Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Environ Res. 2010 Jun 1;110(6):565–570. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2010.05.002

Environmental toxins and risk of narcolepsy among people with HLA DQB1*0602

Thanh GN Ton 1,2, WT Longstreth Jr 1,2,3,5, Thomas D Koepsell 1,3,4,5
PMCID: PMC2930404  NIHMSID: NIHMS212156  PMID: 20519130

Abstract

One etiologic model for narcolepsy suggests that some environmental toxin selectively and irreversibly destroys hypocretin-producing cells in individuals with human leukocyte antigen (HLA) DQB1*0602. Between 2001-2005, the authors conducted a population-based case-control study in King County, Washington to examine narcolepsy risk in relation to toxins found in jobs, hobbies and other non-vocational activities. Sixty-seven cases and 95 controls were enrolled; all were between ages 18-50 and positive for HLA DQB1*0602. All were administered in-person interviews about jobs, hobbies or other non-vocational activities before age 21. All analyses were adjusted for African American race and income. Risk increased significantly for jobs involving heavy metals (odds ratio [OR]=4.7; 95% confidence interval [CI]: 1.5, 14.5) and for highest levels of exposure to woodwork (OR: 3.0; 95% CI: 1.0, 8.9), fertilizer (OR=3.1; 95% CI: 1.1, 9.1), and bug or weed killer (OR=4.5; 95% CI: 1.5, 13.4). Associations were of borderline significance for activities involving ceramics, pesticides, and painting projects. Significant dose-response relationships were evident for jobs involving metals (p<0.03), paints (p<0.03), and bug or weed killer (p<0.02). Additional studies are needed to replicate these findings and continue the search for specific toxins that could damage hypocretin neurons in genetically susceptible people.

Keywords: case-control, environmental exposure, epidemiology, HLA-DQ antigens, narcolepsy

Introduction

Narcolepsy is a sleep disorder characterized by excessive daytime sleepiness, cataplexy, hypnagogic hallucinations, and sleep paralysis with an onset typically before age 21 (Yoss and Daly, 1957). It likely results from the selective loss of cells in the lateral hypothalamus that secrete neurotransmitters called hypocretins (Longstreth et al., 2007; Peyron et al., 2000; Thannickal et al., 2000). A genetic tendency for narcolepsy has been noted since the nineteenth century, and in fact, narcolepsy has one of the tightest associations with specific genetic markers. First-degree relatives are 20-40 times more likely to develop the disease than the general population (Chabas et al., 2003). However, familial clustering accounts for only 10% of patients with narcolepsy, and only 25%-31% of monozygotic twins are reported to be concordant for disease (Mignot, 1998), suggesting that environmental factors are also involved in the etiology of narcolepsy. One etiologic model for the development of narcolepsy proposes that an environmental factor selectively destroys hypocretin-producing cells in genetically susceptible individuals, defined as carriers of the human leukocyte antigen (HLA) DQB1*0602 allele (Longstreth et al., 2007; Mignot, 1998). We were particularly interested in the etiology of narcolepsy specifically among genetically susceptible individuals, since they may be particularly sensitive to the effects of environmental precipitating factors yet to be identified. Driven by this theoretical framework, we restricted our population-based case-control study to individuals who were positive for HLA DQB1*0602. In this group of people, we assessed the risk of narcolepsy associated with environmental toxins that may be found in jobs, hobbies, and other activities performed before age 21.

Materials and Methods

Recruitment

Recruitment methods for cases and controls are detailed elsewhere (Koepsell et al., 2009; Longstreth et al., 2009a; Ton et al., 2009). Briefly, we attempted to identify all prevalent cases of physician-diagnosed narcolepsy who were 18 years and older and residing in King County, Washington as of July 1, 2001. For controls, residents of King County without narcolepsy and within the 18-50 year-old age range were identified through random-digit dialing. Potential controls within households were sampled using randomized recruitment methods in which age and gender-specific sampling fractions were applied to create a distribution of controls similar to that of the projected distribution of cases (Koepsell et al., 2009; Ton et al., 2009; Weinberg and Sandler, 1991). To be considered eligible for the case-control study, we required both cases and controls to carry at least one HLA DQB1*0602 allele and be within the 18-50-year-old age range. All eligible cases and controls were asked to participate in the study. Consent to participate and provide a buccal specimen was requested from each participant. Samples were assessed for the presence of HLA DQB1*0602 alleles (Gersuk and Nepom, 2009).

We identified and enrolled 425 patients into a narcolepsy registry. Of the 279 interviewed cases, 138 (49%) were positive for the HLA DQB1*0602 allele, and 67 of these were age-eligible for the study. Of 1,203 controls eligible to participate, 448 (37%) completed interviews. Of these, 95 controls carried the HLA DQB1*0602 allele and were eligible for the analyses reported here. The University of Washington's institutional review board approved the study.

Data collection

Trained professionals administered in-person interviews to cases and controls using a standardized exposure questionnaire. Interviews were scheduled according to availability of each subject. Questionnaires were developed using a combination of previously validated questions (Anic-Labat et al., 1999; Anonymous, 1997; Baldwin et al., 2004; Chervin and Aldrich, 1999; Douglass et al., 1994; Hublin et al., 1994; Johns, 1991; Ware and Gandek, 1998) and other questions designed for exploratory purposes. Because the onset of narcolepsy typically occurs during adolescence, questions assessing exposures specifically referred to those occurring before age 21. To determine exposure to environmental toxins commonly found in specific jobs around and outside the home, subjects were asked if they had ever held jobs which involved lawn care, garden work, painting projects, cleaning indoors as well as other odd jobs. Subjects who answered “yes” were asked whether they had engaged in such jobs infrequently, irregularly for years, or regularly for years. In addition, subjects were asked if any jobs had exposed them to toxins in general, and specifically, to heavy metals (e.g., lead, mercury, arsenic), pesticides (e.g., insecticides, herbicides, fungicides), solvents (e.g., paints, glues, gasoline) and carbon monoxide. Those who answered affirmatively were asked if they had become ill from the exposure.

In addition to jobs held before age 21, we asked subjects to recall their hobbies and other activities for the same time period. Subjects reported any involvement in activities involving leather, ceramics, house painting, oil painting, woodworking, engines or motors, wooden or plastic models, chemistry sets, and the application of fertilizer and bug or weed killer. Subjects were also asked to report the average number of hours per month or average number of times per month as well as the total number of years they had engaged in each of these activities. Information on demographic factors including age, gender, race, income and education was also obtained during the interview.

Analysis

Variables were constructed to characterize exposure in two ways: (1) dichotomously (any reported exposure vs. none), and (2) ordinally according to increasing dose. For jobs relating to lawn care, garden care, painting and cleaning, we used frequency as a measure of dose. For exposures to pesticides, heavy metals, solvents and carbon monoxide, we constructed a variable defined by no exposure, exposed but not ill, and exposed and became ill. For hobbies and other activities, cumulative hours of exposure were first calculated as continuous variables from information on frequency and duration, and then transformed into ordinal variables with three categories (none, low, and high) using the median value among exposed controls as the cutoff between low and high exposure. For activities involving fertilizer and bug or weed killer, exposure was calculated in terms of cumulative frequency of application. All dose variables contained an unexposed category that served as the reference group.

We used percentages to represent prevalence of characteristics and exposures among cases and controls. Because controls were selected according to age- and gender-specific selection probabilities, percentages were weighted by the inverse of the final selection probability to obtain accurate proportions for each characteristic. We used unconditional multivariate logistic regression to obtain estimated odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) adjusted for African American race and household income, and adapted to account for the sampling mechanism for controls (Weinberg and Sandler, 1991). Because household income was missing for five cases and seven controls, we multiply imputed household income using age, African American race, interview year and case status as predictors (van Buuren et al., 1999). Imputation was performed using the mice package in R statistical language, version 2.5 (R Development Core Team, 2006). Tests for linear trend were conducted for all dose variables by placing continuous variables of dose into the logistic models. We assessed the significance of linear trends using the Wald test. All other analyses were conducted in Stata version 9.2 (StataCorp, 2006).

As a secondary analysis to address potential disease misclassification, we restricted our analyses to 45 cases with cataplexy as determined by review of medical chart by a study neurologist, or a positive screen developed by Anic-Labat (Anic-Labat, 1999).

Results

Cases and controls did not differ significantly in regards to age and gender, as intended under the control-selection protocol (Table 1). However, cases were significantly more likely to be African American, and significantly less likely to have higher levels of household income. Given these imbalances, all regression models were adjusted for African American race and income. Cases had a mean age of onset of 13.8 (SD=6.6).

Table 1.

Sociodemographic Characteristics of Cases and Controls Ages 18-50

Who Were Also Positive for HLA DQB1*0602

Percentages*

Characteristic Cases
(N=67)
Controls
(N=95)
OR 95% CI Linear Trend P-value
Age group (years) (matched)
 18-25 22.4 18.0 1.0 Reference
 26-30 13.4 18.2 0.5 0.2, 1.5
 31-35 22.4 17.7 0.8 0.3, 2.2
 36-40 7.5 15.8 0.2 0.1, 0.8
 41-45 16.4 17.2 0.7 0.2, 1.9
 46-50 14.9 13.0 0.7 0.2, 2.2
Gender
 Male 29.9 41.9 1.0 Reference (matched)
 Female 70.1 58.1 1.1 0.5, 2.4
Race
 White 80.6 92.1 1.0 Reference
 African-American 14.9 1.4 8.2 2.1, 31.1
 Asian 1.5 0.9 1.2 0.1, 13.8
 Hispanic 1.5 0.5 2.5 0.2, 40.2
 Other 1.5 5.1 0.3 0.0, 2.6
Annual income (after imputation) <0.001
 Under $20,000 39.7 18.2 1.0 Reference
 $20,000-$39,999 18.8 23.3 0.3 0.1, 0.8
 $40,000 - $59,999 17.6 11.7 0.5 0.2, 1.6
 $60,000+ 23.9 46.8 0.2 0.1, 0.4
Education 0.15
 High school or less 28.4 24.3 1.0 Reference
 College 59.7 54.8 0.9 0.4, 1.9
 Graduate School 11.9 20.9 0.4 0.1, 1.2
*

Percentages were weighted by the inverse of the final selection probability

Among jobs that subjects performed around the house or in their neighborhoods before the age of 21, only painting projects suggested an association with narcolepsy in both the primary and sensitivity analyses (Table 2).

Table 2.

Risk of Narcolepsy Associated With Jobs Performed by Subjects Around the House or in Their Neighborhoods Before the Age of 21

Among 67 cases with and without cataplexy, and 95 controls Among 45 cases with cataplexy, and 95 controls


Percentages*

Exposure Cases Controls OR 95% CI P-value OR 95% CI P-value
CUT LAWN
 Any exposure 59.7 75.2 0.6 0.3, 1.3 0.2 0.4 0.2, 0.9 0.03
 Frequency of exposure 0.5 0.3
  None 40.9 24.8 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
  Infrequently 12.1 19.5 0.3 0.1, 0.9 0.2 0.5, 0.6
  Irregularly for years 15.2 9.6 2.1 0.7, 6.3 0.7 0.2, 2.8
  Regularly for years 31.8 46.1 0.6 0.2, 1.3 0.5 0.2, 1.3
OTHER LAWN CARE
 Any exposure 62.7 71.7 0.8 0.4, 1.8 0.6 0.7 0.3, 1.7 0.4
 Frequency of exposure 0.7 0.9
  None 38.5 28.3 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
  Infrequently 15.4 12.2 1.0 0.4, 3.1 0.4 0.1, 1.7
  Irregularly for years 13.9 26.0 0.5 0.2, 1.3 0.4 0.1, 1.4
  Regularly for years 32.3 33.5 1.0 0.4, 2.5 1.1 0.4, 3.0
GARDEN WORK
 Any exposure 70.1 68.9 1.3 0.6, 2.8 0.4 1.2 0.5, 2.6 0.7
 Frequency of exposure 0.9 0.8
  None 30.3 31.1 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
  Infrequently 21.2 11.9 2.4 0.8, 6.5 2.3 0.7,7.2
  Irregularly for years 13.6 23.7 0.8 0.3, 2.3 0.6 0.2, 2.1
  Regularly for years 34.9 33.3 1.2 0.5, 2.8 1.1 0.4, 2.9
PAINTING PROJECTS
 Any exposure 61.2 56.9 1.7 0.9, 3.5 0.13 1.7 0.8, 3.8 0.2
 Frequency of exposure 0.031 0.031
  None 40.0 43.1 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
  Infrequently 32.2 42.9 1.2 0.5, 2.6 1.0 0.4, 2.6
  Irregularly for years 21.5 11.6 2.4 0.9, 6.7 2.6 0.8, 8.3
  Regularly for years 6.2 2.4 5.2 0.8, 35.0 8.4 1.0, 73.2
ODD JOBS
 Any exposure 58.2 54.9 1.1 0.5, 2.2 0.9 1.2 0.5, 2.7 0.7
 Frequency of exposure 0.8 0.3
  None 41.8 45.8 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
  Infrequently 4.5 4.6 0.7 0.1, 2.1 0.3 0.03, 2.7
  Irregularly for years 14.9 8.9 1.9 0.6, 6.0 1.4 0.3, 5.6
  Regularly for years 38.8 40.7 1.0 0.5, 2.2 1.4 0.6, 3.4
CLEANING INDOORS
 Any exposure 98.5 95.8 1.5 0.1, 19.1 0.8 0.8 0.05, 12.3 0.9
 Frequency 0.5 0.6
  None 1.5 4.2 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
  Infrequently 4.5 5.6 0.9 0.5, 17.7 0.6 0.02, 13.5
  Irregularly for years 22.4 13.7 3.6 0.3, 53.3 1.5 0.08, 25.4
  Regularly for years 71.6 76.4 1.2 0.9, 15.7 0.7 0.05, 11.2
*

Percentages were weighted by the inverse of the final selection probability

Cataplexy defined as present after medical chart review, or by a positive screen developed by Anic-Labat and colleagues.

p-value from Wald test in multivariate model adjusted for African American race and household income; for ordinal variables with ≥2 categories, Wald test of continuous variable constitutes test for linear trend

Risk of narcolepsy was significantly increased over two-fold for jobs exposing subjects to toxins in general (Table 3), with subjects naming a variety of exposures including chlorine, mold, cleaning products, kerosene, asbestos, fiberglass, jet fuel, mace, and fertilizer. A nearly five-fold significant increase in risk of narcolepsy was observed for jobs with exposure to heavy metals such as lead, mercury or arsenic, with a significant trend according to severity of illness from exposure.

Table 3.

Risk of Narcolepsy Associated With Jobs Where Subjects Reported Exposure to Toxins Before the Age of 21

Among 67 cases with and without cataplexy, and 95 controls Among 45 cases with cataplexy, and 95 controls


Percentages*

Exposure Cases Controls OR 95% CI P-value OR 95% CI P-value
TOXINS
 Any exposure 23.8 12.7 2.6 1.0, 6.9 0.053 2.0 0.6, 6.5 0.3
HEAVY METALS
 Any exposure 17.7 7.9 4.7 1.5, 14.5 0.006 3.1 0.9, 11.4 0.09
 Severity of illness 0.027 0.2
  Not exposed 82.3 89.8 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
  Not ill 16.1 9.2 3.4 1.1, 10.3 2.0 0.6, 7.5
  Ill 1.6 1.0 3.7 0.2, 69.4 5.6 0.2, 132.7
PESTICIDES
 Any exposure 24.6 21.1 2.1 0.9, 5.2 0.092 2.2 0.8, 6.7 0.15
 Severity of Illness 0.2 0.3
  Not exposed 76.6 77.9 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
  Not ill 21.9 22.1 1.6 0.7, 4.0 1.7 0.6, 5.0
  Ill 1.6 0.0 Inf n/a Inf n/a
SOLVENTS
 Any exposure 40.3 40.4 1.1 0.6, 2.4 0.7 1.1 0.5, 2.4 0.9
 Severity of Illness 0.8 0.8
  Not exposed 59.7 59.6 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
  Not ill 34.3 33.6 1.2 0.5, 2.5 1.0 0.4, 2.4
  Ill 6.0 6.8 1.0 0.2, 5.1 1.3 0.2, 7.8
CARBON MONOXIDE
 Any exposure 11.9 7.5 2.1 0.6, 7.0 0.2 1.9 0.5, 7.8 0.4
 Severity of Illness 0.064
  Not exposed 88.1 92.5 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
  Not ill 4.5 7.5 0.5 0.1, 2.5 0.5 0.8, 3.0
  Ill 7.4 0.0 Inf n/a Inf n/a
*

Percentages were weighted by the inverse of the final selection probability

Cataplexy defined as present after medical chart review, or by a positive screen developed by Anic-Labat and colleagues.

p-value from Wald test in multivariate model adjusted for African American race and household income; for ordinal variables with ≥2 categories, Wald test of continuous variable constitutes test for linear trend

Risk of narcolepsy was significantly increased in the highest exposure category for woodworking, applications of fertilizer, and applications of bug or weed killer (Table 4). A significant trend was only observed for cumulative applications of bug or weed killer (p<0.02).

Table 4.

Risk of Narcolepsy Associated With Hobbies and Other Non-Vocational Activities That Subjects Performed Before the Age of 21

Among 67 cases with and without cataplexy, and 95 controls Among 45 cases with cataplexy, and 95 controls


Percentages*

Exposure Cases Controls OR 95% CI P-value OR 95% CI P-value
LEATHER WORK
 Any exposure 14.9 7.9 1.3 0.4, 4.1 0.6 1.5 0.5, 5.3 0.4
 Cumulative hours 0.6 0.9
 None 85.1 92.1 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
 1-479 11.9 3.1 3.7 0.9, 14.4 4.5 1.0, 20.0
 480+ 3.0 4.8 0.2 0.02, 1.2 0.3 0.04, 2.0
CERAMICS
 Any exposure 23.9 14.7 2.0 0.9, 4.7 0.11 2.8 1.1, 7.0 0.034
 Cumulative hours 0.11 0.030
  None 76.1 85.3 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
  1-239 10.5 7.2 1.7 0.5, 5.8 2.1 0.5, 8.5
  240+ 13.4 7.5 2.3 0.7, 6.8 3.3 1.0, 20.8
HOUSE PAINTING
 Any exposure 13.4 15.7 1.0 0.3, 3.1 1.0 1.3 0.4, 4.5 0.7
 Cumulative hours 1.0 0.8
  None 86.6 84.3 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
  1-83 6.0 5.0 1.0 0.2, 5.4 1.6 0.3, 8.8
  84+ 7.4 10.7 1.0 0.2, 4.2 1.0 0.2, 5.9
OIL PAINTING
 Any exposure 10.5 7.2 1.3 0.3, 4.8 0.7 1.2 0.3, 4.9 0.8
 Cumulative hours 0.8 0.9
  None 89.6 92.8 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
  1-119 4.5 1.5 1.8 0.3, 12.5 2.5 0.4, 17.1
  120+ 5.9 5.7 1.0 0.2, 5.3 0.5 0.06, 3.9
WOODWORKING
 Any exposure 23.4 16.0 1.6 0.6, 3.9 0.3 1.5 0.6, 4.2 0.4
 Cumulative hours 0.07 0.2
  None 77.6 84.5 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
  1-119 6.0 6.3 0.7 0.2, 3.4 1.0 0.2, 5.2
  120+ 16.4 9.2 3.0 1.0, 8.9 2.5 0.7, 8.7
ENGINES OR MOTORS
 Any exposure 13.4 15.9 0.9 0.3, 2.4 0.8 0.8 0.2, 2.5 0.7
 Cumulative hours 0.9 0.5
  None 86.6 84.1 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
  1-179 4.5 6.6 0.8 0.2, 3.4 1.3 0.3, 6.1
  180+ 8.9 9.3 1.0 0.3, 3.6 0.5 0.1, 2.4
WOODEN OR PLASTIC MODELS
 Any exposure 14.9 23.0 0.7 0.3, 1.8 0.5 0.5 0.2, 1.5 0.2
 Cumulative hours 0.7 0.4
  None 85.1 77.0 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
  1-155 6.0 9.6 0.4 0.1, 1.8 0.2 0.03, 1.1
  156+ 8.9 13.4 1.0 0.3, 3.3 1.0 0.2, 3.8
CHEMISTRY SETS
 Any exposure 31.4 23.2 1.5 0.6, 3.4 0.4 1.3 0.5, 3.3 0.6
 Cumulative hours 0.2 0.6
  None 68.7 76.8 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
  1-119 10.5 12.7 1.0 0.3, 3.4 1.2 0.3, 4.5
  120+ 20.9 10.5 1.9 0.7, 5.1 1.3 0.4, 4.0
APPLICATION OF FERTILIZER
 Any exposure 26.9 19.7 1.8 0.7, 4.6 0.2 1.4 0.4, 3.9 0.6
 Cumulative times 0.08 0.4
  No 73.1 80.3 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
  1-3 4.5 9.7 0.4 0.1, 2.3 0.6 0.1, 3.6
  4+ 22.4 10.0 3.1 1.1, 9.1 1.9 0.6, 5.6
APPLICATION OF BUG OR WEED KILLER
 Any exposure 23.9 14.2 2.1 0.8, 5.4 0.12 1.6 0.5, 4.9 0.4
 Cumulative times 0.02 0.026
  None 76.1 85.8 1.0 Reference 1.0 Reference
  1-5 3.0 7.6 0.3 0.04, 1.9 -- --
  6+ 20.9 6.6 4.5 1.5, 13.4 4.0 1.2, 13.8
*

Percentages were weighted by the inverse of the final selection probability

Cataplexy defined as present after medical chart review, or by a positive screen developed by Anic-Labat and colleagues.

p-value from Wald test in multivariate model adjusted for African American race and household income; for ordinal variables with ≥2 categories, Wald test of continuous variable constitutes test for linear trend

When we restricted our analysis to cases with cataplexy, stronger estimates of association emerged for lawn cutting (OR=0.4; 95% CI: 0.2, 0.9), hobbies involving ceramics (OR=2.8; 95% CI; 1.1, 7.0), and for the highest exposure category to painting projects (OR=8.4; 95% CI; 1.0, 73.2). Estimates of association became non-significant for toxins, and heavy metals. Results were notably weakened only for application of fertilizer, with estimates for dichotomous exposure, cumulative exposure, and linear trend all attenuated.

Discussion

In this population-based study in which all subjects had HLA DQB1*0602, we found a nearly five-fold increase in narcolepsy risk associated with jobs containing heavy metals and with six or more applications of bug or weed killer. Risk was also elevated, but less impressively so, for job-related exposures to pesticides, toxins, and paint; for activities involving ceramics; and for the highest exposure level to activities involving wood. Results were similar when cases were restricted to those with cataplexy.

Our study is limited by possibly incomplete ascertainment of all population-based cases, small sample size and hence low statistical power to detect more subtle associations, and a multiple comparison issue arising from the exploratory nature of our aims. Because clinical sleep studies are not feasible to incorporate in the recruitment strategy for a population-based epidemiologic study, we may have sacrificed some diagnostic precision typically afforded by measures obtained in sleep laboratories. We did attempt to address disease misclassification by repeating our analyses restricted to cases with HLA DQB1*0602 who also had cataplexy. In doing so, however, we found stronger associations for certain exposures, perhaps reflecting reduced disease misclassification, but other estimates lost statistical significance, possibly due to a reduction in power. In addition, although the overall response rate of 37% among controls is typical of that documented in recent studies using similar randomized digit-dialing techniques (Bunin et al., 2007), our study is vulnerable to selection bias as a result. We attempted to control for this bias by adjusting for African-American race and annual household income, both of which were significantly associated with case-control status in our study. Also, because we relied exclusively on questionnaires to examine past exposures, our results are vulnerable to differential recall bias, whereby cases may be more likely than controls to recall exposures. We attempted to limit our assessment of exposures to an etiologically relevant time window by specifying that subjects report exposures before age 21; however because the mean age of symptom onset reported by cases was 13.8 (SD = 6.6), our exposures may have included those occurring in a non-relevant time period for some early-onset cases. Because our study was exploratory in nature, we questioned subjects not only on a broad range of topics but also in terms of broad categories of exposure. Pesticides, for instance, encompasses an array of compounds used to deter or kill insects (insecticides), rodents (rodenticides), plants (herbicides), and fungi (fungicides) (Hatcher et al., 2008). Similarly, our findings for toxins, metals, and activities involving woodwork, ceramics and painting projects represent broad exposure categories that may each encompass a variety of constituents. Which specific factors may drive the observed associations remains to be determined.

Numerous studies have been conducted assessing certain risk factors for narcolepsy including obesity, immunologic and genetic factors, and associated diseases (Longstreth et al., 2007), but few have evaluated environmental exposures as potential risk factors for narcolepsy. Picchioni and colleagues assessed infectious agents and psychological stress as risk factors for narcolepsy (Picchioni et al., 2007). They observed an increased risk of narcolepsy from flu infections, unexplained fevers, and major change in sleep habits; the authors did not ask about exposures related to toxins. Orellano and colleagues conducted a case-control study and observed the life events in the year preceding age of onset were significantly associated with narcolepsy risk. (Orellana et al., 1994). However, neither of these two studies matched controls to cases by HLA status, a limitation that was noted in both reports.

Deficient in the literature is an understanding of how environmental toxins may contribute to narcolepsy risk. Solvents, pesticides and heavy metals such as mercury, lead, and arsenic are widely known to be neurotoxic (Firestone and Longstreth, 1994; Graham Doyle G et al., 2003). One possible mechanism is that toxins can directly injure the brain. Pesticides may be of particular interest because almost 40% of all pesticides used in the United States contain organophosphorous (OP) compounds (Feldman, 1999), and because OP compounds inhibit cholinesterase resulting in excess acetylcholine, a neurotransmitter used in the circuitry for regulation of sleep, wakefulness, and arousal (Nieuwenhuys et al., 2008). Possibly, OP compounds affect acetylcholine-rich areas of the midbrain, disturbing the sleep-wake cycle involved in narcolepsy. In humans, exposure to OP compounds has been associated with difficulty in maintaining alertness, lethargy, drowsiness, sleep problems and other narcoleptic-like symptoms (Metcalf and Holmes, 1969), and many OP compounds have been shown to increase rapid eye movement sleep by inhibiting acetylcholinesterase (Duffy et al., 1979; Metcalf and Holmes, 1969). Hypersensitivity to acetylcholine has also been implicated in the pathophysiology of narcolepsy (Kilduff et al., 1986; Mignot, 2000; Nishino and Mignot, 1997). Another plausible mechanism involves excitotoxicity, a process by which neurons are damaged or killed as a result of activation of excitatory amino acids (Hauser and Beal, 2008). In the case of narcolepsy, glutamate-induced excitotoxity may play a role in selectively damaging hypocretin neurons. Katsuki and colleagues demonstrated that hypocretin neurons were selectively and irreversibly damaged by N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA), an excitotoxin that mimics the neurotransmitter glutamate (Katsuki and Akaike, 2004). Hypocretin neurons contain both functional and non-functional NMDA receptors, and their electrical activity appears to be influenced by excitatory glutaminergic input (Katsuki and Akaike, 2004). Therefore, any exogenous or endogenous compound with the potential to elicit glutamate-related toxicity should be explored as a potential risk factor. Finally, given the mounting evidence of an immunologic basis for narcolepsy (Black, 2005; Carlander et al., 1993; Cvetkovic-Lopes et al., 2010; Hallmayer et al., 2009; Mignot et al., 1995; Nepom, 1993), it is also possible that an environmental factor can trigger an autoimmune response leading to the selective destruction of hypocretins in the lateral hypothalamus. Such a mechanism for narcolepsy has been recently speculated for exposure to streptococcal infections (Koepsell et al., 2010; Longstreth et al., 2009b) and second hand smoke (Ton et al., 2009).

Our findings for narcolepsy provide etiologic clues that warrant not only replication by future studies but also attempts to identify the specific environmental toxins that may plausibly increase the risk of narcolepsy among genetically susceptible individuals.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke [grant number R01 NS38523]. The authors thank Dr. Vivian Gersuk and Dr. Gerald Nepom for assistance with HLA genotyping and Ms. Audrey Hendrickson and Dr. Sarah Velde for study coordination. This work was presented at the 2009 regional meeting in Seattle, Washington of the World Federation of Neurology Neuroepidemiology Research Group. The authors do not have any conflicts of interest.

The University of Washington's institutional review board approved the study.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Anic-Labat S, et al. Validation of a cataplexy questionnaire in 983 sleep-disorders patients. Sleep. 1999;22:77–87. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anic-Labat S, Guilleminault C, Kraemer HC, Meehan J, Arrigoni J, Mignot E. Validation of a cataplexy questionnaire in 983 sleep-disorder patients. Sleep. 1999;22:77–87. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anonymous. Sleepiness in America. The Gallup Organization; Princeton, NJ: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  4. Baldwin CM, et al. Associations between gender and measures of daytime somnolence in the Sleep Heart Health Study. Sleep. 2004;27:305–11. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.2.305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Black JL., 3rd Narcolepsy: a review of evidence for autoimmune diathesis. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2005;17:461–9. doi: 10.1080/02646830500381492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bunin GR, et al. Secular trends in response rates for controls selected by random digit dialing in childhood cancer studies: a report from the Children's Oncology Group. Am J Epidemiol. 2007;166:109–16. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Carlander B, et al. Autoimmune hypothesis in narcolepsy. Neurophysiol Clin. 1993;23:15–22. doi: 10.1016/s0987-7053(05)80279-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chabas D, et al. The genetics of narcolepsy. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet. 2003;4:459–83. doi: 10.1146/annurev.genom.4.070802.110432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Chervin RD, Aldrich MS. The Epworth Sleepiness Scale may not reflect objective measures of sleepiness or sleep apnea. Neurology. 1999;52:125–31. doi: 10.1212/wnl.52.1.125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cvetkovic-Lopes V, et al. Elevated Tribbles homolog 2-specific antibody levels in narcolepsy patients. J Clin Invest. 2010;120:713–9. doi: 10.1172/JCI41366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Douglass AB, et al. The Sleep Disorders Questionnaire. I: Creation and multivariate structure of SDQ. Sleep. 1994;17:160–7. doi: 10.1093/sleep/17.2.160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Duffy FH, et al. Long-term effects of an organophosphate upon the human electroencephalogram. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 1979;47:161–76. doi: 10.1016/0041-008x(79)90083-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Feldman RG. Organophosphorous Compounds. In: John JR, editor. Occupational and Environmental Neurotoxicology. Lippencott-Raven; Philadelphia: 1999. p. 500. [Google Scholar]
  14. Firestone J, Longstreth W. Central Nervous System Diseases. In: Rosenstock L, et al., editors. Textbook of clinical occupational and environmental medicine. Saunders; Philadelphia: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  15. Gersuk VH, Nepom GT. A real-time polymerase chain reaction assay for the rapid identification of the autoimmune disease-associated allele HLA-DQB1*0602. Tissue Antigens. 2009;73:335–340. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-0039.2009.01219.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Graham Doyle G, et al. “Chapter 16: Toxic Responses of the Nervous System” (Chapter) In: Curtis JBWI, Klaassen D, editors. Casarett & Doull's Essentials of Toxicology. The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  17. Hallmayer J, et al. Narcolepsy is strongly associated with the T-cell receptor alpha locus. Nat Genet. 2009;41:708–11. doi: 10.1038/ng.372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hatcher JM, et al. Parkinson's disease and pesticides: a toxicological perspective. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2008;29:322–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2008.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hauser SL, Beal MF. Chapter 360. Mechanisms of Neurologic Diseases. In: B E, Kasper Dennis L, Fauci Anthony S, Hauser Stephen L, Longo Dan L, Jameson J Larry, Isselbacher Kurt J, editors. Harrison's Principles of Internal Medicine. The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc.; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  20. Hublin C, et al. The Ullanlinna Narcolepsy Scale: validation of a measure of symptoms in the narcoleptic syndrome. J Sleep Res. 1994;3:52–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1994.tb00104.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep. 1991;14:540–5. doi: 10.1093/sleep/14.6.540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Katsuki H, Akaike A. Excitotoxic degeneration of hypothalamic orexin neurons in slice culture. Neurobiol Dis. 2004;15:61–9. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2003.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kilduff TS, et al. Muscarinic cholinergic receptors and the canine model of narcolepsy. Sleep. 1986;9:102–6. doi: 10.1093/sleep/9.1.102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Koepsell TD, et al. Medical exposures in youth and frequency of narcolepsy: A population-based case-control study in persons with HLA DQB1*0602. Journal of Sleep Research. 2009 doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00756.x. In Press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Koepsell TD, et al. Medical exposures in youth and the frequency of narcolepsy with cataplexy: a population-based case-control study in genetically predisposed people. J Sleep Res. 2010;19:80–86. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00756.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Longstreth WT, Jr, et al. The epidemiology of narcolepsy. Sleep. 2007;30:13–26. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.1.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Longstreth WT, Jr, et al. Prevalence of narcolepsy in King County, Washington, USA. Sleep Med. 2009a;10:422–6. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2008.05.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Longstreth WT, Jr, et al. Narcolepsy and streptococcal infections. Sleep. 2009b;32:1548. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.12.1548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Metcalf DR, Holmes JH. VII. Toxicology and physiology. EEG, psychological, and neurological alterations in humans with organophosphorus exposure. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1969;160:357–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1969.tb15857.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Mignot E. Genetic and familial aspects of narcolepsy. Neurology. 1998;50(suppl 1):S16–S22. doi: 10.1212/wnl.50.2_suppl_1.s16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Mignot E. Pathophysiology of narcolepsy. In: Kryger MH, et al., editors. Principles and practices of sleep medicine. W.B. Saunders Company; Philadelphia: 2000. pp. 663–675. [Google Scholar]
  32. Mignot E, et al. Narcolepsy and immunity. Adv Neuroimmunol. 1995;5:23–37. doi: 10.1016/0960-5428(94)00043-n. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Nepom GT. MHC and autoimmune diseases. In: Bach JF, editor. Monoclonal Antibodies and Peptide Therapy in Autoimmune Diseases. Marcel Dekker; New York: 1993. pp. 143–164. [Google Scholar]
  34. Nieuwenhuys R, et al. The Human Central Nervous System. Springer; Berlin: 2008. Diencephalon: Hypothalamus; p. 967. [Google Scholar]
  35. Nishino S, Mignot E. Pharmacological aspects of human and canine narcolepsy. Prog Neurobiol. 1997;52:27–78. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(96)00070-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Orellana C, et al. Life events in the year preceding the onset of narcolepsy. Sleep. 1994;17(Suppl):S50–S53. doi: 10.1093/sleep/17.suppl_8.s50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Peyron C, et al. A mutation in a case of early onset narcolepsy and a generalized absence of hypocretin peptides in human narcoleptic brains. Nat Med. 2000;6:991–7. doi: 10.1038/79690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Picchioni D, et al. A case-control study of the environmental risk factors for narcolepsy. Neuroepidemiology. 2007;29:185–92. doi: 10.1159/000111581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Thannickal TC, et al. Reduced number of hypocretin neurons in human narcolepsy. Neuron. 2000;27:469–74. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00058-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Ton TG, et al. Active and passive smoking and risk of narcolepsy in people with HLA DQB1*0602: a population-based case-control study. Neuroepidemiology. 2009;32:114–21. doi: 10.1159/000177037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. van Buuren S, et al. Multiple imputation of missing blood pressure covariates in survival analysis. Stat Med. 1999;18:681–94. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-0258(19990330)18:6<681::aid-sim71>3.0.co;2-r. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Ware JEJ, Gandek B. Overview of the SF-36 Health Survey and the International Quality of Life Assessment (IQOLA) Project. J Clin Epidemiol. 1998;51:903–12. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(98)00081-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Weinberg CR, Sandler DP. Randomized recruitment in case-control studies. Am J Epidemiol. 1991;134:421–32. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a116104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Yoss RE, Daly DD. Criteria for the diagnosis of the narcoleptic syndrome. Mayo Clin Proc. 1957;32:320–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES