Abstract
Objective
Cognitive impairment is common in epilepsy, particularly in memory function. Interictal spikes are thought to disrupt cognition, but it is difficult to delineate their contribution from general impairments in memory produced by etiology and seizures. We investigated the transient impact of focal interictal spikes on the hippocampus, a structure crucial for learning and memory and yet highly prone to interictal spikes in temporal lobe epilepsy.
Methods
Bilateral hippocampal depth electrodes were implanted into fourteen Sprague-Dawley rats, followed by intrahippocampal pilocarpine or saline infusion unilaterally. Rats that developed chronic spikes were trained in a hippocampal-dependent operant behavior task, delayed-match-to-sample. Depth EEG was recorded during 5,562 trials among five rats, and within-subject analyses evaluated the impact of hippocampal spikes on short-term memory operations.
Results
Hippocampal spikes that occurred during memory retrieval strongly impaired performance (p<0.001). However, spikes that occurred during memory encoding or memory maintenance did not affect performance in those trials. Hippocampal spikes also affected response latency, adding approximately 0.48 seconds to the time taken to respond (p<0.001).
Interpretation
We found that focal interictal spike-related interference in cognition extends to structures in the limbic system, which required intrahippocampal recordings. Hippocampal spikes seem most harmful if they occur when hippocampal function is critical, extending human studies showing that cortical spikes are most disruptive during active cortical functioning. The cumulative effects of spikes could therefore impact general cognitive functioning. These results strengthen the argument that suppression of interictal spikes may improve memory and cognitive performance in patients with epilepsy.
Introduction
One of the most common yet troublesome co-morbidities in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) is cognitive impairment 1-3. In particular, many patients have disturbances in memory, an essential higher cognitive function for continuity in time, personal history, and awareness. While both etiology 4-5 and recurrent seizures 6 have been implicated, an important controversy is whether interictal spikes (IIS) are a harmless biomarker of the disease or contribute to this dysfunction.
IIS are transient, abnormal focal neural discharges seen on electroencephalogram (EEG) recordings during periods between seizures. Lasting 50-200 milliseconds, they are a result of synchronous, paroxysmal depolarizations of neurons producing a rapid succession of action potentials 7-8. IIS usually occur close to the seizure focus 9-10 and they are one of the most important factors in the diagnosis of epilepsy 11-12. However, IIS may interfere with cognitive functions that are already jeopardized by disease, which would be particularly troublesome given the prominence of focal IIS in epileptic syndromes such as temporal lobe epilepsy and the epileptic encephalopathies 13.
The effect of IIS on cognition is likely ephemeral, coinciding with timing of the event on the EEG. The clinical term transitory cognitive impairment has been used to describe brief disruptions in brain function concurrent with IIS 14. This term encompasses impairments observed with both focal IIS and generalized spike-and-wave complexes. The latter have produced unquestionable demonstrations of transitory cognitive impairment, including impairments in reaction time 15-17, perception 16-19, verbal and spatial tasks 20-22, and even driving behavior 23. Furthermore, some studies suggest that the neuroanatomical site and timing of the EEG disturbance predicts the specific cognitive function affected. For example, disturbances localized to the occipital cortex briefly disrupt visual perception, particularly during a stimulus presentation 17, 19.
Focal IIS-induced cognitive disruption has been more difficult to identify than that associated with generalized spike-and-wave. This may be due to the use of scalp EEG in all studies of transient IIS effects thus far. Scalp EEG does not allow precise neuroanatomical localization of IIS, which weakens the determination of which cognitive function might be affected. Furthermore, non-cortical focal IIS, such as those in the hippocampus or other deep medial temporal lobe structures, are not effectively detected by scalp EEG 24. This presents problems in testing for effects of IIS on learning and memory, for which these structures are critical. Hence, mixed results in previous studies may be related to inconsistencies between IIS localization and the cognitive function tested.
Depth electrode studies can help to resolve these issues, through visualization of focal IIS in subcortical structures that play a central role in memory. However, patients with intracranial electrodes present other difficulties for extended neuropsychological testing, mainly due to post-surgical discomfort and cognitive side effects of pain medication. We investigated the effects of IIS on operant behavior, a particularly useful tool to examine IIS in an animal model which enables rigorous assessment of a fleeting and potentially subtle disruption. Pilocarpine-treated rats with chronic IIS were trained to perform a delayed-match-to-sample task (DMTS), which relies largely on intact hippocampal function 25, and involves short-term memory encoding, maintenance, and retrieval. We used within-subject analysis to compare trials with IIS versus trials without IIS from the same animal, isolating the specific effect of IIS on cognition from differences that might otherwise vary between animals.
While cortical IIS have been shown to disrupt stimulus perception for later recall 19, patients with TLE are not particularly prone to problems in perception 26, despite high incidence of IIS in temporal lobe structures including the hippocampus 27. However, neuronal firing and neurophysiological rhythms in the hippocampus are closely related to short-term memory, particularly the retrieval phase, and may be vulnerable to the electrophysiological impact of hippocampal IIS 28-30. Therefore we hypothesized that focal IIS in the hippocampus momentarily disrupt short-term memory, specifically short-term memory retrieval.
We report that IIS in the hippocampus are associated with a transient disruption in function and an increase in response latency. This evidence illustrates that focal IIS in limbic structures, which are very common in epileptic conditions such as TLE, can provide an independent contribution to cognitive impairments.
Methods
Animals
All animal procedures were approved by the Dartmouth IACUC, under USDA and AAALAC-approved conditions, in accordance with National Institutes of Health guidelines Fourteen Adult male Sprague-Dawley rats, approximately eight weeks old, were housed individually with a 12-hour light/dark cycle and ad libitum access to food and water. All rats underwent surgical implantations and infusion with either pilocarpine or saline, and were later recorded to screen for IIS. Specifically, nine rats were implanted and infused with pilocarpine at least two weeks before behavioral training began, while two rats were trained before surgery and pilocarpine infusion. Within-subject analyses accounted for any individual differences between rats such as the timing of training, therefore all behavioral data was pooled together in the analysis (see Data Analysis section).
Three control rats were implanted and infused with saline at least two weeks before training to provide a comparison to pathological EEG phenomena in pilocarpine-treated rats, and to verify that the surgical and infusion techniques alone did not produce IIS. Control rats were not involved in the behavioral analysis since this study aimed to use within-subject comparisons to assess the independent impact of IIS, which were not exhibited at any time following a saline infusion. Female rats were not utilized in this study to prevent the potential effects of fluctuating estrous cycle hormones on behavior and cognition31.
Surgery
Rats were anesthetized with isoflurane (2-3% in oxygen) and custom electrodes were implanted in three sites 32 in each rat: bilateral ventral hippocampal CA1 (±5.7 mm lateral, 5.4 mm posterior, and 6.0 mm ventral from the bregma skull fissure; Fig 1A), and prelimbic prefrontal cortex (0.6 mm lateral, 3.2 mm anterior, and 4.3 mm ventral). A 26-gauge guide cannula (Plastics One, Roanoke, VA) was bonded to the right ventral CA1 bipolar electrode to allow close proximity (<1 mm) of EEG recording to the infusion focus. A ground wire was soldered to a bone screw, and a reference wire was implanted in the cerebellum. All wires were plugged into an interconnect socket array (Mill-Max Mfg. Corp., Oyster Bay, NY), and encased in dental cement (Dentsply International Inc., Milford, DE), leaving only the gold pin sockets exposed to allow connection to the amplifiers (see Electrophysiology section).
Pilocarpine infusion
One week following surgery, pilocarpine dissolved in 0.9% saline (0.5 mg/μl; Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) was infused into the hippocampus at a rate of 0.05 μl/minute with concurrent depth-EEG recordings. To minimize mortality while ensuring IIS occurrence, pilocarpine infusions were terminated for a given animal when continuous EEG status epilepticus (SE) was observed in the hippocampus. The amount of solution infused varied from 0.9 μL to 2.3 μl (0.45-1.15 mg pilocarpine). Importantly, individual differences in infusion amount were accounted for by the within-subject design of the analysis (see Data Analysis section). Control rats received an infusion of 1-2 μl of 0.9% saline, to provide later qualitative comparison of their long-term EEG activity to that of pilocarpine-treated rats. Rats were monitored on EEG for one half hour after SE began, and behavior was monitored for another 12 hours. To assess IIS development, 30-60 minute EEG recordings were made each day after the infusion for two weeks, and once per week for an additional 3 weeks.
Electrophysiology
Electrodes consisted of two insulated 25 μm nichrome wires (California Fine Wire, Grover Beach, CA), twisted and inserted into a 25-gauge stainless steel guide tube (Small Parts Inc., Miami Lakes, FL). Operational amplifiers plugged directly to the rat connector to reduce mechanical artifacts. EEG signals, transmitted via a custom cable and a rotating commutator (Dragonfly Research & Development Inc., Ridgeley, WV), were amplified (10K) and acquired at 2.2kHz with an Axoscope analog/digital converter and software acquisition set-up (DigiData 1322, Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA).
Apparatus
Two operant conditioning chambers contained in sound-attenuating cubicles (Lafayette Instruments Inc., Lafayette, IN; Med Associates Inc., St. Albans, VT) were controlled with behavior software packages (ABET II; MED-PC IV). Two retractable levers were separated by a pellet dispenser on one of the walls. A stimulus light was located above each lever. An infrared photobeam nosepoke detector was placed on the opposite wall, with a stimulus light above it. A white incandescent house light bulb was illuminated during each trial and extinguished at its end. Three DC outputs from the behavioral software computer interface were sent directly to the EEG data acquisition system to additional input ports. Stimulus and response events were recorded as binary (on/off) combinations of these signals, enabling microsecond synchronization with EEG data.
Procedure
Rats were food-restricted and maintained at 85% of their ad libitum weight throughout operant behavior study. They were trained in operant chambers to lever-press and poke their nose into the infrared beam for food reward (45 mg Noyes food pellet; Research Diets, Incorporated, New Brunswick, NJ). Training sessions were run at approximately the same time each day for each rat. Once the rats reliably pressed levers and broke the infrared beam for food, the DMTS training paradigm commenced (Fig 1B). At the beginning of each trial, one lever (right or left) was randomly presented [Sample step]. Upon pressing the sample lever, rats were then required to poke their nose into an infrared beam [Delay step], which was positioned on the opposite wall of the chamber to prevent rats from relying on body positioning (non-hippocampal-dependent) strategies to gain reinforcers 33. Immediately after the beam was broken, both levers were presented [Match step]. A correct response was defined as pressing the same lever that was presented in the Sample Phase (i.e. match-to-sample), which produced a food reward. An incorrect response was pressing the lever that had not been presented, which produced no food reward. Daily sessions consisted of 60-100 trials, and when accuracy reached >85% for 3 continuous days, the delay period was increased. This was accomplished by designating nosepoke beam-breaks as unproductive during the delay step until a specified amount of time had elapsed. The delay length was increased for each rat until performance reached >80% accuracy on sessions with 6-15 second variable delays, and EEG data was recorded for all sessions thereafter. Rats with at least 10 IIS/hour were included in DMTS analysis. Five rats qualified under both performance and IIS prerequisites, generating a total of 5,562 trials for analysis.
Following spontaneous seizures, rats tended to return to normal behavioral activity levels 5-15 minutes. Previous studies from our laboratory show that post-ictal effects are rather minimal after three hours, in terms of hippocampal function and related cell firing34. Therefore, if an animal happened to have a spontaneous seizure before a DMTS session, the session was delayed at least 3 hours to help minimizes influences of post-ictal impairment on general performance. In addition, the within subject design of our analysis, which compared trials with and without IIS in the same session, was designed to negate such influences.
Data Analysis
Custom IIS detection software, based on White et al. 35, was written using MATLAB (The Mathworks, Inc., Natick, MA). Briefly, potential IIS were detected each time the derivative of the EEG recording (re-sampled at 222 Hz) was greater than five times the standard deviation. Potential waveforms were then classified as IIS, artifacts, or false-detections of normal physiological waveforms36 based on comparison EEG sessions from saline-treated rats (Fig 2C). IIS differentiation was based on morphology, by plotting the characteristic features of the waveforms against one another (e.g. amplitude). IIS included in the analysis had a peak amplitude of at least 1 mV, and lasted less than 50 ms (examples in Fig 2B), with the entire transient lasting less than 200 ms if including the subsequent slow wave component. We evaluated the effectiveness of our IIS detection programs by comparing their performance to the evaluation of an experienced epileptologist (GLH) in three randomly chosen sessions. Comparison of manually detected IIS to those detected by the program revealed a sensitivity of >95% and a specificity of >99%. Using the binary input signals from the behavioral software, the exact timings of IIS within trials were documented relative to behavioral events, to designate each trial as one with or without an IIS (Fig 1C and D).
DMTS performance as a function of delay length and timing of IIS occurrence was analyzed using logistic regression in Stata 10 (StataCorp LP, College Station, TX). Each DMTS trial was treated as an individual observation and standard errors were adjusted for within animal effects (N=5,562, clustered for five rats), to control for differences between animals such as pilocarpine dose, lesion extent, and general cognitive ability. Three stages of short-term memory processing were inferred in the DMTS task: encoding (4 seconds surrounding the sample press), maintenance (2 seconds after the sample press to 1 second before the match lever presentation), and retrieval (1 second before the match lever presentation to 1 second before it was pressed). The timing of these epochs was based on rat behavior in short-term memory tasks 29 and the pronounced effects in neuronal firing produced in the first second following a hippocampal IIS 30. Additionally, Pearson correlation coefficients were used to assess the timing of IIS within trials versus trial delay lengths, as well as test for any relationships between IIS frequency and general performance across sessions (defined as percent correct in a given session).
The effect of hippocampal IIS on response latency was assessed by quantifying the amount of time taken by the rat to press one of the match levers, following their extension and stimulus light illumination after the last nosepoke. This length of time was compared in trials in which no IIS occurred versus trials in which an IIS occurred during this period, using a Mann-Whitney U test.
Hippocampal theta oscillations have been related to greater vigilance during behavioral tasks, and would likely associate with improved performance 28. Likewise, IIS occur largely in the absence of hippocampal theta rhythm, i.e. during non-attentive states 37. To rule out the possibility that vigilance could account for effects of IIS on DMTS performance, a measure of vigilance ratio was calculated (sum of theta band (4-11Hz) power divided by the sum of delta band (0.5-3Hz) power)38-41, and compared between trials with versus without IIS, using a Mann-Whitney U test.
Results
Seizures and IIS
During intrahippocampal pilocarpine infusion, all rats exhibited electrographic seizures initiating in the ipsilateral hippocampus and eventually generalizing to both the contralateral hippocampus and the ipsilateral prefrontal cortex leads. The initial SE lasted from 3-7 hours for all rats, corresponding to Stage III-V behavioral seizures 42. In the subsequent weeks following infusion, seven rats had witnessed spontaneous seizures. Because long-term monitoring was not performed, we could not verify the presence of spontaneous seizures in the other animals with IIS. It should be noted that our use of the term “interictal” when referring to IIS is based on the pathological waveform characteristics (deCurtis & Avanzini, 2001), and not the apparition of previous and subsequent seizures. IIS developed within the first few days following infusion, subsequently increasing and reaching stable rates after approximately two weeks. IIS in the left hippocampus (contralateral to infusion site) tended to be more frequent than right hippocampal IIS in the first two weeks (Fig 2A), while prefrontal cortex IIS were seen in only one rat. Ten of eleven pilocarpine treated rats developed hippocampal IIS following pilocarpine infusion, ranging from 0.5 to 256.8 IIS per hour. IIS morphology and lateralization was variable between rats as illustrated in Figure 2B and C, although these features did not predict the degree of impairment induced. IIS included in the analysis were distinct from normal physiological waveforms seen in both pilocarpine- and saline-treated rats (Fig 2C). Electrode placements were verified in the majority of rats (N=9) to verify our surgical procedures, using thionin staining techniques of coronal sections43. Electrodes were found to be in the hippocampus, in or very near the CA1 sub-region (Figure 1A). Although there was some variability in the amount of pilocarpine-induced cell loss, the hilus and CA1 appeared most affected by the pilocarpine. As shown in Figure 1A, cell loss tended to be greater ipsilateral to the injection site.
IIS-related impairment
Increasing delays produced overall decreases in DMTS accuracy (odds ratio (OR) – 1.08; 95% CI – 1.03 to 1.14, p<0.01), confirming findings by Hampson and colleagues 25. Furthermore, IIS were associated with additional impairments in performance in a trial-specific or transient manner (Fig 3). IIS that occurred during the retrieval epoch of trials were related to markedly impaired performance, with rats more than 3 times as likely to make an error in those trials (OR – 3.19; 95% CI – 2.34 to 4.35, p<0.001). However, IIS during the encoding epoch (OR - 0.97; 95% CI - 0.59 to 1.59, p=0.91) or the maintenance epoch were not associated with impaired performance (OR – 1.06; 95% CI - 0.85 to 1.33, p=0.61). The number of IIS in a trial was not a significant predictor of accuracy, although this was likely due to a statistical paucity of trials with multiple IIS. There was a greater likelihood of IIS occurring in longer delays rather than shorter delays (p<0.001), but there was no relationship between the length of the delay and the epoch within which IIS happened (r=0.003, p=0.92). Vigilance ratio (see Methods) did not differ in trials with and without IIS (p=0.96). Finally, we found no relationships between the frequency of IIS and overall accuracy across sessions (r=0.02, p=0.78), nor between the frequency of IIS and the average performances of individual rats (r=0.24, p=0.69).
Response latency
The median response latency to press the match lever after it was extended into the chamber was 2.28 seconds in trials without IIS, whereas if an IIS occurred anywhere during this period, the median increased to 2.76 seconds (p<0.001; Fig 4). This addition in reaction time did not differ between correct or incorrect trials.
Discussion
We demonstrate that hippocampal IIS are associated with a dramatic alteration in on both accuracy and response latency in a rodent model. We were able to show this effect due to our use of depth electrodes, which have not previously been used to study the transient impact of IIS. Therefore, the transitory effects from cortical IIS described previously have a corollary in a deep brain structure. Furthermore, transient impairments in short-term memory were related to focal IIS in the hippocampus, illustrating that a more generalized discharge is not necessary to disrupt this cognitive function.
The hippocampus is required for accurate performance of the DMTS task, particularly in trials with long delays 25. Using this cognitive paradigm, we found that the effect of depth-detected hippocampal IIS is contrary to the effect of scalp-detected IIS 19. Specifically, hippocampal IIS had no effect on accuracy if they occurred during the encoding epoch (analogous to stimulus presentation in human neuropsychological studies), or during the maintenance epoch. However, hippocampal IIS during the memory retrieval epoch were associated with severe transient impairments, probably because of the intimate role of the hippocampus in this function 28-29. These selective effects reveal that hippocampal operations may be devastated if hippocampal IIS occur during their neural processing windows.
The specific effect of IIS on retrieval but not encoding or maintenance may also relate to the transient nature of the hippocampal process involved in memory recall29. Memory encoding and maintenance may involve a longer time-span of processing, and may be buffered by structures in addition to the hippocampus 44, and thus less vulnerable to a transient focal disruption. Future studies will be required to delineate the mechanism of the process-specific impact on hippocampal function, potentially utilizing multi-structure depth recordings.
Response latency was increased in trials with hippocampal IIS by approximately half a second. These additions are a fraction of the total latency to respond, and might be easily overlooked in a clinical context. However, they may be cumulatively detrimental to patients, particularly those with frequent IIS 17. Similar increases in response latency were seen in both correct and incorrect trials, suggesting that this additional time does not reflect a hesitancy of the rat due to faulty memory recollection. Rather, in addition to their deleterious effect on informational integrity, hippocampal IIS may mark a temporary offline state of the brain, manifested by this delay in responding.
IIS-related memory impairments may be a consequence of the incorporation of local neurons into a synchronized burst 45. Drawing neurons away from their normal processing may disorder the representation of pertinent information within a hippocampal neuronal assembly 46, causing inaccurate recall of which lever was pressed just seconds ago. In addition, local changes in cellular environment immediately following an IIS 7 may be sufficient to briefly depress neuron firing 30 and local oscillations47, and this delay in brain processing could manifest as an increase in response latency.
IIS were observed throughout the first two weeks following pilocarpine infusion and reached relatively stabile levels afterward, illustrating pathophysiological activity during the supposed “latent period” before the generation of spontaneous seizures48-49. The development of IIS contralateral to the infusion site was an unexpected finding, and may indicate a bilateral epileptogenic process following unilateral intrahippocampal pilocarpine infusion. The laterality of the IIS did not affect our results. Whether the spike occurred in either hippocampus, or propagated from one to the other, impairments were still observed.
Correlations between the frequency of IIS and cognitive impairment in patients have been controversial, and the clinical significance of such findings has been questioned 13. For example, deficits in IQ and school performance among patients with Benign Epilepsy with Centro-Temporal Spikes have been correlated with the frequency of IIS but not seizure frequency in some but not all studies 50-51. Differentiating the transient effects of IIS from the underlying pathological factors is challenging in clinical settings since factors such as anti-epileptic drugs and seizures may contribute to cognitive deficits. IIS rate and general measures of performance were not related in the current study (see also Chauviere et al., 200952). However, we show that the timing of IIS may be decisive in producing adverse cognitive effects, since complex functions are often supported by multiple brain structures with differential time windows of processing 44. Therefore, IIS in other brain structures may be equally disruptive and clinically evident if both the timing of the IIS, and the putative neurological substrate of the function tested, are taken into consideration.
While extrapolating results from rats to humans must be done cautiously, our study strongly suggests that hippocampal IIS produce intermittent hippocampal dysfunction. It is therefore not unreasonable to be concerned that cumulative influences of transient IIS-related disruptions may account for a portion of the cognitive impairment seen in epilepsy 14. By extension, IIS-related disruptions could be devastating in children with epileptic encephalopathies, in whom high numbers of IIS may systematically obstruct learning functions in the brain, hence impeding normal cognitive development 19.
Further studies are required using other tasks that also require an intact hippocampus, since this brain area contributes to a multitude of neural processing functions 53. Human studies of focal hippocampal IIS are also warranted, to elucidate the extent of their impact on more complex cognitive functions, and to determine whether therapy improves those cognitive functions if IIS can be effectively suppressed.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (5R01NS056170-02 and 1F30NS064624-01). We thank Gregory Richard and Qian Zhao for assistance.
References
- 1.Helmstaedter C. Effects of chronic epilepsy on declarative memory systems. In: Thomas S, Asla P, editors. Progress in Brain Research. Volume 135. Elsevier; 2002. pp. 439–453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hermann BP, Seidenberg M, Schoenfeld J, Davies K. Neuropsychological characteristics of the syndrome of mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Arch Neurol. 1997;54:369–376. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1997.00550160019010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Jokeit H, Ebner A. Effects of chronic epilepsy on intellectual functions. Prog Brain Res. 2002;135:455–463. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(02)35042-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Theodore WH, Bhatia S, Hatta J, et al. Hippocampal atrophy, epilepsy duration, and febrile seizures in patients with partial seizures. Neurology. 1999;52:132–136. doi: 10.1212/wnl.52.1.132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Elger CE, Helmstaedter C, Kurthen M. Chronic epilepsy and cognition. Lancet Neurol. 2004;3:663–672. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00906-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Cheung MC, Chan AS, Chan YL, et al. Effects of illness duration on memory processing of patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia. 2006;47:1320–1328. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00556.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.de Curtis M, Avanzini G. Interictal spikes in focal epileptogenesis. Progress in Neurobiology. 2001;63:541–567. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(00)00026-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Prince DA, Connors BW. Mechanisms of interictal epileptogenesis. Adv Neurol. 1986;44:275–299. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Blume WT, Kaibara M. Localization of epileptic foci in children. Can J Neurol Sci. 1991;18:570–572. doi: 10.1017/s0317167100032728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Engel J., Jr A practical guide for routine EEG studies in epilepsy. J Clin Neurophysiol. 1984;1:109–142. doi: 10.1097/00004691-198404000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Barry E, Sussman NM, O’Connor MJ, Harner RN. Presurgical electroencephalographic patterns and outcome from anterior temporal lobectomy. Arch Neurol. 1992;49:21–27. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1992.00530250025010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Blume WT. Current trends in electroencephalography. Curr Opin Neurol. 2001;14:193–197. doi: 10.1097/00019052-200104000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Holmes GL, Lenck-Santini PP. Role of interictal epileptiform abnormalities in cognitive impairment. Epilepsy Behav. 2006;8:504–515. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2005.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Binnie CD. Cognitive impairment during epileptiform discharges: is it ever justifiable to treat the EEG? The Lancet Neurology. 2003;2:725–730. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(03)00584-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Hutt SJ, Newton J, Fairweather H. Choice reaction time and EEG activity in children with epilepsy. Neuropsychologia. 1977;15:257–267. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(77)90034-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Shewmon DA, Erwin RJ. The effect of focal interictal spikes on perception and reaction time. I. General considerations. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1988;69:319–337. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(88)90004-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Shewmon DA, Erwin RJ. The effect of focal interictal spikes on perception and reaction time. II. Neuroanatomic specificity. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1988;69:338–352. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(88)90005-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Shewmon DA, Erwin RJ. Focal spike-induced cerebral dysfunction is related to the after-coming slow wave. Ann Neurol. 1988;23:131–137. doi: 10.1002/ana.410230205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Shewmon DA, Erwin RJ. Transient impairment of visual perception induced by single interictal occipital spikes. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1989;11:675–691. doi: 10.1080/01688638908400924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Aarts JH, Binnie CD, Smit AM, Wilkins AJ. Selective cognitive impairment during focal and generalized epileptiform EEG activity. Brain. 1984;107(Pt 1):293–308. doi: 10.1093/brain/107.1.293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Binnie CD. Significance and management of transitory cognitive impairment due to subclinical EEG discharges in children. Brain Dev. 1993;15:23–30. doi: 10.1016/0387-7604(93)90003-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Rugland AL. Neuropsychological assessment of cognitive functioning in children with epilepsy. Epilepsia. 1990;31(Suppl 4):S41–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1990.tb05869.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Trenite DG Kasteleijn-Nolst, Riemersma JB, Binnie CD, et al. The influence of subclinical epileptiform EEG discharges on driving behaviour. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1987;67:167–170. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(87)90040-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Nayak D, Valentin A, Alarcon G, et al. Characteristics of scalp electrical fields associated with deep medial temporal epileptiform discharges. Clin Neurophysiol. 2004;115:1423–1435. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hampson RE, Jarrard LE, Deadwyler SA. Effects of ibotenate hippocampal and extrahippocampal destruction on delayed-match and -nonmatch-to-sample behavior in rats. J Neurosci. 1999;19:1492–1507. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-04-01492.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Grant AC, Donnelly KM, Chubb C, et al. Temporal lobe epilepsy does not impair visual perception. Epilepsia. 2008;49:710–713. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2007.01483.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Niedermeyer E, Rocca U. The diagnostic significance of sleep electroencephalograms in temporal lobe epilepsy. A comparison of scalp and depth tracings. Eur Neurol. 1972;7:119–129. doi: 10.1159/000114418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Jones MW, Wilson MA. Theta Rhythms Coordinate Hippocampal–Prefrontal Interactions in a Spatial Memory Task. PLoS Biol. 2005;3:e402. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Montgomery SM, Buzsaki G. Gamma oscillations dynamically couple hippocampal CA3 and CA1 regions during memory task performance. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:14495–14500. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0701826104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Zhou JL, Lenck-Santini PP, Zhao Q, Holmes GL. Effect of interictal spikes on single-cell firing patterns in the hippocampus. Epilepsia. 2007;48:720–731. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00972.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Daniel JM. Effects of oestrogen on cognition: what have we learned from basic research? J Neuroendocrinol. 2006;18:787–795. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2006.01471.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Paxinos G, Watson C. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. 6th ed. 1 v. Academic Press/Elsevier; Amsterdam ; Boston: 2007. (unpaged) [Google Scholar]
- 33.Chudasama Y, Muir JL. A behavioural analysis of the delayed non-matching to position task: the effects of scopolamine, lesions of the fornix and of the prelimbic region on mediating behaviours by rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1997;134:73–82. doi: 10.1007/s002130050427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Boukhezra O, Riviello P, Fu DD, et al. Effect of the postictal state on visual-spatial memory in immature rats. Epilepsy Res. 2003;55:165–175. doi: 10.1016/s0920-1211(03)00111-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.White AM, Williams PA, Ferraro DJ, et al. Efficient unsupervised algorithms for the detection of seizures in continuous EEG recordings from rats after brain injury. J Neurosci Methods. 2006;152:255–266. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2005.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Buzsaki G. Hippocampal sharp waves: their origin and significance. Brain Res. 1986;398:242–252. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(86)91483-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Leung LW. Hippocampal interictal spikes induced by kindling: relations to behavior and EEG. Behav Brain Res. 1988;31:75–84. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(88)90160-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Dzirasa K, Ribeiro S, Costa R, et al. Dopaminergic control of sleep-wake states. J Neurosci. 2006;26:10577–10589. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1767-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Lee AK, Wilson MA. Memory of sequential experience in the hippocampus during slow wave sleep. Neuron. 2002;36:1183–1194. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01096-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Louie K, Wilson MA. Temporally structured replay of awake hippocampal ensemble activity during rapid eye movement sleep. Neuron. 2001;29:145–156. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00186-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Santos LM, Dzirasa K, Kubo R, et al. Baseline hippocampal theta oscillation speeds correlate with rate of operant task acquisition. Behav Brain Res. 2008;190:152–155. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.02.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Racine RJ. Modification of seizure activity by electrical stimulation. II. Motor seizure. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1972;32:281–294. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(72)90177-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kohane DS, Holmes GL, Chau Y, et al. Effectiveness of muscimol-containing microparticles against pilocarpine-induced focal seizures. Epilepsia. 2002;43:1462–1468. doi: 10.1046/j.1528-1157.2002.11202.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Lee I, Kesner RP. Time-dependent relationship between the dorsal hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex in spatial memory. J Neurosci. 2003;23:1517–1523. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-04-01517.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Miles R, Wong RK. Single neurones can initiate synchronized population discharge in the hippocampus. Nature. 1983;306:371–373. doi: 10.1038/306371a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Pastalkova E, Itskov V, Amarasingham A, Buzsaki G. Internally generated cell assembly sequences in the rat hippocampus. Science. 2008;321:1322–1327. doi: 10.1126/science.1159775. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Halasz P, Kelemen A, Clemens B, et al. The perisylvian epileptic network. A unifying concept. Ideggyogy Sz. 2005;58:21–31. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Williams PA, Hellier JL, White AM, et al. Development of spontaneous seizures after experimental status epilepticus: implications for understanding epileptogenesis. Epilepsia. 2007;48(Suppl 5):157–163. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2007.01304.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Williams PA, White AM, Clark S, et al. Development of spontaneous recurrent seizures after kainate-induced status epilepticus. J Neurosci. 2009;29:2103–2112. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0980-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Fonseca LC, Tedrus GM, Tonelotto JM, et al. School performance in children with benign childhood epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2004;62:459–462. doi: 10.1590/s0004-282x2004000300015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Weglage J, Demsky A, Pietsch M, Kurlemann G. Neuropsychological, intellectual, and behavioral findings in patients with centrotemporal spikes with and without seizures. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1997;39:646–651. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1997.tb07357.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Chauviere L, Rafrafi N, Thinus-Blanc C, et al. Early deficits in spatial memory and theta rhythm in experimental temporal lobe epilepsy. J Neurosci. 2009;29:5402–5410. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4699-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Andersen P. The hippocampus book. Oxford University Press; Oxford ; New York: 2007. p. xx.p. 832. [Google Scholar]