Skip to main content
American Journal of Public Health logoLink to American Journal of Public Health
. 2010 Sep;100(9):1687–1695. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.179424

Integrated Schools, Segregated Curriculum: Effects of Within-School Segregation on Adolescent Health Behaviors and Educational Aspirations

Katrina M Walsemann 1,, Bethany A Bell 1
PMCID: PMC2920985  PMID: 20634462

Abstract

Objectives. We examined the extent to which within-school segregation, as measured by unevenness in the distribution of Black and White adolescents across levels of the English curriculum (advanced placement–international baccalaureate–honors, general, remedial, or no English), was associated with smoking, drinking, and educational aspirations, which previous studies found are related to school racial/ethnic composition.

Methods. We analyzed data from wave 1 of the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health, restricting our sample to non-Hispanic Blacks (n = 2731) and Whites (n = 4158) who from 1994 to 1995 attended high schools that enrolled Black and White students.

Results. White female students had higher predicted probabilities of smoking or drinking than did Black female students; the largest differences were in schools with high levels of within-school segregation. Black male students had higher predicted probabilities of high educational aspirations than did White male students in schools with low levels of within-school segregation; this association was attenuated for Black males attending schools with moderate or high levels of within-school segregation.

Conclusions. Our results provide evidence that within-school segregation may influence both students' aspirations and their behaviors.


The role of segregation in maintaining and perpetuating racial health disparities is receiving increased attention in public health research.17 The vast majority of these studies, however, have focused on residential segregation, with few investigating the impact of school segregation on health or health behaviors. Yet schools wield considerable influence on individuals' social and economic trajectories. Because the distribution of school resources often falls along racial and class lines, schools are 1 of the prime institutions that engage in the reproduction and perpetuation of social8 and racial9 inequities in the United States. For example, Black students today are more likely to attend predominately minority schools than they were in the 1990s1012; in 1991 to 1992, 66% of Black students attended a school where 50% to 100% of the student body was non-White; 77% attended such schools in 2003 to 2004.12 Predominately minority schools are less able than majority-White schools to provide the full array of educational opportunities.11,1315

Black students who attend racially mixed and predominately White schools are more likely than Whites to be assigned to less academically rigorous coursework, even when Black students' abilities are equivalent to those of the White students.1518 This process is often referred to as tracking. Academic tracks correspond to the sequences of courses within subject domains (e.g., English, math, science) that differ in content, rigor, and instruction.18 In theory, the process of tracking should be based on student ability, previous academic achievement, and course availability. In reality, however, other factors often influence track assignment, including pressure placed on school decision makers by parents, as well as students' race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status (SES), and gender.18 Academic tracking by race has been termed second-generation segregation and often occurs in racially mixed and predominately White schools in an attempt to recreate middle-class White privilege under the guise of meritocracy and ability.1921 Many have argued that racial segregation within schools (hereafter “within-school segregation”) affects the educational opportunities of racial minorities at least as much as does racial segregation between schools.16,17,22,23

Recent studies suggest that inequalities across and within the education system affect the health and health behaviors of individuals in the short term and long term.2428 In the short term, the racial composition of schools appears to influence adolescents' smoking and drinking behaviors. For example, Kandel et al. found that as the percentage of minority students at a school increased, the odds of smoking initiation among all students decreased.24 Johnson and Hoffmann found lower rates of smoking initiation among Black students, but not White students, attending predominately minority schools.28 Hoffmann also found lower odds of drinking in predominately minority schools, but only for female students.27

Schools may also have long-term effects on health. Walsemann et al. found that the accumulation of educational advantages (e.g., college preparatory coursework, expectations to attend college, attending high-SES schools) decreased the odds of reporting a health limitation in early adulthood and slowed the rate of increase in the odds of health limitations up to middle age, independent of educational attainment.26 In another study, respondents who reported being in the general academic track, who had taken remedial coursework, or who did not expect to attend college experienced consistently higher levels of depressive symptoms in early and mid-adulthood, independent of family background and adult SES.25 These studies help elucidate the role that the education system plays in population health, but we know of no studies that have examined how within-school segregation affects adolescent health.

Within-school segregation may influence the health and health behaviors of adolescents through multiple pathways. Segregating students into different levels of course curriculum on the basis of race is discriminatory. Previous research has suggested that perceived discrimination increases the odds of smoking and drinking among Black individuals.2935 It is therefore possible that attending schools with high levels of within-school segregation may lead Black students to engage in riskier health behaviors as a means of coping with the chronic stress of marginalization.

Another possibility is that schools with high levels of within-school segregation may create an environment in which stereotypes about group differences are magnified36 and Black students are marginalized by teachers and peers.37 In response to such an environment, Black students may seek out same-race friendships and safe places where they feel a sense of belonging and identity.37 This, in turn, may confer a level of protection against some risky behaviors (e.g., smoking, drinking) by insulating Black students from the riskier behaviors of White students.24,3840

Students are often aware of tracking in their school, even if classes are not marked as high or low ability.41 Because of the interplay between social, cultural, and political processes involved in tracking decisions at the school level, tracking often results in the conflation of ability with race,9 which in turn may socialize students to accept their positions in their schools' social hierarchy.17 Even in schools that provide some opportunity for track mobility, students who have a history of being tracked often do not enroll in high-ability courses because they have internalized the labels and status attached to them by their peers, teachers, and administrators.37 Thus, another pathway through which within-school segregation may affect health is demoralization.4244 Repeated failures and grade retentions, for example, increase the likelihood of dropping out.15 Dropping out of high school decreases access to economic and social resources in adulthood that have been linked to better physical and mental health.4547 In addition, students who are tracked into low-ability courses are less likely to attend or complete college because they do not have the prerequisite coursework to apply to college18 or do not have access to important postsecondary informational networks.20

We examined the extent to which within-school segregation, as measured by racial segregation across levels of the English curriculum, was associated with smoking, drinking, and the educational aspirations of Black and White adolescents.

METHODS

We analyzed wave 1 (1994–1995) restricted data from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health, a nationally representative sample of adolescents in grades 7 through 12 in 1994 to 1995,48 and the Adolescent Health and Academic Achievement study.49 We analyzed data from 4 sources: an in-home interview of the student, an in-home interview of the parent, a self-administered questionnaire completed by a school administrator, and retrospectively collected transcript data.48,49 Transcript data provided information on educational achievement, courses taken, curricular exposure, and school context during the respondents' 9th- through 12th-grade years (if applicable). We limited the use of transcript data to courses taken during the 1994–1995 school year.

The original sample had 80 high schools. Of these, 78 provided transcript data. We excluded 13 schools whose student body was either 100% White or 100% Black, 1 school that had too few students to calculate a meaningful measure of within-school segregation, and 17 schools with no Blacks in attendance. Our total sample comprised 47 schools.

We restricted our analysis to students who self-reported as non-Hispanic Black (n = 2731) or non-Hispanic White (n = 4158). We excluded 76 students with missing data. The total analytic sample consisted of 6889 adolescents dispersed across the 47 schools (density = 27–472 students for the entire sample, 10–243 male students, and 14–229 female students).

Measures

Dependent variables.

We created a variable indicating whether the respondent smoked cigarettes on 1 or more occasions in the past 30 days or whether the respondent consumed alcohol on at least 1 day in the past 12 months (excluding drinking alcohol with adult relatives). Those who reported smoking or drinking were coded 1, and all others were coded 0. As a measure of educational aspirations, respondents were asked, “On a scale of 1 to 5, where 1 is low and 5 is high, how much do you want to go to college?” We categorized respondents as having high educational aspirations if they selected 4 or 5, and low–medium if not.

Within-school segregation.

We calculated the index of dissimilarity for each school as a measure of within-school segregation. In our study, the measure of dissimilarity captured the unevenness in the distribution of Black and White students across levels of the English curriculum. We used the index of dissimilarity because, as we defined it, the index reflects the process of assigning students to levels of the curriculum according to race, it is a measure of segregation that is widely understood in the field of segregation research, and it is independent of the racial composition of the school.50 We focused on the English curriculum, rather than math or science, because (1) most students are required to take English throughout high school, whereas math and science requirements vary more across schools (e.g., many schools only require 2 years of science),51 and (2) students can enroll in advanced English courses without completing prerequisites, which are often required for math and science courses.

We used the following formula to calculate the index of dissimilarity for each school:

graphic file with name 1687equ1.jpg

where bi was the number of Black students in the ith English curriculum track at the school (i.e., advanced placement–international baccalaureate–honors, general, remedial, or no English), B was the total number of students at the school, wi was the number of White students in the ith English curriculum track at the school, and W was the total number of White students at the school. We used the weights provided by the Adolescent Health and Academic Achievement study to calculate the proportion of Black and White students at each level of the English curriculum in 1994 to 1995. The dissimilarity index could range from 0, indicating no segregation, to 100, indicating complete segregation.

Our index ranged from 0.96 to 100, with a mean dissimilarity index of 23. Exploratory analyses suggested a nonlinear relation between the index of dissimilarity and the dependent variables, so we created 3 dummy variables to represent low (0–9.95), medium (9.96–29.10), and high (> 29.10) within-school segregation. Interaction terms between within-school segregation and student race were also created to allow us to investigate whether the relation between within-school segregation and each dependent variable was different for Black and White students.

Individual-level covariates.

We included the age of the respondent as a continuous variable. Respondents self-reported their race, which we categorized as non-Hispanic Black or non-Hispanic White. We created a measure of family structure categorized as nuclear (2 biological parents), stepfamily (1 biological and 1 stepparent), female headed, or other. Finally, we constructed a composite measure of family SES, calculated as the mean of standardized (z score) measures of family income, parental educational level, and parental occupational prestige, with higher values representing higher levels of SES (Cronbach α = 0.69).

School-level covariates.

We included several school characteristics that might be related to our dependent variables or to our main variable of interest, within-school segregation. School SES was a composite measure calculated as the mean of standardized (z score) measures of school-level parental education, school-level parental occupational prestige, and the proportion of students not receiving free or reduced-priced lunch, with higher values representing higher levels of school SES (Cronbach α = 0.85). Additional covariates were the percentage of non-Hispanic White students in the school (a continuous measure, ranging from 3 to 95), school size (small = 1–400 students; medium = 401–1000 students; large = 1001–4000 students); the region of the country (West, Midwest, Northeast, or South); the school community (urban–suburban = consolidated metropolitan statistical area or metropolitan statistical area with ≥ 2500 residents; rural = all others); and school type (public or private).

Analyses

Previous research suggests that different factors may influence the behaviors and aspirations of male and female students,27,52 so we stratified all analyses by gender. We began with descriptive statistics to understand the data distribution. Next, because of the nonindependent nature of the data (i.e., students tested in schools), we used multilevel modeling techniques to investigate the extent to which within-school segregation was associated with smoking, drinking, and the educational aspirations of Black and White students. Because both of our outcomes were binary variables, we used random-intercept 2-level generalized linear mixed models to answer our research questions.

After examining an unconditional model with no predictors to assess between-school variation in our dependent variables, we ran a model that examined the association between our primary predictor variable—within-school segregation—and our dependent variables, after adjusting for individual-level covariates (model 1). Finally, we added school-level covariates to determine whether the effects of within-school segregation were attenuated once other correlated school characteristics were included (model 2). The equation from our final model for predicting adolescent drinking or smoking was

graphic file with name 1687equ2.jpg

where logit[P(Yij1 X, Z, μ0j)] was the log odds that student i in school j smoked or drank and assumed that conditional on X, Z, and μ0j, Yij to Ynj were independent; j = 1 … Jk was the number of schools in our sample; Inline graphic was a vector of individual-level covariates (e.g., race, family SES); Inline graphic was a vector of school-level covariates (e.g., medium segregation) and cross-level interactions (e.g., Black x medium segregation); and μ0j was a random effect representing unobserved heterogeneity for school j and was assumed to be randomly and normally distributed with mean 0. We built similar models to predict the log odds of high educational aspirations.

To make the interpretation of the intercept more meaningful, age was centered at 16 years, the approximate mean age of the sample; the percentage of non-Hispanic White students at a school was mean centered at 57%. Other than the assumption of independence, which was not violated by the study design, no other assumptions needed to be evaluated because the models were nonlinear. We specified random-intercept models with xtlogit in Stata version 10.53

RESULTS

Overall, the adolescents were primarily White (60%), tended to live in nuclear families (39%), and had slightly above-average family SES (mean = 0.07; SE = 0.009; Table 1). The sample had slightly more girls than boys (52% versus 48%) and the mean age was 16.7 years (SE = 0.017). Fifty-six percent of the adolescents reported smoking or drinking, and 83% had high educational aspirations.

TABLE 1.

Sample Characteristics of White and Black Students, by Gender: Wave 1, National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health, 1994–1995

All Students, % (SE) or Mean (SE) Female Students, % (SE) or Mean (SE) Male Students, % (SE) or Mean (SE)
Dependent variables
Smoking or drinking 0.56 (0.006) 0.56 (0.008) 0.57 (0.009)
High educational aspirations 0.83 (0.005) 0.86 (0.006) 0.80 (0.006)
Individual-level characteristics
Mean age,a y 16.7 (0.017) 16.7 (0.023) 16.8 (0.024)
Gender
    Female 0.52 (0.006)
    Male 0.48 (0.006)
Race
    White 0.60 (0.006) 0.59 (0.008) 0.62 (0.008)
    Black 0.40 (0.006) 0.41 (0.008) 0.38 (0.008)
Family structure
    Nuclear 0.39 (0.006) 0.39 (0.008) 0.40 (0.008)
    Stepfamily 0.10 (0.004) 0.09 (0.005) 0.10 (0.005)
    Female headed 0.18 (0.005) 0.18 (0.006) 0.18 (0.007)
    Other 0.33 (0.006) 0.34 (0.008) 0.32 (0.008)
Family SESb 0.07 (0.009) 0.05 (0.012) 0.09 (0.013)
School-level characteristics
Within-school segregationc
    Low (≤ 9.95) 0.21 (0.005) 0.22 (0.007) 0.20 (0.007)
    Medium (9.96–29.10) 0.50 (0.006) 0.50 (0.008) 0.50 (0.009)
    High (> 29.10) 0.29 (0.005) 0.29 (0.008) 0.30 (0.008)
School SESd 0.06 (0.008) 0.08 (0.011) 0.04 (0.11)
Mean % of White studentsa 57.3 (0.347) 57.0 (0.005) 58.0 (0.005)
School size
    Small (1–400 students) 0.07 (0.003) 0.08 (0.004) 0.07 (0.004)
    Medium (401–1000 students) 0.31 (0.006) 0.30 (0.008) 0.31 (0.008)
    Large (1001–4000 students) 0.62 (0.006) 0.62 (0.008) 0.62 (0.008)
Region
    West 0.16 (0.004) 0.16 (0.006) 0.15 (0.006)
    Midwest 0.22 (0.005) 0.23 (0.007) 0.21 (0.007)
    Northeast 0.16 (0.004) 0.16 (0.006) 0.17 (0.006)
    South 0.46 (0.006) 0.46 (0.008) 0.47 (0.009)
School communitye
    Rural 0.12 (0.004) 0.12 (0.005) 0.12 (0.006)
    Urban–Suburban 0.88 (0.004) 0.88 (0.005) 0.88 (0.006)
School type
    Private 0.09 (0.003) 0.09 (0.005) 0.09 (0.005)
    Public 0.91 (0.003) 0.91 (0.005) 0.91 (0.005)

Note. SES = socioeconomic status. Unless otherwise noted, variables are binary or categorical, and estimates can be interpreted as proportions. Columns may not equal 1 because of rounding. The sample size was n = 6889 respondents and n = 47 schools.

a

Continuous variable.

b

Family SES was a composite measure calculated as the mean of standardized (z score) measures of family income, parental educational level, and parental occupational prestige, with higher values representing higher levels of family SES.

c

Within-school segregation was calculated with the dissimilarity index, a measure of the unevenness in the distribution of Black and White students across levels of the English curriculum. The index ranged from 0.96 to 100, with a mean dissimilarity index of 23; the ranges shown correspond to 3 dummy variables.

d

School SES was a composite measure calculated as the mean of standardized (z score) measures of school-level parental education, school-level parental occupational prestige, and the proportion of students not receiving free or reduced-priced lunch, with higher values representing higher levels of school SES.

e

Urban–suburban included consolidated metropolitan statistical areas or metropolitan statistical areas with 2500 or more residents; rural included all others.

On average, the majority of adolescents were enrolled in large (62%), urban–suburban (88%), public (91%) schools in the South (46%). On average, schools were primarily White (57.3%) in their student body composition and had slightly above-average SES (mean = 0.06; SE = 0.008). Half of the students attended schools with medium levels of within-school segregation. More details on the sample characteristics are provided in Table 1.

After adjustments for individual covariates, within-school segregation appeared to be related to female students' predicted log odds of smoking or drinking (Table 2, model 1). Specifically, the results suggested a statistically significant interaction between student race and high levels of within-school segregation. These findings held after adjustment for school-level covariates (Table 2, model 2). As shown in Figure 1, not only did White female students have higher predicted probabilities of current smoking or ever drinking in the past 12 months, but the nature of these racial differences varied by levels of within-school segregation. As the level of within-school segregation increased, racial differences in the predicted probabilities of current smoking or ever drinking in the past 12 months also increased, with the largest Black–White differences found in schools with high levels of within-school segregation (predicted probabilities of 0.54 for White female students versus 0.25 for Black female students). However, we did not observe similar relationships in the educational aspirations model for female students; within-school segregation was not a significant main effect, nor did it interact significantly with race (Table 2, model 2).

TABLE 2.

Log-Odds Estimates From Random Intercept Logit Models Predicting Smoking or Drinking and Educational Aspirations Among Non-Hispanic Black and White Female Students, Regressed on Within-School Segregation: Wave 1, National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health, 1994–1995

Smoking or Drinkinga (n = 3579)
Educational Aspirationsb (n = 3579)
Model 1, b (SE) Model 2, b (SE) Model 1, b (SE) Model 2, b (SE)
Intercept 0.09 (0.14) −0.11 (0.15) 2.04*** (0.21) 2.07*** (0.23)
Individual-level variables
Age,c y 0.21*** (0.03) 0.22*** (0.03) −0.10*** (0.04) −0.10* (0.04)
Race
    White (Ref) 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
    Black −0.68*** (0.16) −0.69*** (0.16) 0.61* (0.24) 0.65** (0.24)
Family structure
    Nuclear (Ref) 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
    Stepfamily 0.58*** (0.14) 0.58*** (0.14) −0.37 (0.19) −0.34 (0.19)
    Female headed 0.42*** (0.10) 0.40*** (0.10) −0.28 (0.16) −0.26 (0.16)
    Other 0.47*** (0.09) 0.47*** (0.09) −0.51*** (0.13) −0.48*** (0.13)
Family SES 0.08 (0.05) 0.06 (0.06) 0.61*** (0.08) 0.56*** (0.08)
School-level variables
Within-school segregationd
    Low (≤ 9.95; Ref) 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
    Medium (9.96–29.10) 0.12 (0.16) 0.08 (0.15) 0.11 (0.24) 0.14 (0.22)
    High (> 29.10) 0.31 (0.17) 0.28 (0.16) 0.11 (0.26) 0.11 (0.23)
School SES 0.02 (0.11) 0.34* (0.17)
White students,c % 0.003 (0.002) 0.001 (0.003)
School size
    Small (1–400 students) 0.04 (0.24) 0.32 (0.44)
    Medium (401–1000 students) 0.15 (0.11) −0.33* (0.16)
    Large (1001–4000 students; Ref) 1.00 1.00
Region
    West 0.35* (0.15) −0.35 (0.21)
    Midwest 0.37** (0.12) −0.03 (0.18)
    Northeast 0.27* (0.14) −0.06 (0.21)
    South (Ref) 1.00 1.00
School communitye
    Rural −0.08 (0.16) 0.07 (0.25)
    Urban–suburban (Ref) 1.00 1.00
School type
    Public (Ref) 1.00 1.00
    Private −0.28 (0.27) 0.69 (0.49)
Cross-level interactions
Black x medium segregation −0.24 (0.20) −0.16 (0.19) −0.30 (0.29) −0.29 (0.28)
Black x high segregation −0.72** (0.23) −0.59** (0.23) 0.07 (0.34) 0.04 (0.34)
ρ 0.02* 0.006* 0.05* 0.02*
Log likelihood −2291.7 −2282.8 −1348.6 −1334.7

Note. SES = socioeconomic status.

a

Unconditional model: ρ = 0.05; log likelihood = −2406.

b

Unconditional model: ρ = 0.11; log likelihood = −1407.

c

Age was centered at 16, percentage of White students at 57%.

d

Within-school segregation calculated with the dissimilarity index, a measure of the unevenness in the distribution of Black and White students across levels of the English curriculum. The index ranged from 0.96 to 100, with a mean dissimilarity index of 23; the ranges shown correspond to 3 dummy variables.

e

Urban–suburban included consolidated metropolitan statistical areas or metropolitan statistical areas with 2500 or more residents; rural included all others.

*P < .05, **P < .01, ***P < .001.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Predicted probability, by level of within-school segregation and race, of (a) smoking or drinking among female adolescents and (b) high desire to attend college among male adolescents: Wave 1, National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health, 1994–1995.

Note. Adjusted for age, family structure, family SES, school SES, percentage non-Hispanic Whites in school, school size, region, rurality, and school type. Age centered at 16 years, and percentage non-Hispanic Whites centered at 57%. Predicted probability calculated using fully adjusted model 2.

We observed opposite patterns for male and female students (Table 3). Within-school segregation did not appear to be related to male students' predicted log odds of smoking or drinking but was related to the predicted log odds of reporting high educational aspirations, after adjustment for individual covariates (Table 3, model 1). Indeed, in predictions of male students' log odds of current smoking or ever drinking in the past 12 months, within-school segregation was not a statistically significant main effect, nor did it interact significantly with race. These findings remained after further adjustments for school-level covariates (Table 3, model 2).

TABLE 3.

Log-Odds Estimates From Random-Intercept Logit Models Predicting Smoking or Drinking and Educational Aspirations Among Non-Hispanic Black and White Male Students, Regressed on Within-School Segregation: Wave 1, National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health, 1994–1995

Smoking or Drinkinga (n = 3310)
Educational Aspirationsb (n = 3310)
Model 1, b (SE) Model 2, b (SE) Model 1, b (SE) Model 2, b (SE)
Intercept 0.20 (0.14) 0.06 (0.14) 1.43*** (0.20) 1.60*** (0.20)
Individual-level variables
Age,c y 0.32*** (0.03) 0.32*** (0.03) −0.18*** (0.04) −0.18*** (0.04)
Race
    White (Ref) 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
    Black −1.16*** (0.18) −1.18*** (0.17) 1.18*** (0.24) 1.25*** (0.24)
Family structure
    Nuclear (Ref) 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
    Stepfamily 0.35** (0.13) 0.35** (0.13) −0.39* (0.16) −0.41** (0.16)
    Female headed 0.42*** (0.11) 0.43*** (0.11) −0.09 (0.14) −0.11 (0.14)
    Other 0.51*** (0.09) 0.51*** (0.09) −0.26* (0.12) −0.26* (0.11)
Family SES 0.05 (0.06) 0.02 (0.06) 0.76*** (0.07) 0.70*** (0.07)
School-level variables
Within-school segregationd
    Low (≤ 9.95) (Ref) 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
    Medium (9.96–29.10) −0.05 (0.16) −0.09 (0.14) 0.10 (0.23) 0.16 (0.19)
    High (> 29.10) −0.03 (0.17) −0.10 (0.15) 0.15 (0.24) 0.23 (0.20)
School SES −0.03 (0.10) 0.51*** (0.15)
White students,c % 0.003 (0.002) −0.000 (0.002)
School size
    Small (1–400 students) −0.23 (0.22) −0.10 (0.33)
    Medium (401–1000 students) 0.15 (0.10) −0.32* (0.13)
    Large (1001–4000 students; Ref) 1.00 1.00
Region
    West 0.08 (0.13) −0.28 (0.18)
    Midwest 0.14 (0.11) −0.12 (0.15)
    Northeast 0.13 (0.12) −0.21 (0.17)
    South (Ref) 1.00 1.00
School communitye
    Rural 0.01 (0.14) −0.30 (0.19)
    Urban–suburban (Ref) 1.00 1.00
School type
    Public (Ref) 1.00 1.00
    Private 0.50 (0.26) 0.20 (0.40)
Cross-level interactions
Black x medium segregation 0.18 (0.21) 0.28 (0.20) −0.55 (0.29) −0.65* (0.28)
Black x high segregation 0.12 (0.24) 0.28 (0.23) −0.70* (0.32) −0.73* (0.32)
ρ 0.01* 0.00 0.05* 0.01*
Log likelihood −2102.1 −2091.4 −1517.4 −1501.5

Note. b = the estimate for the coefficient produced from the analysis; SES = socioeconomic status.

a

Unconditional model: ρ = 0.04; log likelihood = −2238.

b

Unconditional model: ρ = 0.11; log likelihood = −1614.

c

Age was centered at 16, percentage of White students at 57%.

d

Within-school segregation was calculated with the dissimilarity index, a measure of the unevenness in the distribution of Black and White students across levels of the English curriculum. The index ranged from 0.96 to 100, with a mean dissimilarity index of 23; the ranges shown correspond to 3 dummy variables.

e

Urban–suburban included consolidated metropolitan statistical areas or metropolitan statistical areas with 2500 or more residents; rural included all others.

*P < .05; **P < .01; ***P < .001.

In models predicting male students' log odds of having high educational aspirations, we observed statistically significant interaction effects between student race and within-school segregation, after we controlled for individual covariates. When we included school-level covariates (Table 3, model 2), the interaction effect between race and medium within-school segregation became statistically significant (b = −0.65), and the interaction effect between race and high within-school segregation remained significant (b = −0.73). As shown in Figure 1, racial differences in the predicted probabilities of male students reporting high educational aspirations varied by levels of within-school segregation. In schools with low levels of within-school segregation, the predicted probabilities of reporting high educational aspirations were greater for Black than for White male students (0.94 and 0.83, respectively). However, as the level of within-school segregation increased, racial differences in the predicted probabilities of reporting high educational aspirations dissipated.

DISCUSSION

Consistent with previous research, we found higher rates of smoking or drinking among White students24,3840,54 and higher educational aspirations among Black students, regardless of gender.52,55 Our findings suggest, however, that attending schools with higher levels of within-school segregation may confer some level of protection against risky behaviors for Black female students while simultaneously dampening educational aspirations for Black male students. These findings held even with adjustment for a range of individual- and school-level covariates.

The significant negative association we detected between high within-school segregation and smoking or drinking among Black female students may be attributable to several factors. For example, within-school segregation may affect risk behaviors by fostering same-race friendships. Black female students may befriend other Black students as a buffer against an environment that systematically marginalizes them.37 Because smoking and drinking are more common among White adolescents,24,39 the lack of cross-racial friendships may protect Black female students from the riskier behaviors of their White counterparts. Alternatively, the association between within-school segregation and smoking or drinking may reflect engagement in other, unmeasured resiliency strategies among Black female students. Such analyses were beyond the scope of our study; however, our findings suggest the importance of investigating the different types of resistance strategies Black youths adopt in an effort to overcome segregated environments.

Although Black adolescents are less likely than are White adolescents to smoke, the reverse is true in adulthood.5658 It is plausible that within-school segregation has short-term protective effects but also has long-term negative effects, especially on smoking. Within-school segregation restricts the educational opportunities of Black students.18 The long-term consequences of within-school segregation on individuals' social and economic trajectories may not be fully perceived until they attempt to enter college or find gainful employment, at which point they may initiate smoking as a coping mechanism. Indeed, such speculation is consistent with previous studies that found lagged effects of perceived discrimination on health5961 and higher rates of smoking and drinking among those who reported current or lifetime discrimination.29,32 Our study was cross sectional, and we were therefore unable to test this hypothesis, but exploring such questions is an important next step in understanding the long-term effects of within-school segregation.

Students attending desegregated schools are more likely to attend college, be employed, and work in less segregated occupations.20,62 Our findings, however, suggest a possible exception to this finding: desegregated schools that resegregate within the curriculum by race may dampen the educational aspirations of Black male students, which may ultimately lead to lower educational achievement.41,42,63 This dampening of aspirations could have significant long-term consequences on the social, economic, and health outcomes of Black men. Indeed, low-level curricula often leave students unchallenged, discouraged, and unprepared to attend college42; their resultant effects on educational achievement may lead to higher rates of unemployment and poverty, greater engagement in risky health behaviors, and increased likelihood of premature morbidity and mortality.4547

Limitations

Our measure of within-school segregation likely underestimated the extent of segregation within a school because it focused on how unevenly Blacks and Whites were distributed across levels of the English curriculum. Segregation across math, science, and social science courses also occurs and is likely to exist in schools that use academic tracking in the English curriculum,17 but our ability to calculate the dissimilarity measure was limited because of the greater variability in requirements for these subjects. We were also unable to examine Hispanic–White segregation because not enough schools (n = 39) had sufficient numbers of Hispanics and Whites to provide sufficient statistical power for our random-intercept models.

Because our analytic sample comprised only 47 schools, our ability to generalize beyond our sample schools and the students who attended them in 1994 to 1995 is limited. We know of no other data, however, that would allow us to test our novel research questions. In addition, the restricted nature of our sample limited our ability to detect variation across schools in other measures of health and health behaviors. Studies that collect sufficient data on course enrollment and adolescent health across a larger sample of high schools are needed. Finally, we relied on cross-sectional data and were therefore unable to determine causation.

Conclusions

Our findings provide preliminary evidence that within-school segregation may influence not only students' worldview (i.e., aspirations) but also their behaviors (i.e., smoking or drinking). Caution is warranted, however, because our results indicate that the effects of within-school segregation on health behaviors may not be entirely negative, at least in the short term. Efforts to promote a positive racial climate and reduce the risky behaviors of Whites are vital to ensuring that desegregated schools provide the best social and health environment for all of their students.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the University of Michigan's Robert Wood Johnson Foundation Health & Society Scholars Program (grant N008885).

We thank Debeshi Maitra for her assistance with data management and Gilbert Gee for helpful comments and suggestions on an earlier draft of this article.

This research used data from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health, a program project by Kathleen Mullan Harris and designed by J. Richard Udry, Peter S. Bearman, and Kathleen Mullan Harris at the University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, and funded by grant P01-HD31921 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, with cooperative funding from 23 other federal agencies and foundations. Special acknowledgment is due Ronald R. Rindfuss and Barbara Entwisle for assistance in the original design. Information on how to obtain the Add Health data files is available on the Add Health Web site (http://www.cpc.unc.edu/addhealth). No direct support was received from grant P01-HD31921 for this analysis.

This research also used data from the Adolescent Health and Academic Achievement study, which was funded by a grant (R01 HD040428-02, Chandra Muller, PI) from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, and a grant (REC-0126167, Chandra Muller, PI, and Pedro Reyes, Co-PI) from the National Science Foundation. This research was also supported by grant 5 R24 HD042849, Population Research Center, awarded to the Population Research Center at the University of Texas at Austin by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Health and Child Development.

Note. Opinions reflect those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect those of the granting agencies.

Human Participation Protection

Institutional review board approval was obtained from the University of South Carolina.

References

  • 1.Kramer MR, Hogue CR. Is segregation bad for your health? Epidemiol Rev. 2009;31:178–194 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Williams DR, Collins C. Racial residential segregation: a fundamental cause of racial disparities in health. Public Health Rep. 2001;116(5):404–417 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Osypuk TL, Acevedo-Garcia D. Are racial disparities in preterm birth larger in hypersegregated areas? Am J Epidemiol. 2008;167(11):1295–1304 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bell JF, Zimmerman FJ, Mayer JD, Almgren GR, Huebner CE. Associations between residential segregation and smoking during pregnancy among urban African American women. J Urban Health. 2007;84(3):372–388 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Robert SA, Ruel E. Racial segregation and health disparities between Black and White older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2006;61(4):S203–S211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Acevedo-Garcia D, Lochner KA, Osypuk TL, Subramanian S. Future directions in residential segregation and health research: A multilevel approach. Am J Public Health. 2003;93(2):215–221 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Polednak AP. Segregation, discrimination and mortality in US Blacks. Ethn Dis. 1996;6(1–2):99–147 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Bourdieu P, Passeron JC. Reproduction in Education, Society, and Culture. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications; 1990 [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Lewis AE. Race in the Schoolyard: Negotiating the Color Line in Classrooms and Communities. Piscataway, NJ: Rutgers University Press; 2003 [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Orfield G. Schools More Separate: Consequences of a Decade of Resegregation. Cambridge, MA: The Civil Rights Project at Harvard University; 2001. Available at: http://www.civilrightsproject.ucla.edu/research/deseg/Schools_More_Separate.pdf. Accessed July 27, 2009 [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Orfield G, Eaton SE. Dismantling Desegregation: The Quiet Reversal of Brown v. Board of Education. New York, NY: W.W. Norton & Company; 1996 [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Orfield G, Lee C. Racial Transformation and the Changing Nature of Segregation. Cambridge, MA: The Civil Rights Project at Harvard; 2006. Available at: http://www.civilrightsproject.ucla.edu/research/deseg/Racial_Transformation.pdf. Accessed July 27, 2009 [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Darling-Hammond L, Post L. Inequality in teaching and schooling: supporting high-quality teaching and leadership in low-income schools. : Kahlenberg R, A Notion at Risk: Preserving Public Education as an Engine for Social Mobility. New York, NY: Century Foundation Press; 2000:127–167 [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Rothstein R. Equalizing education resources on behalf of disadvantaged children. : Kahlenberg R, A Notion at Risk: Preserving Education as an Engine for Social Mobility. New York, NY: Century Foundation Press; 2000:31–92 [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Darling-Hammond L. Inequality and the right to learn: access to qualified teachers in California's public schools. Teach Coll Rec. 2004;106(10):1936–1966 [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lucas SR. Tracking Inequality: Stratification and Mobility in American High Schools. New York, NY: Teachers College Press; 1999 [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Mickelson RA. Subverting Swann: first- and second-generation segregation in the Charlotte-Mecklenburg schools. Am Educ Res J. 2001;38(2):215–252 [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Mickelson RA, Everett BJ. Neotracking in North Carolina: how high school courses of study reproduce race and class-based stratification. Teach Coll Rec. 2008;110(3):535–570 [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Welner K, Oakes J. (Li)ability grouping: the new susceptibility of school tracking systems to legal challenges. Harv Educ Rev. 1996;66(3):451–470 [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Wells AS, Crain RL. Perpetuation theory and the long-term effects of school desegregation. Rev Educ Res. 1994;64(4):531–555 [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Meier KJ, Stewart J, Jr, England RE. Race, Class, and Education: The Politics of Second-Generation Discrimination. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press; 1989 [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Oakes J. Multiplying Inequalities: The Effects of Race, Social Class, and Ability Grouping on Opportunities to Learn Mathematics and Science. Santa Monica, CA: RAND; 1990. Available at: http://www.rand.org/pubs/reports/2006/R3928.pdf. Accessed July 27, 2009 [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Wells AS, Oakes J. Potential pitfalls of systemic reform: early lessons from research on detracking. Sociol Educ. 1996;69(extra issue):135–143 [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kandel DB, Kiros G-E, Schaffran C, Hu M- C. Racial/ethnic differences in cigarette smoking initiation and progression to daily smoking: a multilevel analysis. Am J Public Health. 2004;94(1):128–135 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Walsemann KM, Gee GC, Geronimus AT. Ethnic differences in trajectories of depressive symptoms: disadvantage in family background, high school experiences, and adult characteristics. J Health Soc Behav. 2009;50(1):82–98 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Walsemann KM, Geronimus AT, Gee GC. Accumulating disadvantage over the life course: evidence from a longitudinal study investigating the relationship between educational advantage in youth and health in middle-age. Res Aging. 2008;30(2):169–199 [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Hoffmann JP. Extracurricular activities, athletic participation, and adolescent alcohol use: gender-differentiated and school-contextual effects. J Health Soc Behav. 2006;47(3):275–290 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Johnson RA, Hoffmann JP. Adolescent cigarette smoking in U.S. racial/ethnic subgroups: findings from the National Education Longitudinal Study. J Health Soc Behav. 2000;41(4):392–407 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Borrell LN, Jacobs DR, Jr, Williams DR, Pletcher MJ, Houston TK, Kiefe CI. Self-reported racial discrimination and substance use in the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Adults Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2007;166(9):1068–1079 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Bennett GG, Wolin KY, Robinson EL, Fowler S, Edwards CL. Perceived racial/ethnic harassment and tobacco use among African American young adults. Am J Public Health. 2005;95(2):238–240 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Paradies Y. A systematic review of empirical research on self-reported racism and health. Int J Epidemiol. 2006;35(4):888–901 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Martin JK, Tuch SA, Roman PM. Problem drinking patterns among African Americans: the impacts of reports of discrimination, perceptions of prejudice, and “risky” coping strategies. J Health Soc Behav. 2003;44(3):408–425 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Yen IH, Ragland DR, Greiner BA, Fisher JM. Workplace discrimination and alcohol consumption: findings from the San Francisco Muni Health and Safety Study. Ethn Dis. 1999;9(1):70–80 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Yen IH, Ragland DR, Greiner BA, Fisher JM. Racial discrimination and alcohol-related behavior in urban transit operators: findings from the San Francisco Muni Health and Safety Study. Public Health Rep. 1999;114(5):448–458 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Guthrie BJ, Young AM, Williams DR, Boyd CJ, Kintner EK. African American girls' smoking habits and day-to-day experiences with racial discrimination. Nurs Res. 2002;51(3):183–190 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Moody J. Race, school integration, and friendship segregation in America. Am J Sociol. 2001;107(3):679–716 [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Yonezawa S, Wells AS, Serna I. Choosing tracks: “freedom of choice” in detracking schools. Am Educ Res J. 2002;39(1):37–67 [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Bachman JG, Wallace JM, Jr, O'Malley PM, Johnston LD, Kurth CL, Neighbors HW. Racial/ethnic differences in smoking, drinking, and illicit drug use among American high school seniors, 1976–89. Am J Public Health. 1991;81(3):372–377 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Blum RW, Beuhring T, Shew ML, Bearinger LH, Sieving RE, Resnick MD. The effects of race/ethnicity, income, and family structure on adolescent risk behaviors. Am J Public Health. 2000;90(12):1879–1884 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Ellickson PL, Orlando M, Tucker JS, Klein DJ. From adolescence to young adulthood: racial/ethnic disparities in smoking. Am J Public Health. 2004;94(2):293–299 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Oakes J. Keeping Track: How Schools Structure Inequality. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 1985 [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Rubin BC. Detracking in context: how local constructions of ability complicate equity-geared reform. Teach Coll Rec. 2008;110(3):646–699 [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Murphy J, Hallinger P. Equity as access to learning: curricular and instructional treatment differences. J Curriculum Stud. 1989;21(2):129–149 [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Oakes J, Gamoran A, Page R. Curriculum differentiation: opportunities, outcomes, and meanings. : Jackson P, Handbook of Research on Curriculum. New York, NY: Macmillan; 1992:570–608 [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Lewis SK, Ross CE, Mirowsky J. Establishing a sense of personal control in the transition to adulthood. Soc Forces. 1999;77:1573–1599 [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Gore S, Aseltine RH., Jr Race and ethnic differences in depressed mood following the transition from high school. J Health Soc Behav. 2003;44(3):370–389 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Ross CE, Wu C-l. The links between education and health. Am Sociol Rev. 1995;60(5):719–745 [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Harris KM, Halpern CT, Whitsel E, et al. 2009 The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health: Research Design. Chapel Hill: Carolina Population Center, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill; 2009. Available at: http://www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth/design. Accessed July 27, 2009 [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Muller C, Pearson J, Riegle-Crumb C, et al. National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health: Wave III Data: Design and Implementation of the Adolescent Health and Academic Achievement Study. Chapel Hill: Carolina Population Center, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill; 2007. Available at: http://www.laits.utexas.edu/ahaa/docs/StudyDesign_2007.pdf. Accessed July 27, 2009 [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Weinberg DH, Iceland J, Steinmetz E. Racial and Ethnic Residential Segregation in the United States, 1980–2000. Census 2000 Special Reports Washington DC: US Census Bureau; 2002. Available at: http://www.census.gov/hhes/www/housing/housing_patterns/pdf/paa_paper.pdf. Accessed July 27, 2009 [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Planty M, Provasnik S, Daniel B. High School Coursetaking: Findings From the Condition of Education 2007. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics; 2007. NCES 2007-065. Available at: http://nces.ed.gov/pubs2007/2007065.pdf. Accessed July 27, 2009 [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Mau W-C, Bikos LH. Educational and vocational aspirations of minority and female students: a longitudinal study. J Couns Dev. 2000;78(2):186–194 [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Stata [computer program] Version 10.0. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP; 2007 [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Pampel FC. Differences in the influence of family background and social activities on smoking of minority and White high school seniors, 1976–2004. J Immigr Minor Health. 2008;10(6):507–515 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Chang ES, Chen C, Greenberger E, Dooley D, Heckhausen J. What do they want in life?: the life goals of a multi-ethnic, multi-generational sample of high school seniors. J Youth Adolesc. 2006;35(3):302–313 [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Karlamangla AS, Singer BH, Williams DR, et al. Impact of socioeconomic status on longitudinal accumulation of cardiovascular risk in young adults: the CARDIA Study (USA). Soc Sci Med. 2005;60(5):999–1015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Hayward MD, Miles TP, Crimmins EM, Yang Y. The significance of socioeconomic status in explaining the racial gap in chronic health conditions. Am Sociol Rev. 2000;65(6):910–930 [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Kiefe CI, Williams OD, Lewis CE, Allison JJ, Sekar P, Wagenknecht LE. Ten-year changes in smoking among young adults: are racial differences explained by socioeconomic factors in the CARDIA study? Am J Public Health. 2001;91(2):213–218 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Gee G, Walsemann K. Does health predict reports of racial discrimination or do reports of discrimination predict health? Findings from the National Longitudinal Study of Youth. Soc Sci Med. 2009;68(9):1676–1684 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Pavalko EK, Mossakowski KN, Hamilton VJ. Does perceived discrimination affect health? Longitudinal relationships between work discrimination and women's physical and emotional health. J Health Soc Behav. 2003;44(1):18–33 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Brown TN, Williams DR, Jackson JS, et al. “Being Black and feeling blue”: the mental health consequences of racial discrimination. Race Soc. 2000;2(2):117–131 [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Wells AS, Holme JJ, Revilla AT, Atanda AK. How desegregation changed us: the effects of racially mixed schools on students and society. Teachers College, Columbia University, and University of California, Los Angeles: 2005. Available at: http://faculty.tc.columbia.edu/upload/asw86/ASWells041504.pdf. Accessed July 27, 2009 [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Rumberger RW, Palardy GJ. Does segregation still matter? The impact of student composition on academic achievement in high school. Teach Coll Rec. 2005;107(9):1999–2045 [Google Scholar]

Articles from American Journal of Public Health are provided here courtesy of American Public Health Association

RESOURCES