Abstract
Empirical studies have shown that animals often focus on short-term benefits under conditions of predation risk, which reduces the likelihood that they will cooperate with others. However, some theoretical studies predict that animals in adverse conditions should not avoid cooperation with their neighbors since it may decrease individual risks and increase long-term benefits of reciprocal help. We experimentally tested these two alternatives to find out whether increased predation risk enhances or diminishes the occurrence of cooperation in mobbing, a common anti-predator behavior, among breeding pied flycatchers, Ficedula hypoleuca. Our results show that birds attended mobs initiated by their neighbors more often, approached the stuffed predator significantly more closely, and mobbed it at a higher intensity in areas where the perceived risk of predation was experimentally increased. This study demonstrates a positive impact of predation risk on cooperation in breeding songbirds, which might help to explain the emergence and evolution of cooperation.
Key words: cooperation, predation risk, social behavior, pied flycatcher
Predation is one of the main factors responsible for mortality in the wild.1 To enhance chances of survival, prey individuals often join together to mob a predator by cooperatively attacking it.2,3 Mobbing activities of prey usually cause the predator to vacate the area and increase the probability that the predator will not return to the area where it has been unsuccessful in obtaining prey.5 Thus, mobbing reduces the threat to nearby prey individuals and allows them to resume daily activities.6 Beside these benefits, mobbing incurs costs and there appears to be a group size effect in mobbing, which indicates the importance of cooperation among prey individuals in driving predators away.7,8
It has been long assumed that organisms are less cooperative in stressful environmental conditions. Since participation in a cooperative behavior such as mobbing is costly over a short-time scale, animals often value smaller, immediate benefits over large, future benefits, thus inhibiting cooperation under adverse conditions.9 However, there are examples showing that the occurrence of positive interactions among organisms may be enhanced when living in physiologically stressful environments. 10–12 Increasing empirical evidence suggests a correlation between adverse conditions and cooperative behavior,13,14 but it is still not known whether adverse conditions play a causal role in the evolution of cooperation because experimental support is lacking.
In our study we found that breeding pied flycatchers were more willing to cooperate with their neighbors after many consecutive encounters with predators that increased their perceived risk of predation. These birds were more willing to mob at their neighbors’ nests (Fig. 1) and mobbed more intensely in areas with an increased predation risk than birds unexposed to predators. Moreover, in areas with a higher perceived predation risk, birds approached predators at their neighbors’ nests more closely. Field studies suggest that mobbing during nest defense increases birds’ fitness despite of the immediate risks.1,8 Since mobbing in a group enhances these fitness benefits, this helps explain why birds mob cooperatively.5 Through cooperation, prey individuals might significantly intensify their effect on the behavior of predators and alleviate the risks associated with engaging in mobbing, which might make mobbing in groups profitable even under increased predation risk.
We also found that birds that had received mobbing assistance from neighbors at their own nests were more likely to mob at the nests of neighbors in the future. This result is consistent with previous studies showing that breeding birds engage in reciprocal cooperation when mobbing.15–19 Reciprocal cooperation produces benefits for participants only when they interact with other co-operators.20 In our study participants interacted during the course of relatively short breeding season, which appeared to be long enough to establish reciprocal interactions. Together with the previous results revealing that pied flycatchers that encountered predators more frequently were more willing to support their neighbors, who, as a result, were more willing to join these mobs in the future, our results support the theoretical prediction21–23 and suggest that birds facing a risky environment can reduce their individual risks by cooperating with others. We show experimentally that the risk of predation can have a direct effect on cooperation, which might help to explain the origin and evolution of cooperation. However, more data are badly needed to demonstrate how adversity and uncertainty of environmental conditions might improve fitness prospects for cooperating individuals in mutualistic cooperation based on the principles of reciprocity in different taxa.
Footnotes
Previously published online: www.landesbioscience.com/journals/cib/article/11395
References
- 1.Lima SL. Predators and the breeding bird: behavioral and reproductive flexibility under the risk of predation. Biol Rev. 2009;84:485–513. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2009.00085.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Curio E. The adaptive significance of avian mobbing I. Teleonomic hypotheses and predictions. Z Tierpsychol. 1978;48:175–183. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Krams I, Krama T. Interspecific reciprocity explains mobbing behaviour of the breeding chaffinches, Fringilla coelebs. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2002;269:2345–2350. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Griesser M. Mobbing calls signal predator category in a kin group-living bird species. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2009;276:2887–2892. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.0551. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Pettifor RA. The effects of avian mobbing on a potential predator, the European kestrel, Falco tinnunculus. Anim Behav. 1990;39:821–827. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Flasskamp A. The adaptive significance of avian mobbing V. An experimental test of the ‘move on’ hypothesis. Ethology. 1994;96:322–333. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Mori K, Saito Y. Variation in social behavior within a spider mite genus, Stigmaeopsis (Acari: Tetranychidae) Behav Ecol. 2004;16:232–238. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Krams I, Berzins A, Krama T. Group effect in nest defense of breeding pied flycatchers, Ficedula hypoleuca. Anim Behav. 2009;77:513–517. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Mazur JE. An adjusting procedure for studying bird delayed reinforcement. The effect of delay and of intervening events on reinforcement value. In: Commous ML, Mazur JE, Nevin JA, editors. The effect of delay and intervening events on reinforcement value. Quantitative analyses of behaviour. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1987. pp. 55–73. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Callaway RM, Brooker RW, Choler P, Kikvidze Z, Lortie CJ, Michalet R, et al. Positive interactions among alpine plants increase with stress. Nature. 2002;417:844–847. doi: 10.1038/nature00812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Forsman JT, Hjernquist MB, Taipale J, Gustafsson L. Competitor density cues for habitat quality facilitating habitat selection and investment decisions. Behav Ecol. 2008;19:539–545. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kokko H, Ekman J. Delayed dispersal as a route to breeding: territorial inheritance, safe havens, and ecological constraints. Am Nat. 2002;160:468–484. doi: 10.1086/342074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Spinks AC, Jarvis JUM, Bennett NC. Comparative patterns of philopatry and dispersal in two common mole-rat populations: implications for the evolution of mole-rat sociality. J Anim Ecol. 2000;69:224–234. [Google Scholar]
- 14.de Bono M, Tobin DM, Davis MW, Avery L, Bargmann CI. Social feeding in Caenorhabditis elegans is induced by neurons that detect aversive stimuli. Nature. 2002;419:899–903. doi: 10.1038/nature01169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Dugatkin LA. Cooperation among animals: an evolutionary perspective. New York: Oxford University Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Krams I, Krama T, Igaune K. Mobbing behaviour: reciprocity-based co-operation in breeding pied flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca. Ibis. 2006;148:50–54. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Krams I, Krama T, Igaune K, Mänd R. Experimental evidence of reciprocal altruism in the pied flycatcher. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2008;62:599–605. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Wheatcroft DJ, Price TD. Reciprocal cooperation in avian mobbing: playing nice pays. Trends Ecol Evol. 2008;23:416–419. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2008.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wheatcroft DJ, Krams I. Response to Russell and Wright: avian mobbing. Trends Ecol Evol. 2009;24:5–6. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2008.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Axelrod R, Hamilton WD. The evolution of cooperation. Science. 1981;211:1390–1396. doi: 10.1126/science.7466396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Andras P, Roberts G, Lazarus J. Environmental risk, cooperation and communication complexity. In: Alonso EKD, editor. Adaptive Agents and Multi-Agent Systems. Berlin: Springer-Verlag; 2003. pp. 49–65. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Andras P, Lazarus J. Cooperation, risk and the evolution of teamwork. In: Gold N, editor. Teamwork: Multi-Disciplinary Perspectives. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan; 2005. pp. 56–77. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Andras P, Lazarus J, Roberts G. Environmental adversity and uncertainty favour cooperation. BMC Evol Biol. 2007;7:240. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-7-240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]