Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Jul 15.
Published in final edited form as: Neuron. 2010 Jul 15;67(1):156–170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.025

A Parcellation Scheme for Human Left Lateral Parietal Cortex

Steven M Nelson 1,*, Alexander L Cohen 1, Jonathan D Power 1, Gagan S Wig 1, Francis M Miezin 1,2, Mark E Wheeler 3,4, Katerina Velanova 4,5, David I Donaldson 6, Jeffrey S Phillips 3,4, Bradley L Schlaggar 1,2,7,8, Steven E Petersen 1,2,7,9
PMCID: PMC2913443  NIHMSID: NIHMS211738  PMID: 20624599

SUMMARY

The parietal lobe has long been viewed as a collection of architectonic and functional subdivisions. Though much parietal research has focused on mechanisms of visuospatial attention and control-related processes, more recent functional neuroimaging studies of memory retrieval have reported greater activity in left lateral parietal cortex (LLPC) when items are correctly identified as previously studied (“old”) vs. unstudied (“new”). These studies have suggested functional divisions within LLPC that may provide distinct contributions towards recognition memory judgments. Here, we define regions within LLPC by developing a novel parcellation scheme that integrates data from resting state functional connectivity MRI (rsfcMRI) and functional MRI (fMRI). This combined approach results in a six-fold parcellation of LLPC based on the presence (or absence) of memory retrieval-related activity, dissociations in the profile of task-evoked timecourses, and membership in large-scale brain networks. This parcellation should serve as a roadmap for future investigations aimed at understanding LLPC function.

Keywords: parietal, memory, functional connectivity, fMRI, fcMRI, parcellation, module, angular gyrus, intraparietal sulcus, inferior parietal lobule

INTRODUCTION

In humans, parietal cortex has traditionally been linked to processing mechanisms involving attention (Corbetta et al., 1998; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Dosenbach et al., 2007; Dosenbach et al., 2006; Rushworth et al., 2001; Yantis et al., 2002). Other accounts of parietal cortex function, particularly focused on the left hemisphere, have examined its role in reading (Turkeltaub et al., 2002), as well as numerosity judgments and arithmetic (Göbel and Rushworth, 2004; Hubbard et al., 2005). More recently, there has been a surge in research devoted to understanding the contributions of left lateral parietal cortex (LLPC) to memory retrieval (for review, see Wagner et al., 2005). The multitude of processing descriptions arising from studies in these domains suggests that distinct regions in parietal cortex might subserve unique functional contributions. As Devlin and Poldrack (2007) have argued, the success of functional neuroimaging is contingent on the ability to accurately and precisely map function to underlying neuroanatomy. The primary goal of the current study is to better define divisions that exist within LLPC. As a means of identifying potential distinctions, we use a combined resting state functional connectivity MRI (rs-fcMRI) and functional MRI (fMRI) approach. The fMRI data includes studies related to memory retrieval, which will be used to leverage the distinctions seen with rs-fcMRI-based analyses and to better understand how specific LLPC regions contribute to this domain.

Determining the functional-neuroanatomical correlates of memory retrieval has driven a considerable amount of work in cognitive neuroscience. In particular, a great deal of research has been aimed at understanding how humans distinguish between previously experienced information (“old”) and that which is novel (“new”) (Henson et al., 2000; Konishi et al., 2000; McDermott et al., 2000; Wheeler and Buckner, 2003). Using fMRI, researchers have classified a set of regions in the brain that tend to be more active when human subjects correctly identify an item as old (“hit”) than when a given item is correctly identified as being new (“correct rejection”). This difference in activation has come to be called the “retrieval success effect” and is present in a highly distributed set of brain regions. The most common regions showing retrieval success effects are in lateral parietal cortex (Simons, 2008), and though this differential activation is typically bilateral, the most robust effects include a large expanse of LLPC (McDermott, 2009).

A number of laboratories (Cabeza et al., 2008; Ciaramelli et al., 2008; Vilberg and Rugg, 2008) have begun exploring the role of LLPC contributions to memory retrieval by performing meta-analyses in which data from large numbers of studies involving a variety of different retrieval tasks are analyzed in a common stereotactic space. The anatomical location of foci within LLPC can be differentiated on the basis of responses in paradigms contrasting recollection and familiarity, or source and item memory, as well as a number of other tasks in which old and new information is embedded. A consistent finding across these studies is the presence of a dorsal-ventral distinction in LLPC that appears to dissociate regions near intraparietal sulcus (IPS) involved in familiarity judgments (dorsal) and regions near angular gyrus involved in recollection (ventral) (Henson et al., 1999; Wheeler and Buckner, 2004).

Mapping estimates of recollection and familiarity onto distinct regions of LLPC represents one way in which the functional neuroanatomy underlying separate types of processing can be distinguished. However, even within the domain of memory retrieval, this distinction does not represent an extensive view of possible parcellations of LLPC. Further, a failure to incorporate “non-retrieval” related accounts of parietal mechanisms could result in an incomplete description of boundaries that exist within LLPC. A deeper understanding of the processing roles of LLPC is likely to be bolstered by the ability to apply objective parcellation schemes using a variety of analysis techniques.

One such technique, rs-fcMRI boundary mapping, is based on the observation that rs-fcMRI can dissociate regions within the cortex using edge detection algorithms (Cohen et al., 2008). This procedure uses a seed-based approach (Biswal et al., 1995; Fox et al., 2005) to identify regions whose rs-fcMRI-derived timecourse is significantly correlated with the timecourse of the seed location. The result is a whole-brain correlation map that indicates the degree of correlation between the seed and all other voxels in the brain. Specifically, the technique developed in Cohen et al. (2008) detects transitions between whole-brain correlation maps of nearby cortical seeds, and translates these abrupt transitions into boundaries between cortical regions. A key concept underlying this approach that will be underscored by the present report is that global brain relationships can facilitate the parcellation of anatomically adjacent pieces of cortex.

The current study begins with such rs-fcMRI boundary mapping to identify “correlationally” distinct regions in LLPC. These regions then serve as a launching point from which to probe a set of recognition memory fMRI studies for distinctions within LLPC. We find that LLPC regions divide along anatomical lines into an anterior group that does not display retrieval success effects, and a posterior group that does. These findings are augmented with large-scale network analysis of rs-fcMRI signal correlations between LLPC regions and regions located outside of LLPC using tools from graph theory. This analysis confirms the anterior/posterior distinction and divides the retrieval success regions into four groups embedded in distinct whole-brain rs-fcMRI networks. This final LLPC parcellation scheme is then corroborated by demonstrating that within the distinct whole-brain networks, the task-evoked signals shown by LLPC regions are shared by distinct sets of regions outside of LLPC.

RESULTS

Description of analysis stream

The results presented here can be viewed as the application of multiple analyses to two distinct imaging methodologies (fMRI and rs-fcMRI) both locally (within LLPC) and for distributed sets of regions (large-scale cortical networks) (Figure 1).

Figure 1. An outline of the analysis stream.

Figure 1

The analyses presented in the results comprise four sections represented in this 2×2 diagram.

Left Lateral Parietal Cortex: rs-fcMRI

Fifteen regions of interest were defined within a grid applied to LLPC

In order to parcellate LLPC and investigate its functional properties, an understanding of the local topography and heterogeneity is a critical starting point. The discovery that boundaries can be defined on the basis of abrupt changes in whole-brain rs-fcMRI maps (Cohen et al., 2008) provided a method (rs-fcMRI boundary mapping) by which to place regions of interest (ROI) in locations where rs-fcMRI maps are relatively stable and interrogate their function.

For the purposes of this experiment, a 27×27 grid of small spherical foci (6mm diameter) was generated over the extent of LLPC (Figure 2A) using Caret software (Van Essen et al., 2001). The grid extended outside of traditional bounds of parietal cortex to decrease the chance that any functional borders near the anatomical boundaries of LLPC would go undetected.

Figure 2. rs-fcMRI data were used to generate probabilistic boundary maps for the purpose of defining regions in LLPC.

Figure 2

(A) A square patch of 729 spherical foci (6mm diameter, 27 × 27 grid, spaced 6mm apart), was created using Caret software (Van Essen et al., 2001) and is shown here on an inflated cortical surface rendering. The surface is rotated to allow better visualization of LLPC.

(B) rs-fcMRI boundary map indicating the likelihood of a border at each seed. ‘Cooler’ colors represent stable rs-fcMRI patterns, whereas ‘hotter’ colors represent high border likelihood, i.e., rapidly changing rs-fcMRI patterns.

(C) Inverted rs-fcMRI boundary map demonstrating peaks of stability from the previous map. Centers are shown as dark gray spheres (10mm diameter) on the inflated surface. The blue circle indicates regions of interest (ROI) located within LLPC.

(D) Unprojected data from previous panel (C) allowing better visualization of borders. Gray dots represent ROIs with those circled in blue indicating regions located within LLPC. For orientation purposes, the grid contains anatomical labels that roughly correspond to these locations on the cortical surface. SMG = supramarginal gyrus, SPL = superior parietal lobule, IPS = intraparietal sulcus.

The resulting rs-fcMRI boundary map that depicts the boundary likelihood at any given focus in the patch is shown in Figure 2B. ‘Hot’ and ‘cool’ colors indicate high and low probabilities, respectively, of the existence of a boundary.

The apparent centers of the bounded regions in LLPC were obtained by inverting the map such that ‘hot’ colors now indicated rs-fcMRI map consistency between nearby seeds (Figure 2C,D). ROIs were defined as 10mm diameter spheres at peak locations using 2-D peak-finding algorithms. This resulted in 25 ROIs across the grid. Ten of the defined ROIs were outside of parietal cortex and were excluded from further analyses, leaving 15 LLPC ROIs as the targets of additional investigation.

Left Lateral Parietal Cortex: fMRI

Regions located more posteriorly in LLPC showed retrieval success effects

We next applied these LLPC ROIs to a number of studies that contained a contrast of “old” vs. “new” items and performed a meta-analysis (Table 1). Only the 7 more posterior ROIs showed consistent retrieval success effects (Figure 3A, green circles), defining a strong functional boundary between region sets.

Table 1.

Experimental details for tasks included in the analysis.

Study/Publication Task Condition Input Modality Output Modality Subjects
Donaldson et al., Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2010 Source Memory (Target
Correct (Hit) vs. CR)
Words Button 26
Phillips et al., Neuroimage, 2009 Old/New (Hit vs. CR) Words Button 12
Velanova et al., Journal of Neuroscience, 2003 Old/New (1× Hit vs. 1× CR)
Old/New (20× Hit vs. 20× CR)
Words
Words
Button
Button
29
Wheeler & Buckner, Journal of Neuroscience, 2003 Source Memory ((1×
Hit + 20× Hit) vs. CR)
Pictures/Sounds/
Words
Button 29
Wheeler & Buckner, Neuroimage, 2004 (Remember (Hit) + Know (Hit) vs.
CR
Pictures/Words Button 20
Unpublished data* Old/New (Hit vs. CR)
Old/New (Hit vs. CR)
Words
Words
Button
Button
24

Additional details can be found in the published manuscripts listed in the table.

*

Unpublished data from two tasks wherein subjects are asked to respond “old” or “new” to words that they had either seen once or had not seen at all (standard old/new paradigm).

Figure 3. Regions showing retrieval success effects are located in posterior parietal cortex.

Figure 3

(A) ROIs circled in green indicate those that showed retrieval success effects across the eight studies that comprised the meta-analysis, while ROIs circled in red did not. The thick black line indicates this distinction. ROIs are displayed on inflated cortical surface renderings of the human brain using Caret software.

(B), (C), (D) Timecourses from regions showing retrieval success effects. posterior middle IPS (pmIPS), posterior IPL (pIPL), and angular gyrus (AG) timecourses correspond to B,C, and D as labeled in (A). P-values indicate level of significance for hit > correct rejection.

Previously, it was speculated that retrieval success regions may be divided through the examination of differential timecourse dynamics in adjacent regions and that this would be critical in determining an appropriate level of parcellation in LLPC (Wheeler and Buckner, 2004). With this in mind, we extracted timecourses from 3 LLPC ROIs near those investigated in Wheeler and Buckner (2004) located in posterior middle intraparietal sulcus (pmIPS), posterior intraparietal lobule (pIPL), and angular gyrus (AG) (Figure 3B–D) to determine whether there was a difference in their profiles. A region × time repeated measures ANOVA with 3 levels of region and 14 levels of time (7 timepoints each for hit and cr) showed a significant interaction effect (F(12,84) = 17.35, p < 0.001). Post-hoc analyses revealed a significant region × time interaction for all three pairwise comparisons of timecourses including pmIPS vs. pIPL (F(12,84) = 13.13, p < 0.001), pmIPS vs. AG (F(12,84) = 28.43, p < 0.001), and pIPL vs. AG (F(12,84) = 6.99, p < 0.001).

Although there is a clear statistical dissociation between IPS and AG timecourses (Figures 3B and 3D), the timecourse in pIPL (Figure 3C) does resemble an average of IPS and AG timecourses. Thus, while the timecourse in pIPL may reflect a truly distinct signal, the possibility exists that this is simply an artifact of the spatial averaging of the signals from pmIPS and AG. The close proximity of these regions poses problems as we attempt to draw functional distinctions. The question of how to resolve this ambiguity motivated, in part, the remaining analyses (Table 2).

Table 2.

Response × time repeated measures ANOVA performed on 15 LLPC ROIs.

LLPC Region X Y Z F-statistic (6,42) P-value Effect
Posterior Intraparietal Sulcus (IPS) −24 −67 48 4.18 0.002 Hit > CR
Posterior Middle IPS −32 −62 48 9.22 < 0.001 Hit > CR
Anterior Middle IPS −35 −53 49 12.93 < 0.001 Hit > CR
Anterior IPS −39 −44 44 4.86 0.001 Hit > CR
Anterior Inferior Parietal Lobule (IPL) −54 −47 48 3.09 0.01 Hit > CR
Angular Gyrus (AG) −45 −67 36 10.97 < 0.001 Hit > CR
Posterior IPL −40 −62 48 8.97 < 0.001 Hit > CR
Supramarginal Gyrus (SMG) −56 −42 31 1.49 0.21 N.S.
Anterior SMG −63 −29 26 1.11 0.37 N.S.
Anterior IPL −60 −40 44 1.35 0.26 N.S.
Lateral Anterior IPS −55 −28 38 0.87 0.52 N.S.
Anterior IPS −40 −37 47 1.64 0.16 N.S.
Dorsal Anterior IPS −36 −41 54 2.98 0.02 CR > Hit
Superior Parietal Lobule (SPL) −29 −44 65 5.53 < 0.001 CR > Hit
Dorsal SPL −24 −58 62 0.29 0.94 N.S.

Significant statistical effects (p < 0.05) are shown in bold text. X,Y,Z values correspond to stereotactic coordinates in MNI space.

Parietal-Cortical Networks: rs-fcMRI

Since the initial definition of the LLPC regions was driven by differences in whole-brain rs-fcMRI relationships, one avenue towards addressing this ambiguity is to interrogate the whole-brain relationships of specific regions of LLPC. In other words, which regions outside of LLPC are most strongly functionally connected to each of the LLPC regions (the region’s “neighborhood”), and do regions in different neighborhoods show distinct functional timecourses? If an LLPC region, such as pIPL, possesses a functional (fMRI), task-evoked timecourse reflecting some functional process, do other regions in its neighborhood share similar functional timecourses? The following sections aim to provide answers to these questions through the exploration of relationships of LLPC regions to regions elsewhere in the brain. The next step is to define sets of regions that are related to LLPC regions using rs-fcMRI data.

“Neighborhood” generation identified regions outside of LLPC related to the LLPC ROIs

Although rs-fcMRI boundary mapping and subsequent peak-finding algorithms can separate adjacent cortex into distinct regions based on underlying differences in rs-fcMRI correlation maps, they do not reveal what the actual underlying differences are that drive these spatial distinctions. To explore these differences, we generated rs-fcMRI “neighborhoods”, defined as the sets of regions most highly correlated with each of the 15 LLPC ROIs.

Figure 4A shows an example neighborhood derived from a seed location in left AG, one of the LLPC ROIs examined in the previous analysis (see Figure 3D). For a given seed location, the 15 most correlated regions were included to ensure a manageable aggregate set from all LLPC ROIs (see Methods section on rs-fcMRI “Neighborhood” generation). It is important to note that since boundaries indicate abrupt change in whole-brain rs-fcMRI maps, adjacent regions will likely contain different, but potentially overlapping, sets of neighbors.

Figure 4. Generation of neighborhoods for each location within LLPC yielded 87 additional regions of interest (ROI).

Figure 4

(A) An example neighborhood (yellow spheres) derived from a seed location (gray sphere) in left angular gyrus (AG) (one neighbor in right cerebellum not shown).

(B) Left hemisphere view of each seed’s z-score normalized correlation map. Seed ROIs are indicated by a gray sphere and neighbors for each seed are indicated by yellow spheres.

(C) Aggregate neighbor ROIs from all fifteen neighborhoods. Overlapping regions derived from multiple neighborhoods were combined by averaging in stereotactic space. Gray circle indicates the original 15 LLPC ROIs.

Figure 4B depicts the seed maps for all 15 LLPC ROIs and a portion of the identified neighbors for each ROI on the left lateral surface of the cortex. Neighbors that appeared in more than one seed map were consolidated to eliminate overlap (see Methods), resulting in 87 final neighbors that spanned the cortex and cerebellum (Figure 4C, cerebellum not shown). The 87 neighbors and 15 LLPC ROIs formed a collection of 102 ROIs, which could then be viewed as a network of 102 nodes related to each other by rs-fcMRI correlations.

Modularity optimization identified four modules within the 102 ROIs

The next set of analyses was aimed at understanding this network, and in particular, whether distinct groupings or “modules” existed within the network, which might provide further distinctions between the LLPC ROIs. Tools from graph theory have become instrumental in determining structure within large-scale brain networks (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009; Sporns and Zwi, 2004). Networks are collections of items (nodes) possessing pairwise relations (edges) between each item, and community-detection analyses can decompose networks into functionally related subsets of nodes called “communities”, or “modules”. For example, a person's social network might include a module of coworkers, a module of relatives, and a module of teammates, each of which is richly connected internally, but possesses few connections to other modules. To assess the underlying grouping of our LLPC ROIs and their neighbors, community detection analysis using modularity optimization (Newman, 2006) was performed on the matrix of pairwise rs-fcMRI correlations between the 102 ROIs.

Modularity optimization separated the ROIs derived from the neighborhood analysis into four distinct sets of regions, or modules (Figure 5, Figure S1). The supramarginal gyrus (SMG) module (blue, Figure 5A,B, Table 3) included regions in bilateral SMG, anterior and posterior insula, frontal operculum, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), and supplementary motor area (SMA). The superior parietal lobule (SPL) module (orange, Figure 5A,B, Table 3) contained regions in bilateral SPL, putative human “frontal eye fields” (FEF), anterior IPS, and postcentral gyrus. The IPS module (brown, Figure 5A,B, Table 4) included regions in bilateral IPS, dorsal frontal cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), anterior PFC (aPFC), and lateral temporal cortex. The AG module (light purple, Figure 5A,B, Table 4) consisted of regions in bilateral AG, posterior cingulate, medial PFC (mPFC), superior frontal gyrus (FG), ventrolateral PFC, anterior lateral temporal cortex, posterior IPL, and right cerebellum.

Figure 5. Modularity optimization identified four modules within the neighborhoods generated from the fifteen seed locations.

Figure 5

(A) Modularity optimization assignments shown in LLPC. Lines drawn in LLPC delineate the module assignments (SMG, blue; SPL, orange; IPS, brown; AG, light purple). The thick yellow line indicating the functional distinction made based on the presence or absence of retrieval success effects (Figure 3) was replicated using modularity optimization.

(B) Modularity optimization assignments shown on lateral and medial views using Caret software. Colors are same as in (A).

(C) Modularity optimization assignments are represented by the color of each of the nodes (colors are same as in (A)). The placement of each node is determined by a spring-embedding algorithm that takes into account both the presence and strength of node-node connections (rs-fcMRI correlations). The graph represents the global relaxation of the connections at a threshold (r ≥ 0.10) into a low energy state (see also Figure S1).

Table 3.

Regions belonging to the SPL and SMG modules and their corresponding sub-module assignment

Region X Y Z Module Sub-Module
R Lateral Anterior IPS (1) 55 −20 42 SPL SPL/FEF
R FEF (2) 26 −4 55 SPL SPL/FEF
R SPL (3) 25 −51 64 SPL SPL/FEF
R Anterior IPS (4) 42 −33 51 SPL SPL/FEF
L FEF (5) −27 −7 55 SPL SPL/FEF
R FEF (6) 25 −15 69 SPL SPL/FEF
L FEF (7) −18 −10 63 SPL SPL/FEF
L SPL (8) −17 −47 68 SPL SPL/FEF
R SPL (9) 35 −37 62 SPL SPL/FEF
R SPL (10) 14 −46 60 SPL SPL/FEF
L Anterior IPS(11) −44 −25 56 SPL SPL/FEF
R Postcentral Gyrus (12) 39 −17 60 SPL SPL/FEF
R SPL (13) 18 −33 60 SPL SPL/FEF
L Medial Anterior IPS (14) −40 −37 47 SPL SPL/FEF
L Lateral Anterior IPS (15) 55 −28 38 SPL SPL/FEF
L SPL (16) −24 −58 62 SPL SPL/FEF
L SPL (17) −29 −44 65 SPL SPL/FEF
L Anterior IPS(18) −36 −41 54 SPL SPL/FEF
R SMA(19) 9 −11 52 SPL SMA
L SMA (20) −1 −1 52 SMG SMA
L Dorsal Anterior Cingulate (21) −5 8 37 SMG SMA
R SMA (22) 2 −6 64 SMG SMA
R SMA (23) 8 3 59 SMG SMA
R Dorsal Anterior Cingulate (24) 1 16 28 SMG SMA
R pre-SMA (25) 5 6 42 SMG SMA
L Anterior IPL (26) −60 −40 44 SMG SMG/Insula
L Inferior Frontal (27) −47 −1 13 SMG SMG/Insula
R Inferior Frontal (28) 39 −2 13 SMG SMG/Insula
R Middle Insula (29) 39 5 2 SMG SMG/Insula
R Inferior Frontal (30) 60 7 3 SMG SMG/Insula
L Anterior Insula (31) −31 17 5 SMG SMG/Insula
R Anterior Insula (32) 36 20 7 SMG SMG/Insula
R SMG (33) 65 −24 25 SMG SMG/Insula
L Middle Insula (34) −40 2 −1 SMG SMG/Insula
L Inferior Frontal (35) −56 6 5 SMG SMG/Insula
R Inferior Frontal (36) 51 0 8 SMG SMG/Insula
R Inferior Frontal (37) 58 16 11 SMG SMG/Insula
R Anterior PFC (38) 35 49 32 SMG SMG/Insula
R SMG (39) 68 −39 30 SMG SMG/Insula
L Posterior Insula (40) −36 −16 −4 SMG SMG/Insula
L Inferior Frontal (41) −56 −16 12 SMG SMG/Insula
L Inferior Frontal (42) −51 −22 22 SMG SMG/Insula
R Inferior Frontal (43) 45 12 −4 SMG SMG/Insula
R SMG (44) 53 −34 26 SMG SMG/Insula
L SMG (45) −56 −42 31 SMG SMG/Insula
L SMG (46) −63 −29 26 SMG SMG/Insula

X,Y,Z values correspond to stereotactic coordinates in MNI space. IPS = intraparietal sulcus, FEF = “frontal eye fields”, SPL = superior parietal lobule, SMA = supplementary motor area, SMG = supramarginal gyrus.

Table 4.

Regions belonging to the AG and IPS modules and their corresponding “sub-module” assignment.

Region X Y Z Module Sub-Module
L Posterior Cingulate (47) −1 −31 39 AG AG/mPFC
L Anterior Temporal (48) −59 −7 −17 AG AG/mPFC
R Posterior Cingulate (49) 1 −60 36 AG AG/mPFC
R Posterior Cingulate (50) 3 −47 35 AG AG/mPFC
R Superior Frontal (51) 19 35 50 AG AG/mPFC
R Angular Gyrus (52) 50 −60 34 AG AG/mPFC
L Posterior Cingulate (53) −7 −54 31 AG AG/mPFC
L Dorsomedial PFC (54) −3 50 24 AG AG/mPFC
R Anterior Temporal (55) 63 −7 −17 AG AG/mPFC
L Angular Gyrus (56) −45 −67 36 AG AG/mPFC
L Superior Frontal (57) −18 29 54 AG AG/mPFC
R Lateral Cerebellum (58) 35 −72 −32 AG Cerebellum
R Medial Cerebellum (59) 15 −84 −28 AG Cerebellum
R Lateral Cerebellum (60) 35 −65 −41 AG Cerebellum
L Anterior PFC (61) −32 51 1 AG pIPL/sFG
L Middle Temporal (62) −63 −39 −4 AG pIPL/sFG
R Middle Temporal (63) 71 −37 −6 AG pIPL/sFG
R Posterior IPL (64) 44 −56 41 AG pIPL/sFG
R Superior Frontal (65) 40 17 57 AG pIPL/sFG
R Posterior IPL (66) 44 −63 52 AG pIPL/sFG
L Dorsal Anterior Cingulate (67) −10 40 41 AG pIPL/sFG
L Superior Frontal (68) −31 17 55 AG pIPL/sFG
L Superior Frontal (69) −44 12 46 AG pIPL/sFG
L Posterior IPL (70) −40 −62 48 AG pIPL/sFG
L Ventrolateral PFC (71) −45 47 −11 AG alPL/aPFC
L Superior Frontal (72) −29 6 61 IPS pIPL/sFG
R Anterior PFC (73) 38 30 42 IPS alPL/aPFC
L Anterior I PL (74) −54 −47 48 IPS alPL/aPFC
R Anterior IPL (75) 56 −38 52 IPS alPL/aPFC
R Anterior PFC (76) 43 61 2 IPS alPL/aPFC
R Anterior PFC (77) 40 48 −2 IPS alPL/aPFC
R Ventrolateral PFC (78) 47 52 −13 IPS alPL/aPFC
L Anterior PFC (79) −44 51 4 IPS alPL/aPFC
L Dorsolateral PFC (80) −38 35 30 IPS LIPS/dlPFC
L Dorsal Frontal (81) −47 3 32 IPS LIPS/dlPFC
L Posterior Temporal (82) −52 −58 −6 IPS LIPS/dlPFC
L Dorsolateral PFC (83) −42 38 11 IPS LIPS/dlPFC
L Dorsolateral PFC (84) −45 29 25 IPS LIPS/dlPFC
L Dorsal Frontal (85) −39 15 25 IPS LIPS/dlPFC
L Anterior Middle IPS (86) −35 −53 49 IPS LIPS/dlPFC
L Posterior IPS (87) −24 −67 48 IPS LIPS/dlPFC
L Anterior IPS (88) −39 −44 44 IPS LIPS/dlPFC
L Posterior Middle IPS (89) −32 −62 48 IPS LIPS/dlPFC
R Medial IPS (90) 18 −64 59 IPS RIPS/dlPFC
R Posterior Temporal (91) 53 −56 −9 IPS RIPS/dlPFC
R Dorsolateral PFC (92) 47 42 19 IPS RIPS/dlPFC
R Dorsal Frontal (93) 47 9 31 IPS RIPS/dlPFC
R Middle IPS (94) 41 −43 45 IPS RIPS/dlPFC
R Precuneus (95) 9 −69 54 IPS RIPS/dlPFC
R Posterior IPS (96) 28 −68 50 IPS RIPS/dlPFC
R Posterior Middle IPS (97) 32 −51 48 IPS RIPS/dlPFC
R Dorsolateral PFC (98) 38 30 22 IPS RIPS/dlPFC
L Posterior Temporal (99) −48 −72 −2 IPS RIPS/dlPFC
L Superior Occipital (100) −26 −80 28 IPS Superior Occipital
R Superior Occipital (101) 47 −77 13 IPS Superior Occipital
R Superior Occipital (102) 38 −80 23 IPS Superior Occipital

X,Y,Z values correspond to stereotactic coordinates in MNI space. PFC = prefrontal cortex, IPL = inferior parietal lobule, IPS = intraparietal sulcus.

These assignments can be visualized using a spring-embedding algorithm in which the placement of each region or node in 2-D space (Figure 5C) reflects relative node-node connection strengths. Thus, there should be visual overlap between relationships defined by modularity and spring-embedding as both are dictated by correlations between the same set of regions, and this is the case here (Figure 5C).

The graph theory analyses revealed four modules, all of which contained regions both within LLPC and throughout the rest of the brain (Figure 5A,B). Strikingly, even though the modularity analysis contained no a priori information with regard to retrieval success effects, it still finds the same distinction within LLPC between regions in SPL and SMG and those located in AG and IPS. The dissociation found based on the presence or absence of a retrieval success effect across the LLPC ROIs (see Figure 3A) is replicated exactly and is extended in the modularity optimization analysis. This result is indicated by the thick yellow line in Figure 5A, separating anterior from posterior modules/regions in an identical manner.

Hierarchical structure in modules containing retrieval success effects revealed a further distinction in LLPC ROIs

We next conducted two modularity optimization analyses separately on retrieval success regions (the AG and IPS modules) and non-retrieval success regions (the SMG and SPL modules). This was done to determine whether there was further division among functionally similar regions that might have been overshadowed in a modularity optimization procedure containing functionally disparate regions.

Performing modularity optimization on the set of non-retrieval success regions did not result in any additional parietal distinctions from the SMG and SPL module assignments seen in the initial large-scale analysis. However, regions in SMA and dACC were placed into a small separate “sub-module” (dark green, Figure 6A,D, Figure S2, Table 3). Because the SMG module also contained regions along the extent of the bilateral insula, the final sub-module will be referred to as the SMG/Insula sub-module (blue, Figure 6A,C,D, Table 3). Additionally, because the final SPL sub-module also contained regions in putative human FEF, it will be referred to as the SPL/FEF sub-module (orange, Figure 6A,C,D, Table 3).

Figure 6. Modularity optimization performed separately on modules not showing retrieval success effects (SPL and SMG) and retrieval success modules (AG and IPS).

Figure 6

(A) The SPL and SMG modules did not split into separate sub-modules, though regions within the supplementary motor area (SMA) and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) separate from the two (SMA/dACC, dark green). The SPL module is now labeled SPL/FEF and the SMG module is now labeled SMG/Insula to more appropriately describe the distributed regions contained therein. Graph methods are as described in Figure 5 and text.

(B) The AG and IPS modules each split into multiple separate sub-modules, four of which (AG/mPFC (red), pIPL/sFG (light yellow), LIPS/dlPFC (light blue), aIPL/aPFC (light green)) contained regions within LLPC showing retrieval success effects. Regions in the right IPS and right dlPFC (RIPS/dlPFC, purple), cerebellum (light brown) and superior occipital cortex (SOC, teal) were also found to be distinct from the other regions. Parameters dictating the placement of nodes in network space are the same as in (A).

(C) Modularity optimization assignments shown in LLPC. Lines drawn in LLPC delineate sub-module assignments. Colors are same as in (A) and (B).

(D) Modularity optimization shown on lateral and medial views of the cortex using Caret software. Cerebellum (light brown in (B)) not shown. Colors are same as in (A) and (B) (see also Figure S2).

Modularity optimization of the LLPC retrieval success regions (AG and IPS modules) revealed distinctions not seen in the large-scale modularity analysis. The original IPS module was separated into four distinct sub-modules, two of which contained original LLPC ROIs. One of these sub-modules (light blue, here labeled LIPS/dlPFC) contained 4 LLPC ROIs that lined the IPS, as well as other regions in frontal and temporal cortex (Figure 6B-D, Figure S2, Table 3). The second sub-module (light green, here labeled aIPL/aPFC) contained an LLPC region in left aIPL, along with a region in right aIPL, and regions in bilateral aPFC (Figure 6B–D, Table 3). The “non-LLPC sub-modules” included a sub-module that contained regions in right IPS and right dlPFC (purple, RIPS/dlPFC; Figure 6B,D, Table 3), while a fourth small sub-module contained regions in superior occipital cortex (teal, Figure 6B,D, Table 3).

The original AG module was separated into three distinct sub-modules, two of which included original LLPC regions. An AG/mPFC sub-module (red, Figure 6B–D, Table 3) contained regions in medial frontal, temporal, and medial parietal cortex, while a pIPL/sFG sub-module (light yellow, Figure 6B–D, Table 3) included regions in superior frontal, anterior prefrontal, and lateral temporal cortex. A third sub-module that was not highly inter-connected with either the AG/mPFC or pIPL/sFG sub-modules consisted of three regions in right cerebellum (light brown, Figure 6B, Table 3).

Thus, as a result of modularity optimization on the AG and IPS modules, a total of four sub-modules emerged that included a region or regions within LLPC that exhibited retrieval success effects in the initial fMRI analysis. In total, six sub-modules were defined in LLPC. Importantly, the three LLPC regions that showed the apparently distinct timecourse profiles (see Figure 3) were also dissociated in the sub-module analysis.

Parietal-Cortical Networks: fMRI

The final analysis seeks, in part, to further corroborate whether the previously described intermediate timecourse in pIPL (see Figure 3C) is functionally distinct by examining the timecourses in regions within its sub-module. If they show a similar pattern to the region in pIPL and are distinct from the AG/mPFC and LIPS/dlPFC sub-modules, this would indicate that the pIPL timecourse is indeed not an artifact of spatial blurring in LLPC. More generally, this analysis asks to what degree the distinctions found using rs-fcMRI are reflected in task-evoked signals.

Regions outside of LLPC demonstrate similar retrieval success effects and timecourse dynamics

The characterization of sub-modules that consist of regions outside of, but closely related to, each of the LLPC ROIs now provides an opportunity to assess task-evoked signals at the sub-module level. Timecourses for hits and correct rejections were extracted from the regions comprising each of the four sub-modules that contained an LLPC ROI exhibiting retrieval success effects (see Table 2) across the eight tasks that defined the initial meta-analysis. It is important to note that the following analyses were performed on sub-module ROIs in which the original timecourses from LLPC ROIs were excluded. As such, any observed effects are necessarily independent of the initial fMRI analysis that only examined LLPC ROIs.

We first conducted a sub-module × time repeated measures ANOVA with 4 levels of sub-module and 14 levels of time (7 timepoints each for hit and cr) to determine whether the distinctions among the timecourse profiles in specific LLPC ROIs (Figure 7A–D, and see Figure 3) were also present at the sub-module level. The omnibus ANOVA yielded a significant effect of sub-module × time (F(18,174) = 13.27, p < 0.001) and post-hoc tests were performed on all pairwise combinations of sub-modules (Table 5). This yielded significant effects of sub-module × time for 5 of the 6 comparisons, with the exception being pIPL/sFG vs. aIPL/aPFC (Table 5). Thus, the timecourse profiles between the AG/mPFC, pIPL/sFG, and LIPS/dlPFC sub-modules are distinct from one another, mirroring both the apparent timecourse distinctions of Figure 3, and the modularity analysis that defined sub-modules (see Figure 6). Additionally, regions in the pIPL/sFG and aIPL/aPFC sub-modules were dissociable throughout all levels of the rs-fcMRI analyses (see Figure 5 and Figure 6); however, the lack of a sub-module × time interaction suggests that each may contain a similar processing mechanism by virtue of their timecourse profiles.

Figure 7. Four rs-fcMRI derived sub-modules show different task-evoked fMRI timecourse dynamics and retain retrieval success effects independent of the LLPC ROIs.

Figure 7

(A) A region in angular gyrus (AG) is shown on a lateral view of the left hemisphere using Caret software. Timecourses (below) were extracted for hits and correct rejections across the eight studies that comprised the meta-analysis. P-values represent the significance of the difference between hits and correct rejections determined by a response × time repeated measures ANOVA with 2 levels of response and 7 levels of time.

(B), (C), and (D) Same as in (A) but for pIPL (B), LIPS (C) and aIPL (D).

(E) All ROIs in the AG/mPFC sub-module excluding AG are shown on lateral and medial views of the cortex using Caret software. Timecourse data was extracted as in (A) but is averaged across all ROIs shown here. P-values are same as in (A).

(F), (G), (H) Same as in (E) but for pIPL/sFG (F), LIPS/dlPFC (G), and aIPL/aPFC (H).

Table 5.

Post-hoc statistical effects from the sub-module × time repeated measures ANOVA and response × time repeated measures ANOVA within each sub-module.

Sub-Modules (w/o LLPC) Sub-Module × Time
AG/mPFC vs. pIPL/sFG F(6,108) = 6.75, p < 0.001
AG/mPFC vs. alPL/aPFC F(6,90) = 6.21, p < 0.001
AG/mPFC vs. LIPS/dlPFC F(6,84) = 47.06, p < 0.001
pIPL/sFG vs. alPL/aPFC F(6,90) = 0.38, p = 0.89
pIPL/sFG vs. LIPS/dlPFC F(6,84) = 15.62, p < 0.001
alPL/aPFC vs. LIPS/dlPFC F(6,66) = 15.40, p < 0.001
Sub-Module (w/o LLPC) Response × Time
AG/mPFC F(6,54) = 12.50, p < 0.001
pIPL/sFG F(6,54) = 3.41, p < 0.01
alPL/aPFC F(6,36) = 4.85, p < 0.01
LIPS/dlPFC F(6,90) = 26.36, p < 0.001

We were also interested in determining which sub-modules, if any, showed retrieval success effects. Importantly, each of the four sub-modules showed a significant response×time interaction (2 levels of response and 7 levels of time) when averaging across regions outside of LLPC (Figure 7E–H, Table 5), such that hits were greater than correct rejections. Additionally, we examined whether or not response × time interactions were present in sub-modules whose LLPC ROIs did not originally show retrieval success effects (SPL/FEF and SMG/Insula). Neither sub-module exhibited an effect of response×time (data not shown), which again mirrors the effects seen in the related LLPC regions.

DISCUSSION

By applying graph theoretic tools and image analysis algorithms to a combination of functional and resting state data, we were able to tease apart details of cortical network architecture. The result of these analyses was a novel parcellation scheme for LLPC that was informed by distinct types of data, the convergence of which lends credence to the LLPC boundaries and networks defined here. We think that this work provides a new lens by which to view LLPC and should inform functional studies by providing an anatomical platform from which to map information processing within this large region of cortex.

Generally speaking, the rs-fcMRI boundary mapping approach has the ability to provide unique insight into the structure and function of locations spanning the entire cortex. In addition, it provides a set of ROIs that can be used to interrogate local functional distinctions and global network properties. The discussion that follows outlines the importance of the integrated analysis approach, as well as potential roles for the four “retrieval success” sub-modules defined in the graph theoretic analyses. In addition, other distinctions are outlined between our sub-modules and similar regions and networks that have been described in previous studies.

An integrated fMRI and rs-fcMRI approach

One of the themes in the work presented here is the correspondence between networks discovered from data collected while subjects lie in the scanner fixating a central crosshair (rs-fcMRI) and data collected while subjects are engaged in a cognitively demanding task. These types of data differ not only on the basis of task demands (or lack thereof), but also in the time scales they sample. rs-fcMRI analyses here reflect correlations in low frequency fluctuations of the BOLD response (0.009 - 0.08 Hz) over many minutes, whereas task-related fMRI analyses reflect transient activations or deactivations over several seconds.

Why would such a high degree of similarity exist in networks defined from these seemingly disparate forms of data and analyses? We have hypothesized (Dosenbach et al., 2008; Fair et al., 2007) the answer to be rooted in a Hebbian-like mechanism whereby regions that tend to coactivate across task settings reduce the overall threshold for propagation of signals between them via direct or indirect connections. Over time, the correlation between the ‘spontaneous’ activity in two regions will tend to reflect their statistical history of co-activation across the myriad everyday “tasks” performed by the individual.

The approach employed here provides a means of identifying regions whose relationship can be gleaned from rs-fcMRI data and interrogated further in a variety of task settings, the combination of which has the potential to yield a richer set of results than can be obtained by each method individually.

Regions in the LIPS/dlPFC sub-module have been implicated in processes related to familiarity judgments and attentional control

In the context of memory retrieval studies, regions in left IPS have been implicated in a number of processing roles including, most notably, familiarity judgments (Henson et al., 1999; Wheeler and Buckner, 2004; Yonelinas et al., 2005). Recent meta-analytic studies have revealed the presence of consistent familiarity-related activation along the lateral bank of the IPS (Cabeza et al., 2008; Vilberg and Rugg, 2008), at or near the location of the regions described in our experiments (see Figure 7C). In addition, other experiments have shown familiarity-related activation in left dlPFC and dorsal frontal cortex (Yonelinas et al., 2005).

Studies not explicitly testing memory retrieval have also noted activity in similar regions. For instance, a meta-analysis published by Dosenbach et al. (2006) highlighted the presence of consistent start-cue effects in left IPS. This effect, which occurs when subjects are alerted that trials within a task block are about to begin, appears to be domain-general and is also seen when subjects are cued in a trialwise manner to switch tasks or response contingencies (Bunge et al., 2002; Rushworth et al., 2001). Reliable start cue effects are also seen in regions at or near bilateral dlPFC and dorsal frontal cortex.

Furthermore, this same set of regions appears to show greater responses when subjects make an error than when they respond correctly to a given item (Dosenbach et al., 2006; Wheeler et al., 2008). This effect, as with start-cues, seems to occur largely independently of domain or task parameters. In addition to the task-evoked data, there is also evidence from functional connectivity data that regions in left IPS and dlPFC are part of a network (Dosenbach et al., 2007; Seeley et al., 2007) that supports flexible attentional control mechanisms.

Thus, activation of these regions during familiarity judgments may reflect control processes that are deployed when subjects are unable to determine with certainty whether an item is one they remember having encoded or not. In addition, reaction times for familiarity decisions tend to be greater than “remember” or “new” judgments (Wheeler and Buckner, 2004; Yonelinas et al., 2005), perhaps reflecting additional demand on attentional resources.

It is important to note that the attention-related effects described here may be distinct from shifts in visuospatial attention and orienting that are characteristic of regions in the dorsal attention system (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002), which we believe are captured in the SPL/FEF sub-module. Hutchinson et al. (2009) provide evidence along these lines using meta-analytic data to distinguish between regions at or near the LIPS/dlPFC sub-module that show memory-retrieval related effects and regions in the SPL that show consistent effects related to spatial attention in tasks not involving memory retrieval.

Regions in the AG/mPFC sub-module may play a role in re-instantiating context-specific perceptual information

The AG/mPFC sub-module consists of a number of regions that have been previously defined by task-induced deactivations (Shulman et al., 1997). More recently, rs-fcMRI analyses have isolated a similar set of regions as being strongly correlated with one another when a subject is not in any specific task setting (Fox et al., 2005; Greicius et al., 2003). These regions have been termed the “default mode network” and encompass those obtained in the current study.

From the standpoint of memory retrieval, many of these regions have been implicated in a wide variety of processing roles including retrieval of autobiographical events or imagined future events (Schacter et al., 2007; Szpunar et al., 2007), remember judgments in the context of tasks probing recollection/familiarity distinctions (Henson et al., 1999; Wheeler and Buckner, 2004), and high confidence recognition memory judgments (Yonelinas et al., 2005). In addition, a study from Kelley et al. (2002) has also highlighted a role for posterior cingulate and mPFC in making judgments related to trait adjectives that are self-relevant. These regions, similar to those highlighted in autobiographical retrieval studies (McDermott, 2009), seem to be important when stimulus information is related more specifically to one’s own past. Similarly, the presence of activation in angular gyrus and other “default” regions for remember judgments may reflect the recollection of specific details from the encoding event, eliciting a greater sense that this item is “from their past”. Thus, the mechanism by which these regions support retrieval-related processing may be related to information that is not just simply “old”, but has a more specific autobiographical signature.

Support for the importance of “my” past in understanding activation in these regions comes from a study by Szpunar et al. (2009), in which subjects were asked to either remember events from their past or imagine personal events in the future; critically, some of these future events were to be set in places well-known to the person, whereas others were to be set in unfamiliar settings. Activation in regions near angular gyrus and posterior cingulate was similar for conditions in which subjects remembered or imagined episodes in known contextual settings (e.g., one’s backyard), but was attenuated when the future event was projected to take place in a novel context (e.g., a safari). In short, activation in these regions was modulated not by whether a person was remembering an event or envisioning a novel event, but was instead modulated by whether the event was placed in a well-known contextual setting or a novel setting.

Regions in the aIPL/aPFC sub-module are consistent with those implicated in post-retrieval monitoring

Regions in left lateral aPFC have been shown to support the retrieval of source information (Dobbins et al., 2002) and controlled semantic retrieval (Bunge et al., 2005) during memory-based tasks but appear to show little or less activation during simple item memory judgments (Ranganath et al., 2000). Additionally, regions in right lateral aPFC have been most commonly attributed to source or post-retrieval monitoring (Dobbins et al., 2004). The claim that this signal occurs post-retrieval relies on the fact that information related to the temporal signature of this activity in aPFC can be extracted. While the nature of the hemodynamic response can make it difficult to determine much in the way of timing relationships, late onset of activity has been noted in these regions both in fMRI (Henson et al., 2000; Reynolds et al., 2006; Schacter et al., 1997) and during ERP studies (Rugg and Allan, 2000).

An examination of the literature on aPFC will often show that IPL is activated for the same contrast (Dobbins et al., 2003; Dobbins et al., 2004), even though IPL regions are not typically highlighted. Regarding the late onset of the response, a recent study by Phillips et al. (2009) shows the timecourse of activity for a region very near our left aIPL region (see Figure 7D) that exhibits an early onset deactivation and a positive-going response that appears to take place much later than in regions showing a more canonical pattern of activity. This pattern of activation is similar to the timecourses observed in a study by Reynolds et al. (2006) that also showed early onset deactivation with a very late positive onset, which was noted to likely be occurring post-response.

Our meta-analytic timecourse data are consistent with similar types of processing that may occur post-retrieval or post-decision. Our data also appear to show an early onset deactivation of the timecourse relative to baseline that looks similar to the initial phase of the response in regions in the AG/mPFC sub-module. This finding may be evidence that the activity in this region cannot be easily explained by a singular processing account. Further studies will need to be done in an attempt to tease out the implications of these timing dissociations to better understand their meaning in the context of memory retrieval and other task settings.

The pIPL/sFG sub-module may implement processes similar to those in the aIPL/aPFC sub-module

The sub-module analyses revealed a distinction between the pIPL/sFG sub-module and the AG/mPFC sub-module. There is little precedent in the literature for discussing processing distinctions in regions that may be interposed between AG and IPS, since typical regions of interest encompass much of one or the other. Based on the initial modularity optimization analysis (see Figure 5), the AG/mPFC and pIPL/sFG sub-modules appear closely linked as they comprised only one module. Interestingly, however, the timecourses derived from the task-evoked fMRI data in AG/mPFC appear to be most similar to those in the aIPL/aPFC sub-module (Figure 7, Table 3), with a negative response on CRs and little change from baseline on hits. One possibility is that the pIPL/sFG sub-module engages in similar post-decision processing mechanisms as the aIPL/aPFC sub-module by virtue of its similar timecourse profile, but preferentially communicates with regions in the AG/mPFC sub-module, as indicated by the rs-fcMRI analyses, to relay information related to the outcome of a trial.

Three distinctions exist within a previously defined unitary fronto-parietal network

A study from Vincent et al. (2008) used rs-fcMRI to identify a network anatomically located between regions in the superior parietal lobule and the angular gyrus. In the current study, we find further separation of this network, which they termed the fronto-parietal control system (FPCS). Anatomically, the regions in LLPC that correspond to left IPS (LIPS/dlPFC sub-module), aIPL (aIPL/aPFC sub-module), and pIPL (pIPL/sFG sub-module) all lie within the FPCS boundaries. Important additional evidence comes from the separation that we observe in bilateral frontal cortex. The extent of cortex corresponding to the FPCS in Vincent et al. (2008) contains regions that, in our hands, separate into the same distinctions we see in LLPC including the presence of separable members in superior frontal gyrus (pIPL/sFG sub-module), dorsal frontal cortex and dlPFC (LIPS/dlPFC sub-module), and aPFC (aIPL/aPFC sub-module).

Conclusion

Taken together, we found evidence that LLPC contains regions that are members of six sub-modules that span the cortex. This set of observations underscores the importance of interpreting data at a number of levels and with a number of different techniques. We think that this work reveals key organizational features of LLPC at both the region (local) and network (global) level that should inform future attempts to map function to structure.

EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES

Left Lateral Parietal Cortex: rs-fcMRI (Figure 2)

Subjects

Subjects were recruited from the Washington University community and were screened with a self-report questionnaire to ensure that they had no current or previous history of neurological or psychiatric diagnosis. Informed consent was obtained from all subjects, and the study was approved by the Washington University Human Studies Committee. rs-fcMRI data were collected on 28 subjects (14 female, age 21–29 years) who were instructed to relax while fixating a white cross on a black background. Three runs (2.5 s TR, 133 frames per run) were acquired on 21 subjects and 2 runs on the remaining 7 subjects.

Data acquisition

Images were acquired in adherence to a standard protocol. To help stabilize head position, each subject was fitted with a thermoplastic mask fastened to the head coil. All images were obtained with a Siemens MAGNETOM Tim Trio 3.0T Scanner (Erlangen, Germany) and a Siemens 12 channel Head Matrix Coil. A T1-weighted sagittal MPRAGE structural image was obtained (TE=3.08ms, TR(partition)=2.4 s, TI =1000ms, flip angle=8 degrees, 176 slices with 1×1×1mm voxels) (Mugler and Brookeman, 1990). A T2-weighted turbo spin echo structural image (TE=84ms, TR=6.8 s, 32 slices with 2×1×4mm voxels) in the same anatomical plane as the BOLD images was also obtained to improve alignment to an atlas. An auto align pulse sequence protocol provided in the Siemens software was used to align the acquisition slices of the functional scans parallel to the anterior commissure-posterior commissure (AC-PC) plane and centered on the brain. This plane is parallel to the slices in the Talairach atlas (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988) which is used for subsequent data analysis. Functional imaging was performed using a BOLD contrast sensitive gradient echo echo-planar sequence (TE =27 ms, flip angle=90°, in-plane resolution= 4×4mm). Whole brain EPI volumes (MR frames) of 32 contiguous, 4mm-thick axial slices are obtained every 2.5 seconds. The first four image acquisitions were discarded to allow net magnetization to reach steady state.

MR data preprocessing

Imaging data from each subject were pre-processed to remove noise and artifacts, including: a) correction for movement within and across runs using a rigid-body rotation and translation algorithm (Snyder, 1996), b) whole brain normalization to a common mode of 1000 to allow for comparisons across subjects (Ojemann et al., 1997), and c) temporal re-alignment using sinc interpolation of all slices to the temporal midpoint of the first slice, accounting for differences in the acquisition time of each individual slice. Functional data were then resampled into 3mm isotropic voxels and transformed into stereotaxic atlas space (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988). Atlas registration involved aligning each subject’s T1-weighted image to a custom atlas-transformed (Lancaster et al., 1995) target T1-weighted template (711-2B) using a series of affine transforms (Michelon et al., 2003).

rs-fcMRI preprocessing

Pre-processing for rs-fcMRI analyses was performed on the fMRI data as in Fox et al. (2005), to optimize the time-series data and remove spurious variance unlikely to reflect neuronal activity (e.g., heart rate and respiration). These steps included: 1) a temporal band-pass filter (0.009 Hz < f < 0.08 Hz) and spatial smoothing (6mm full width at half maximum), 2) regression of six parameters obtained by rigid body head motion correction, 3) regression of the whole brain signal (the average timecourse of the whole brain), 4) regression of ventricular signal extracted from ventricular ROIs, and 5) regression of white matter signal extracted from white matter ROIs. The first order derivative terms for the movement, whole brain, ventricular, and white matter signals were also included in the correlation preprocessing.

Surface-based analysis

In Cohen et al. (2008), seeds were defined directly on the individual subject’s flattened cortical representation. Here, the PALS atlas family of surfaces was used as a starting point to obtain common surface coordinates to use across our group of subjects (Van Essen, 2005). The PALS atlas “fiducial” surface represents the average of 12 individual gray mid-thickness surfaces each volumetrically registered to the 711-2B atlas. While the PALS fiducial surface does not represent the actual fiducial surface of any specific individual, it can be used to approximate average fiducial surface locations in group-averaged data in the same volumetric atlas space. Using Caret 5.3 software (Van Essen et al., 2001), a grid of seed points was generated on the spherical PALS surface over LLPC and the volumetric locations on the corresponding PALS fiducial surface in 711-2B atlas space were obtained.

rs-fcMRI boundary mapping

rs-fcMRI boundary mapping was then performed as in Cohen et al. (2008). For our purposes, the set of volumetric coordinates for a patch over LLPC was used to generate a set of 729 spherical ROIs (6mm diameter). For each seed, in each subject, volumetric correlation maps were generated as in Fox et al. (2005), i.e., correlating the timecourse of a region of interest with the timecourses of all other voxels over the entire volume of the brain. This method creates a volumetric correlation map for each seed. To generate maps of statistical significance, correlation maps were normalized using the Fisher r-to-z-transform, and then converted into statistical z-scores. A volumetric average correlation map was then created for the group of subjects for each seed region, producing a single set of correlation maps representing the whole group, which is then used for the remaining analysis.

Eta2 coefficients were calculated for all possible pairs of volumetric correlation maps, generating a 729 × 729 eta2 matrix where each column represents the similarities between a particular region’s volumetric correlation map and all other regions’ volumetric correlation maps. The full 729 × 729 eta2 matrix was then re-organized into a series of 729 2D ‘patch-shaped’ matrices such that each matrix represents the spatial organization of similarity between all the other seeds’ correlation maps to the current seed’s correlation map.

Edge detection algorithms

Since the matrix of eta2 coefficients (i.e., eta2 profile) is a 2D array of values across the cortical surface, it can also be treated as a flat image. To find salient boundaries in these arrays, the Canny edge detection algorithm (Canny, 1986), as implemented in the Image Processing Toolbox (v7.2) of the MATLAB software suite, was applied to each seed’s eta2 profile ‘image’.

The Canny method smoothes the image with a Gaussian filter to reduce noise, and then creates a gradient image that locates regions that retain high spatial derivatives. High gradient values represent locations where the similarity between rs-fcMRI correlation maps is rapidly changing (i.e., they are peaks in the first derivative). After eliminating pixels in the 2D array that are not local maxima in the gradient image, the algorithm tracks along the highlighted regions of the image and categorizes each location as a boundary or not. To prevent hysteresis, both a high and low threshold are used. If the gradient magnitude of the pixel is below the low threshold, it is set to zero. If the magnitude is above the high threshold, it is considered a boundary. If the magnitude of the pixel is between the two thresholds, then the location is only considered a boundary if a neighboring pixel has a gradient above the high threshold. The primary goal is to identify and differentiate locations with strong, spatially coherent peaks as being different from locations that are relatively smooth or have incoherent gradient peaks across many of the eta2 profiles. The result of processing the eta2 profile set with an edge detection algorithm is a set of binary images representing the locations of rapid changes in each grid point’s eta2 profile.

Since the boundary determination is binary, averaging across the entire set of binary edge maps generates a probabilistic boundary location map in which intensity represents how likely a location is to be a putative functional boundary.

LLPC ROI identification

Inverting the boundary location map provides a map of centroid locations, which can be used to generate regions using 2D local extrema algorithms (MATLAB v7.2, Image Processing Toolbox) as well as custom-written software that approximates established methods for detecting volumetric peaks of activation in task-evoked fMRI studies. The corresponding volumetric coordinates for each region identified are then used to generate spherical ROIs (10mm diameter) for further analyses as described below.

Left Lateral Parietal Cortex and Parietal-Cortical Networks: fMRI (Figure 3,Figure 7)

Functional MRI meta-analysis of studies contrasting hits vs. correct rejections

Studies included a total of 140 neurologically normal adults between the ages of 18–35 who were recruited from both the Washington University and University of Pittsburgh communities. Data were collected on either a 1.5T Siemens MAGNETOM Vision Scanner (Washington University in St. Louis) or a 3T Siemens Allegra Scanner (University of Pittsburgh). Studies included a variety of different tasks in which judgments about item status were embedded within various source attribution, remember/know or basic old/new decisions (Donaldson et al., 2010; Phillips et al., 2009; Velanova et al., 2003; Wheeler and Buckner, 2003, 2004) (Table 1). In addition, the encoding tasks contained either visual or auditory stimuli that were either presented once or many times to enhance retrieval success.

MR data preprocessing

The same MR data preprocessing steps as used for the rs-fcMRI data (see above) were used for the fMRI data.

Functional MRI data analysis using the general linear model

BOLD activity related to the trials was modeled using a general linear model (GLM) approach. Additionally, baseline and trend terms for each BOLD run were included in the GLM. In each study, correct responses to both old and new items were coded separately, as were other manipulations that were specific to each experiment. This approach is equivalent to estimating the finite impulse response evoked by each event and eschews assumptions about the form of the hemodynamic response function (Miezin et al., 2000; Ollinger et al., 2001; Zarahn et al., 1997).

Analysis of timecourses

rs-fcMRI derived-regions were applied to the functional data to identify those that showed reliable retrieval success activity. Spherical ROIs with a diameter of 10mm were created from the ‘peak’ foci and the mean timecourse from all eight tasks for each of the regions was obtained for both hits and correct rejections.

Parietal-Cortical Networks: rs-fcMRI (Figure 4,Figure 5,Figure 6)

rs-fcMRI “Neighborhood” generation

As a means of determining the way in which LLPC regions differ from one another, the rs-fcMRI “neighborhoods”, or the sets of regions that most strongly correlated with each identified region, were generated (See Figure 4B). A peak finding algorithm was employed to determine the 15 most significant peaks of positive correlation for each LLPC ROI. The number of “neighbors” (15) represents an attempt to strike a balance between defining too few and too many neighbors for a given ROI. Too few neighbors would fail to adequately characterize a network corresponding to a given seed and too many neighbors would lead to the analysis of neighbors that are weakly correlated. As a result of this criterion, the lowest z-score of the Fisher r-to-z transformed values that corresponded to the most minimally significant peak was not necessarily the same for each region. Nonetheless, all regions within each of the neighborhoods showed strongly significant effects (z-scoremin = 5.31). In order to exclude regions with a significant degree of overlap, regions closer together than 10mm were replaced by the average of their stereotactic coordinates.

Modularity optimization analysis

Networks with N nodes (regions) can be mathematically represented as a N×N matrix of relationships where cell ij contains the correlation coefficient of region i with region j. Our networks are formed from correlation matrices, which possess values from -1 to 1. These matrices may be thresholded, where cells below some value are set to zero. As thresholds rise, the density of edges in the network decreases, and at some point the network begins to fragment into disconnected components. The properties of graphs may change as edges are dropped from the network, and so it is important to explore a range of thresholds to determine whether network properties are dependent upon the threshold(s) being examined. An upper bound to this range is usually set by fragmentation considerations, and the lower bound is set to avoid negative and insignificant correlations.

Here, we report analyses from a (positive) range of thresholds over which significant modular structure was found (Q > 0.30), and over which most nodes (> 80%) could reach most other nodes, indicating that the network was minimally fragmented. Our module detection was performed with algorithms that optimize “modularity” (Fair et al., 2009; Newman, 2006; Newman and Girvan, 2004), a metric of modular structure in networks. Modularity (Q) measures, for a given partition of a network into modules, the number of connections found within modules compared to the number of connections within modules expected by chance given the number of nodes and connections in the network. Our algorithm maximizes this metric, and returns the node assignments into modules that yield the highest value. Typically, Q values over 0.30 are thought to indicate strong community structure (Fortunato, 2010; Newman, 2006).

Spring embedding using Social Network Image Animator (SoNIA)

Though the correlation matrix contains a complete description of the network, it is information-dense and difficult to inspect visually. To visualize the network structure, we used a standard technique called spring embedding. Graphs were created by using the Kamada-Kawai spring-embedding algorithm implemented in the Social Network Image Animator (SoNIA) software package (Bender-deMoll and McFarland, 2006).

Nodes were given a fixed repulsive force and placed randomly in a plane, and springs with force constants related to the pairwise correlation coefficients of the matrix were placed between all nodes. This spring system was allowed to iteratively reposition nodes in order to reduce the energy of the spring system, resulting in a final low-energy state, where nodes with high correlations are positioned near one another, and those with weak or no connections are placed more distantly.

Spring-embedding diagrams were combined with module assignments to reveal individual node-node interactions, and the relationships of modules to one another.

Supplementary Material

01

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank Kathleen McDermott, Kelly Barnes, Nico Dosenbach, Jessica Church, Alecia Vogel, Christopher Fetsch and Maital Neta for helpful discussion, Mark McAvoy and Avi Snyder for technical support, and Randy Buckner for generously providing data. Funding for this work came from NIH grants NS32979 (S.E.P), NS41255 (S.E.P), NS46424 (S.E.P.), NS062489 (A.L.C.), and NS53425 (B.L.S.).

Footnotes

This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

SUPPLEMENTAL DATA

Supplemental data include modularity optimization assignments from Figure 5 and Figure 6 across a broad range of correlation thresholds as well as plots of modularity optimization parameters.

REFERENCES

  1. Bender-deMoll S, McFarland DA. The art and science of dynamic network visualization. J Soc. Struct. 2006;7:2. [Google Scholar]
  2. Biswal B, Yetkin F, Haughton V, Hyde J. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn. Res. Med. 1995;34:537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bullmore E, Sporns O. Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009;10:186–198. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bunge SA, Hazeltine E, Scanlon MD, Rosen AC, Gabrieli JD. Dissociable contributions of prefrontal and parietal cortices to response selection. Neuroimage. 2000;17:1562–1571. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bunge SA, Wendelken C, Badre D, Wagner AD. Analogical reasoning and prefrontal cortex: evidence for separable retrieval and integration mechanisms. Cereb. Cortex. 2005;15:239–249. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Cabeza R, Ciaramelli E, Olson IR, Moscovitch M. The parietal cortex and episodic memory: an attentional account. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;9:613–625. doi: 10.1038/nrn2459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Canny J. A computational approach to edge detection. IEEE. Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell. 1986;PAMI-8:679–698. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Ciaramelli E, Grady C, Moscovitch M. Top-down and bottom-up attention to memory: A hypothesis (AtoM) on the role of the posterior parietal cortex in memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:1828–1851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.03.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cohen AL, Fair DA, Dosenbach NU, Miezin FM, Dierker D, Van Essen DC, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Defining functional areas in individual human brains using resting functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage. 2008;41:45–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Corbetta M, Akbudak E, Conturo TE, Snyder AZ, Ollinger JM, Drury HA, Linenweber MR, Petersen SE, Raichle ME, Van Essen DC, Shulman GL. A common network of functional areas for attention and eye movements. Neuron. 1998;21:761–773. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80593-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Corbetta M, Shulman G. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;3:201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Devlin JT, Poldrack RA. In praise of tedious anatomy. Neuroimage. 2007;37:1033–1041. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dobbins IG, Foley H, Schacter DL, Wagner AD. Executive control during episodic retrieval: multiple prefrontal processes subserve source memory. Neuron. 2002;35:989–996. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00858-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dobbins IG, Rice HJ, Wagner AD, Schacter DL. Memory orientation and success: separable neurocognitive components underlying episodic recognition. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:318–333. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00164-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dobbins IG, Simons JS, Schacter DL. fMRI evidence for separable and lateralized prefrontal memory monitoring processes. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2004;16:908–920. doi: 10.1162/0898929041502751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Donaldson DI, Wheeler ME, Petersen SE. Remember the source: dissociating frontal and parietal contributions to episodic memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2010;22:377–391. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dosenbach NUF, Fair DA, Cohen AL, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. A dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2008;12:99–105. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dosenbach NUF, Fair DA, Miezin FM, Cohen AL, Wenger KK, Dosenbach RAT, Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME, et al. Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 2007;104:11073–11078. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704320104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dosenbach NUF, Visscher KM, Palmer ED, Miezin FM, Wenger KK, Kang HC, Burgund ED, Grimes AL, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. A core system for the implementation of task sets. Neuron. 2006;50:799–812. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fair DA, Cohen AL, Power JD, Dosenbach NUF, Church JA, Miezin FM, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Functional brain networks develop from a "local to distributed" organization. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2009;5 doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fair DA, Dosenbach NUF, Church JA, Cohen AL, Brahmbhatt S, Miezin FM, Barch DM, Raichle ME, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL. Development of distinct control networks through segregation and integration. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 2007;104:13507–13512. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705843104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fortunato S. Community Detection in Graphs. Phys. Rep. 2010;486:75–174. [Google Scholar]
  23. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 2005;102:9673–9678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Göbel SM, Rushworth MF. Cognitive neuroscience: acting on numbers. Curr. Biol. 2004;14:R517–R519. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.06.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 2003;100:253–258. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0135058100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Henson RN, Rugg MD, Shallice T, Dolan RJ. Confidence in recognition memory for words: Dissociating right prefrontal roles in episodic retrieval. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2000;12:913–923. doi: 10.1162/08989290051137468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Henson RN, Rugg MD, Shallice T, Josephs O, Dolan RJ. Recollection and familiarity in recognition memory: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:3962–3972. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-10-03962.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hubbard EM, Piazza M, Pinel P, Dehaene S. Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2005;6:435–448. doi: 10.1038/nrn1684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hutchinson JB, Uncapher MR, Wagner AD. Posterior parietal cortex and episodic retrieval: convergent and divergent effects of attention and memory. Learn. Mem. 2009;16:343–356. doi: 10.1101/lm.919109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kelley WM, Macrae CN, Wyland CL, Caglar S, Inati S, Heatherton TF. Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2002;14:785–794. doi: 10.1162/08989290260138672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Konishi S, Wheeler ME, Donaldson DI, Buckner RL. Neural correlates of episodic retrieval success. Neuroimage. 2000;12:276–286. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lancaster JL, Glass TG, Lankipalli BR, Downs H, Mayberg H, Fox PT. A Modality-Independent Approach to Spatial Normalization of Tomographic Images of the Human Brain. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1995;3:209–223. [Google Scholar]
  33. McDermott KB, Jones TC, Petersen SE, Lageman SK, Roediger HL., III Retrieval Success is Accompanied by Enhanced Activation in Anterior Prefrontal Cortex During Recognition Memory: An Event-Related fMRI Study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2000;12:965–976. doi: 10.1162/08989290051137503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. McDermott KB, Szpunar KK, Christ SE. Laboratory-based and autobiographical retrieval tasks differ substantially in their neural substrates. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2290–2298. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.12.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Michelon P, Snyder AZ, Buckner RL, McAvoy M, Zacks JM. Neural correlates of incongruous visual information. An event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2003;19:1612–1626. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00111-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Miezin FM, Maccotta L, Ollinger JM, Petersen SE, Buckner RL. Characterizing the hemodynamic response: Effects of presentation rate, sampling procedure, and the possibility of ordering brain activity based on relative timing. Neuroimage. 2000;11:735–759. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Mugler JP, 3rd, Brookeman JR. Three-dimensional magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo imaging (3D MP RAGE) Magn. Res. Med. 1990;15:152–157. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910150117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Newman ME. Modularity and community structure in networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 2006;103:8577–8582. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601602103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Newman MEJ, Girvan M. Finding and evaluating community structure in networks. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonlin. Soft Matter Phys. 2004;69:026113. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.69.026113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Ojemann JG, Akbudak E, Snyder AZ, McKinstry RC, Raichle ME, Conturo TE. Anatomic localization and quantitative analysis of gradient refocused echo-planar fMRI susceptibility artifacts. Neuroimage. 1997;6:156–167. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Ollinger JM, Shulman GL, Corbetta M. Separating processes within a trial in event-related functional MRI I. The method. Neuroimage. 2001;13:210–217. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Phillips JS, Velanova K, Wolk DA, Wheeler ME. Left posterior parietal cortex participates in both task preparation and episodic retrieval. Neuroimage. 2009;46:1209–1221. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Ranganath C, Johnson MK, D'Esposito M. Left anterior prefrontal activation increases with demands to recall specific perceptual information. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:1–5. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-22-j0005.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Reynolds JR, McDermott KB, Braver TS. A direct comparison of anterior prefrontal cortex involvement in episodic retrieval and integration. Cereb. Cortex. 2006;16:519–528. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Rugg MD, Allan K. Event-related potential studies of memory. In: Tulving E, Craik FIM, editors. In The Oxford Handbook of Memory. New York: Oxford; 2000. pp. 521–537. [Google Scholar]
  46. Rushworth MFS, Paus T, Sipila PK. Attention systems and the organization of the human parietal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2001;21:5262–5271. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-14-05262.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Schacter DL, Addis DR, Buckner RL. Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;8:657–661. doi: 10.1038/nrn2213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Schacter DL, Buckner RL, Koutstaal W, Dale AM, Rosen BR. Late onset of anterior prefrontal activity during true and false recognition: An event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage. 1997;6:259–269. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, Reiss AL, Greicius MD. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Shulman GL, Fiez JA, Corbetta M, Buckner RL, Miezin FM, Raichle ME, Petersen SE. Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: II. Decreases in cerebral cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1997;9:648–663. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Simons JS, Peers PV, Hwang DY, Ally BA, Fletcher PC, Budson AE. Is the parietal lobe necessary for recollection in humans? Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:1185–1191. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Snyder AZ. Difference image vs. ratio image error function forms in PET-PET realignment. In: Myer R, Cunningham VJ, Bailey DL, Jones T, editors. Quantification of Brain Function Using PET. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1996. pp. 131–137. [Google Scholar]
  53. Sporns O, Zwi JD. The small world of the cerebral cortex. Neuroinformatics. 2004;2:145–162. doi: 10.1385/NI:2:2:145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Szpunar KK, Chan JCK, McDermott KB. Contextual processing in episodic future thought. Cereb. Cortex. 2009;19:1539–1548. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Szpunar KK, Watson JM, McDermott KB. Neural substrates of envisioning the future. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:642–647. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0610082104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Talairach J, Tournoux P. New York: Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc.; 1988. Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. [Google Scholar]
  57. Turkeltaub PE, Eden GF, Jones KM, Zeffiro TA. Meta-analysis of the functional neuroanatomy of single-word reading: method and validation. Neuroimage. 2002;16:765–780. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Van Essen DC. A Population-Average, Landmark- and Surface-based (PALS) Atlas of Human Cerebral Cortex. Neuroimage. 2005;28:635–662. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Van Essen DC, Dickson J, Harwell J, Hanlon D, Anderson CH, Drury HA. An integrated software suite for surface-based analyses of cerebral cortex. J. Am. Med. Inform. Assoc. 2001;41:1359–1378. doi: 10.1136/jamia.2001.0080443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Velanova K, Jacoby LL, Wheeler ME, McAvoy MP, Petersen SE, Buckner RL. Functional-anatomic correlates of sustained and transient processing components engaged during controlled retrieval. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:8460–8470. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-24-08460.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Vilberg KL, Rugg MD. Memory retrieval and the parietal cortex: a review of evidence from a dual-process perspective. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:1787–1799. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Vincent JL, Kahn I, Snyder AZ, Raichle ME, Buckner RL. Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 2008;100:3328–3342. doi: 10.1152/jn.90355.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Wagner AD, Shannon BJ, Kahn I, Buckner RL. Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005;9:445–453. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Wheeler ME, Buckner RL. Functional dissociation among components of remembering: Control, perceived oldness, and content. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:3869–3880. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-09-03869.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Wheeler ME, Buckner RL. Functional-anatomic correlates of remembering and knowing. Neuroimage. 2004;21:1337–1349. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Wheeler ME, Petersen SE, Nelson SM, Ploran EJ, Velanova K. Dissociating Early and Late Error Signals in Perceptual Recognition. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2008;20:2211–2225. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Yantis S, Schwarzbach J, Serences JT, Carlson RL, Steinmetz MA, Pekar JJ, Courtney SM. Transient neural activity in human parietal cortex during spatial attention shifts. Nat. Neurosci. 2002;5:995–1002. doi: 10.1038/nn921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Yonelinas A, Otten L, Shaw K, Rugg M. Separating the brain regions involved in recollection and familiarity in recognition memory. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:3002–3008. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5295-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Zarahn E, Aguirre G, D'Esposito M. A trial-based experimental design for fMRI. Neuroimage. 1997;6:122–138. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

01

RESOURCES