Skip to main content
American Journal of Public Health logoLink to American Journal of Public Health
. 2010 Sep;100(9):1758–1764. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.165019

The Relative Health Burden of Selected Social and Behavioral Risk Factors in the United States: Implications for Policy

Peter Muennig 1,, Kevin Fiscella 1, Daniel Tancredi 1, Peter Franks 1
PMCID: PMC2920959  PMID: 20019300

Abstract

Objectives. We sought to quantify the potential health impact of selected medical and nonmedical policy changes within the United States.

Methods. Using data from the 1997–2000 National Health Interview Surveys (linked to mortality data through 2002) and the 1996–2002 Medical Expenditure Panel Surveys, we calculated age-specific health-related quality-of-life scores and mortality probabilities for 8 social and behavioral risk factors. We then used Markov models to estimate the quality-adjusted life years lost.

Results. Ranked quality-adjusted life years lost were income less than 200% of the poverty line versus 200% or greater (464 million; 95% confidence interval [CI] = 368, 564); current-smoker versus never-smoker (329 million; 95% CI = 226, 382); body mass index 30 or higher versus 20 to less than 25 (205 million; 95% CI = 159, 269); non-Hispanic Black versus non-Hispanic White (120 million; 95% CI = 83, 163); and less than 12 years of school relative to 12 or more (74 million; 95% CI = 52, 101). Binge drinking, overweight, and health insurance have relatively less influence on population health.

Conclusions. Poverty, smoking, and high-school dropouts impose the greatest burden of disease in the United States.


Heart disease, cancer, and stroke are the leading causes of death in the United States and much of the world. These deaths arise in part from behavioral risk factors such as smoking, obesity, and excessive alcohol use and in part from social risk factors, such as income and education disparities.14 Social risk factors also affect population health.5,6 However, although public health policy has been directed at individual social and behavioral risks, there has been little systematic investigation of their relative contribution to US population health.

Building on prior research,710 we examined social risk in the context of health disparities resulting from an individual's membership in a socially identifiable and disadvantaged group compared with membership in a nondisadvantaged counterpart.11 The poor health outcomes of such groups has long been linked, in part, to differential access to everyday social goods, including access to medical care, transportation, housing, and disability insurance.4,12 Social risk factors affect health through a wide array of pathways including stress, discrimination, social exclusion, environmental exposures, and health behaviors.3,1317

We examined the potential health gains associated with selected policy goals that are at the forefront of the current national debates. These include smoking prevention, increased access to medical care, poverty reduction, and early childhood education programs targeted toward increasing high school graduation rates. To contextualize these findings, we also explored some of the priority areas from Healthy People 2010.18 We focused on specific policies. For example, we quantified the population health benefits of poverty reduction rather than the entire income gradient19; however, we did not attempt to estimate the population health impact of any 1 policy. Rather, we simply present the total burden of disease associated with each risk factor that targeted policies might address. The objective is to provide policymakers with a rough sense of how different policy priorities might influence population health.

METHODS

We estimated the burden of disease in the United States associated with race (non-Hispanic Black versus non-Hispanic White); income (< 200% of the federal poverty line versus ≥ 200%); schooling (< 12 years versus ≥ 12); and health insurance (none versus private). The selected behavioral risk factors we studied were smoking (current versus never); obesity (body mass index [BMI; weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared] ≥ 30 kg/m2 versus 20 kg/m2 to 24.9 kg/m2); overweight (BMI 25 kg/m2 to 29.9 kg/m2 versus 20 kg/m2 to 24.9 kg/m2); and binge drinking (≥ 5 drinks per day at least 1 day per year versus < 5).

Datasets

We conducted all analyses by using consistent nationally representative datasets of the US noninstitutionalized population: the National Health Interview Survey (NHIS) linked both to the National Death Index and the Medical Expenditures Panel Survey (MEPS). We constructed mortality models with data from the 1997–2000 NHIS (157 809 adults aged 18 to 85 years) linked to mortality data via the National Death Index (1 904 535 person-years, 5749 deaths), with follow-up through the end of 2002 (the most recent follow-up publicly available).20,21 This linkage is achieved via participant identifiers within the NHIS (e.g., their social security number), and permits prospective follow-up of mortality data. The 1 exception was the insurance analysis, for which we used 1996–2000 MEPS data because the MEPS contains more detailed information than the NHIS. We exploited the MEPS linkage to the NHIS to obtain mortality follow-up through the end of 2002.

We constructed health-related quality-of-life (HRQL) models by using the EuroQol (EQ-5D) from the 2000–2002 MEPS with US weights (38 305 adults).22,23 The EQ-5D is a 5-item preference-based HRQL measure where participants rate their problems with mobility, self-care, usual activities, pain or discomfort, and anxiety or depression with a 3-category scale (no problems, some problems, extreme problems). This score is translated into a value ranging from zero (equivalent to death) to 1 (equivalent to perfect health).

The MEPS is a subsample of NHIS respondents, so the 2 surveys can be linked.24 Persons were eligible for inclusion in these analyses if they were aged 18 years or older. There was no upper age limit except for the insurance analysis, which ran to age 65 (the age at which almost all persons become insured through Medicare).

Risk Factor Selection

Our choice of risk factors was guided by the published literature1,19,2529 but limited by data available in both NHIS and MEPS.

Behavioral risk factors.

We identified smoking, obesity, overweight, and alcohol misuse as 4 important behavioral risk factors.1,26,29 Because we lacked reliable measures of alcohol dependence in our data, we substituted binge drinking to at least capture some of the risk. Other behavioral risk factors such as exercise level, sexual behavior, and the use of illicit drugs either appear to have less impact or there were inadequate data for estimation.

Social risk factors.

The most studied social factors are those related to socioeconomic status and race, which appear to have powerful independent effects on mortality.17,3033 We also included absence of health insurance as a social risk factor. Although having health insurance may also be viewed as a mediator of social risk rather than a social risk factor itself, it shares many common characteristics of social risk factors, including being largely shaped by social policy and strongly clustering with other social risk factors.34 Occupation also shapes health both indirectly through income and directly through social status, social networks, occupational hazards, stress, agency, and behavioral norms. However, unlike education and income, there are no generally accepted ordinal (or dichotomous) measures in the available data that fully capture these disparate aspects of occupational risk, so we did not examine it.

Risk Factor Thresholds

We developed a series of dichotomous thresholds to compare individuals with the risk factor present to those with no risk factor present. Thresholds were primarily chosen based upon the potential health benefits associated with current policy goals.

Most social programs aimed at addressing income disparities in the United States are targeted toward poverty alleviation. These include earned income tax credits, school lunch programs, and Medicaid. Most programs vary by state, but are targeted to those earning less than 200% of the federal poverty line.3537 Thus, we used less than 200% relative to 200% or more of the poverty line, per 2000 Census definitions obtained via the MEPS, as a threshold.

Most education programs are geared toward improving high-school graduation.38 We therefore chose the threshold for educational attainment (less than 12 years versus 12 years or more) based on whether individuals possessed a high school diploma, as it is the basic social credential needed for employment.

The smoking analysis compared current smokers with never smokers. Former smokers were excluded because many have quit only after developing serious underlying disease, and in the cross-sectional data available their mortality and HRQL were worse than that of current smokers.

The overweight (BMI ≥ 25 kg/m2 to 29.9 kg/m2) and obesity (BMI ≥ 30 kg/m2) analyses used standard thresholds.39 However, we used normal BMI (≥ 20 kg/m2 to 24.9 kg/m2) as our reference rather than BMI from 18.5 kg/m2 to 24.9 kg/m2 to ensure that we did not include chronically ill persons (who are prevalent in the 18.5 kg/m2 to 19.9 kg/m2 range) in the reference group.40 This analysis reflects the potential health benefits associated with overweight and obesity prevention.

The race analyses compared non-Hispanic Blacks with non-Hispanic Whites. African Americans have the worst life expectancy of any major minority group. We conducted 2 analyses, first without controlling for income and education and then again controlling for these factors. The former analysis may be useful for estimating the health benefits associated with community-targeted education interventions, such as Head Start or reduced class size programs. The latter analysis is meant to estimate the health benefits associated with antidiscrimination policies.

The health insurance analysis compared those with no coverage for 12 months in the survey year with those having 12 months of private coverage. We excluded those on public health insurance because these persons often have severe medical morbidity or disabilities complicating the assessment of health insurance effects. This analysis helps estimate the potential health benefits associated with universal health insurance.

The binge drinking analysis was constrained by the available data to comparing those drinking 5 or more drinks per day at least 1 day per year with those drinking less. This analysis helps estimate the potential health benefits associated with either community- or office-based anti–binge drinking programs.

Outcome Measure

To assess relative effects of social and behavioral risk factors on population health, we used quality-adjusted life years (QALYs), which capture both morbidity and mortality in a single metric. The QALY is capable of comparing disparate conditions, such as depression and stroke, or effects of disparate risk factors.41 Central to the QALY is the HRQL score, here measured as the EQ-5D.42

Regression Analyses

We conducted regression analyses with Stata version 10.1 (Statacorp, College Station, TX), adjusting for the complex survey designs of the NHIS and MEPS. We estimated risk factor stratified regression models separately for mortality and for HRQL. Confidence intervals for estimated regression coefficients were adjusted for the survey design and were based on the linearized (robust) variance estimator.

To derive the age-specific mortality risks and HRQL scores, we used the sample of participants with the risk factor present as a standard population as follows. First, we estimated regression coefficients from a pair of mortality and HRQL regression analyses of the datasets involving only persons with the risk factor present. From these regression analyses, we generated age-specific mortality rates and HRQL scores for the “risk factor present” condition. Second, we estimated regression coefficients from mortality and HRQL regression analyses involving only those with the risk factor absent. We then applied the regression coefficients from the analyses of those with the risk factor absent to the covariate values of those with the risk factor present to predict the corresponding counterfactual (“risk factor absent”) age-specific mortality rates and HRQL scores.

We adjusted all regression models for a base set of covariates: age, log age (to address nonlinear age effects), gender, region of the country (Northeast, South, Midwest, West), and survey year (as a series of dummy variables). When included as covariates, we measured income and educational attainment with 5 categories each (< 100%, 100% to 124%, 125% to 199%, 200% to 399%, and ≥ 400% of the federal poverty level; and < 12 years, 12 years, 13 to 15 years, 16 years, and > 16 years of schooling completed), and race/ethnicity was measured as 4 categories (non-Hispanic White, non-Hispanic Black, Hispanic, or Asian). See Table 1 for a listing of variables included as covariates in each analysis.

TABLE 1.

Covariates Used for Each Risk Factor Regression Model for Estimating the Impact of Common Social and Behavioral Risk Factors on US Population Health: 1997–2000 National Health Interview Survey Followed Through 2002

Risk Factor Model Covariates
< 12 years schooling Base, race/ethnicity
< 200% of federal poverty line Base, race/ethnicity
< 200% of federal poverty linea Base, race/ethnicity, education
Non-Hispanic Black Base
Non-Hispanic Blackb Base, income, education
No health insurance Base, race/ethnicity, income, education
Smoking Base, race/ethnicity, income, education
Obesityc Base, race/ethnicity, income, education, smoking status
Overweight Base, race/ethnicity, income, education, smoking status
Binge drinkingd Base, race/ethnicity, income, education, smoking status

Notes. Base covariates were age, log age, region, survey year. Race/ethnicity was categorized as non-Hispanic White, non-Hispanic Black, Hispanic, Asian. Income categories were < 100%, 100% to 124%, 125% to 199%, 200% to 399%, ≥ 400% of federal poverty line. Education categories were < 12 years, 12 years, 13 to 15 years, 16 years, > 16 years schooling completed.

a

Per 2000 Census definitions obtained via the MEPS. The poverty analysis was conducted twice, once with and once without education included as a covariate to isolate the effects of adult income programs and childhood intervention programs.

b

The race analysis was conducted twice, once with and once without income and education added as covariates to isolate the burden of disease associated with childhood intervention programs and anti–racial discrimination programs, respectively.

c

Defined by body mass index (BMI; weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared). Obesity indicated by BMI ≥ 30 kg/m2.

d

Defined as more than 5 or more drinks per day at least 1 day per year.

We estimated mortality regression coefficients with a multiplicative hazards parametric regression model of age-at-event failure time data, specified as a log-linear model by using Poisson regression.43,44 To better estimate the impact of time-varying age on the baseline hazard, this model used person-years as the unit of analysis, with each participant contributing an observation for each full or partial year of follow-up. A person-year observation could represent only a partial-year of follow-up because of the timing of death relative the NHIS interview. We used standard methods in the Poisson regression model to account for differences in observation lengths.44,45 We estimated HRQL regression coefficients with a survey weighted least squares linear regression model with EQ-5D scores as the dependent variable.

Markov Cohort Models

We used Markov cohort models to compute the expected QALYs gained when risk factors are removed from the US population. The models used our regression-derived mortality probabilities and mean HRQL scores (in 1-year age intervals) as inputs. We ran models to age 85 years. We also ran them to age 65 years to (1) provide prime-of-life estimates, (2) facilitate comparisons with the insurance analysis (which also ran only to age 65 years), and (3) explore survivor effects.

During each 1-year cycle, the age-specific mortality risk was used to transition participants to the “dead” state beginning at the next cycle, at which point they exited the model with all subsequent HRQL scores set to zero. The remaining participants were assigned to the “alive” state and received the HRQL score for the next age (based on the appropriate age and risk-factor–specific HRQL score input parameter). No discounting was applied to our estimates to ensure comparability with other burden of disease study methodology. All Markov models were built with DATApro 2006 (TreeAge Software, Williamstown, MA).

We estimated the overall societal health impact by multiplying the incremental differences in quality-adjusted life expectancy in QALYs by the number of adults in the United States at risk to obtain total QALYs lost associated with the risk factor.

We used the 95% confidence intervals around each age-specific HRQL and mortality parameter estimate to derive 95% Monte Carlo confidence intervals for each estimate of QALYs lost associated with a given risk factor. Each distribution was sampled 100 times per trial, and 1000 trials were run for each analysis. The final output for each model was normally distributed.

RESULTS

Sample sizes, population proportions, and person-years of follow-up are shown in Table 2. The mean age of the sample was 46.3 years (standard error [SE] = 0.14), with 55.4% (SE = 0.2) women. The mean EQ-5D score was 0.87 (SE = 0.001).

TABLE 2.

Sample Sizes, the Percentage of Persons Within Each Risk Category, and Person-Years at Risk for Mortality Used in Our Analyses (Through Age 85 Years): 1997–2000 National Health Interview Survey Followed Through 2002

Person-Years at Risk for Mortality
Risk Factor No. % (SE)a Person-Yearsb HRQL, No.
Race
    Non-Hispanic Black 17 075 11.8 (0.2) 57 103 6259
    Non-Hispanic White 83 663 75.9 (0.3) 287 696 27 583
Education
    < High school 26 672 18.4 (0.2) 88 120 4986
    ≥ High school 97 121 81.6 (0.2) 338 946 39 881
Poverty
    < 200% poverty line 36 345 32.7 (0.3) 153 158 15 558
    ≥ 200% poverty line 64 345 67.3 (0.3) 273 908 29 718
Health insurance
    None 8609 14.6 (0.4) 35 104 7684
    Private insurance 28 985 65.0 (0.6) 121 499 30 381
Smoker
    Current 29 936 24.6 (0.2) 106 707 9067
    Never 65 957 52.3 (0.2) 224 818 31 556
BMI, kg/m2
    Normal (20 to 24.9) 44 477 37.8 (0.2) 124 144 10 812
    Overweight (25 to 29.9) 42 251 34.6 (0.2) 109 949 12 189
    Obese (≥ 30) 25 579 20.2 (0.2) 66 134 8840
Binge drinking
    ≥ 5 drinks per d 23 800 24.5 (0.2) 72 725 777
    < 5 drinks per d 78 065 75.5 (0.2) 211 074 2806

Notes. BMI = body mass index; HRQL = health-related quality of life. Sample sizes for HRQL scores are from the 2000–2002 Medical Expenditure Panel Survey.

a

Percentages are population (not sample) percentages; some percentages do not add to 100 because those with risk factor and their comparison group do not represent the whole population.

b

Person-years = person-years of follow-up in mortality analysis.

Table 3 presents the quality-adjusted life expectancy at age 18 years with and without the risk factor alongside rankings of the incremental health impact (the gains associated with eliminating the risk) of each risk factor over the lifespan of the average American. On average, living at less than 200% federal poverty level showed the greatest impact on health, resulting in a net loss of 8.2 QALYs per person exposed. Smoking was second, with 6.6 QALYs lost per person over his or her lifetime. These were followed by the following risk factors in rank order: less than 12 years of school, being non-Hispanic Black, obesity, binge drinking, lack of health insurance, and overweight. When modeling was truncated at age 65 years, smoking dropped in ranking.

TABLE 3.

Quality-Adjusted Life Expectancy at Age 18 for Persons With and Without Each Risk Factor Under Study, and Incremental Quality Adjusted Life-Years (QALYs) Lost by Age 65 or 85 Years: 1997–2000 National Health Interview Survey Followed Through 2002

Risk Factor QALYs From Age 18 to 65 Years, Mean (SD) Incremental QALYs Lost to Age 65 Years, Mean QALYs From Age 18 to 85 Years, Mean (SD) Incremental QALYs Lost to Age 85 Years, Mean
Poverty
    ≥ 200% poverty line 41.2 (0.6) 52.9 (1.2)
    < 200% poverty line 36.8 (0.7) 4.5 44.7 (1.2) 8.2
Smokera
    Never 40.1 (0.6) 50.9 (1.1)
    Current 37.4 (0.7) 2.7 44.2 (1.1) 6.6
Poverty adjusted for education
    ≥ 200% poverty line 40.1 (0.6) 51.0 (1.1)
    < 200% poverty line 36.7 (0.7) 3.4 44.7 (1.1) 6.4
Education
    ≥ High school 40.1 (0.6) 50.8 (1.1)
    < High school 37.8 (0.7) 2.2 45.8 (1.2) 5.1
Race
    Non-Hispanic White 40.2 (0.6) 50.9 (1.1)
    Non-Hispanic Black 38.4 (0.7) 1.8 46.2 (1.2) 4.7
Obesityb
    Normal (BMI = 20 to 24.9 kg/m2) 40.6 (0.6) 52.2 (1.1)
    Obese (BMI ≥ 30 kg/m2) 38.2 (0.6) 2.4 48.0 (1.1) 4.2
Race adjusted for income, educationa
    Non-Hispanic White 38.8 (0.6) 48.0 (1.1)
    Non-Hispanic Black 38.3 (0.7) 0.5 46.3 (1.2) 1.7
Binge drinkingb
    < 5 drinks per d 40.2 (0.7) 50.5 (1.2)
    ≥ 5 drinks per d 39.6 (0.6) 0.62 49.3 (1.1) 1.2
Health insurancea
    Private insurance 40.0 (0.7)
    None 39.4 (0.6) 0.6
Overweightb
    Normal (BMI = 20 to < 25 kg/m2) 40.9 (0.6) 52.7 (1.2)
    Overweight (BMI = 25 to 29.9 kg/m2) 40.4 (0.6) 0.5 52.4 (1.2) 0.3

Note. BMI = body mass index. All regressions adjusted for age, log age, gender, race/ethnicity (except race model), region of residence (Northeast, South, Midwest, West), and survey year.

a

Adjusted for poverty (< 100%, 100% to 124%, 125% to 199%, 200% to 399%, ≥ 400% of the federal poverty level) and education levels (< 12 years, 12 years, 13 to 15 years, 16 years, > 16 years of schooling completed).

b

Adjusted for poverty, education levels, and smoking status.

Table 4 presents estimates of the US burden of disease associated with each risk factor. This analysis takes into account both the average impact on individuals and the number of individuals exposed. Here, poverty was again the leading risk factor, resulting in 544 million QALYs lost (range = 434 million to 666 million). Smoking was again second, at 332 million QALYs lost (range = 246 million to 415 million). Having less than 12 years of education was again third, followed by being obese, being non-Hispanic Black, binge drinking, being overweight, and being uninsured. The Monte Carlo confidence intervals for the estimates for no health insurance and overweight overlap with zero, indicating the effects were not statistically significant. In the analyses through age 65 years, poverty was again the leading risk factor, with other rankings largely unchanged.

TABLE 4.

The Impact of Selected Social and Behavioral Risk Factors on Millions of Quality-Adjusted Life Years (QALYs) Lost in the United States: 1997–2000 National Health Interview Survey Followed Through 2002

Risk Factor Impact to Age 65 Years, Millions of QALYs (95% CI) Impact to Age 85 Years, Millions of QALYs (95% CI)
< 200% poverty line 226 (182,276) 544 (434, 666)
< 200% poverty line adjusted for education 171 (128, 223) 421 (315, 537)
Current smokera 129 (92,167) 332 (246, 415)
< High-school education 56 (35, 80) 190 (134, 259)
Obesity (BMI ≥ 30 kg/m2)b 100 (81, 131) 149 (115,193)
Non-Hispanic Black race 40 (21,59) 113 (78, 153)
Binge drinkingbc 29 (3, 67) 56 (15, 118)
Non-Hispanic Black racea 13 (–2, 32) 42 (13, 83)
Overweight (BMI ≥ 25 kg/m2 to 29.9 kg/m2)b 20 (–24, 42) 35 (10,45)
Uninsureda 11 (–15, 29)

Note. BMI = body mass index. All regressions adjusted for age, log age, gender, race/ethnicity (except race model), region of residence (Northeast, South, Midwest, West), and survey year.

a

Adjusted for poverty (< 100%, 100% to 124%, 125% to 199%, 200% to 399%, ≥ 400% of the federal poverty level) and education levels (< 12 years, 12 years, 13 to 15 years, 16 years, > 16 years of schooling completed).

b

Adjusted for poverty, education levels, and smoking status.

c

Defined as 5 or more drinks per day at least 1 day per year.

DISCUSSION

There is debate over the extent to which health policies should be directed at broader social risk factors (such as race, education, poverty, and health insurance) relative to narrower, intermediate, and behavioral risk factors (such as smoking, obesity, and alcohol consumption).32,46 Although there is substantial overlap, the former are generally addressed by nonmedical social policies, some of which are controversial in the US context, and the latter via the public health and medical systems.27 One missing piece of information in this debate is the relative extent to which these major risk factors impact population health. Numerous studies have examined the impact of behavioral and social risk factors on health,17,30,31,4752 but this is the first study to examine these together by using a consistent analytic approach applied to the same set of nationally representative data.

We found that living at less than 200% of the federal poverty level reduced quality-adjusted life expectancy more than any other risk factor, even after we controlled for effects of education. However, smoking also proved to be a major contributor to population health, outweighing the overall impact of less than 12 years of schooling and being non-Hispanic Black. The individual impacts on QALYs of living at less than 200% of the federal poverty level, failing to graduate from high school, and being non-Hispanic Black were each greater than that of obesity. However, on a population level, the high prevalence of obesity renders it the fourth largest public health problem we, falling behind being poor, a smoker, or a high school dropout in importance. The relatively small impact of schooling in our analyses was surprising, and may reflect the declining health-related significance of high-school graduation, effects of social grade promotion, and inclusion of those with general equivalency diplomas.

Our analysis captured age-related differences in mortality; used consistent, nationally representative data sources that captured relevant confounders; used predictive modeling; and included measures of morbidity. The primary limitation was that risk factor categories are not entirely independent, but rather overlap in complex ways. For instance, low-income persons are more likely to smoke, so the smoking analysis will capture some income effects. To address this limitation, we introduced control variables into our models. However, it is likely that other unadjusted confounders may have biased our findings.

Confounding was also a challenge in our smoking and drinking analyses. Those with either of these risk factors may be more likely to engage in other risk-taking behaviors for which we were not able to account. Thus, our estimates of the burden of disease associated with these factors may be overstated. Other unmeasured confounders, such as genetic factors or personality profiles, may also have resulted in overestimates of the burden of disease reported here.

A further limitation is that policy solutions aimed at addressing the burden of disease associated with earning less than 200% of the poverty line are less than clear because the income–health relationship is bidirectional.53 Poverty produces a wide array of risk factors (e.g., crime, poor housing, limited access to medical care) thereby damaging the health and longevity of low-income populations. But sickness also leads to unemployment and poverty. The policy implications for these 2 pathways differ. Poverty itself might be addressed through policies that minimize inequalities in schooling (e.g., through early intervention programs) or through the Earned Income Tax Credit.52,53 In contrast, to address the effect of health on income, policies such as universal health insurance and disability insurance would better stem the loss of income among those who are poor because they are sick.54,55 Likewise, our health insurance analysis was designed to detect potential benefits associated with improved access to medical care. However, health insurance would also protect health by reducing income shocks associated with illness; health care costs are a leading cause of home foreclosure.

Differences in the follow-up interval for mortality estimates, the time intervals over which the risk factors are captured, and regression model design (e.g., treating inputs as moderators versus mediators) also affect our estimates of population health impact of the risk factor under study. Risk factor status was assessed at baseline only, so that misclassification over time would produce biased estimates. This bias was somewhat reduced by the short follow-up interval. However, the risk factors we estimated were of a similar magnitude to those in the medical literature.17,30,31,4752

We were only able to explore the limited range of socio–behavioral risk factors available in the datasets we used. As with other burden of disease studies, our estimates can only serve as guideposts for policymakers; the policies we identify will not eliminate the risk factor in the population. Conversely, policies can have spillover effects. For example, the overall income–health gradient is much larger than the poverty gradient,19 so spillover effects from poverty reduction programs could produce larger gains. Finally, inputs were self-reported.

Others have shown that intermediate causes of deaths—behavioral risk factors, microbial agents, toxic agents, motor vehicle accidents, and firearms—account for more than half of all the deaths in the United States.1,29 We extend these earlier analyses to include measures of morbidity and key social determinants of health. We find that living at less than 200% of the federal poverty level imposes a greater societal health burden than either of the leading behavioral causes of death—tobacco and obesity. Consistent with this, the original planning committee for Healthy People 2010 strongly considered including poverty and educational attainment among its leading indicators.56 Our findings provide additional justification for including social determinants in Healthy People 2020 and may help inform priority setting aimed at optimizing population health.

Human Participant Protection

This study used only anonymous nationally representative datasets and no additional institutional review board approval was needed.

References

  • 1.McGinnis JM, Foege WH. Actual causes of death in the United States. JAMA. 1993;270(18):2207–2212 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Link BG, Phelan J. Social conditions as fundamental causes of disease. J Health Soc Behav. 1995;35:80–94 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Adler NE, Ostrove JM. Socioeconomic status and health: what we know and what we don't. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;896:3–15 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Virchow R. Notes on the typhoid epidemic prevailing in Upper Silesia. Arch Pathologische Anatomie Physiologic Klinische Medizin. 1849;2:143–322 [Google Scholar]
  • 5.World Health Organization Commission on the social determinants of health. Available at: http://www.who.int/social_determinants/en. Accessed October 19, 2008
  • 6.Robert Wood Johnson Foundation Commission to Build a Healthier America. Available online at: http://www.commissiononhealth.org. Accessed October 19, 2008
  • 7.Smedly BD, Stith AY, Nelson AR. Unequal Treatment: Confronting Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Health Care. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2002 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Starfield B. Pathways of influence on equity in health. Soc Sci Med. 2007;64(7):1355–1362 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Rathore SS, Krumholz HM. Differences, disparities, and biases: clarifying racial variations in health care use. Ann Intern Med. 2004;141(8):635–638 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Braveman P, Gruskin S. Defining equity in health. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003;57(4):254–258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Franks P, Fiscella K. Reducing disparities downstream: prospects and challenges. J Gen Intern Med. 2008;23(5):672–677 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Sen A. Development as Freedom. New York, NY: Knopf; 1999 [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Christakis NA, Fowler JH. The spread of obesity in a large social network over 32 years. N Engl J Med. 2007;357(4):370–379 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Christakis NA, Fowler JH. The collective dynamics of smoking in a large social network. N Engl J Med. 2008;358(21):2249–2258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lantz PM, House JS, Lepkowski JM, Williams DR, Mero RP, Chen J. Socioeconomic factors, health behaviors, and mortality: results from a nationally representative prospective study of US adults. JAMA. 1998;279(21):1703–1708 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.McEwen BS, Mirsky AE. How socioeconomic status may “get under the skin” and affect the heart. Eur Heart J. 2002;23(22):1727–1728 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Williams DR. Race, socioeconomic status, and health. The added effects of racism and discrimination. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;896:173–188 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Healthy People 2010: Understanding and Improving Health. Washington, DC: US Department of Health and Human Services; 2000:30–31 [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Muennig P, Franks P, Jia H, Lubetkin E, Gold MR. The income-associated burden of disease in the United States. Soc Sci Med. 2005;61(9):2018–2026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.National Center for Health Statistics National Health Interview Survey. Available at: http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nhis.htm. Accessed August 3, 2008
  • 21.National Center for Health Statistics National Death Index. Available at: http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/r&d/nchs_datalinkage/nhis_data_linkage_mortality_activities.htm. Accessed August 3, 2008
  • 22.Agency for Health and Research Quality Medical Expenditure Panel Survey. Available at: http://www.meps.ahrq.gov/mepsweb. Accessed August 3, 2008
  • 23.Shaw JW, Johnson JA, Coons SJ. US valuation of the EQ-5D health states: development and testing of the D1 valuation model. Med Care. 2005;43(3):203–220 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.National Center for Health Statistics National Health Interview Survey and Medical Expenditure Panel Survey Linkage Files. Available at: http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/about/major/nhis/nhismep.htm. Accessed June 7, 2009
  • 25.Murray CJL, Lopez AD. Alternative projections of mortality and disability by cause 1990-2020: Global Burden of Disease Study. Lancet. 1997;349(9064):1498–1504 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Murray CJL, Lopez AD, Harvard School of Public Health, World Health Organization, World Bank. The Global Burden of Disease: A Comprehensive Assessment of Mortality and Disability From Diseases, Injuries, and Risk Factors in 1990 and Projected to 2020. Cambridge, MA: Harvard School of Public Health; 1996 [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Schoeni RF, House JS, Kaplan GA, Pollack H, Making Americans Healthier: Social and Economic Policy as Health Policy. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation; 2008 [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Franks P, Muennig P, Lubetkin E, Jia H. The burden of disease associated with being African American in the United States and the contribution of socioeconomic status. Soc Sci Med. 2006;62(10):2469–2478 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Mokdad AH, Marks JS, Stroup DF, Gerberding JL. Actual causes of death in the United States, 2000. JAMA. 2004;291(10):1238–1245 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Lynch JW, Kaplan GA, Shema SJ. Cumulative impact of sustained economic hardship on physical, cognitive, psychological, and social functioning. N Engl J Med. 1997;337(26):1889–1895 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Lleras-Muney A. The relationship between education and adult mortality in the United States. Rev Econ Stud. 2005;72(1):189–221 [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Deaton A. Policy implications of the gradient of health and wealth. An economist asks, would redistributing income improve population health? Health Aff (Millwood). 2002;21(2):13–30 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Muennig P, Schweinhart L, Montie J, Neidell M. The effect of a pre-kindergarten education intervention on adult health : 37-year follow-up results of a randomized controlled trial. Am J Public Health. 2009;99(8):1431–1437 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Mechanic D. Policy Challenges in Modern Health Care. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press; 2005 [Google Scholar]
  • 35.United States Department of Agriculture National school lunch program. Available at: http://www.fns.usda.gov/cnd/lunch/AboutLunch/NSLPFactsheet.pdf. Accessed May 29, 2009
  • 36.Dickert S, Houser S, Scholz JK. The earned income tax credit and transfer programs: a study of labor market and program participation. Tax Policy Econ. 1995;9:1–50 [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Iglehart JK. The American health care system–Medicaid. N Engl J Med. 1999;340(5):403–408 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Levin H, Belfield C. The Price We Pay: Economic and Social Consequences of Inadequate Education. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press; 2007 [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Defining overweight and obesity. Available at: http://www.cdc.gov/nccdphp/dnpa/obesity/defining.htm. Accessed May 30, 2009
  • 40.Fontaine KR, Redden DT, Wang C, Westfall AO, Allison DB. Years of life lost due to obesity. JAMA. 2003;289(2):187–193 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Weinstein MC, Stason WB. Foundations of cost-effectiveness analysis for health and medical practices. N Engl J Med. 1977;296(13):716–721 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.The EuroQol Group. EuroQol–a new facility for the measurement of health-related quality of life. Health Policy. 1990;16(3):199–208 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Holford TR. The analysis of rates and of survivorship using log-linear models. Biometrics. 1980;36:299–305 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Andersen PK, Borgan O, Gill RD, Keiding N. Statistical Models Based on Counting Processes. New York, NY: Springer; 1993 [Google Scholar]
  • 45.McCullagh P, Naelder JA. Generalized Linear Models. 2nd ed.London, England: Chapman and Hall; 1989 [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Mechanic D. Disadvantage, inequality, and social policy. Health Aff (Millwood). 2002;21(2):48–55 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Kitagawa EM, Hauser PM. Differential Mortality in the United States; a Study in Socioeconomic Epidemiology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1973 [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Rogot E, Sorlie PD, Johnson NJ. Life expectancy by employment status, income, and education in the National Longitudinal Mortality Study. Public Health Rep. 1992;107(4):457–461 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Doll R, Peto R. Mortality in relation to smoking: 20 years' observations on male British doctors. BMJ. 1976;2(6051):1525–1536 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Allison DB, Fontaine KR, Manson JE, Stevens J, VanItallie TB. Annual deaths attributable to obesity in the United States. JAMA. 1999;282(16):1530–1538 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Muennig P, Lubetkin E, Jia H, Franks P. Gender and the burden of disease attributable to obesity. Am J Public Health. 2006;96(9):1662–1668 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Muennig P, Franks P, Gold M. The cost effectiveness of health insurance. Am J Prev Med. 2005;28(1):59–64 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Muennig P. Health selection vs. causation in the income gradient: what can we learn from graphical trends? J Health Care Poor Underserved. 2008;19(2):574–579 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Muennig P, Woolf SH. Health and economic benefits of reducing the number of students per classroom in US primary schools. Am J Public Health. 2007;97(11):2020–2027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Levin H, Belfield C, Muennig P, Rouse C. The Price We Pay: Economic and Social Consequences of Inadequate Education. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press; 2007 [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Institute of Medicine Leading Health Indicators for Healthy People 2010: A Final Report. Available at: http://www.nap.edu/openbook.php?record_id=9436&page=R1. Accessed November 10, 2008 [PubMed]

Articles from American Journal of Public Health are provided here courtesy of American Public Health Association

RESOURCES