD = 5.54–3.03; vegetable preference range M = 11.43–11.56; SD = 2.93–3.06). After adjusting for Pre-intervention fruit and vegetable preferences, the study groups did not differ in preference for fruit in either Year 1 or Year 2 (Experimental minus control fruit preference, Year 1 = 0.10, 95% CI: −1.01, 1.21, p = .85; Year 2 = 0.33, 95% CI: −0.63, 1.29, p = .50), nor were there any group differences in vegetable preferences in Years 1 or 2 (Experimental minus control vegetable preference, Year 1 = 0.24, 95% CI: −1.00, 1.48, p = .69; Year 2 < 0.01, 95% CI: −1.02, 1.03, p = .99).
Group Differences in Fruit and Vegetable Consumption
Means for the F&V consumption measures by study group and year are shown in Table III. Preliminary analyses showed that at pre-intervention, the groups differed only on vegetable intake, which was about 3 g/lunch greater among children in the experimental group (p < .05). All remaining analyses examining group differences statistically controlled for F&V intake at Pre-intervention.
Table III.
Mean (95% CI) and for Measures of Fruit and Vegetable Consumption by Study Group and Year
Consumption measure | Study year |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
Group | Pre-intervention | Year 1 | Year 2 | |
Fruit intake (g) | Experimental | 27 (22, 32) | 45 (40, 51)*** | 34 (30, 39)*** |
Control | 29 (24, 34) | 26 (21, 31) | 23 (18, 28) | |
Vegetable intake (g) | Experimental | 11 (9, 13)* | 18 (15, 20)** | 15 (12, 17) |
Control | 8 (7, 10) | 8 (6, 10) | 9 (7, 11) |
Note. Multiple imputations were used to handle missing data. Significant differences between the experimental and control groups are noted as follows: ***p < .001; ** p < .01; *p < .05.
Group Differences in Fruit Consumption
Children in the experimental group consumed more fruit compared with children in the control group in Years 1 and 2 of the program. In Year 1 children in the experimental group consumed 29 more grams of fruit per lunch (95% CI: 21–38 g, p < .0001, Cohen's d = .86, ICC = .09; 0.32 portions/lunch; 0.21 serving/lunch) compared with children in the control group. In Year 2, children in the experimental group consumed 21 more grams of fruit per lunch (95% CI: 10–32 g, p < .0005, Cohen's d = .55; 0.23 portions/lunch; 0.15 serving/lunch) compared with children in the control group.
Group Differences in Vegetable Consumption
In Year 1 children in the experimental group consumed 6 more grams of vegetables per lunch (95% CI: 2–11 g, p < .01, Cohen's d = .34, ICC = .15; .11 portions/lunch; .07 servings/lunch) compared with children in the control group. However, in Year 2, grams of vegetables consumed did not differ between the groups.
Discussion
The multi-component, school-based program was implemented with good levels of integrity and resulted in increased F&V consumption. At the end of Year 1, children in the experimental group ate 0.43 additional portions and 0.28 additional servings of F&V per day served in the school lunch. This finding is consistent with the review of Knai et al. (2006), which found that among the 15 studies examined, 10 demonstrated increased F&V consumption, ranging from 0.30 to 0.99 servings per lunch. The effect of the current study was reduced at the end of Year 2 with students in the experimental group eating 0.23 additional portions of fruit (0.15 additional servings) and no additional vegetables with children in the control group. These changes are consistent with other research findings in this area, and are important from a public health perspective as they represent a sustained increase in fruit consumption at the group level.
Although it is impossible to identify the main variable(s) responsible for student behavior change, it is likely that the intermittent reinforcement component implemented at the point of performance played a major role in increasing students’ consumption. Although the other program components are probably less important in terms of impacting behavior change, they provided a context for the lunchtime component. The CD-ROM and cafeteria posters helped the students identify where the F&V were located in their school lunch; the cafeteria poster reminded the students to take F&V on their lunch tray, the loudspeaker announcements and CD-ROM motivated the students to eat F&V by raising the profile of these foods on an on-going basis, and the family component helped to bring the messages home.
Fidelity checks for the lunchtime components indicated that integrity for the reinforcement procedures was very high; however, the extent to which the correct F&V of the day was hung on the posters was lower. It is possible that the high degree of integrity for the reinforcement component may have been inflated due to lunch monitor reactivity to the presence of the RA. However, anecdotal comments from teachers and administrators regarding the frequency that stickers were given out, the high rate with which the sticker supply needed to be refilled, and student responses on the questionnaire that they needed to eat a fruit or vegetable at lunch to receive a sticker all provided further evidence that stickers were given out at a high rate contingent on F&V consumption. The lower rates of fidelity for the posters were primarily due to difficulties with the coaches implementing this component of the program. These difficulties were resolved by having the lunch monitors take over the implementation of this component at one school. Notably, when this modification was made, fidelity was nearly perfect.
Blanchette and Brug (2005) found three variables most strongly related to F&V consumption: availability, accessibility, and taste preference. Experimental studies have indicated that when children are given repeated opportunities to taste unfamiliar foods, increases in preference and consumption occur (Cooke, 2007). In this study F&V consumption during school lunch increased without concurrent increases in F&V preferences. Consistent with prior studies (Horne et al., 2004; Perry et al., 2004), the encouragement to try F&V by lunch aides may have contributed to increased intake. Increased F&V consumption in the absence of increased preferences can be explained by the fact that almost all of the behavior change was related to increased fruit consumption and children reported very high levels of fruit preference at all three time points, making it difficult to increase fruit preferences further. The program did not directly target increasing students’ preferences for vegetables. Future research should focus on addressing barriers to student vegetable consumption in the school lunch (e.g., palatability of the foods served). A thorough understanding of students’ perceptions of barriers interfering with vegetable consumption should be sought and incorporated into the design of future programs.
Importantly, although our program incorporated reinforcement contingent upon student eating behavior, we were able to avoid overjustification effects (Lepper et al., 1973). Students’ reports of F&V preferences remained highly stable across the 15-month period, indicating that the program did not adversely impact or increase their F&V preferences. Also, students reported high levels of program acceptability, suggesting that they were still motivated by the program and satiation effects did not occur.
Study Limitations and Strengths
Limitations included the lack of longer term follow-up data, no attention control group, some attrition, moderate participation rates, and the potential for inflated acceptability reports due to social desirability. However, the latter concern was minimized by having teachers and lunch monitors complete the questionnaires anonymously and return them by mail. In this study direct measurement of consumption only occurred during school lunch. However, recently published data indicated that for a sub-set of students in this study whose caregivers were able to be reached for three phone interviews, caregivers of children in the experimental group reported that their children ate more F&V than caregivers of children in the control group (Blom-Hoffman et al., 2008). Another limitation was related to the participation rate, which in this study was slightly lower than the average participation rates found in school-based intervention and prevention studies that use active parental consent and reported participation rates (Blom-Hoffman et al., 2009). Finally, we do not know if the intervention strategies used in this study are relevant for older elementary school students.
These limitations are off-set by several strengths. This study involved a theory-based, cost effective method to reinforce a racially and ethnically diverse group of young children for their F&V consumption over 15 months. This is one of the only school-based F&V promotion studies that included children as young as kindergarten. School staff served as interventionists and the reinforcement system was simple, acceptable, and confined to the cafeteria setting. The longitudinal design allowed us to assess program decrement over time. The study included a precise, direct assessment of student consumption using weighed plate waste and our statistical modeling controlled for multiple confounders in the analyses. The study also included a careful process evaluation that assessed program implementation and acceptability.
Funding
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development [K23HD047480].
Conflict of interest: None declared.
Acknowledgements
The content of this article is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development and the National Institutes of Health. The authors are deeply appreciative of the children and school staff who participated in the development, implementation and evaluation of the fruit and vegetable promotion program. Finally, the authors are indebted to the support of the Center for Study of Sport in Society at Northeastern University, Sports4Kids, and the graduate student research assistants who helped make this study possible.
References
- Allison P. Missing data. Thousand Oaks, California: Sage; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Auld GW, Romaniello C, Heimendinger J, Hambidge C, Hambidge M. Outcomes from a school-based nutrition education program using resource teachers and cross-disciplinary models. Journal of Nutrition Education. 1998;30:268–280. [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall; 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Beunckens C, Molenberghs G, Kenward MG. Direct likelihood analysis versus simple forms of imputation for missing data in randomized clinical trials. Clinical Trials. 2005;2:379–86. doi: 10.1191/1740774505cn119oa. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birch LL, Marlin DW, Rotter J. Eating as the “means” activity in a contingency: Effects on young children's food preference. Child Development. 1984;55:431–439. [Google Scholar]
- Birch LL, Sullivan SA. Measuring children's food preferences. The Journal of School Health. 1991;61:212–214. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.1991.tb06015.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blanchette L, Brug J. Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among 6-12-year-old children and effective interventions to increase consumption. Journal of Human Nutrition and Dietetics. 2005;18:431–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-277X.2005.00648.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blom-Hoffman J. School-based promotion of fruit and vegetable consumption in multi-culturally diverse, urban schools. Psychology in the Schools. 2008;45:16–27. doi: 10.1002/pits.20275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blom-Hoffman J, Dai J, Franko DL. 2004. Assessing young children's fruit and vegetable preferences: Reliability and validity of the adapted fruit and vegetable preference questionnaire. Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Blom-Hoffman J, Leff SS, Franko DL, Weinstein E, Beakley K, Power TJ. Using a multi-component, partnership-based approach to improve participation rates in school-based research. School Mental Health. 2009;1:3–15. doi: 10.1007/s12310-008-9000-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blom-Hoffman J, Wilcox KA, Dunn L, Leff SS, Power TJ. Family involvement in school-based health promotion: Bringing nutrition information home. School Psychology Review. 2008;37:567–577. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown H, Prescott R. Applied mixed models in medicine. New York: Wiley; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Bryk AS, Raudenbush SW. Hierarchical linear models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Code of Federal Regulations. National School Lunch Program regulations. 2007 7 CFR Part 210.10. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd. Hilldale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Cooke L. The importance of exposure for healthy eating in childhood: A review. Journal of Human Nutrition and Dietetics. 2007;20:294–301. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-277X.2007.00804.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Domel SB, Baranowski T, Davis H, Leonard SB, Riley P, Baranowski J. Measuring fruit and vegetable preferences among 4th- and 5th-grade students. Preventive Medicine. 1993;22:866–879. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1993.1078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dole Food Company. 5 a day adventures [Computer Software] Thousand Oaks, California: Author; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Dole Food Company. 5 a day kids cookbook. Thousand Oaks, California: Author; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Eldridge AL, Smith-Warner SA, Lytle LA, Murray DM. Comparison of 3 methods for counting fruits and vegetables for fourth-grade students in the Minnesota 5 a day power plus program. Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 1998;98:777–782. doi: 10.1016/S0002-8223(98)00175-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hendy HM, Williams KE, Camise TS. “Kids Choice” school lunch program increases children's fruit and vegetable acceptance. Appetite. 2005;45:250–263. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2005.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horne PJ, Tapper K, Lowe CF, Hardman CA, Jackson MC, Woolner J. Increasing children's fruit and vegetable consumption: A peer modeling and rewards-based intervention. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2004;58:1649–1660. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Institute of Medicine. Fact Sheet: Schools can play a role in preventing childhood obesity. 2004. Sep, Retrieved January 15, 2009 from http://www.iom.edu/Object.File/Master/22/615/Fact%20Sheet%20-%20Schools%20FINALBitticks.pdf.
- Institute of Medicine. Preventing childhood obesity: Health in the balance. Washington DC: The National Academies Press; 2005. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Institute of Medicine. Nutrition standards for foods in schools: Leading the way toward healthier youth. Washington DC: The National Academies Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Kelder SH, Perry CL, Klepp K-I, Lytle LL. Longitudinal tracking of adolescent smoking, physical activity, and food choice behaviors. American Journal of Public Health. 1994;84:1121–1126. doi: 10.2105/ajph.84.7.1121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knai C, Pomerleau J, Lock K, McKee M. Getting children to eat more fruit and vegetables: A systematic review. Preventive Medicine. 2006;42:85–95. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2005.11.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lepper MR, Greene D, Nisbett RE. Undermining children's intrinsic interest with extrinsic reward: A test of the “overjustification hypothesis”. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1973;28:129–137. [Google Scholar]
- Lohman TG, Roche AR, Martorell R. Anthropometric standardization reference manual. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Ogden CL, Kuzmarski RJ, Flegal KM, Mei Z, Guo S, Wei R, et al. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention 2000 growth charts for the United States: Improvements to the 1977 National Center for Health Statistics version. Pediatrics. 2002;109:45–60. doi: 10.1542/peds.109.1.45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry CL, Bishop DB, Taylor GL, Davis M, Story M, Gray C, et al. A randomized school trial of environmental strategies to encourage fruit and vegetable consumption among children. Health Education and Behavior. 2004;31:65–76. doi: 10.1177/1090198103255530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubin DB. Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys. New York: Wiley; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Striegel-Moore RH, Thompson DR, Affenito SG, Franko DL, Barton BA, Schreiber, et al. Fruit and vegetable intake: Few adolescent girls meet national guidelines: The National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (NHLBI) Growth and Health Study. Preventive Medicine. 2006;42:223–228. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2005.11.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tang L, Song J, Belin TR, Unutzer J. A comparison of imputation methods in a longitudinal randomized clinical trial. Statistics in Medicine. 2005;24:2111–2128. doi: 10.1002/sim.2099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Department of Health and Human Services and U.S. Department of Agriculture. Dietary guidelines for Americans. 6th. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Verbeke G, Molenberghs G. Linear mixed models for longitudinal data. New York: Springer-Verlag; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Witt J, Elliott SN. Acceptability of classroom management strategies. In: Kratochwill T, editor. Advances in school psychology. Vol. 4. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1985. pp. 251–288. [Google Scholar]