Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2010 Apr–Jun;24(2):159–164. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181c9983f

NIA FUNDED ALZHEIMER CENTERS ARE MORE EFFICIENT THAN COMMERCIAL CLINICAL RECRUITMENT SITES FOR PERFORMING SECONDARY PREVENTION TRIALS OF DEMENTIA

SD Edland 1,2, JA Emond 1, PS Aisen 1, RC Petersen 3
PMCID: PMC2922976  NIHMSID: NIHMS167201  PMID: 20505433

Abstract

Study subject dropout compromises clinical trials by reducing statistical power and potentially biasing findings. We use data from a trial of treatments to delay the progression of mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (NEJM 2005;352(23):79–88) to determine predictors of study subject dropout and inform the design and implementation of future trials. Time to study discontinuation was modeled by proportional hazards regression with censoring at incident dementia or trial completion. Of 769 subjects, 230 (30%) discontinued prematurely. Risk of dropout was higher among nonwhites (hazard ratio (HR) 2.1, p=0.0007), subjects with less than college education (HR=1.6, p=0.02), subjects with a Hamilton Depression score of six or more (HR=1.3, p=0.04), unmarried men (HR=2.1 relative to married men, p=0.003) and subjects recruited by commercial clinical sites (HR=2.2 relative to subjects recruited by NIA funded AD research centers, p<0.0001). A trial using commercial sites with the discontinuation rates and incident dementia event rates experienced in this trial would require 80% more subjects than a comparably powered trial using NIA funded AD research center sites. Targeted retention efforts and utilization of academic sites could substantively improve the statistical power and validity of future clinical trials of cognitively impaired elderly.

Keywords: attrition, loss to follow-up, sample size, power, dropout, retention, clinical trial methodology

INTRODUCTION

Missing data is a major concern for all clinical trials. Prevention trials with long-term follow-up are especially vulnerable to missing data due to study subject attrition. Based on the experience of cohort studies, which find that cognitive impairment is a risk factor for study subject attrition1,2, loss of subjects to follow-up may be especially problematic for secondary prevention trials in dementia. As recently reviewed3, this in fact has been the case for most secondary prevention trials performed to date. Moreover, the literature on causes and risk factors for loss to follow-up within the context of clinical trials is limited48. To address these concerns, we investigated predictors of study subject attrition in the Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study (ADCS) trial of donepezil and vitamin E to delay conversion from amnestic MCI to AD (the ADCS MCI trial9). Knowledge of the factors that influence subject attrition may suggest recruitment and retention strategies to improve the validity and statistical power of future trials.

METHODS

Data from the ADCS MCI trial9 were used to examine predictors of loss to follow-up due to death, adverse events, or voluntary subject withdrawal. The trial is described in detail in the report of primary study results (Petersen, et al.9). Briefly, this three-year trial recruited 769 participants from 69 clinical sites in the U.S. and Canada. Inclusion criteria were amnestic MCI of a degenerative nature (insidious onset and gradual progression), impaired memory, a Logical Memory delayed-recall score10 1.5 standard deviations below educated-adjusted norms, a Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR11) scale score of 0.5, a score of 24 to 30 on the mini-mental state examination (MMSE12), an age of 55 to 90 years, a Modified Hachinski score13 of less than or equal to four, and lack of clinically important depression as indicated by a Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HAM-D14) score of less than or equal to 12. Sites were also instructed to only recruit subjects whose general health and physical condition would not preclude them from completing a three-year trial. The recruitment period spanned March 1999 to January 2004. Randomization to one of placebo, 2000 IU vitamin E, or 10 mg donepezil was balanced on MMSE score, age, and APOE e4 status. The study was conducted according to Good Clinical Practice guidelines, the Declaration of Helsinki, and the US Code of Federal Regulations Title 21 Part 50 (Protection of Human Subjects) and Title 21 Part 56 (Institutional Review Boards). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants and study partners. Full cognitive exams for incident dementia were conducted on site at six month intervals for up to 36 months. Incident dementia was diagnosed by site clinicians, with validation by a secondary review committee9. Two-hundred and twelve of 214 incident dementia cases met National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Diseases and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association criteria15 for possible or probable AD.

Subjects who missed scheduled follow-up visits were contacted by site investigators and if they choose to discontinue participation in the study were asked to return for one final cognitive exam. Clinical personnel obtained additional information at the final visit or via the telephone if the subject refused to return. The additional information included predefined questions regarding the reasons for study discontinuation. The predefined reasons queried were concern over placebo condition, length of protocol, travel requirements, frequency of assessments, potential adverse effects of medication, concern about medication effectiveness, alternative treatment chosen by subject, alternative treatment chosen by caregiver, or caregiver unwilling to continue.

The analysis plan for the present investigation consisted of univariate assessments of possible predictors of study discontinuation followed by a model-building exercise to derive a single multivariate predictive model. The outcome variable was time to discontinuation of the closed-label portion of the trial, defined as the time from baseline to the earliest of the date of progression to dementia or the date of last completed cognitive diagnostic assessment. Subjects who did not complete the closed-label portion of the trial were considered events, subjects who converted to dementia were censored at the date of dementia diagnosis, and subjects who completed the trial without converting to dementia were censored at the date of their month 36 assessment.

Univariate assessments included unweighted log-rank tests of overall association of the covariate with time to study discontinuation, descriptive summaries of the proportion discontinuing by level of the covariate, and Cox proportional hazards model estimates of the relative risk of discontinuation by level of the covariate. Variables significantly related to study discontinuation in univariate log-rank tests at the p<0.1 level were selected for inclusion in the final model; variables related at the p=0.10–0.15 level were subsequently tested for inclusion in the final model and selected for inclusion if likelihood ratio tests to add the term were significant at the p<0.05 level or if there was evidence of confounding by the variable. A covariate was considered to have a mediating or confounding effect if its inclusion in the model altered the model coefficients for other covariate terms by greater than 15% (Hosmer et al.16). Interactions with treatment arm were assessed for all variables in the final model and tested with likelihood ratio tests with a Bonferonni adjusted p-value for an overall significance level of 0.05. Interactions significant at the p<0.05 level were included in the final model. The proportional hazards assumption was checked for all term in the final model by Schoenfeld residuals tests using the log transform of time17. To investigate the extent to which observed associations were driven by deaths and other serious adverse events, we performed two sensitivity analyses, first refitting the final model with censoring on death, and again refitting the final model with censoring on both death and discontinuation due to other serious adverse events. The distribution of continuous variables was compared using the Wilcoxon signed rank test and the frequency of categorical variables was compared using Fisher’s exact test, as indicated. All analyses were conducted using the R statistical programming language18, with log-rank tests and Cox modeling performed using the R library ‘survival’.

Covariates considered included the screening measurements of Logical Memory, MMSE, modified Hachinski scale, and HAM-D, as well as measurements of activities of daily living using the ADCS Mild Cognitive Impairment Activities of Daily Living Scale19, BMI (weight in kilograms divided by the square of height in meters), the Clinical Dementia Rating scale sum of boxes (CDR sum of boxes20), and the Global Deterioration Scale21. Informant questionnaires and interviews were used to obtain study subject age, race/ethnicity, primary language, education, marital status, the Beck Informant Depression Scale22, duration of cognitive impairment prior to enrollment, and living arrangement, as well as information on the informant’s relation to the study subject and hours per week informant spends with subject. Treatment arm was categorized per the ADCS MCI trial as placebo, vitamin E, or donepezil. Type of recruitment site was categorized as National Institute of Aging program project AD research centers (ADC sites), non-ADC, university-affiliated research centers (university-affiliated sites), or commercial sites not affiliated with a university (commercial sites).

RESULTS

Of the 769 subjects randomized into the trial, 214 incident dementias were observed, 325 subjects completed the 36 month trial without converting to dementia, and 230 (30%) did not complete the closed-label phase of the trial. Of the 230 non-completers, 17 subjects died, 47 were discontinued by site clinicians due to adverse events, 105 withdrew consent during the trial, 55 discontinued treatment and ultimately withdrew consent during the trial, and six moved or otherwise refused further contact. Study discontinuation occurred within the first six months for 117 subjects and within the first year for 172 subjects. Over four-fifths of those lost (193, 83.9%) dropped by month 18, with 9.6%, 5.7%, and 0.9% dropping in each of the remaining six month intervals. Overall, the mean length in study was 23.2 months (standard deviation (sd) 13.4), with subjects who discontinued completing an average of 9.5 months (sd 8.9) of follow-up, compared to 18.4 months (sd 9.1) for incident dementia cases and 36.0 (sd 0.4) for those who completed the closed-label portion of the trial without converting to dementia.

Baseline characteristics by discontinuation status are presented in Table 1. Subjects who dropped were different from those who continued in the study on several demographic measures. In univariate analyses, risk of study discontinuation was higher among females (HR: 1.36, p=0.020), among non-white subjects compared to white, non-Hispanic subjects (HR: 2.12, p<0.001), and among those with less than high school education (HR: 1.51, p=0.047). Rates of study discontinuation were highest at commercial sites (43.8%), and lowest at ADCs (23.0%, p<0.001). University-affiliated sites had slightly higher rates of study discontinuation compared to ADCs (28.5%, p=0.150). Subjects who were married had lower rates of study discontinuation than their unmarried counterparts (27.0% vs 39.8%, p=0.001). Only one clinical measure was a statistically significant predictor of study discontinuation in the univariate analysis: subjects with a Ham-D score of ≥6 to 12 were more likely to discontinue than subjects scoring < 6 (HR: 1.6, p=0.004). Subjects in the donepezil arm were more likely to discontinue than subjects in the placebo arm (HR: 2.2, p<0.001).

Table 1.

Univariate Comparisons of Baseline Measures with Study Discontinuation (SD).

Overall (N=769) SD (N=230) SD % p-value* HR 95% CI p-value
Age (cut at the median)
 <74 381 114 29.92% 0.713 0.95 (0.74, 1.24) 0.719
 >=74 388 116 29.90% 1.00 Referent
Gender
 Male 417 110 26.38% 0.023 1.00 Referent
 Female 352 120 34.09% 1.36 (1.04, 1.74) 0.024
Race/ethnicity
 White, non-Hispanic 708 198 27.97% <0.001 1.00 Referent
 Non-White 61 32 52.46% 2.11 (1.45, 3.07) <0.001
Education
 <12 76 31 40.79% 0.016 1.51 (1.01, 2.26) 0.047
 >=12, <16 (High School) 313 95 30.35% 1.00 Referent
 >=16 380 104 27.37% 0.84 (0.64, 1.11) 0.224
Primary Language
 English 709 206 29.06% 0.110 1.00 Referent
 Not English 60 24 40.00% 1.43 (0.46, 1.07) 0.097
Marital Status
 Married 597 161 26.97% 0.001 1.00 Referent
 Not married 171 68 39.77% 1.59 (1.20, 2.11) 0.001
BMI at screening
 <25 326 97 29.75% 0.696 1.00 Referent
 >=25, 30 322 98 30.43% 0.99 (0.76, 1.31) 0.961
 >=30 120 34 28.33% 0.85 (0.60, 1.26) 0.417
Hours per week informant spends with subject
 <24 142 49 34.51% 0.229 1.00 Referent
 >=24, 48 38 15 39.47% 1.31 (0.73, 2.34) 0.360
 >=48, 72 48 15 31.25% 0.90 (0.51, 1.61) 0.726
 >=72 541 151 27.91% 0.81 (0.59, 1.11) 0.194
Living Arrangements
 House 566 162 28.62% 0.695 1.00 Referent
 Condominium/Apartment 150 52 34.67% 1.26 (0.92, 1.72) 0.140
 Trailer 12 3 25.00% 0.82 (0.26, 2.57) 0.730
 Retirement Community 33 11 33.33% 1.17 (0.63, 2.16) 0.610
 Assisted Living 3 1 33.33% 1.59 (0.22, 11.36) 0.650
 Other 5 1 20.00% 0.64 (0.09, 4.54) 0.650
Site Classification
 NIA funded ADC 291 67 23.02% <0.001 1.00 Referent
 University-affiliated 302 86 28.48% 1.27 (0.92, 1.75) 0.144
 Commercial site 176 77 43.75% 2.11 (1.52, 2.93) <0.001
Treatment Arm
 Placebo 259 66 25.48% 0.0316 1.00 Referent
 Vitamin E 257 72 28.02% 1.11 (0.80, 1.55) 0.536
 Donepezil 253 92 36.36% 1.49 (1.09, 2.05) 0.013
MMSE at baseline (cut at median)
 >=23, <28 397 113 28.46% 0.905 1.02 (0.79, 1.32) 0.985
 >=28, <=30 372 117 31.45% 1.00 Referent
CDR Sum of Boxes at baseline (cut at median)
 <=1.5 389 128 32.90% 0.287 1.00 Referent
 >1.5 380 102 26.84% 0.87 (0.67, 1.13) 0.290
Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression
 < 6 637 175 27.47% 0.004 1.00 Referent
 >=6 132 55 41.67% 1.56 (1.15, 2.12) 0.004
Beck Informant Depression Scale at baseline
 0–2 159 46 28.93% 0.122 0.82 (0.59, 1.15) 0.253
 3–5 205 50 24.39% 0.72 (0.52, 1.00) 0.053
 6–34 394 128 32.49% 1.00 Referent
Modified Hachinski
 <2 729 222 30.45% 0.108 1.00 Referent
 >=2 40 8 20.00% 0.56 (0.28, 1.14) 0.113
*

Overall log-rank (unweighted) p-value.

From unadjusted Cox proportional hazard model.

Marital status missing for one subject.

Per our pre-specified model building algorithm, covariates selected for the final multivariate model based on a univariate log-rank test p-value < 0.1 were treatment arm, site classification, marital status/informant relationship, education, gender, race/ethnicity, and HAM-D score. Age was not significantly related to study discontinuation at the p<0.1 level, but was forced into the model. Of potential pairwise interactions, only the gender by marital status term was statistically significant (likelihood ratio test p-value < 0.0001); this interaction was added to the model using married men as the referent gender by marital status category. The two measures with univariate p-values between 0.1 and 0.15, the Beck Informant Depression Scale and the primary language indicator, were added separately to the model and tested for significance. Neither significantly improved the model (likelihood ratio test p-values > 0.3), nor did their inclusion modify coefficients of covariates already in the model by more that 15%, and therefore these terms were not included in the final model. Proportional hazards assumptions were not rejected for any terms in the model (p>0.3).

The final multivariate model is presented in Table 2. Hazard ratios and p-values in the full multivariate model are largely consist with those observe in the univariate analyses, although in the final model we observe that the effect of marital status on discontinuation is present only within men (HR=2.24, p=0.001 for unmarried versus married men). The gender effect remains, but individual terms in the final model comparing married and unmarried women to the referent married men category are not statistically significant because of the reduced per comparison sample size. If these two female categories are pooled, the gender effect is statistically significant (HR=1.34, p=0.044 for women versus married men).

Table 2.

Multivariate Cox Proportional Hazards Model of Time to Study Discontinuation*.

Baseline Measure HR 95 % CI p-value
Age
 <74 0.92 (0.70, 1.20) 0.519
 >=74 1.00 Referent
Race/ethnicity
 White, non-Hispanic 1.00 Referent
 Non-White 2.03 (1.34, 3.06) <0.001
Education
 <12 1.62 (1.07, 2.47) 0.023
 >=12, <16 (High School) 1.00 Referent
 >=16 0.97 (0.73, 1.30) 0.852
Marital Status/gender
 Married, male 1.00 Referent
 Married, female 1.35 (0.98, 1.86) 0.066
 Not married, male 2.08 (1.29, 3.35) 0.003
 Not married, female 1.32 (0.90, 1.94) 0.151
Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression
 < 6 1.00 Referent
 >=6 1.38 (1.00, 1.90) 0.047
Treatment Arm
 Placebo 1.00 Referent
 Vitamin E 1.05 (0.75, 1.47) 0.771
 Donepezil 1.57 (1.14, 2.15) 0.006
Site Classification
 NIA funded ADC 1.00 Referent
 University-affiliated 1.31 (0.95, 1.81) 0.097
 Commercial 2.21 (1.58, 3.08) <0.001
*

N=768 with 229 events. One subject missing marital status was excluded from the multivariate analysis.

When we reran the model censoring subjects who died rather than counting them as study discontinuation events, the coefficients and p-values for all of the statistically significant predictors in Table 2 were not appreciably changed except for the HAM-D term, which was attenuated and no longer a significant predictor of study discontinuation (HR 1.21, p=0.27). When we reran the model further censoring both deaths and other serious adverse events, the coefficients for all of the significant predictors were not appreciably changed, although in this model the p-value for the education less than high school term became only marginally statistically significant (p-value changing from 0.02 to 0.07 after censoring of deaths and other serious adverse events).

The final disposition of study subjects is summarized by type of clinical recruitment site in Table 3. Subjects recruited by commercial sites were less likely to convert to dementia, and more likely to die or discontinue compared to non-commercial sites. Reasons for voluntary study discontinuation by type of clinical recruitment site are summarized in Table 4. “Caregiver unwilling or unable to participate” was the most commonly sited reason, scored by 35 of 160 caregivers (22%). There were no significant differences in the frequency of reasons by site type, although there was a trend toward subjects recruited by commercial sites discontinuing because of concern that the treatment medication was not effective (p = 0.07) and a trend toward subjects recruited by non-commercial sites discontinuing because of travel requirements (p = 0.06).

Table 3.

Final Disposition of Study Subjects, by Clinical Site Type.

ADC University Affiliated Commercial

N (%) N (%) N (%) Total

Dementia 97 (33.3%) 85 (28.1%) 32 (18.2%) 214
Death 2 (0.7%) 8 (2.6%) 7 (4.0%) 17
Serious Adverse Event 16 (5.5%) 14 (4.6%) 17 (9.7%) 47
Voluntary Discontinuation 49 (16.8%) 61 (20.2%) 50 (28.4%) 160
Lost to Follow-up 0 (0.0%) 3 (1.0%) 3 (1.7%) 6
Completed Trial 127 (43.6%) 131 (43.4%) 67 (38.1%) 325

Table 4.

Reasons for Voluntary Study Discontinuation, by Clinical Site Type.

ADC (N=49) University Affiliated (N=61) Commercial (N=50)

N (%*) N (%*) N (%*) p-value

Concern over placebo condition 8 (16.3%) 11 (18.0%) 7 (14.0%) 0.83
Length of protocol 3 (6.1%) 2 (3.3%) 3 (6.0%) 0.73
Travel requirements 11 (22.4%) 8 (13.1%) 3 (6.0%) 0.06
Frequency of assessments 5 (10.2%) 2 (3.3)% 1 (2.0%) 0.19
Potential adverse (side) effects 9 (18.4%) 11 (18.0%) 7 (14.0%) 0.83
Concern about effectiveness of medications 10 (20.4%) 6 (9.8%) 13 (26.0%) 0.07
Subject chose alternative treatment 9 (18.4%) 12 (19.7%) 6 (12.0%) 0.56
Caregiver chose alternative treatment 5 (10.2%) 11 (18.0%) 5 (10.0%) 0.37
Caregiver unwilling or unable to participate 7 (14.3%) 13 (21.3%) 15 (30.0%) 0.17
Other 22 (44.9%) 32 (52.5%) 21 (42.0%) 0.53
*

Numbers do not sum to 100% because multiple items could be scored.

Fisher exact test p-value.

DISCUSSION

We found that subjects in the donepezil arm had a 57% higher risk of study discontinuation compared to subjects in the placebo arm after controlling for covariates (Table 2, HR 1.57, p = 0.006). This association was described in the primary report of trial findings and is attributable to the known side-effect profile of cholinesterase inhibitor treatment9.

We also found that subjects with more depression symptoms reported on the HAM-D were more likely to discontinue the trial. While we did not perform a formal competing risk analysis, when we refit the final model with censoring of those subjects who discontinued due to death, the HAM-D association with study termination was attenuated; this suggests that the association was mainly due to depressive symptoms predicting death during the trial. In fact, subjects with a HAM-D greater than or equal to six were more likely to die during the trial (5.6% died versus 1.6% among subjects with a HAM-D less than six, Fisher exact test p-value = 0.016).

We also found that subjects with less educational attainment and non-white subjects had a higher rate of study discontinuation. Although minority race or ethnicity is a commonly perceived barrier to recruitment and retention6,7, there are little published data on clinical trial retention rates as a function of race or ethnicity, and we know of no published data on this issue within the elderly.

We found that commercial recruitment sites had twice the study discontinuation rate of non-commercial sites. All sites attended the same protocol training meetings and followed the same protocol. However, the commercial sites differ from the non-commercial sites in a number of ways. There are some indications the commercial sites recruited from a different pool of study subjects. While there was no difference between commercial and non-commercial sites in terms of the distribution of age, gender, and ethnicity, the subjects recruited by commercial sites had slightly less education (mean 14.1 years vs 14.8 years, Wilcoxon sign rank test p-value = 0.003). Lower education did predict a higher dropout rate in our sample (Table 1). However, in the multivariate analysis with both education and site in the model the HR for commercial sites was unchanged and remained highly statistically significant (Table 2), indicating that the site effect was not mediated by education in any appreciable way. We also observed that, among subjects who withdrew consent, caregivers from commercial sites were more likely to endorse as a reason for study discontinuation “Concern about effectiveness of medications” (26.0% versus 14.5% for non-commercial sites). It is possible that recruitment methods at commercial sites increased the proportion of subjects seeking active treatment via enrollment in clinical trials as opposed to subjects participating for more altruistic motivations. Subjects who seek active treatment via enrollment in clinical trials may be more likely to discontinue early if they perceive they are randomized to an arm with inactive or ineffective treatment (as reviewed in Vozdolska et al.23). Subjects from commercial sites were also more likely to die during the trial or to be discontinued because of other serious adverse events (Table 4). This does not entirely explain the higher loss rate in commercial sites, however. When the multivariate survival analysis was repeated with censoring of the deaths and adverse event-related discontinuations, the HR for commercial sites remained statistically significant (p = 0.0002) and was attenuated only modestly, from HR = 2.21 to HR = 2.09.

Beyond study subject characteristics, we also suspect that staff members at ADCs and university-affiliated sites are more familiar with the scientific research process and more sensitive to the importance of complete data for the validity of a trial. Staff members at ADCs and university-affiliated sites often have developed skills and standards for maintaining study subject continuity in the course of their own research. These skills and standards may transfer to their performance of recruitment and retention for multicenter clinical trials. Furthermore, academic sites are more likely to recruit subjects from their own clinic populations, while commercial sites tend to rely more heavily on advertising; longstanding relationships among staff and subjects at academic sites may improve retention at these sites.

Early study discontinuation at commercial sites did reduce the statistical power of the ADCS MCI trial. Average person-years of observation for subjects recruited by commercial sites was 1.75 person-years per subject recruited, compared to 2.01 person-years per subject recruited by ADC sites and 1.95 person-years per subject recruited by non-ADC university-affiliated sites. Compounding the high study discontinuation rate was a low incident dementia rate among subjects recruited by commercial sites (Table 3). There were only 10.4 incident dementia cases per 100 person-years of observation at the commercial sites compared to 16.6 and 14.4 incident dementia cases per 100 person-years of observation at the ADC and university-affiliated sites. We speculate that this is can be attributed to a lack of specificity of diagnosis for amnestic MCI by commercial sites when screening subjects for inclusion in the ADCS MCI trial (the inclusion criteria specified that non-amnestic subtypes of MCI should not be enrolled). Clinicians at commercial sites may have been less sensitive than their ADC and university-affiliated research counterparts to those inclusion criteria intended to narrow recruitment to the amnestic subtype of MCI that is most consistent with prodromal AD.

Cumulatively, high study discontinuation rates and low incident dementia rates such as experienced by commercial sites in the ADCS MCI trial can substantially compromise statistical power. One practical measure of the effect of incidence rates and discontinuation rates on power is in the calculation of sample size requirements when planning future trials. The statistical power of a survival analysis is determined by the expected number of incident events observed24. During this three-year trial, the commercial sites observed 0.1818 dementia events per study subject recruited, while the ADC sites observed 0.3333 dementia events per study subject recruited. Based on the number of events observed per subject recruited, a clinical trial using commercial sites with the discontinuation rates and incident dementia rates experienced in the ADCS MCI trial would require 1.8 times as many subjects as a comparably powered clinical trial using ADC clinical sites.

Conclusions

Subject dropout has been problematic for many MCI trials performed to date (reviewed in Jelic et al.3). Missing data due to study subject dropout compromises the power and validity of statistical analyses of trial results. We identified several demographic parameters that predict a higher dropout rate, including sex, marital status, education, and race/ethnicity. Retention efforts targeting the identified groups may improve the representativeness and power of future trials. We also found that commercial recruitment sites have a higher dropout rate and lower incident dementia rate than ADCs and university-affiliated sites. Overall, the ADCS MCI trial had a relatively low dropout rate compared to most MCI trials performed to date3, presumably because the ADCS MCI trial was less reliant on commercial sites for recruitment. We conclude that utilization of academic sites could substantively decrease dropout and improve the statistical power and validity of future clinical trials of cognitively impaired elderly.

Acknowledgments

Supported by the National Institute on Aging grants to the Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study (U01 AG010483) and the University of California San Diego Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center (P50 AG005131).

References

  • 1.Jacova C, Hsiung G-YR, Feldman HH. Dropouts and refusals in observational studies: lessons for prevention trials. Neurology. 2006;67(9 Suppl 3):S17–20. doi: 10.1212/wnl.67.9_suppl_3.s17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Chatfield MD, Brayne CE, Matthews FE. A systematic literature review of attrition between waves in longitudinal studies in the elderly shows a consistent pattern of dropout between differing studies. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2005;58:13–19. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2004.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Jelic V, Kivipelto M, Winblad B. Clinical trials in mild cognitive impairment: lessons for the future. J Neurol Neurosurg Psych. 2006;77:429–438. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2005.072926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Matthews F, Chatfield M, Brayne C. An investigation of whether factors associated with short-term attrition change or persist over ten years: data from the Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Ageing Study (MRC CFAS) BMC Public Health. 2006;6:185. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-6-185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Schulz KF, Grimes DA. Sample size slippages in randomised trials: exclusions and the lost and wayward. The Lancet. 2002;359:781–785. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(02)07882-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Yancey AK, Ortega AN, Kumanyika SK. Effective recruitment and retention of minority research participants. Annual Review of Public Health. 2006;27:1–28. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.27.021405.102113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Arean PA, Gallagher-Thompson D. Issues and recommendations for the recruitment and retention of older ethnic minority adults into clinical research. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1996;64:875–880. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.64.5.875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Arnow BA, Blasey C, Manber R, et al. Dropouts versus completers among chronically depressed outpatients. J Affect Disord. 2007;97:197–202. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Petersen RC, Thomas RG, Grundman M, et al. Vitamin E and donepezil for the treatment of mild cognitive impairment. N Engl J Med. 2005;352:2379–2388. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa050151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale, Revised: Administration and Scoring Manual. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Berg L. Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) Psychopharmacol Bull. 1988;24:637–639. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Rosen WG, Terry RD, Fuld PA, et al. Pathological verification of ischemic score in differentiation of dementias. Ann Neurol. 1980;7:486–8. doi: 10.1002/ana.410070516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hamilton M. Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness. Br J Soc Clin Psychol. 1967;6:278–296. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1967.tb00530.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, et al. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurol. 1984;34:939–944. doi: 10.1212/wnl.34.7.939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hosmer DW, Jr, Wang CY, Lin IC, et al. A computer program for stepwise logistic regression using maximum likelihood estimation. Comput Programs Biomed. 1978;8:121–134. doi: 10.1016/0010-468x(78)90047-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Schoenfeld D. Partial residuals for the proportional hazards regression-model. Biometrika. 1982;69:239–241. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.R Development Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Galasko D, Bennett DA, Sano M, et al. ADCS Prevention Instrument Project: Assessment of instrumental activities of daily living for community-dwelling elderly individuals in dementia prevention clinical trials. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2006;20:S152–S169. doi: 10.1097/01.wad.0000213873.25053.2b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Fillenbaum GG, Peterson B, Morris JC. Estimating the validity of the clinical dementia rating scale: The CERAD experience. Aging Clin Exp Res. 1996;8:379–385. doi: 10.1007/BF03339599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Reisberg B, Ferris SH, de Leon MJ, et al. The global deterioration scale for assessment of primary degenerative dementia. Am J Psychiatry. 1982;139:1136–1139. doi: 10.1176/ajp.139.9.1136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1961;4:561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Vozdolska R, Sano M, Aisen P, et al. The net effect of alternative allocation ratios on recruitment time and trial cost. Clinical Trials. 2009;6:126–132. doi: 10.1177/1740774509103485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Collett D. Modelling Survival Data in Medical Research. New York, New York: Chapman and Hall; 2005. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES