Abstract
Background
The majority of depressed adults exhibit dysregulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis, including cortisol hyperreactivity to psychosocial challenge. In contrast, remarkably little is known about HPA axis activity in response to psychosocial challenge among at-risk children and adolescents. This study examined cortisol response to psychosocial challenge in nondepressed, at-risk, dysphoric and control, non-dysphoric youth across different developmentally salient age groups (preschool, 3rd, 6th, and 9th graders).
Methods
Two samples of youth (Study 1—preschoolers; Study 2—3rd, 6th, and 9th graders) without a history of clinical depression were administered developmentally appropriate psychosocial challenges. Of these non-depressed children, we examined youth at high-risk (n = 60) and low-risk (n = 223) status, as defined by elevated, but sub-threshold, dysphoric symptoms according to multiple informants. Cortisol levels were assessed before and after psychosocial stressor.
Results
Non-dysphoric control youth at all ages displayed the expected cortisol rise to challenge followed by return to baseline. However, pre-pubertal at-risk, dysphoric children, specifically preschoolers and 3rd graders, exhibited cortisol hyporeactivity to challenge, whereas post-pubertal dysphoric adolescents (9th graders) displayed hyperreactivity to the stressor. Additional analyses revealed that this switch from cortisol hyporeactivity to hyperreactivity among at-risk, dysphoric youth occurred as a function of pubertal development.
Conclusions
Findings suggest a developmental switch in cortisol response for at-risk, dysphoric youth from preschool through adolescence and have implications for a developmental pathophysiological understanding of how at-risk youth across the lifespan may develop depressive disorder.
Keywords: Stress, Cortisol, Depression, Children
INTRODUCTION
Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a prevalent, costly psychiatric disorder, a serious public health concern, and leading cause of disability worldwide (1). Since Carlson and Cantwell (2) “unmasked” masked depression, young children (ages 3–6) have been shown to meet diagnostic criteria for MDD (3). Rates of depression then increase from childhood into adolescence (4,5). Most individuals experience first MDD onset during adolescence, and subsequently many adults experience recurrent episodes (6). Developing effective interventions, especially for those most at risk, is a clear priority. It is important to identify risk factors and pathophysiological processes contributing to depression. As many mechanisms may be in place before first MDD onset, examining physiologic mechanisms prior to MDD onset may be particularly fruitful.
Dysregulation of the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis (HPA) axis is a robust biomarker for depression among many adults (7,8,9), who show cortisol hyperreactivity in response to psychosocial stressor (e.g., Trier Social Stress Test, TSST (10)). Psychosocial challenge paradigms have the advantage of testing endogenous activity of the whole HPA system7 and may better mimic effects of stress exposure, which predicts depression onset and maintenance.
Surprisingly little research has investigated whether cortisol levels are significantly different between dysphoric and control youth in response to psychosocial lab stressor. Indeed, only 3 papers used a psychosocial laboratory challenge to investigate cortisol levels in depressed versus control youth (11). Using the same sample of preschoolers, Luby and colleagues (3,12) demonstrated cortisol reactivity differences across an extensive laboratory challenge between depressed and non-depressed preschoolers with what appear to be lower pre-challenge levels in depressed preschoolers. Rao and colleagues (13) showed cortisol hyperreactivity in depressed adolescents compared with healthy controls, as is typical in adult work.
The primary aim of this study was to examine cortisol reactivity to developmentally appropriate psychosocial challenges in youth, across a wide age range, who had never been depressed but were at high-risk for depression by virtue of elevated dysphoria. Dysphoric (i.e., subthreshold for MDD, but not clinically depressed) youth are at particular risk for developing later MDD (14). By studying at-risk youth who have never experienced depression across different developmental periods, this study can advance knowledge about potential pathophysiological processes contributing to MDD onset without concern about confounds of current or prior MDD.
We hypothesized that normal children would exhibit an increase in cortisol levels in response to psychosocial stressor (i.e., reactivity) and then would return to baseline (i.e., recovery). Dysphoric children were hypothesized to exhibit dysregulated HPA axis activity. Given wide age ranges of prior research (3, 12, 13), we recruited children of different ages, including preschoolers, 3rd, 6th, and 9th graders, to systematically examine whether developmental level affected cortisol levels among dysphoric and non-dysphoric youth. Recent research with healthy youth revealed changes in cortisol reactivity to laboratory-stressor around puberty (15,16). We hypothesized that younger dysphoric children (e.g., preschoolers) may display cortisol hyporeactivity in response to psychosocial stressor based on our interpretation of data from Luby and colleagues (3), whereas older dysphoric adolescents (9th graders) would exhibit cortisol hyperreactivity (13).
Study 1
Methods and Materials
Participants were 81 preschoolers (53% female; M age = 4.2 years) recruited as part of a larger project from community child care centers. Preschoolers were excluded from this subsample if they had a diagnosed pervasive developmental disability (e.g. autism), were taking medications for asthma or allergy, or were currently ill.
Assessment of Depression Risk
Mothers completed the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/ 1½ – 5/LDS (17)) and a background health questionnaire on their child to screen for current illness and medication use. Lead teachers from the child’s classroom also completed the teacher report form of the CBCL.
Cortisol Collection and Stress Tasks
Preschoolers participated in a 20-minute semi-structured interaction with their primary caregiver designed to elicit a mild stress-reaction during a 1-hour home visit. Children were asked to not eat or drink for 20 minutes prior to the visit. The interaction was adapted from the NICHD Early Child Care Research Network three boxes task (18). During the interaction, the dyad was instructed to play with four different sets of materials for five-minutes each in the following order: a set of books as a warm-up activity, two hand puppets to elicit child-directed play, a scary robot to elicit fear, and a box with an attractive toy that was difficult to open to elicit frustration.
Three saliva samples were collected in 20-min intervals: at baseline before the challenge (immediately after arriving at the home), at the end of the psychosocial challenge reflecting possible initial cortisol reactivity, and after quiet play time/ rest reflecting possible recovery. Importantly, individuals vary in the timing of both their initial response to a challenge and to the length of time it takes to recover after mounting a physiologic response to a stressor, therefore these labels of “reactivity” and “recovery” are simply convenient labels of expected average responses. Saliva samples were obtained via synthetic salivette collection devices without the use of stimulants (Sarstedt, Nuembrecht, Germany). Saliva was extracted by centerfuging for 4 minutes at 2500 RPM. Vials and salivettes were frozen at −20° C until data collection was complete. Samples were sent to the Biochemical Laboratory, Psychobiology, University of Trier, Germany to be assayed. All samples were assayed in duplicate. Cortisol levels were determined by employing a competitive solid phase time-resolved fluorescence immunoassay with fluorometric end point detection (DELFIA (19)). The mean intra-assay coefficients of variation (CV) for blind controls of real saliva of high and low concentration were 6.6% to 8.5%. For duplicates of the samples used in this study and in study 2, the inter-assay CV was 6.5%.
Results
As indicated in Table 1, of the 81 preschool participants, 18 (22.2%) had combined parent and teacher reported anxious/depressed scores from the internalizing dimension of the CBCL that fell within the clinical range (t scores > 70). Of these, 8 were boys and 10 were girls. Chi-square revealed no sex difference in dysphoric group (p = .51). Still, given the small sample, the main analysis was conducted both controlling and not controlling for sex. Cortisol values were not normally distributed and exhibited negative skew for all three time points, so ln transformations were used for analyses. There was considerable variation in scheduled time of home visits (M = 15:48; range = 8:15 to 20:15). A one-way ANOVA revealed no differences between dysphoric groups in sampling time, F (1, 79) = .27, p = .61. Still, to be conservative all analyses were also recomputed controlling for time of first saliva sample. To examine potential effects of sleep on cortisol values, half of the preschoolers (36 non-dysphoric, 5 dysphoric) were fitted with actigraphy watches. Average time of first saliva sample from nearest awakening time (i.e. morning wake up or afternoon nap) was 3.51 hours (range = .90 to 6.80) for the non-dysphoric group and 3.05 hours (range = 2.05 to 3.63) for the dysphoric group.
Table 1.
Demographic and clinical characteristics of never depressed dyshoric and non-dysphoric children and adolescents
Dysphoric Group | Non-dysphoric control group | |
---|---|---|
Preschoolers (n = 81) | (n = 16) | (n = 65) |
Age | 4.47 (.70) | 4.13 (.72) |
Sex | 63% girls | 51% girls |
Ethnicity | 27% white/ not Hispanic | 44% white/ not Hispanic |
CBCL-P,T | P: 4.83 (2.37)/ T: 7.25 (2.92) | P: 1.72 (1.48)*/ T: 1.63 (1.93)* |
3rd Graders (n = 66) | (n = 12) | (n = 54) |
Age | 8.43 (.64) | 8.75 (.49) |
Sex | 57% girls | 53% girls |
Ethnicity | 66% white/ not Hispanic | 65% white/ not Hispanic |
CDI-C,P | 15.91 (4.37) | 4.34 (2.89) * |
6th Graders (n = 70) | (n = 15) | (n = 55) |
Age | 11.41 (.67) | 11.73 (.52) |
Sex | 64% girls | 53% girls |
Ethnicity | 65% white/ not Hispanic | 68% white/ not Hispanic |
CDI-C,P | 14.4 (3.12) | 4.46 (3.08) * |
9th Graders (n = 69) | (n = 17) | (n = 52) |
Age | 14.50 (1.04) | 14.30 (.54) |
Sex | 62.5% girls | 60.5% girls |
Ethnicity | 58% white/ not Hispanic | 56% white/ not Hispanic |
CDI-C,P | 15.19 (5.95) | 4.86 (3.06) * |
Note: CBCL=Child Behavior Checklist; CDI=Child Depression Inventory. P=Parent report; T=Teacher report; C=Child report.
p < .05
To examine the primary hypothesis, we employed a 2 (dysphoric group) × 3 (cortisol time point: baseline, reactivity, recovery) repeated measures Analysis of Variance (ANOVA). The time X dysphoric group interaction was significant, F (2,73) = 3.72, p = .03, and results were unchanged controlling for sex and sampling time, F (2, 71) = 3.63, p = .03. As illustrated in Figure 1, dysphoric preschoolers demonstrated hyporeactivity to the challenge, with significantly lower cortisol 20-minutes into the challenge, but similar cortisol to non-dysphoric preschoolers at baseline and recovery. Our first sample should be a true baseline sample, as it reflects cortisol prior to the arrival of the research team to the home. However, to verify that dysphoric and non-dysphoric children did not differ at baseline, we also compared their average baseline cortisol available from the larger study from which these data were drawn. For each child we selected their closest baseline sample matched for the type of day (weekend vs. weekday) and time (morning versus afternoon) they completed their home visit. The non-dysphoric (mean = .13) and dysphoric groups (mean = .13) did not differ on this true baseline cortisol measure (p = .95).
Figure 1.
Mean (±SEM) cortisol response to psychosocial challenge among preschoolers in Study 1 as a function of at-risk, dysphoric group and control, non-dysphoric group.
To determine whether children among the dysphoric group with greater dysphoria might be driving our results, we also compared cortisol patterns between the 9 dysphoric children with the lower CBCL scores (mean = 15.8, range = 14 to 17) to the 9 dysphoric children with the higher CBCL scores (mean = 24.7, range = 20 to 38). Both groups displayed an overall flat patterning of cortisol across the interaction. Therefore, while the more dysphoric preschoolers may have especially low cortisol, both groups of dysphoric preschoolers were hyporesponsive to the challenge.
Study 2
Methods and Materials
Children and adolescents in 3rd, 6th, and 9th grades were recruited through public schools. Interested parents called the laboratory and responded to a brief phone screen that established that both the parent and the child were fluent in English, and the child did not carry an autism spectrum or psychotic disorder diagnosis and had an IQ > 70. At the laboratory, parents gave consent, and children gave assent. A total of 246 youth in 3rd, 6th, or 9th grades participated, and 205 did not have a current or past diagnosis of depression. These never depressed youth comprised the sample that was the primary focus of Study 2. Third graders were 52% female; M age = 8.7 years, 6th graders were 62% female; M age = 11.7 years, and 9th graders were 58% female; M age = 14.3 years.
Assessment of Depression and Diagnostic Interviews
Mothers and youth were interviewed separately with the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (K-SADS (20)). Of the total sample of 246, 7 youth met criteria for current MDD (2.8%) and 34 for prior MDD (13.8%); none met criteria for any bipolar or psychotic disorder. Diagnostic interviewers, who were advanced graduate students in clinical psychology, completed an intensive training program for administering the K-SADS interview and for assigning DSM-IV diagnoses. The training program consisted of attending a minimum of 40 hours of didactic instruction, listening to audiotaped interviews, and conducting practice interviews. The first author (BLH) held weekly supervision sessions for the interviewers and reviewed interviewers’ notes and tapes in order to confirm the presence or absence of a diagnosis. To assess interrater reliability, an independent trained rater, with numerous years of experience conducting diagnostic interviews and who was blind to parent or child diagnostic status, randomly evaluated 20% of the K-SADS interviews. Reliability was good (κ=.87), and presence/absence of current MDD in youth was confirmed.
Also during the laboratory visit, both parent and child completed the Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI-C for Child and CDI-P for parent informant (21)). The CDI is a self-report measure of general dysphoria with good reliability and validity for children aged 8–17 (21,22), although it does not enable depression diagnosis and most accurately assesses dysphoria and broad negative affect (22).
Pubertal Development
All youth were administered the Pubertal Development Scale (PDS (23)), which includes five questions about physical development, scored from 1 (no) to 4 (development complete). Reliability and validity of the PDS is high (23,24), including evidence that PDS scores are strongly associated with physical exam for pubertal development (24). We followed standard PDS scoring to create prepubertal and postpubertal groups separately for girls and boys.
Cortisol Collection and Stress Tasks
The majority of 3rd, 6th, and 9th grade participants (n = 129; 52%) completed questionnaires and K-SADS interview during the first 60 minutes. The other youth (n = 117; 48%) completed K-SADS interview after the stress challenge and collection of cortisol samples. Timing of K-SADS did not affect any results. After an hour in the laboratory, youth participated in an approximately 15–20 minute psychosocial challenge. This included a 5–10 minute parent-child discussion concerning a recent fight or argument. Then, youth auditioned for a “reality TV show” by giving a speech directly into a video camera; youth were instructed that judges would evaluate their performance. As there is no current consensus on stress/challenge paradigms that elicit cortisol reactivity across different ages (25), we selected this laboratory challenge task as it was likely to be developmentally appropriate across 3rd, 6th, and 9th graders, and it involved the essential elements (e.g., threat of social rejection and social evaluation, anticipatory and processive stress) known to activate cortisol response across the lifespan (26). For 45 minutes after the challenge, youth quietly completed additional questionnaires. Youth were debriefed at the end of the lab visit. On average, the majority of the first cortisol samples were collected at 5PM (range 4 PM-6:30 PM). Cortisol collection intervals, assay procedures, and coefficients of variation were identical to that of Study 1.
Results
We used the CDI (child and parent ratings) to form groups of at-risk, dysphoric children and non-dysphoric healthy controls. As indicated in Table 1, 44 of the 205 (21.4%) never clinically depressed youth from 3rd, 6th, and 9th grades had CDI scores in the at-risk, dysphoric range (21). Across ages, the nondysphoric and dysphoric groups did not differ on any demographic variable. Overall, 49% of the mothers reported a prior history of clinical depression, as ascertained via the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID (27)).
Stress reactivity, depression level, and age group
Cortisol values were not normally distributed and exhibited negative skew for all three time points, so ln transformations were used for analyses, and Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were reported where sphericity was evident. There was a main effect of sex on cortisol levels, F(1, 204) = 4.19, p =.04 (boys M = .068, SE = .004; girls M = .058, SE = .003), but no significant sex effect across time, F(1.62, 325.21) = .02, p = .96. Moreover, sex did not moderate any of the findings reported next. Likewise, there was no main effect of mothers’ history of depression on cortisol levels, F(1, 204) = .29, p =.59, or across time, F(1.61, 380.91) = .01, p = .98, and maternal depression history did not moderate any findings.
To examine the primary hypotheses, we employed a 3 (age group) × 2 (dysphoric group) × 3 (cortisol time point) repeated measures ANOVA. The 3-way interaction was significant, F(3.21, 314.94) = 3.21, p = .02. Planned follow-up analyses to decompose the 3-way interaction involved repeated measures ANOVAs for 2 (dysphoric group) × 3 (cortisol time point) within each age group. This 2 × 3 repeated measures ANOVA was significant for the 3rd graders, F(1.88, 139.53) = 3.83, p = .02, was not significant for 6th graders, F(1.38, 114.53) = .97, p = .36, and was significant for 9th graders, F(1.38, 114.53) = 3.29, p = .04. Figure 2 shows that the overall stress response was quadratic, with increases from baseline to reactivity followed by decreases from reactivity to recovery. This quadratic component was significant for 3rd graders F(1, 65) = 7.32, p = .008, was not significant for 6th graders, F(1, 69) = .61, p = .43, and was significant for 9th graders, F(1, 68) = 7.66, p = .007).
Figure 2.
Mean (±SEM) cortisol response to modified TSST among 3rd, 6th, and 9th graders in Study 2 as a function of at-risk, dysphoric group and control, non-dysphoric group.
As illustrated in Figure 2, dysphoric 3rd graders exhibited cortisol hyporeactivity to the stress challenge, while the healthy control youth displayed the expected quadratic cortisol response. The 6th grade dysphoric youth displayed a trend toward hyporeactivity, although the dysphoric group’s pattern did not significantly differ from the controls who exhibited the expected quadratic pattern. Finally, and in contrast to the younger children, the 9th grade dysphoric group exhibited significantly greater cortisol hyperreactivity in response to the psychosocial stressor compared with the healthy control youth, who displayed the expected quadratic cortisol response. Neither temperament (28) nor recent stressors (29) showed this pattern, suggesting that significant results for at-risk, dysphoric youth cannot be explained by temperament or recent stress (see Supplement 1).
Given these results suggesting a developmental switch from cortisol hyporeactivity in 3rd grade to hyperreactivity in 9th grade for the at-risk, dysphoric group and the non-significant pattern in 6th graders, we explored whether pubertal development (prepubertal vs. postpubertal as defined by PDS), rather than grade, operated as a more appropriate developmental marker affecting cortisol response to psychological stress in at-risk, dysphoric youth compared with control youth. The 2 (pubertal group) × 2 (dysphoric group) × 3 (cortisol time point) repeated measures ANOVA was significant, F(1.79, 275.27) = 4.73, p = .01. The 2 (dysphoric group) × 3 (cortisol time point) for prepubertal youth approached significance F(1.92, 144.12) = 2.75, p = .07, and this 2 × 3 ANOVA was significant for the postpubertal youth, F(1.58, 123.49) = 4.02, p = .03. The quadratic component was significant for prepubertal, F(1, 101) = 6.21, p = .01, and postpubertal youth, F(1, 102) = 5.81, p = .01. Figure 3 shows that nondysphoric, control youth, regardless of pubertal status, exhibited the expected quadratic cortisol response. Of interest, at-risk, dysphoric prepubertal youth displayed hyporeactivity (cortisol levels decreased from baseline to reactivity and recovery), and at-risk postpubertal youth exhibited hyperreactivity from baseline to reactivity and then return to baseline at recovery.
Figure 3.
Mean (±SEM) cortisol response to modified TSST among prepubertal and postpubertal youth in Study 2 as a function of at-risk, dysphoric group and control, non-dysphoric group.
Finally, we examined whether youth with current MDD (n=7) exhibited cortisol hyperreactivity to the psychological stressor, as would be expected given research with adults and limited work with depressed adolescents (15). The 2 (pubertal group) × 2 (currently depressed group) × 3 (cortisol time point) repeated measures ANOVA was significant, F(1.76, 326.7) = 9.22, p < .001. As shown in Figure 4, prepubertal youth with current depression exhibited significantly greater cortisol before the challenge compared with nondepressed youth, but no difference in reactivity or recovery. In contrast, the postpubertal currently depressed youth revealed the expected cortisol hyperreactivity compared to nondepressed youth. Last, parallel analyses with remitted depressed youth showed no significant effect in the 2 (pubertal group) × 2 (remitted depressed group) × 3 (cortisol time point) ANOVA: F(1.78, 318.73) = .19, p =.80. This is consistent with work with adult populations showing normalization in cortisol reactivity following MDD remittance.
Figure 4.
Mean (±SEM) cortisol response to modified TSST among prepubertal and postpubertal youth in Study 2 as a function current clinical depression and nondepressed group.
General Discussion
This study investigated cortisol response to a psychosocial stressor in never depressed youth, who are at risk for depression based on subthreshold dysphoria, from preschool through middle adolescence. Results showed that non-dysphoric youth, from preschool through adolescence, exhibited the expected quadratic cortisol response to psychosocial challenge. For these control youth, cortisol levels increased 20 minutes after the challenge (i.e., reactivity) and then returned to baseline 40 minutes later (i.e., recovery). This mild response is appropriate for the challenges posed and provides youth with an extra boost of energy to face the challenge without extended exposure to elevated steroid hormones. In contrast, the at-risk dysphoric groups displayed HPA axis dysregulation, and the particular form of dysregulation depended on developmental level. Younger dysphoric children (i.e., preschoolers and 3rd graders) exhibited cortisol hyporeactivity, whereas the dysphoric adolescents (9th graders) displayed cortisol hyperreactivity. This developmental switch from blunted cortisol response to hyperreactivity to challenge was explained by pubertal stage, such that prepubertal at-risk children exhibited hyporeactivity, and the postpubertal at-risk youth displayed hyperreactivity to challenge. Findings advance knowledge of youths’ biological response to stressful challenges and how this response differs across development between at-risk dysphoric and non-dysphoric youth.
These results are consistent with and extend the limited prior research studying cortisol response to challenge with currently clinically depressed (13) or healthy youth (15, 16). Our results, with never depressed, but at-risk dysphoric 9th graders exhibiting cortisol hyperreactivity, expand on the one prior study (13) with currently depressed vs. control adolescents. Additionally, the small group of currently clinically depressed postpubertal youth exhibited cortisol hyperreactivity, replicating this research. Of interest and new are the findings with currently depressed prepubertal youth. These children exhibited higher baseline cortisol prior to challenge, and then no reactivity to the stressor. As there were few currently depressed youth, these findings should be interpreted cautiously and require replication. Last, we found that the group of dysphoric preschoolers exhibited cortisol hyporeactivity, possibly consistent with Luby and colleagues’ work (3,12) using different tasks and timing with clinically depressed preschoolers.
Taken together, our findings are consistent with recent work with healthy youth showing that HPA-axis activity increases with age, particularly after the pubertal transition (15,16). Our findings, both with currently clinically depressed and nondepressed, at-risk youth, suggest that there is a developmental shift in the pattern of HPA axis activity with increases in cortisol among adolescents compared to children, and this normative HPA axis increase among healthy youth may be exacerbated among at-risk, dysphoric as well as currently depressed individuals.
Surprisingly little research has investigated HPA axis activity throughout different developmental periods, especially in childhood and adolescence (26). The adolescent brain and HPA axis response may be more reactive to stress and associated glucocorticoid exposure. In animals, adolescent rats exhibit elevated release of adrenocorticotropic hormone and glucocorticoids after repeated stress exposure (30). In humans, potentiated basal and stress-induced HPA axis activity starts around mid-puberty (15,16). These developmental shifts in heightened HPA axis activity from childhood into adolescence may be related to the substantial increase in sex hormones during puberty as sex steroids affect HPA axis activity (31).
These normative changes in HPA axis activity during the pubertal transition may be important for understanding the surge in depression observed from adolescence into young adulthood (4,5). The developmental shift in cortisol response may inform a developmental pathophysiological perspective on how at-risk individuals at different salient age periods may become depressed. Hyperreactivity to psychosocial challenge in dysphoric adolescents is also observed in depressed adults (9). These patterns are consistent with strong continuity from adolescent-onset depression into adulthood. However, less continuity is observed between child-onset and adult depression (6), and the processes contributing to child-onset depression differ from adolescent and adult-onset depression (32). The adolescent transition comes with a host of changes, including desire for greater autonomy from parents and a dramatic increase in stressor exposure (33). Individuals begin to experience substantial increases in stress starting in adolescence, when most people first become depressed (33). At-risk, post-pubertal adolescents, who display cortisol hyperreactivity to stressful challenges, may be at particular susceptibility to develop depression as they experience more stressors throughout adolescence and have a dysregulated biological stress response system that is less capable of adaptively coping with increasing stress. We speculate that at-risk pre-pubertal children who exhibit cortisol hyporeactivity to challenge may be more susceptible to depression because they have a less responsive stress system and, as a result, have more difficulty increasing energy to meet challenges and sufficiently cope with stress. Alternatively, young children with early depressive symptoms may be experiencing chronic stress that they are unable to change or escape and are exhibiting HPA-axis suppression as a result of early chronic exposure. Future research would benefit from longitudinal research examining whether the developmental switch in cortisol reactivity occurring with puberty, as found in the current cross-sectional study, can be observed to change in individuals as they progress through pubertal development, as well as accumulating stress exposure over time.
Study strengths include developmentally appropriate psychosocial challenges to test for cortisol reactivity from early childhood through adolescence. Also, studying at-risk, dysphoric youth, who were never depressed, ensures that results are not confounded by depression history. Additional research is needed to examine whether disturbed HPA axis function predicts development of MDD. Moreover, investigating developmentally equivalent, time-limited laboratory stressors that elicit cortisol response across different ages would help to further understand developmental pathophysiology of depression. Our relatively mild challenges sufficiently elicited cortisol responses as well as other ethical tasks used in prior research (e.g., serial subtraction, social competence interview (34); public-speaking task (13)). However, the tasks and procedures differed between Studies 1 and 2, precluding analyses across the two studies. Future research would benefit from greater number of cortisol samplings both before and after challenge. Most studies investigating HPA axis function index cortisol reactivity at approximately 20 minutes post-stressor, as we did. Meta-analytic findings show this is an average time for cortisol reactivity (35), although individual variability in cortisol response over time is likely (36).
Overall, findings highlight developmental factors in understanding the dynamic relation between direction of HPA axis dysregulation and risk to depression over the lifespan. Such knowledge holds promise to inform intervention efforts to reduce occurrence and recurrence of depression among high-risk individuals.
Supplementary Material
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This work was supported, in part, by NICHD grant (1 RO3 HD054718-01) and a grant from the Foundation for Child Development awarded to Sarah Enos Watamura, ACF grant (90YE0091-01) awarded to Lisa S. Badanes, and NIMH grant (5R01 MH077195) awarded to Benjamin L. Hankin and John R.Z. Abela. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Mental Health or National Institutes of Health.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE
The authors declare no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
Contributor Information
Benjamin L. Hankin, University of Denver
Lisa S. Badanes, University of Denver
John R.Z. Abela, Rutgers University
Sarah E. Watamura, University of Denver
References
- 1.Lopez AD, Mathers CD, Ezzati M, Jamison DT, Murray CJL, editors. The World Bank. The Global Burden of Disease and Risk Factors. Washington, DC: Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Carlson GA, Cantwell DP. Unmasking masked depression in children and adolescents. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1980;137:445–449. doi: 10.1176/ajp.137.4.445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Luby JL, Heffelfinger A, Mrakotsky C, Brown K, Hessler M, Spitznagel E. Alterations in stress cortisol reactivity in depressed preschoolers relative to psychiatric and no-disorder comparison groups. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2003;60:1248–1255. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.12.1248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hankin BL, Abramson LY, Moffitt TE, Silva PA, McGee R, Angell KA. Development of depression from preadolescence to young adulthood: Emerging gender differences in a 10 year longitudinal study. J Abnorm Psychology. 1998;107:128–141. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.107.1.128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:593–602. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Rutter M, Kim-Cohen J, Maughan B. Continuities and discontinuities in psychopathology between childhood and adult life. J Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:276–295. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01614.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Burke HM, Davis MC, Otte C, Mohr DC. Depression and cortisol responses to psychological stress: A meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinol. 2005;30:846–856. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Parker KJ, Schatzberg AF, Lyons DM. Neuroendocrine aspects of hypercortisolism in major depression. Horm Behav. 2003;43:60–66. doi: 10.1016/s0018-506x(02)00016-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Bao AM, Menyen G, Swaab DF. The stress system in depression and neurodegeneration: Focus on the human hypothalamus. Brain Res Reviews. 2007;57:531–553. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kirschbaum C, Pirke KM, Hellhammer DH. The “Trier Social Stress Test”—a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology. 1993;28:76–81. doi: 10.1159/000119004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Lopez Duran NL, Kovacs M, George CJ. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis dysregulation in depressed children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinol. 2009;34:1272–1283. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.03.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Luby JL, Mrakotsky C, Heffelfinger A, Brown K, Spitznagel E. Characteristics of depressed preschoolers with and without anhedonia: evidence for a melancholic depressive subtype in young children. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2004;161:1998–2004. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.11.1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rao U, Hammen C, Ortiz LR, Chen LA, Poland RE. Effects of early and recent adverse experiences on adrenal response to psychosocial stress in depressed adolescents. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;64:521–526. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.05.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Klein DN, Shankman SA, Lewinsohn PM, Seeley JR. Subthreshold depressive disorder in adolescents: Predictors of escalation to full-syndrome depressive disorders. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2009;48:703–710. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181a56606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Gunnar MR, Wewerka S, Frenn K, Long JD, Griggs C. Developmental changes in hypothalamus pituitary-adrenal activity over the transition to adolescence: normative changes and associations with puberty. Dev. Psychopathol. 2009;21:69–85. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Stroud LR, Foster E, Papandonatos GD, Handwerger K, Granger DA, Kivlighan KT, Niaura R. Stress response and the adolescent transition: Performance versus peer rejection stressors. Dev. Psychopathol. 2009;21:47–68. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Achenbach TM. Child Behavior Checklist/ 1 ½-5. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Dept. of Psychiatry; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 18.NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Early child care and self-control, compliance and problem behavior at twenty-four and thirty-six months. Child Dev. 1998;69:1145–1170. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Martina H, Sabine K, Gerhard B. Adaptation of DELFIA cortisol kit for determination of salivary cortisol concentration. Archiv der Pharmazie. 2005;338(10):493–497. doi: 10.1002/ardp.200500116. 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, Williamson D, Ryan N. Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children- Present and Lifetime version (K-SADS-PL), initial reliability and validity data. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 1997;36:980–988. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kovacs M. The Children's Depression Inventory (CDI) Psychopharmacological Bulletin. 1985;21:995–998. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Klein DN, Dougherty LR, Olino TM. Toward guidelines for evidence-based assessment of depression in children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2005;34:412–432. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3403_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Petersen AC, Crockett LJ, Richards M, Boxer A. A self-report measure of pubertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 1988;17:117–133. doi: 10.1007/BF01537962. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Shirtcliff EA, Dahl RE, Pollak SD. Pubertal development: Correspondence between hormonal and physical development. Child Development. 2008;80(2):327–337. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01263.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Gunnar MR, Talge NM, Herrera A. Stressor paradigms in developmental studies: What does and does not work to produce mean increases in salivary cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(7):953–967. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behavior, and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10:434–445. doi: 10.1038/nrn2639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Disorders. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Capaldi DM, Rothbart MK. Development and validation of an early adolescent temperament measure. Journal of Early Adolescence. 1992;12:153–173. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Rudolph KD, Flynn M. Childhood adversity and youth depression: The role of gender and pubertal status. Development and Psychopathology. 2007;19:497–521. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407070241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Romeo RD, et al. Stress history and pubertal development interact to shape hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis plasticity. Endocrinology. 2006;147:1664–1674. doi: 10.1210/en.2005-1432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.McCormick CM, Mathews IZ. HPA function in adolescence: role of sex hormones in its regulation and the enduring consequences of exposure to stressors. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2007;86:220–233. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Kaufman J, Martin A, King RA, Charney D. Are child-, adolescent-, and adult-onset depression one and the same disorder? Biol Psychiatry. 2001;49:980–1001. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01127-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hankin BL, Abramson LY. Development of gender differences in depression: An elaborated cognitive vulnerability- transactional stress theory. Psychol Bull. 2001;127:773–796. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.6.773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Gotlib IH, Joormann J, Minor KL, Hallmayer HPA axis reactivity: A mechanism underlying the associations among 5-HTTLPR, stress, and depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:847–851. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Dickerson SS, Kemeny ME. Acute stressors and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychol Bull. 2004;130:355–391. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Lopez-Duran NL, Hajal NJ, Olson SL, Felt BT, Vazquez DM. Individual differences in cortisol responses to fear and frustration during middle childhood. J of Exper Child Psychol. 2009;103(3):285–295. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.