Skip to main content
F1000 Medicine Reports logoLink to F1000 Medicine Reports
. 2009 Jan 21;1:9. doi: 10.3410/M1-9

Prevention of awareness during general anesthesia

Michael S Avidan 1,, George A Mashour 2, David B Glick 3
PMCID: PMC2920697  PMID: 20948688

Abstract

Awareness during general anesthesia with subsequent explicit recall is a serious and frequently preventable problem that is gaining attention from clinicians and patients alike. Cost-effective interventions that increase vigilance should be implemented to decrease the likelihood of this complication.

Introduction and context

Awareness events can lead to post-operative psychological sequelae [1] but are also feared by patients presenting for surgery. As such, a reliable method of detecting or preventing awareness would be an important clinical advance. Although there are a number of candidate ‘awareness monitors’ based mainly on processed electroencephalograms (EEGs) or on evoked potentials, there has been considerable study and controversy regarding one particular processed EEG: the bispectral index (BIS) monitor.

Most of the research into awareness has focused on episodic consciousness during general anesthesia with subsequent explicit recall. Clinical investigators, however, have used Tunstall's isolated forearm technique to show that patients may have frequent episodes of awareness during anesthesia without forming explicit memories [2]. Even at low subanesthetic concentrations, both inhalational and intravenous anesthetic agents have been shown to block emotional and episodic memory before engendering loss of consciousness [3,4]. Debate has arisen as to whether it is acceptable for patients to have conscious perception with possible concomitant unpleasant sensations, even if they do not remember their experiences [5,6].

Current practices: what are the key considerations?

Prospective studies of unintended intra-operative awareness with subsequent explicit recall (AWR) in the general surgical population demonstrated an incidence of 1 to 2 cases per 1000 [7,8]. An increased risk for AWR has been attributed to specific patient and surgical risk factors [9]. Broadly speaking, these have been conceptualized as patients with resistance (genetic or acquired) to anesthetic agents, patients who do not tolerate high-dose anesthetic agents owing to poor cardiac reserve, and surgeries in which anesthetic dose has often been low, such as cardiac surgery and cesarean section using general anesthesia [9,10]. Beyond these risk factors, human error and machine malfunction may also lead to awareness events. Two landmark studies, a large Scandinavian observational study [11] and the B-Aware Study [9], found that incorporating BIS guidance into routine anesthetic practice could decrease the incidence of AWR. Since these studies, BIS monitoring has enjoyed widespread adoption in operating rooms and intensive care units worldwide. Interestingly, the utility of BIS monitoring in decreasing AWR was not found in a large multicenter US study, but in this study patients who received BIS monitoring may not have been well matched with those who did not [8]. In the B-Unaware randomized controlled trial, conducted in patients who were anesthetized with exclusively inhalational agents, a BIS-guided protocol was not found to confer an advantage in preventing AWR compared with a protocol using end-tidal anesthetic gas concentration [10]. The possibility of a small but clinically important benefit attributable to the BIS monitor was not excluded by this study. The B-Unaware Trial raises the question of whether the reduction in awareness reported previously with processed EEG guidance is attributable predominantly to an ‘awareness monitor’ or to a protocol that increases clinical vigilance.

BIS values have been shown to correlate with propofol blood concentrations [12], and routine intra-operative measurement of propofol levels is not presently available. It is therefore plausible that processed EEG-guided adjustment of propofol infusion might decrease the likelihood of awareness with total intravenous anesthesia (TIVA). There are, however, no randomized controlled trials assessing the efficacy of a processed EEG-guided protocol for preventing awareness with subsequent recall in the context of TIVA. Although 43% of patients in the B-Aware Study received TIVA [9], the incidence of definite awareness in this group was not reported separately, nor was this a pre-specified subgroup analysis. If the incidence of awareness had been significantly lower in this subgroup among those who received the BIS protocol, this result would have been hypothesis-generating.

The incidence of AWR in children (5 to 12 years old) has been estimated to be 0.8% [13]. In a survey of European pediatric anesthesiologists, more than 60% identified awareness as a problem in current practice [14]. In the pediatric population, the BIS has demonstrated some degree of correlation with anesthetic concentration using sevoflurane [15,16,17] as well as isoflurane [18]. The BIS has also demonstrated some degree of correlation with propofol sedation and anesthesia [19]. Thus, the use of the BIS or perhaps another ‘awareness monitor’ could potentially provide evidence that consciousness is unlikely in a child. There are no data, however, suggesting that the use of processed EEG or evoked potentials can prevent awareness in the pediatric population. Whether using any ‘awareness monitor’ provides a higher probability of unconsciousness and amnesia compared with other indices, such as movement or respiratory rate, is currently unknown.

Recent advances

In the last decade, there have been important advances in our understanding of anesthetic mechanisms [20], sleep circuits in the brain [21,22], consciousness [23], functional brain imaging [3,5], advanced electroencephalography analysis [24,25], and memory formation [3,4,26]. Recent evidence suggests that anesthetic drugs exert amnestic and hypnotic actions at different receptor subtypes in different brain regions [27]. This raises the possibility that some patients may have genetic resistance to the memory-blocking actions of anesthetic agents, which may increase their susceptibility to the complication of AWR. Improved detection of unconsciousness with general anesthesia has been facilitated by brain monitors based on processed EEG analysis [2,24,25,28] and evoked potentials [29,30]. While the BIS is reasonably good at predicting unconsciousness [2], new approaches, such as permutation entropy, Hilbert-Huang spectral entropy, bicoherence, weighted spectral median frequency, and combination techniques [24,25,28,29,30,31], may be superior at discriminating between consciousness and unconsciousness.

Implications for clinical practice

While methodological approaches may confound the detection of AWR, the vast range from 1/100 [32] to 1/14,000 [33] in the reported incidence of awareness suggests that it may not be only high-risk patients or surgeries that determine the likelihood of AWR, but also high-risk anesthetic techniques. This notion was reinforced by the results of a recent study in 1095 women undergoing cesarean section with general anesthesia [34]. Historically, this has been considered high-risk surgery and anesthesia for explicit recall, with an estimated incidence of awareness of 1% [9]. But only two women experienced awareness with explicit recall in this study, and both cases could likely have been prevented with the implementation of a simple protocol designed to increase vigilance. Inhalational anesthesia may carry less risk for awareness than TIVA since the practitioner can routinely monitor exhaled anesthetic gas and alarms can be set for low concentrations; this hypothesis warrants further examination.

At this time, it is unclear whether current ‘awareness monitors’, or indeed newer and potentially superior technologies, will be both efficacious and cost-efficient in the prevention of awareness during general anesthesia or other related settings [35]. Voss and Sleigh [35] have written an excellent and thought-provoking review on the challenges and limitations associated with EEG-based monitors of ‘depth of anesthesia’. Large prospective randomized trials without commercial influence or bias are currently under way to evaluate BIS guidance both in high-risk patients (NCT00682825) and in all-comers (NCT00689091). The major advance in the past 10 years has been the validation of awareness as a serious problem that requires rigorous study.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Rachel Wilson for her editorial and proofreading contributions to this article.

Abbreviations

AWR

awareness with recall

BIS

bispectral index

EEG

electroencephalogram

TIVA

total intravenous anesthesia

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

The electronic version of this article is the complete one and can be found at: http://F1000.com/Reports/Medicine/content/1/9

References

  • 1.Lennmarken C, Sydsjo G. Psychological consequences of awareness and their treatment. Best Pract Res Clin Anaesthesiol. 2007;21:357–67. doi: 10.1016/j.bpa.2007.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Schneider G, Gelb AW, Schmeller B, Tschakert R, Kochs E. Detection of awareness in surgical patients with EEG-based indices-bispectral index and patient state index. Br J Anaesth. 2003;91:329–35. doi: 10.1093/bja/aeg188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Alkire MT, Gruver R, Miller J, McReynolds JR, Hahn EL, Cahill L. Neuroimaging analysis of an anesthetic gas that blocks human emotional memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:1722–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0711651105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Factor 4.8 Must ReadEvaluated by John Mihic 25 Feb 2008, Misha Perouansky with Cliff Rodgers 18 Mar 2008
  • 4.Veselis RA, Reinsel RA, Feshchenko VA, Johnson R., Jr Information loss over time defines the memory defect of propofol: a comparative response with thiopental and dexmedetomidine. Anesthesiology. 2004;101:831–41. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200410000-00006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Davis MH, Coleman MR, Absalom AR, Rodd JM, Johnsrude IS, Matta BF, Owen AM, Menon DK. Dissociating speech perception and comprehension at reduced levels of awareness. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:16032–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0701309104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hudetz AG. Are we unconscious during general anesthesia? Int Anesthesiol Clin. 2008;46:25–42. doi: 10.1097/AIA.0b013e3181755db5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Factor 3.0 RecommendedEvaluated by Ville Jantti 14 Oct 2008
  • 7.Sandin RH, Enlund G, Samuelsson P, Lennmarken C. Awareness during anaesthesia: a prospective case study. Lancet. 2000;355:707–11. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(99)11010-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Sebel PS, Bowdle TA, Ghoneim MM, Rampil IJ, Padilla RE, Gan TJ, Domino KB. The incidence of awareness during anesthesia: a multicenter United States study. Anesth Analg. 2004;99:833–839. doi: 10.1213/01.ANE.0000130261.90896.6C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Myles PS, Leslie K, McNeil J, Forbes A, Chan MT. Bispectral index monitoring to prevent awareness during anaesthesia: the B-Aware randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2004;363:1757–63. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(04)16300-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Avidan MS, Zhang L, Burnside BA, Finkel KJ, Searleman AC, Selvidge JA, Saager L, Turner MS, Rao S, Bottros M, Hantler C, Jacobsohn E, Evers AS. Anesthesia awareness and the bispectral index. N Engl J Med. 2008;358:1097–108. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa0707361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Changes Clinical PracticeF1000 Factor 11.5 ExceptionalEvaluated by Vesna Jevtovic-Todorovic 18 Mar 2008, Philippa Newfield 28 Mar 2008, Beverley Orser 31 Mar 2008, Emery Brown 1 Apr 2008, Harald Groeben 7 Apr 2008, Benedikt Pannen 8 Apr 2008, Manfred Seeberger 10 Apr 2008, Alain Borgeat 17 Mar 2008, Lynda Wells 22 Apr 2008, Dhanesh Gupta 25 Apr 2008, Alex Sia with Serene Leo 16 May 2008, Paul Barash 22 May 2008, Michael O'Connor 5 Jun 2008, Masahiko Kawaguchi 11 Jun 2008
  • 11.Ekman A, Lindholm ML, Lennmarken C, Sandin R. Reduction in the incidence of awareness using BIS monitoring. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand. 2004;48:20–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-6576.2004.00260.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Rigouzzo A, Girault L, Louvet N, Servin F, De-Smet T, Piat V, Seeman R, Murat I, Constant I. The relationship between bispectral index and propofol during target-controlled infusion anesthesia: a comparative study between children and young adults. Anesth Analg. 2008;106:1109–16. doi: 10.1213/ane.0b013e318164f388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Factor 4.8 Must ReadEvaluated by Francis Veyckemans 23 May 2008, Harald Ihmsen 1 Jul 2008
  • 13.Davidson AJ, Huang GH, Czarnecki C, Gibson MA, Stewart SA, Jamsen K, Stargatt R. Awareness during anesthesia in children: a prospective cohort study. Anesth Analg. 2005;100:653–61. doi: 10.1213/01.ANE.0000150604.73481.00. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Engelhardt T, Petroz GC, McCheyne A, Bissonnette B. Awareness during pediatric anesthesia: what is the position of European pediatric anesthesiologists? Paediatr Anaesth. 2007;17:1066–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9592.2007.02277.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Denman WT, Swanson EL, Rosow D, Ezbicki K, Connors PD, Rosow CE. Pediatric evaluation of the bispectral index (BIS) monitor and correlation of BIS with end-tidal sevoflurane concentration in infants and children. Anesth Analg. 2000;90:872–7. doi: 10.1097/00000539-200004000-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Rodriguez RA, Hall LE, Duggan S, Splinter WM. The bispectral index does not correlate with clinical signs of inhalational anesthesia during sevoflurane induction and arousal in children. Can J Anaesth. 2004;51:472–80. doi: 10.1007/BF03018310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Kim HS, Oh AY, Kim CS, Kim SD, Seo KS, Kim JH. Correlation of bispectral index with end-tidal sevoflurane concentration and age in infants and children. Br J Anaesth. 2005;95:362–6. doi: 10.1093/bja/aei196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Changes Clinical PracticeF1000 Factor 3.0 RecommendedEvaluated by Victor Baum 8 Dec 2005
  • 18.Whyte SD, Booker PD. Bispectral index during isoflurane anesthesia in pediatric patients. Anesth Analg. 2004;98:1644–9. doi: 10.1213/01.ANE.0000117223.84646.36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Powers KS, Nazarian EB, Tapyrik SA, Kohli SM, Yin H, van der Jagt EW, Sullivan JS, Rubenstein JS. Bispectral index as a guide for titration of propofol during procedural sedation among children. Pediatrics. 2005;115:1666–74. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-1979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Franks NP. General anaesthesia: from molecular targets to neuronal pathways of sleep and arousal. Nat Rev. 2008;9:370–86. doi: 10.1038/nrn2372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Factor 8.0 ExceptionalEvaluated by Edward Bertaccini 19 Jun 2008, Douglas Bayliss 8 Aug 2008
  • 21.Kelz MB, Sun Y, Chen J, Cheng Meng Q, Moore JT, Veasey SC, Dixon S, Thornton M, Funato H, Yanagisawa M. An essential role for orexins in emergence from general anesthesia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:1309–14. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707146105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Factor 3.0 RecommendedEvaluated by Douglas Bayliss 8 Apr 2008
  • 22.Lu J, Nelson LE, Franks N, Maze M, Chamberlin NL, Saper CB. Role of endogenous sleep-wake and analgesic systems in anesthesia. J Comp Neurol. 2008;508:648–62. doi: 10.1002/cne.21685. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Mashour GA. Consciousness unbound: toward a paradigm of general anesthesia. Anesthesiology. 2004;100:428–33. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200402000-00035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hayashi K, Sawa T, Matsuura M. Anesthesia depth-dependent features of electroencephalographic bicoherence spectrum during sevoflurane anesthesia. Anesthesiology. 2008;108:841–50. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e31816bbd9b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Factor 3.0 RecommendedEvaluated by Emery Brown 3 Jun 2008
  • 25.Li X, Cui S, Voss LJ. Using permutation entropy to measure the electroencephalographic effects of sevoflurane. Anesthesiology. 2008;109:448–56. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e318182a91b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Eichenbaum H. Conscious awareness, memory and the hippocampus. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:775–6. doi: 10.1038/12137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Cheng VY, Martin LJ, Elliott EM, Kim JH, Mount HT, Taverna FA, Roder JC, Macdonald JF, Bhambri A, Collinson N, Wafford KA, Orser BA. Alpha5GABAA receptors mediate the amnestic but not sedative-hypnotic effects of the general anesthetic etomidate. J Neurosci. 2006;26:3713–20. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5024-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Factor 6.5 Must ReadEvaluated by Gregg Homanics 11 Apr 2006, Emery Brown 14 Aug 2007, Michael Avidan with Troy Wildes 23 May 2008
  • 28.Li X, Li D, Liang Z, Voss LJ, Sleigh JW. Analysis of depth of anesthesia with Hilbert-Huang spectral entropy. Clin Neurophysiol. 2008;119:2465–75. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Rundshagen I, Mast J, Mueller N, Pragst F, Spies C, Cortina K. Nervus medianus evoked potentials and bispectral index during repeated transitions from consciousness to unconsciousness. Br J Anaesth. 2008;101:366–73. doi: 10.1093/bja/aen186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Factor 3.0 RecommendedEvaluated by Gerhard Schneider with Stefanie Pilge 22 Aug 2008
  • 30.Schneider G, Hollweck R, Ningler M, Stockmanns G, Kochs EF. Detection of consciousness by electroencephalogram and auditory evoked potentials. Anesthesiology. 2005;103:934–43. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200511000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Jordan D, Stockmanns G, Kochs EF, Schneider G. Median frequency revisited: an approach to improve a classic spectral electroencephalographic parameter for the separation of consciousness from unconsciousness. Anesthesiology. 2007;107:397–405. doi: 10.1097/01.anes.0000278904.63884.4c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Factor 3.0 RecommendedEvaluated by Ville Jantti 19 Sep 2007
  • 32.Errando CL, Sigl JC, Robles M, Calabuig E, García J, Arocas F, Higueras R, Del Rosario E, López D, Peiró CM, Soriano JL, Chaves S, Gil F, García-Aguado R. Awareness with recall during general anaesthesia: a prospective observational evaluation of 4001 patients. Br J Anaesth. 2008;101:178–85. doi: 10.1093/bja/aen144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Changes Clinical PracticeF1000 Factor 3.2 RecommendedEvaluated by Jonathan Hardman with Matthew Wiles 24 Jul 2008, Michael Avidan with Kevin Finkel 11 Aug 2008
  • 33.Pollard RJ, Coyle JP, Gilbert RL, Beck JE. Intraoperative awareness in a regional medical system: a review of 3 years' data. Anesthesiology. 2007;106:269–74. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200702000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Paech MJ, Scott KL, Clavisi O, Chua S, McDonnell N. A prospective study of awareness and recall associated with general anaesthesia for caesarean section. Int J Obstet Anesth. 2008;17:298–303. doi: 10.1016/j.ijoa.2008.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; F1000 Factor 3.0 RecommendedEvaluated by Michael Avidan with Saima Kamal 3 Sep 2008
  • 35.Voss L, Sleigh J. Monitoring consciousness: the current status of EEG-based depth of anaesthesia monitors. Best Pract Res Clin Anaesthesiol. 2007;21:313–25. doi: 10.1016/j.bpa.2007.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from F1000 Medicine Reports are provided here courtesy of Faculty of 1000 Ltd

RESOURCES