Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: Soc Sci Res. 2010 Sep 1;39(5):814–830. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2010.03.002

ARE BOTH PARENTS ALWAYS BETTER THAN ONE? PARENTAL CONFLICT AND YOUNG ADULT WELL-BEING *

Kelly Musick 1, Ann Meier 2
PMCID: PMC2930824  NIHMSID: NIHMS195431  PMID: 20824195

Abstract

Using data from three waves of the National Survey of Families and Households (N=1,963), we examine associations between adolescent family experiences and young adult well-being across a range of indicators, including schooling, substance use, and family-related transitions. We compare children living with both biological parents, but whose parents differ in how often they argue, to children in stepfather and single-mother families, and we assess the extent to which differences can be understood in terms of family income and parenting practices. Findings suggest that parental conflict is associated with children’s poorer academic achievement, increased substance use, and early family formation and dissolution. Living in single mother and stepfather families tend to be more strongly associated with our indicators of well-being, although differences between these family types and living with high conflict continuously married parents are often statistically indistinguishable. Income and parenting largely do not account for associations between adolescent family type and later life outcomes. We conclude that while children do better, on average, living with two biological married parents, the advantages of two-parent families are not shared equally by all.

Keywords: family structure, parental conflict, transition to adulthood, parenting


The association between family structure and child well-being is frequently cited: children who grow up with two married parents tend to fare better than others (for reviews see Amato, 2005; McLanahan & Sandefur, 1994; Sigle-Rushton & McLanahan, 2004). Most studies of family structure compare children in single-parent and stepparent families to those living with their married, biological parents, treating these marriages as a homogenous group. A somewhat distinct body of work shows the importance of parental conflict for child outcomes. Children whose parents often argue score worse on measures of academic achievement, behavior problems, psychological well-being, and adult relationship quality; they are also more likely to form families early and outside of marriage (Amato & Sobolewski, 2001; Booth & Amato, 2001; Booth & Edwards, 1990; Davies & Cummings, 1994; Emery, 1982; Furstenberg & Teitler, 1994; Grych & Fincham, 1990; Hanson, 1999; Jekielek, 1998; Morrison & Coiro, 1999; Musick & Bumpass, 1999). Examining variation in conflict between married parents is important for social scientists because it expands our understanding of how families matter for children. It is also important for the broader public, with marriage emerging high on the U.S. policy agenda in recent years as a tool for improving child outcomes (Nock, 2005). Increasing marriage rates was an explicit goal of the 1996 welfare reform legislation and a key piece of the latest welfare reform re-authorization package (Ooms, 2007; U.S. DHHS 2008). The success of marriage promotion for the sake of children depends not just on the overall association between marriage and child well-being, but on how this association varies across marriages.

Much of the demographic research on parental conflict and child outcomes stems from an interest in the divorce process (e.g., Booth & Amato, 2001; Booth & Edwards, 1990; Furstenberge & Teitler, 1994; Hanson, 1999). Studies of this sort typically follow children living with continuously married parents and examine the role of parental conflict in explaining or conditioning the effects of subsequent marital disruption. But many poor quality marriages survive, and children may experience parental conflict independent of divorce. Our analysis sets up a comparison to address whether children fare better living with both parents than living with just one, in particular, when parents do not get along. We compare child outcomes across single-parent, stepparent, and high conflict continuously married-parent family types and test key explanations for observed associations. We do this over a range of outcomes covering various dimensions of children’s development and well-being in young adulthood. This work relies on all three waves of the National Survey of Families and Households (NSFH); to our knowledge, it is the first to use the recently fielded third wave to investigate these questions. The NSFH is uniquely suited to our study, with rich, prospective data from multiple members of the same family over time, including both parents’ self-reports of marital conflict and children’s self-reports on a range of outcomes. We provide a broad descriptive portrait of family structure, parental conflict, and child well-being, bringing together literatures on family structure and marital conflict.

FAMILY STRUCTURE, CONFLICT, AND CHILD WELL-BEING

Growing up without both parents is associated with a host of poor child outcomes. Children from single-parent and stepparent families have higher poverty rates and lower levels of educational and occupational attainment than children who grow up with both their biological or adoptive parents (Astone & McLanahan, 1991; Biblarz & Raftery, 1993, 1999; DeLeire & Kalil, 2002; Kiernan, 1992; McLanahan & Sandefur, 1994; Wojtkiewicz, 1993). They report greater substance use and risk-taking behavior, such as smoking, drinking, and drug use (Carlson, 2006; DeLeire & Kalil, 2002; Hoffmann & Johnson, 1998). Further, these children are more likely to have sex at an early age (Davis & Friel, 2001; Thornton & Camburn, 1989), to be young and unmarried when they form their families (Cherlin, Kiernan, & Chase-Lansdale, 1995; Kiernan 1992; Kiernan & Hobcraft, 1997; McLanahan & Sandefur, 1994; Thornton 1991; Wu 1996), and to experience the dissolution of their own romantic unions (Amato & DeBoer, 2001; Kiernan & Cherlin, 1999; McLanahan & Bumpass, 1988; Wolfinger 1999). Most of this literature treats continuously married-parent families as a single, homogenous group.

Another line of research has devoted attention to variation within continuously married two-parent families, particularly with respect to marital conflict. Children whose parents often argue fare worse than those whose parents get along: parental conflict is associated with negative schooling outcomes (Hanson, 1999), behavior problems (Morrison & Coiro, 1999), early and nonmarital family formation (Furstenberg & Teitler, 1994; Musick & Bumpass, 1999), lower quality adult relationships (Amato & Booth, 2001; Booth & Edwards, 1990), and lower psychological well-being (Amato & Sobolewski, 2001; Jekielek, 1998). Much of this work focuses on continuously married-parent families at initial observation and treats conflict as either a selection or moderating factor in the divorce process. Controlling for pre-disruption marital conflict, studies typically report that it accounts for some, but not all, of the association between marital disruption and academic achievement, problem behaviors, family-related transitions, and subjective well-being (Cherlin et al., 1991; Furstenberg & Teitler, 1994; Hanson, 1999). Testing the moderating effect of conflict on divorce (i.e., the interaction between conflict and divorce), many find weaker negative associations between divorce and child outcomes in the case of high conflict marriages, suggesting that divorce may bring relief from the stress of high conflict family environments (Amato & Booth, 1997; Amato, Spencer Loomis, & Booth, 1995; Booth & Amato, 2001; Hanson, 1999; Jekielek, 1998; Strohschein, 2005).

A few studies compare differences in child well-being by family structure, accounting for heterogeneity among continuously married-parent families in parental conflict – these are closest to what we set out to do. Musick and Bumpass (1999) use data from the first two waves of the NSHF to examine associations between adolescent family type and children’s transitions to adulthood (measured when children are 18–23); they further examine whether associations can be understood in terms of parents’ income, attitudes, and behaviors. The authors find similarities in associations between children’s outcomes and high conflict married-parent families on the one hand and single and stepparent families on the other, with children in these families less likely to graduate from high school and more likely to have sex and cohabit at an early age, compared to children from low conflict continuously married-parent families. Associations are, by and large, not explained by parents’ income, attitudes, or behaviors. This study is limited by its young sample, many of whom have not yet aged into adult transitions of interest. Amato and Sobolewski rely on data from the Marital Instability and the Life Course study to test associations between parental discord and divorce (measured when most children are under 19) and children’s young adult psychological well-being (measured when children are 19 to about 40). They find negative associations between both discord and divorce and young adult well-being mediated (Amato & Sobolewski, 2001) and moderated (Sobolewski and Amato, 2007) by closeness between parents and young adult children. Their data come from a 17-year study of individuals married at the first wave of data collection in 1980; they include child interviews in 1992 and 1997. Although many parents divorced over the study period, the sample excludes those who were divorced or unmarried at the first wave. Our analysis relies on new data to provide a broader examination of family structure, parental conflict, and child well-being. We focus on the family environment during children’s adolescence, include a range of young adult outcomes, and investigate key explanations for links between the two.

EXPLAINING FAMILY STRUCTURE AND CONFLICT ASSOCIATIONS

As suggested above, there is strong evidence that both family structure and parental conflict are associated with child well-being. What explains these associations, however, remains a difficult question, to which there is not a clear answer. Selection into single-parenthood on unmeasured characteristics undoubtedly accounts for some of the observed associations between family structure and child outcomes; indeed, a recent generation of studies using longitudinal designs and techniques to address selection report weaker and less consistent associations between growing up without both parents and children’s outcomes (Aughinbaugh, Pierret, & Rothstein, 2005; Cherlin, Chase-Lansdale, & McRae, 1998; Cherlin et al., 1991; Cherlin et al., 1995; Morrison & Cherlin, 1995; Sigle-Rushton, Hobcraft, & Kiernan, 2005; Strohschein, 2005; Sun, 2001). Individual characteristics may similarly select parents into conflict with their partners. Personality traits or mental health conditions, for example, may lead to poor marital relationships, and these traits may independently influence children via parenting or genetic inheritance (Amato, 2005; Gotlib, Lewinsohn, & Seely, 1998).

There are also compelling theoretical reasons to expect family structure and conflict effects on children. Family income and parenting practices, in particular, are two causal pathways that have been discussed extensively in the family structure literature. While estimates vary on how much is due to selection and how much is due to family structure, single-parent families are at much higher risk of poverty than other family types (Duncan & Rodgers, 1991; Eggebeen & Lichter, 1991; McLanahan & Percheski, 2008; Thomas & Sawhill, 2005), which in turn is linked to child health and well-being. Single parents manage work and child care without the help of a second resident parent, and less than 50% receive what is owed to them in child support payments from noncustodial parents (Grall, 2007). Up to half the association between single-parent families and children’s academic performance, teen and premarital childbearing, and idleness is due to the lower incomes of single-parent families (McLanahan & Sandefur, 1994; Thomson, Hanson, & McLanahan, 1994); income does not account for links between stepparent families and child outcomes (McLanahan & Sandefur, 1994; Thomson et al., 1994).

Parenting behaviors further account for differences in child well-being by family structure. Children typically fare best when parents maintain a strong parent-child bond, apply consistent discipline, and respond firmly but warmly to situations at home (Baumrind, 1991) – behaviors that are displayed more often among continuously married parents. Single parents balance the provision of financial support with solo care for children, which can lead to time pressure and stress (McLanahan & Booth, 1989; McLanahan & Percheski, 2008). Stepfamilies negotiate relationships for which rules are often not clearly defined (Cherlin, 1978). Children may compete with new spouses for parents’ time and attention, and stepparents may be less invested in non-biological children (White, 1994). Compared with continuously married parents, single parents are less emotionally supportive of their children, have fewer rules yet dispense harsher discipline, and provide less supervision; stepparents spend less time with children and offer less positive response and encouragement (Astone & McLanahan, 1991; McLanahan & Sandefur, 1994; Thomson, McLanahan, & Curtin, 1992). Parenting practices, including warmth, harshness, time, and involvement, typically explain less than 20% of the association between growing up without both parents, education outcomes, and family-related transitions (Astone & McLanahan, 1991; Musick & Bumpass, 1999; Thomson et al., 1994), although McLanahan & Sandefur (1994) find a stronger role of parenting in explaining high school dropout and idleness among children from single-parent families.

Family income is a selection factor into marital conflict (Conger, Conger & Elder, 1997); it is unlikely a pathway through which conflict affects children. As in the case of stepparent families, two parents remain in the household as potential earners and caretakers. Parenting behaviors, by contrast, may be an important causal mechanism linking parental conflict and child outcomes. Parental conflict may spill over to interactions with children (Erel & Burman, 1995), preoccupying parents and decreasing their availability and time for children. The stress associated with conflict may result in less warmth and harsher discipline (Fauber et al., 1990), and disagreements between parents may prevent their cooperation in decisions regarding child rearing, precluding the clear and consistent rule-setting and supervision that protects youth from risks (Grych & Fincham, 1990). Parents who often argue have weaker relationships with their children (Amato & Sobolewski, 2001; Musick & Bumpass, 1999; Sobolewski & Amato, 2007), relationships that further protect children from risk (Resnick et al., 1997). Musick and Bumpass (1999) find that parents’ time, warmth, negativity, and relationships with children mediate a small share – typically less than 10% – of the association between marital conflict and children’s transitions to adulthood; Amato and Sobolewski (2001) find that parent-child relationships mediate much of the association between marital conflict and children’s psychological well-being in young adulthood. We build on empirical estimates of the extent to which family income and parenting account for differential child outcomes by family structure and conflict, either as selection factors or causal pathways – findings (particularly from representative samples) that to date focus largely on single and stepparents compared to all continuously married parents.

ADOLESCENT FAMILY EXPERIENCES AND YOUNG ADULT WELL-BEING

We examine children’s family experiences during adolescence, a staging ground for the many educational, vocational, and relationship experiences leading into adulthood, experiences that can be both high stakes and hard to reverse. The sheer proximity of adolescence may make it consequential for the transition to adulthood. Moreover, the developmental changes of adolescence may increase the salience of family as an arena of comfort and stability (Shanahan, 2000). The timing and sequencing of the various demographic changes associated with the transition to adulthood are important for success in the subsequent life course (Hogan & Astone, 1986; Rindfuss, 1991). Markers of a “successful” transition to adulthood include financial security, dependable behavior, and stable relationships (Hogan & Astone, 1986; Moffitt, 1993; Oppenheimer, Kalmijn, & Lim, 1997). We examine multiple indicators in each of three related domains: academic achievement, risk-taking behavior, and family-related transitions.

For the purposes of this study, adolescence is defined as encompassing ages 10–18, when measures of children’s family experiences are assessed. Family structure, conflict, and parenting may have different meanings at the younger and older ends of this span, and the effects of family may further depend on the child’s age (Allison & Furstenberg, 1989; Duncan, Yeung, Brooks-Gunn, & Smith, 1998). Given these considerations, a narrower age band would be desirable, although it would lead to a loss of data that would potentially constrain our ability to discern true group differences from sampling error. Within the constraints of our sample, we explored the sensitivity of our results to the child’s age at family assessment, testing differences in the association between family experiences (family structure, conflict, and parenting) and child outcomes by child age at family assessment (10–14 versus 15–18); these tests yielded few significant differences.1 We control for child age in all models, and for certain outcomes, we narrow the age of child at family assessment to ensure that family experiences are measured prior to transitions of interest. Outcomes are assessed when children are in their teens to early thirties, always following our observation of adolescent family experiences, as just noted. Despite limitations, prior studies comparing children in high-conflict continuously married-parent families to those in other family types are not set up to as clearly delineate child age when families are observed (Amato & Sobolewski, 2001; Sobolewski & Amato, 2007), or to follow children so far into adulthood (e.g., Musick & Bumpass, 1999).

In the domain of academic achievement, we focus on high school graduation, high school grades, and college attendance. Adult economic well-being is highly stratified by educational attainment; years of schooling are linked to life-time earnings (Day & Newburger, 2002), health (House, 2002), and marital stability (Raley & Bumpass, 2003). Our risky behaviors include smoking, binge drinking, and marijuana use. Unlike some adolescent-limited risk behaviors (e.g., school misconduct), substance use may start early in life but persist into adulthood, as smoking and drinking become legally sanctioned, and all have physiologically addictive properties. In adulthood, substance use is associated with poor socioeconomic, health, and psychological outcomes (e.g., Gruber, 2001; Kandel, 2002). Finally, in the arena of family-related transitions, we examine early sexual initiation, early cohabitation, nonmarital fertility, and union disruption. While sex, union formation, and childbearing are clearly normative life course transitions, early sex and family formation may have negative consequences not associated with later transitions. Early first sex increases exposure to sexually transmitted diseases and nonmarital pregnancy (Alan Guttmacher Institute, 2002; Resnick et al., 1997), and early (often nonmarital) childbearing truncates educational attainment (Astone & Upchurch, 1989; Teti & Lamb, 1989). Early cohabitation may pull people out of the socialization and interactions that lead to successful partner selection, and young unions are less stable than those formed later (Raley & Bumpass, 2003).

We expect parental conflict and family structure to be associated with schooling, substance use, and family-related transitions, albeit through different pathways. Less parental time associated with parental conflict, single-parenthood, and step parenthood may affect children’s schooling via reduced help with homework; the lower incomes of single parents may constrain college attendance. Likewise, less time may increase children’s substance use via lower levels of supervision. The tendency of parents in high conflict marriages to engage in harsh parenting and their poorer relationships with children may further increase children’s substance use, as well as hasten children’s first sex and cohabitation, as youth look outside the home to peers and romantic partners for support. Early sex and cohabitation among children from single and stepparent families may be more a result of modeling the dating and nonmarital relationships that they see at home (Axinn & Thornton, 1996). Direct modeling of conflict (in the case of high conflict marriages) and the experience of divorce (in the case of single or stepparents) may also raise children’s own risk of union disruption. The transmission of attitudes about the importance and durability of marriage may further affect rates of union dissolution among children from high conflict, step, and single-parent families (Amato & DeBoer, 2001; Axinn & Thornton, 1996).

In sum, we expect that parental conflict and family structure will be associated with young adult well-being, and that parenting and income will account for these associations to varying degrees. For example, in the case of academic achievement, associations may run indirectly through time with mothers; but in the case of union dissolution, associations may be more directly linked to parental conflict and family structure. We hypothesize, then, that parenting plays a mediating role in some of the processes examined here, i.e., that family structure and conflict affect aspects of parenting, which in turn affect child outcomes. Income may also play a mediating role in the relationship between single-parent families and child outcomes, but it should have little to do with stepfamilies or parental conflict (i.e., outside of selection). We recognize that results based on observational data such as ours may ultimately be consistent with multiple interpretations; for example, with an unmeasured, extraneous factor that is correlated with – and drives relationships among – parenting behavior, family conflict, and child well-being. We flesh out this possibility in greater detail below.

PRESENT STUDY

The present study extends the literature on family structure, which generally means how parents’ marital and cohabiting histories sort children into single, step, and two biological-parent families, by highlighting diversity in the family experiences of children living with continuously married parents. We build conflict into our analysis, adding another family type – high conflict continuously married-parent families – to traditional measures of family structure. Drawing attention to conflict as an important family factor in and of itself, we shift the emphasis of the demographic literature on parental conflict, which tends to focus on conflict as a selection or conditioning variable in the divorce process. We extend research both on family structure and conflict by examining the roles played by income and parenting in accounting for differences in the young adult well-being of children from single, step, and high conflict continuously married-parent families.

We combine data from three waves of the NSFH, including the recently fielded third wave, in a way that maximizes the sample while matching children’s family experiences to the same life stage – adolescence. Of the major demographic studies to examine parental conflict, including the Marital Instability over the Life Course Study (e.g. Amato et al., 1995), the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (Jekeliek, 1998; Morrison & Coiro, 1999), and the National Survey of Children (Cherlin et al., 1991; Furstenberg & Teitler, 1994), the NSFH is the only one to include both parents’ reports of marital conflict. It also contains relatively rich information from parents about their own social class backgrounds, education trajectories, and family formation histories, which allows us to control for key factors that potentially select parents into subsequent family structures and conflict with partners. Further, it includes detailed, prospective data on income and parenting that make it possible to look inside family categories to better understand the processes through which families matter. Some of the parenting questions refer specifically to behaviors with respect to the focal child, as opposed to general parenting practices, providing more precise measures of children’s family experiences.

The scope of our analysis – starting with single, step, and continuously married-parent families, accounting for factors that potentially explain differences in child well-being across family type, and examining a range of outcomes associated with young adult well-being – allows us to draw a broad picture of family structure, parental conflict, and child outcomes. It also raises challenging issues. First, including high conflict continuously married-parent families highlights diversity in two-parent families that is often overlooked, but it also treats conflict and divorce as independent when, as noted earlier, research tends to find that pre-divorce conflict accounts for some of the association between divorce and subsequent child outcomes. More generally, observing family experiences at a single point in time, as we do, may result in an underestimate of the association between parental conflict and child well-being, relative to other family types. That is, we will underestimate parental conflict to the extent that young adults in other family types experienced it at some point in time, but not when it was assessed in the NSFH. Unmeasured conflict will be captured in lower levels of child well-being, blurring distinctions between the high and low conflict married-parent family types and inflating associations between step and single-parent families, relative to our high conflict married-parent families.2

Another challenge is parsing out selection and causation. We incorporate relatively rich controls that are prior to parents’ family-related decisions and conflict with partners, narrowing – but not eliminating – the full set of factors that potentially threaten causal inference. It would take much more detailed data to fully disentangle causal pathways from selection. In the meantime, we report patterns of association and are cautious not to make strong causal arguments; we present this analysis in the spirit of learning what we can with the data we have.

DATA AND METHODS

NSFH

The first wave of the NSFH was collected in 1987–1988 and involved interviews with over 13,000 respondents, including a main cross-section and an over-sample of Blacks, Puerto Ricans, Mexican Americans, single-parent families, families with stepchildren, cohabiting couples, and recently married persons. In each household, an adult was randomly selected as the primary respondent, and the spouse or cohabiting partner was asked to complete a shorter, self-administered questionnaire. The second wave (NSFH2) was fielded in 1992–1994, and it included interviews with current partners as well as ex-partners who were in the main respondent’s household at NSFH1. The most recent wave (NSFH3) was fielded in 2001–2002. Of particular interest to this study, a focal child was randomly selected from the household roster at NSFH1 and followed over the subsequent surveys. At NSFH1, primary respondents provided information on the focal child, and at NSFH2 and NSFH3, focal children themselves were also interviewed.

We use reports from main respondents, current partners, and ex-partners at NSFH1 and NSFH2 to construct measures of parental conflict, family structure, family income, and parenting during children’s adolescent years. We use focal child interviews from NSFH2 and NSFH3 to construct indicators of young adult well-being. Our analysis pieces together information from all three waves of data, but we rely on just two waves to construct measures for any given child, with the combination of interviews dependent on the child’s age. Focal children are 4–18 at NSFH1, 10–25 at NSFH2, and 19–34 at NSFH3.3 In order to assess family experiences for all children at the same stage – adolescence – we use parents’ NSFH1 responses to construct family variables for the older children, who are 12–18 at the first wave, and we use parents’ NSFH2 responses for the younger children, who are 10–18 at the second wave.4 We measure outcomes using focal child self-reports from either NSFH2 or NSFH3 for the older children (ages 18–34 at these waves) and NSFH3 only for the younger children (ages 19–27 at NSFH3).

Attrition affects our sample in a few ways, again depending on the age of the focal child. For the younger focal children, cases are lost due to parents’ nonresponse at NSFH2 and their own nonresponse at NSFH3. Eighty-five percent of all younger focal children had at least one parent report from NSFH2, and of these children, over half were interviewed at NSFH3. For the older focal children, parents’ nonresponse is not a factor, since data on the family environment are drawn from NSFH1. Cases are lost if the focal child was not successfully interviewed at either NSFH2 or NSFH3; 69% of the original older focal children were interviewed at one of these time points.5 In all, we are left with about 56% of the potential sample of focal children identified at NSFH1. Attrition was greater among non-White and socioeconomically disadvantaged respondents, as well as those living with a step or single-parent family at NSFH1. Despite these differences, among the older focal children, we found (in results not shown) similar relationships between NSFH1 family environment and young adult well-being, whether outcomes were generated from the NSFH2 or NSFH3 focal child samples. This provides some evidence that our key findings are not affected by attrition.

We restrict our analyses to children who were living with their biological mother at NSFH1, thereby excluding single father and stepmother families, which are relatively rare and cannot be analyzed separately. We also exclude cases who experienced the death of a parent as there are too few deaths to analyze separately, and the processes of divorce and death affect children differently (Biblarz & Gottainer, 2000). Finally, we lose a small number of cases due to missing values on parental conflict and union transitions, child outcomes, and family background characteristics. Our initial sample includes 2,269 children whose parents were interviewed at either NSFH1 or NSFH2, who were living with their parents at the adolescent family observation, and who were interviewed themselves at NFSH2 or NSFH3. Of these, 2,065 were living with their biological mother at NSFH1. We exclude 55 children living with widowed single mothers, and we drop another 47 due to missing values on conflict, family structure, or controls, leaving a baseline sample of 1,963. Final samples vary by outcome due to item nonresponse and censoring of family-related transitions (discussed below).

Adolescent Family Type

We measure conflict between continuously married parents on the basis of couples’ responses to six items concerning frequency of conflict. Main respondents and their spouses/partners were asked: “The following is a list of subjects on which couples often have disagreements. How often, if at all, in the last year have you had open disagreements about each of the following…” The subjects include household tasks, money, spending time together, sex, in-laws, and the children. We generate a conflict scale by averaging all valid responses from mothers and fathers to these six items, keeping observations on conflict when only one parent report is available.6 This measure has good scale reliability (Chronbach’s alpha = 0.81).

We categorize continuously married-parent families by grouping the distribution of average conflict scores into thirds, corresponding to low, medium, and high average conflict. We explored the relationship between this measure and agreement between spouse reports of marital conflict. For those with both a main respondent and spouse report of conflict, the average difference between the main respondent and spouse report was quite small and varied little by the average level of spouse conflict. We examined an alternative measure of conflict based on agreement between spouses: whether they both reported low levels of conflict, both reported high levels of conflict, or disagreed on levels of conflict. We found that agreement on high levels of conflict raised the risks of most of the outcomes we examined relative to agreement on low levels of conflict, much like we found that high average levels of conflict raised the risks relative to low average levels of conflict.

Relying on the frequency of conflict is a common approach to measuring marital discord (e.g., Cherlin et al., 1991; Furstenberg & Teitler, 1994; Jekeliek, 1998; Morrison & Coiro, 1999). Using different data, Amato and colleagues further include indicators of marital problems and divorce proneness (Amato & DeBoer, 2001; Amato & Sobolewski, 2001; Amato et al., 1995; Booth & Amato, 2001). Marital problems and divorce proneness may be associated with the likelihood of divorce, but they may be less directly related to child well-being. Children are more likely to perceive – and be affected by – open disagreements than parents’ feelings about their marriage. We return to a discussion of the links between parental conflict, children’s perceptions, and child well-being in the conclusion.

Using average conflict scores to make distinctions between continuously married-parent families, we generate five family types: low, medium, and high conflict continuously married-parent families; stepfather families; and single-mother families. Family structure is determined on the basis of the parents’ union status and history. Children are coded as living with continuously married parents if their parents were married or living together within one year of the focal child’s birth and are in the same union at the time the family environment is observed, when children are 10–18, or adolescents (we include three cohabiting families by this definition).7 They are coded as coming from stepfather families if mothers are in a union that began more than a year after the focal child’s birth (this includes 58 cohabiting stepfamilies). Finally, they are classified as coming from single-mother families if mothers are not married or cohabiting. Table 1 shows the distribution of our measure of adolescent family type.

Table 1.

Means on Key Variables, by Family Type

All Continuously Married
Stepfather Single Mother
Low Conflict Medium Conflict High Conflict
Proportion 1.00 0.24 0.25 0.26 0.13 0.12
N 1963 340 353 382 401 487
Family income (in 1992 dollars) 60,343 (63,240) 71,621 (67,090) 70,484 (91,086) 58,023 (36,273) 50,469 (35,775) 27,135 (28,312)
Mother-child relationship (1–7) 6.11 (0.95) 6.37 (0.74) 6.12 (0.93) 5.99 (0.94) 6.07 (1.06) 5.90 (1.12)
Time with mother (1–6) 3.90 (1.08) 4.03 (1.09) 3.84 (1.10) 3.91 (1.07) 3.79 (1.00) 3.90 (1.11)
Harsh mothering (0.8–4) 2.02 (0.67) 1.86 (0.68) 1.96 (0.65) 2.14 (0.66) 2.12 (0.64) 2.14 (0.67)
Outcomes
High school dropout 0.12 0.06 0.08 0.12 0.19 0.25
Poor grades 0.22 0.18 0.18 0.24 0.23 0.28
No college 0.37 0.31 0.32 0.34 0.46 0.52
Current smoking 0.31 0.24 0.27 0.33 0.38 0.38
Binge drinking 0.35 0.29 0.38 0.42 0.32 0.32
Marijuana use 0.24 0.20 0.24 0.25 0.29 0.25
Early sex 0.20 0.15 0.14 0.22 0.29 0.28
Early cohabitation 0.20 0.14 0.17 0.17 0.31 0.34
Nonmarital fertility 0.11 0.07 0.07 0.11 0.18 0.22
Union disruption 0.41 0.32 0.38 0.42 0.43 0.57

Note: Standard deviations are in parentheses. Means, proportions, and standard deviations are weighted; N’s are unweighted. N’s refer to the baseline sample. Missing data on family income and mothering are set to their means (and flagged in all models). N’s vary by outcome due to item nonresponse and censoring of family-related transitions; see Tables 2 and 4 for final N’s of all models.

Income and Parenting

We focus on two factors potentially linking family type to young adult well-being: income and parenting. Both are observed, like family type, when children are adolescents. As noted, we expect income to play a role in mediating only associations between single-parent family structure and outcomes, whereas we expect parenting to play a mediating role across family types. Family income includes all sources of income to family members in the past year. It is adjusted to constant 1992 dollars and modeled as the natural log. We include three indicators of parenting: the quality of the mother-child relationship, mother’s time with children, and mother’s frequency of harsh behaviors toward children. We rely on mother’s relationships and practices, as all family types in our study include the biological mother, but not all include a male parent.

The quality of the mother-child relationship is based on a single question about how the mother would describe her relationship with the focal child, with response choices ranging from 1 = very poor to 7 = excellent at NSFH1 and 0 = really bad to 10 = absolutely perfect at NSFH2. We rescaled the NSFH2 item so that our measure of relationship quality ranges from 1 – 7. Mother’s time with children (all children in the household, including the focal child) is an average of four items about how often she spends time with children in leisure activities away from home, at home working on a project or playing together, having private talks, or helping with reading or homework, with responses ranging from 1 = never or rarely to 6 = almost every day. Finally, mother’s harsh behaviors are constructed from questions about how often she yells at or spanks or slaps her children. The wording of questions and the referent differ across waves, but are comparable. At NSFH1, mothers are asked two questions about yelling and spanking/slapping the children, i.e., all children in the household. Response alternatives range from 1 = never to 4 = very often and are averaged across items. At NSFH2, questions refer specifically to the focal child. Mothers are asked two questions about how they respond when the focal child does something especially bad, namely how often they yell at the child and how often they spank or slap the child. They are asked a third question about how they try to influence the focal child’s behavior, including how often they yell or shout. Responses to the three items range from 1 = never to 5 = always and are averaged. We rescaled the NSFH2 mean so that our measure of harsh behaviors ranges from 0.8 – 4. The focal child-specific measures of parenting behaviors should result in a tighter fit between what mothers report about parenting and the focal child’s own perception of parenting.

Missing data on family income and mothering are assigned mean values for the sample, and missing cases are flagged in all models. We tested the sensitivity of our results to including cases that were missing information, and key findings were similar whether we flagged cases with missing data or dropped them from our analyses.8 Income and mothering variables enter models in standardized form, with means of 0 and standard deviations of 1, so that we can more directly compare how each is associated with child well-being.

Table 1 shows means and standard deviations of our income and mothering variables for our full sample and by family type. Differences across family types suggest that these variables may indeed account for associations between family structure, conflict, and child outcomes. While family income varies somewhat by level of marital conflict, variation within continuously married-parent families is small relative to the very large gap between them and single mothers (e.g., nearly $58,000 for high conflict continuously married parents compared to $27,000 for single mothers). Indicators of mothering also vary as expected, with low conflict married-parent families having the highest scores on relationships and time with mother, and the lowest scores on harsh behaviors. Mothering variables for the high conflict married-parent families look more similar to those of stepfather and single mother families than to those of other, low conflict, continuously married-parent families. These associations may be confounded by pre-existing characteristics of families not yet controlled.

Outcomes

We examine indicators of young adult well-being in the areas of schooling, substance use, and family related transitions. For schooling, we model high school dropout,9 poor grades in high school (“C” or below), and never attended a two- or four-year college. For substance use, we model smoking in the past 30 days, binge drinking (five or more drinks in one sitting) in the past 30 days, and marijuana use in the past year. Family-related transitions include early first sex (before age 16), early cohabitation (before age 21), nonmarital childbearing, and union dissolution. Early sex is based on a single question about age at first sex, and other family transitions are pieced together from union and childbearing histories collected at NSFH2 and NSFH3.

Outcomes are measured when children are ages 19 – 34, at NSFH3 for the younger focal children and at either NSFH2 or NSFHF3 for the older focal children. If the older focal children were interviewed only at NSFH2 or NSFH3, we use the available interview. If both interviews are available, we prioritize NSFH2 in the case of dropout, poor grades, substance use, and early sex, since these events either take place prior to age 18 (the youngest age at NSFH2), or are somewhat more common at younger ages. For college attendance, early cohabitation, nonmarital childbearing, and union dissolution, we prioritize the NSFH3 report to give the focal child more time to experience these events. We include a flag in all models for whether the outcome was assessed at NSFH2 or NSFH3.

We consider outcomes for males and females together. Recent literature indicates that there are few differences in the effects of family disruption by child gender (Amato, 2005; Jekielek, 1998). In the one case of nonmarital childbearing, because we were concerned about the quality of men’s nonmarital fertility reports (Rendall, Clarke, Peters, Ranjit, & Verropoulou, 1999), we examined models separately for women. Coefficients in the models including women only were similar to those of the pooled sample, although some lost statistical significance, likely due to a reduction in statistical power. We report pooled results here.

Table 1 shows the frequency of each of our outcomes, for which final samples vary by item nonresponse and censoring: 12% dropped out of high school, over a fifth had poor grades in high school, and 37% never attended college. Substance use is reasonably common, with about a third of the sample reporting smoking and binge drinking in the past month, and about a quarter using marijuana in the past year. The transition to first sex occurred before age 16 for 20% of young men and women (of those who did not make or age out of the transition prior to the time we observed their adolescent family experiences). Twenty percent cohabited by the age of 21, 11% had a child outside of marriage, and 41% experienced the dissolution of their first cohabiting or marital union. Almost across the board, children from low conflict continuously married-parent families have the lowest proportions engaging in measured outcomes, that is, prior to any controls for socio-demographic and other differences across family types.

Controls

The availability of mother’s education, union, and childbearing histories and detailed information on her social class background allow us to control for important characteristics that are prior to family structure and conflict, including: race, highest level of education prior to the focal child’s birth, childhood family structure, age at first birth, and union dissolution prior to the focal child’s birth.10 Many of these factors are associated with both family structure and child well-being, and may be associated with conflict, as well. We also include controls for the focal child’s sex and age. Specifically, as discussed earlier in the text, we control for whether the child was on the younger or older end of adolescence at the time his or her family experiences were observed, adding a dummy for age 10–14 versus 15–18 at family assessment. We flag whether family experiences were measured at NSFH1 or NSFH2 and whether outcomes were measured at NSFH2 or NSFH3. Descriptive statistics on control variables are shown in Appendix Table 1.11

Appendix Table 1.

Means on Control Variables

Focal child 10–14 when family type observed 0.55
Family type observed at NSFH1 0.57
Focal child female 0.53
Mother’s race
 White 0.83
 Black 0.09
 Other 0.09
Mother’s education before focal born
 Less than high school 0.11
 High school 0.42
 Some college 0.25
 College or more 0.22
Mother grew up with single parent 0.24
Mother had teen birth 0.25
Mother had union dissolution before focal’s birth 0.11
Missing data on mother’s background 0.04
N 1963

Note: Proportions are weighted; N’s are unweighted. Flag for whether older focal child outcome measured at NSFH2 or NSFH3 varies by outcome and is not shown here.

Models

We use logistic regression to analyze dichotomous indicators for school-related outcomes and substance use. For these models, exponentiated coefficients represent the proportionate change in the odds associated with a unit change in the observed characteristic xi, holding all else constant (Agresti, 1990). We use Cox proportional hazard models to examine determinants of time to family-related transitions, i.e., sex, cohabitation, nonmarital birth, and union dissolution. The Cox model provides multivariate estimates of the effects of independent variables on the time-dependent risk of transition (Cleves, Gould, and Gutierrez, 2002; Cox, 1972). Hazard models are appropriate for analyzing time to an event, especially when there is censoring. Exponentiated coefficients represent the proportionate change in the baseline hazard associated with characteristic xi, and can be interpreted as a change in the relative risk of an event for a one-unit change in xi. The Cox model assumes that the effect of covariates remains constant at all durations, but it makes no assumptions about the shape of the baseline hazard over time.

We model the age-specific risks of sex, cohabitation, and nonmarital childbearing and the duration-specific risk of union dissolution (i.e., duration since the start of the first union). Age 16 is treated as a competing risk in the model of first sex (i.e., at age 16, individuals are removed from the risk set). For cohabitation and nonmarital childbearing, age 21 and age at marriage are treated as competing risks, respectively. For early sex, early cohabitation, and nonmarital childbearing, children’s exposure to risk starts after we observe their family circumstances in adolescence, and cases are left-truncated if children make or age out of the transition of interest prior to that date. For union dissolution, exposure starts at the time of the first union, and cases are left-truncated if their unions begin prior to their adolescent family observation. The only outcome for which left-truncation results in a significant loss of cases is first sexual intercourse. Because we are modeling age-specific risks of first sex and all ages up to 16 are represented in our models, censoring these cases reduces the precision of estimates but does not lead to bias.12

We posit that adolescence marks the onset or heightening of risk related to key young adult transitions, and that family experiences during this stage matter for the subsequent life course. “Clocking” exposure from the time that a child’s adolescent family experiences are observed rests on the notion that what is critical is measuring family experiences at some point in adolescence and measuring them prior to transitions of interest. That said, our measurement of family experiences – the start of our “clock” – may not precisely correspond to when a child becomes “at risk” of an event (this applies as well, to varying degrees, to our logistic regression models). It is reasonable to expect that family effects vary by their timing relative to a child’s risk of any given event – and worth reiterating that our analyses capture a snapshot of an unfolding process. Our sensitivity tests examining the implications of child age at family assessment (10–14 versus 15–18) suggest that our main findings do not depend on whether our clock starts earlier or later in adolescence, but larger sample sizes and more systematic analyses would be required to speak more fully to the possibility of varying family effects depending on when in a child’s life they are measured.

RESULTS

We start by reviewing results of our logistic regression models of academic achievement, which are found in Table 2, Panel A. This table reports exponentiated coefficients or odds ratios relating our key measures of adolescent family experience to high school dropout, grades, and college attendance. Model 1 includes family type, and Model 2 adds family income and mothering in standardized form. The first set of rows in Table 2 compares the odds of school-related outcomes for children from high conflict married-parent, stepfather, and single-mother families to low conflict married-parent families. In the rows below labeled “key contrasts,” we vary the reference group, examining differences among children from high conflict, stepfather, and single-mother families. While they do not appear in the table, both models include the controls described earlier (mother’s race, education, childhood family structure, and past childbearing and union experiences; child’s age and sex; flags for missing data on mother’s background and the wave at which family experiences and outcomes were assessed); Appendix Table 2 displays the full set of parameter estimates from Model 2.

Table 2.

Logistic Regression Models of Academic Achievement and Substance Use

Panel A: Academic Achievement High School Dropout
Poor Grades
No College
M1 Exp(B)
M2 Exp(B)
% Δ M2-M1
M1 Exp(B)
M2 Exp(B)
% Δ M2-M1
M1 Exp(B)
M2 Exp(B)
% Δ M2-M1
Family type (low conflict reference)
 Medium conflict 1.38 1.25 −10 1.12 1.04 −7 1.06 1.04 −1
 High conflict 1.76 ** 1.53 −13 1.59 ** 1.43 * −10 1.08 1.03 −5
 Stepfather 2.89 *** 2.60 *** −10 1.40 * 1.28 −9 1.49 ** 1.43 ** −4
 Single-mother 3.62 *** 2.99 *** −17 1.48 ** 1.25 −16 1.68 *** 1.47 ** −12
Ln family income z-score 0.91 0.89 ** 0.85 ***
Mother-child relationship z-score 0.76 *** 0.82 *** 0.97
Time with mother z-score 0.89 0.93 0.99
Harsh mothering z-score 1.13 1.06 1.09
Key Contrasts
High conflict v. step 0.61 ** 0.59 ** 1.14 1.13 0.72 ** 0.72 **
High conflict v. single 0.49 *** 0.51 *** 1.07 1.15 0.64 *** 0.70 **
Step v. single 0.80 0.87 0.94 1.02 0.89 0.97
Panel B: Substance Use Current Smoking
Binge Drinking
Marijuana Use
M1 Exp(B)
M2 Exp(B)
% Δ M2-M1
M1 Exp(B)
M2 Exp(B)
% Δ M2-M1
M1 Exp(B)
M2 Exp(B)
% Δ M2-M1
Family type (low conflict reference)
 Medium conflict 1.26 1.19 −5 1.29 1.28 −1 1.24 1.24 0
 High conflict 1.53 ** 1.42 ** −7 1.66 *** 1.66 *** 0 1.35 1.35 0
 Stepfather 1.98 *** 1.87 *** −5 1.33 * 1.35 * 2 1.70 *** 1.74 *** 2
 Single-mother 2.05 *** 1.94 *** −5 1.34 * 1.54 ** 15 1.48 ** 1.61 ** 9
Ln family income z-score 1.00 1.28 *** 1.16 *
Mother-child relationship z-score 0.92 0.97 0.88 **
Time with mother z-score 0.88 ** 0.96 0.97
Harsh mothering z-score 1.08 0.98 0.91
Key Contrasts
High conflict v. step 0.77 0.76 * 1.25 1.23 0.79 0.78
High conflict v. single 0.75 * 0.73 * 1.24 1.08 0.91 0.84
Step v. single 0.96 0.96 0.99 0.88 1.15 1.08

Note: Controls are included in M1 and M2, but not shown, for child’s age and sex and mother’s race, education, childhood family structure, and past childbearing and union experiences.

*

p < 0.10;

**

p < 0.05;

***

p < 0.01

Appendix Table 2.

Odds Ratios (Exp[B]) from Full Logistic Regression Model (Model 2) of Academic Achievement and Substance Use

High School Dropout Poor Grades No College Current Smoking Binge Drinking Marijuana Use
Family type (low conflict reference)
 Medium conflict 1.25 1.04 1.04 1.19 1.28 1.24
 High conflict 1.53 1.43 * 1.03 1.42 ** 1.66 *** 1.35
 Stepfather 2.60 *** 1.28 1.43 ** 1.87 *** 1.35 * 1.74 ***
 Single-mother 2.99 *** 1.25 1.47 ** 1.94 *** 1.54 ** 1.61 **
Ln family income z-score 0.91 0.89 ** 0.85 *** 1.00 1.28 *** 1.16 *
Mother-child relationship z-score 0.76 *** 0.82 *** 0.97 0.92 0.97 0.88 **
Time with mother z-score 0.89 0.93 0.99 0.88 ** 0.96 0.97
Harsh mothering z-score 1.13 1.06 1.09 1.08 0.98 0.91
Focal child female 0.62 *** 0.49 *** 0.65 *** 0.80 ** 0.34 *** 0.56 ***
Focal child 10–14 when family type observed 1.22 0.80 * 1.11 1.04 1.22 * 1.60 ***
Family type observed at NSFH1 1.76 ** 1.22 0.84 1.04 0.85 0.68 *
Outcome reported at NSFH2 (older focals only) 0.67 ** 0.92 1.04 0.68 ** 1.00 1.34
Mother’s race (white reference)
 Black 0.78 0.96 1.18 0.39 *** 0.53 *** 0.76
 Other 1.65 ** 0.98 1.18 0.73 0.89 0.97
Mother’s education (<high school reference)
 High school 0.55 *** 1.19 0.62 *** 1.26 1.02 1.81 ***
 Some college 0.40 *** 1.07 0.31 *** 1.17 1.13 1.95 ***
 College or more 0.24 *** 0.55 ** 0.13 *** 0.93 1.62 ** 2.53 ***
Mother grew up with single parent 1.13 1.11 1.31 ** 1.19 0.91 1.03
Mother had teen birth 1.41 ** 1.32 ** 1.40 *** 1.13 0.85 0.94
Mother union dissolution before focal’s birth 1.45 ** 1.22 1.16 1.12 1.29 * 1.50 ***
Missing data on mother’s background 1.41 1.19 1.03 1.07 0.87 0.78
Constant 0.12 *** 0.30 *** 1.27 0.38 *** 0.66 * 0.14 ***
N 1942 1950 1962 1933 1958 1955

Note: Flags are included, but not shown, for missing data on family income and mothering.

*

p < 0.10;

**

p < 0.05;

***

p < 0.01

Model 1 indicates that, compared to living in a low conflict continuously married-parent family, living in a high conflict family increases the odds of dropping out of high school and poor grades, but is not significantly related to college attendance. Living in a stepfather or single-mother family is associated with all three outcomes, and appears to be more strongly associated with both dropping out and not attending college than conflict (see rows labeled “key contrasts”). For example, while parental conflict is associated with a 76% increased odds of drop out, stepfather and single-mother families are associated with nearly a tripling or greater of these odds.

Model 2 adds family income and mothering variables. The odds ratio on income is less than one for all three academic outcomes, but is not statistically significant in the case of dropping out. A one standard deviation increase in income is associated with about a 10% reduction in the odds of poor grades and a 15% reduction in the odds of not going to college. The quality of the mother-child relationship is negatively associated with dropping out and poor grades, but not college attendance. A one standard deviation increase in mother-child relationship quality is associated with about a 20% decline in the odds of dropping out and poor grades. Somewhat surprisingly, time with mother and harsh mothering are not statistically significantly associated (net of other variables) with any of these academic outcomes. In particular, we hypothesized that time with mother would be important in mediating the association between family type and academic achievement, especially as one dimension of time includes help with homework.

The three mothering variables are jointly significant in models of dropping out and poor grades and, together with income, they modestly reduce (mostly by 10–15%) the coefficients on parental conflict and family structure (see column labeled “% Δ,” which compares coefficients from Models 1 and 2). In the case of high school drop out, including family income and (especially) mothering reduces the coefficient on high parental conflict to statistical insignificance; in the case of poor grades, adding these variables reduces coefficients on stepfather and single mother to statistical insignificance. Mothering appears unrelated to college attendance (i.e., variables are individually and jointly statistically insignificant) and thus does not explain associations between this outcome and family structure. Income, however, explains a modest share of the association between college attendance and living with a single mother (it reduces the single-mother coefficient by 12%).

Table 3 provides a final – more intuitive – look at differences in academic achievement by family type. It shows predicted probabilities of high school dropout, poor grades, and no college attendance generated from Model 2 parameter estimates, setting family income, mothering, and controls to their mean values and varying dummies on family type. In the case of dropout, adjusted proportions range from 6% of children from low conflict married-parent families not graduating to 17% from single-mother families. Differences are narrower in the case of poor grades (ranging from 18–24% of children from low and high conflict married-parent families, respectively) and never attending college (ranging from 34–43% of children from low conflict married-parent families and both stepfather and single-mother families, respectively). Adjustments reflected in the predicted probabilities appear to account for a greater share of the poor schooling outcomes among children from single-parent families compared to others (Table 3 predicted probabilities versus Table 1 unadjusted proportions).

Table 3.

Predicted Values from Logistic Regression Models of Academic Achievement and Substance Use

Continuously Married
Stepfather Single Mother
Low Conflict Medium Conflict High Conflict
High school dropout 0.06 0.08 0.09 0.15 0.17
Poor grades 0.18 0.19 0.24 0.22 0.21
No college 0.34 0.35 0.35 0.43 0.43
Current smoking 0.24 0.27 0.31 0.37 0.38
Binge drinking 0.27 0.33 0.39 0.34 0.37
Marijuana use 0.18 0.22 0.23 0.28 0.27

Note: Predicted probabilities are derived from our full models, varying family structure and conflict and holding all other covariates at their mean values. See Table 2, Model 2, for tests of statistical significance.

Panel B of Table 2 shows the exponentiated coefficients or odds ratios for key variables from logistic regression models of substance use (recall that models include controls that are not displayed in the table; the full set of parameter estimates are shown in Appendix Table 2). Coming from a high conflict married-parent, stepfather, or single-mother family increases the odds of smoking, binge drinking, and marijuana use, relative to a low conflict married-parent family (result just misses statistical significance in the case of high conflict married-parent families and marijuana use). The increase in odds is reasonably substantial, ranging from about 50–100%; e.g., in Model 1, the odds of smoking are 53% higher for children from high conflict families and 105% higher for children from single-mother families. The only differences among high conflict, stepfather, and single-mother family types (see “key constrasts”) appear to be in smoking, where odds are lower for children in the high conflict married-parent families. There is some indication that the odds of binge drinking are higher for children in these families relative to those in stepfather and single-mother families, although differences miss statistical significance. This story is largely the same whether we look at Model 1 or Model 2.

Mother-child relationship quality is negatively associated with marijuana use, as is mother’s time with smoking (Model 2). Across outcomes, the mothering variables explain little of the family conflict and structure associations – and are individually and jointly insignificant in the case of binge drinking. Family income is statistically significant in the binge drinking and marijuana models, with a standard deviation increase in income increasing the odds of these outcomes by 28 and 16%, respectively. This is consistent with income increasing the ability of youths to purchase alcohol and drugs. Income acts as a suppressor in the associations between single-mother families and drinking and marijuana use; including income increases the coefficient on single-mother families by 15% in the case of drinking and 9% in the case of marijuana (column labeled “% Δ”). This is reflected in the predicted probabilities (Table 3), which show higher levels of binge drinking and marijuana use among single-mother families (37 and 27%, respectively), relative to the unadjusted proportions shown in Table 1 (32 and 25%, respectively).

Table 4 shows Cox hazard results for early sex, early cohabitation, nonmarital childbearing, and union disruption (models include controls that are not displayed in the table; the full set of parameter estimates are shown in Appendix Table 3). Exponentiated coefficients are given for our key family variables, and these can be interpreted as relative risks. Compared to those from low conflict married-parent families, the risks of each of our family-related transitions are anywhere from 25% to upwards of 100% greater for children from high conflict married-parent, stepfather, and single-mother families (not statistically significant in the case of high conflict married-parent families and early cohabitation). Risks also tend to be greater for children from stepfather and single-mother families, relative to children from high conflict married-parent families, although differences are not consistently statistically significant (see “key contrasts”). Whether we focus on Model 1 or 2, differences are statistically significant in the case of early cohabitation, with the risk being about 30% lower for children from high conflict married-parent families, relative to those from either stepfather or single-mother families. But differences among these families are not statistically significant in the case of early sex, and the picture is mixed when we look at nonmarital childbearing (in Model 2, no significant differences) and union dissolution (in Model 2, higher risks among children from single-mother families, relative to both high conflict and stepfather families).

Table 4.

Cox Hazard Models of Family-Related Transitions

First Sex by Age 16
Cohabitation by Age 21
Non-Marital Birth
Union Dissolution
M1
Exp(B)
M2
Exp(B)
% Δ
M2-M1
M1
Exp(B)
M2
Exp(B)
% Δ
M2-M1
M1
Exp(B)
M2
Exp(B)
% Δ
M2-M1
M1
Exp(B)
M2
Exp(B)
% Δ
M2-M1
Family type (low conflict reference)
 Medium conflict 0.91 0.91 0 1.10 1.09 −1 1.14 1.15 1 1.37 * 1.34 −2
 High conflict 1.54 * 1.53 * −1 1.33 1.24 −7 1.59 * 1.50 * −5 1.52 ** 1.42 ** −6
 Stepfather 1.69 ** 1.64 ** −3 1.93 *** 1.86 *** −4 1.80 ** 1.67 ** −7 1.47 ** 1.43 ** −3
 Single-mother 2.02 *** 2.05 *** 2 2.07 *** 1.84 *** −11 2.30 *** 1.96 *** −15 1.88 *** 1.84 *** −2
Ln family income z-score 1.08 0.92 ** 0.88 ** 1.06
Mother-child relationship z-score 0.98 0.89 ** 0.90 * 0.91 **
Time with mother z-score 0.90 1.01 0.98 1.08
Harsh mothering z-score 1.04 1.08 1.11 1.07
Key Contrasts
High conflict v. step 0.91 0.93 0.69 ** 0.67 *** 0.88 0.90 1.04 1.00
High conflict v. single 0.76 0.75 0.64 *** 0.67 *** 0.69 ** 0.76 0.81 0.77 *
Step v. single 0.84 0.80 0.93 1.01 0.78 0.85 0.78 ** 0.78 **

Note: Controls are included in M1 and M2, but not shown, for child’s age and sex and mother’s race, education, childhood family structure, and past childbearing and union experiences.

*

p < 0.10;

**

p < 0.05;

***

p < 0.01

Appendix Table 3.

Hazard Ratios (Exp[B]) from Full Cox Regression Model (Model 2) of Family-Related Transitions

Early Sex Early Cohabitation Nonmarital Childbearing Union Disruption
Family type (low conflict reference)
 Medium conflict 0.91 1.09 1.15 1.34
 High conflict 1.53 * 1.24 1.50 * 1.42 **
 Stepfather 1.64 ** 1.86 *** 1.67 ** 1.43 **
 Single-mother 2.05 *** 1.84 *** 1.96 *** 1.84 ***
Ln family income z-score 1.08 0.92 ** 0.88 ** 1.06
Mother-child relationship z-score 0.98 0.89 ** 0.90 * 0.91 **
Time with mother z-score 0.90 1.01 0.98 1.08
Harsh mothering z-score 1.04 1.08 1.11 1.07
Focal child female 0.89 1.47 *** 1.85 *** 0.74 ***
Focal child 10–14 when family type observed 0.95 1.29 ** 1.07 1.43 ***
Family type observed at NSFH1 1.06 0.76 ** 1.02 0.53 ***
Outcome reported at NSFH2 (older focals only) 1.25 1.61 *** 1.35 * 3.35 ***
Mother’s race (white reference)
 Black 1.55 ** 0.54 *** 2.37 *** 1.37 **
 Other 1.21 0.73 1.76 ** 0.76
Mother’s education (<high school reference)
 High school 1.32 0.90 0.67 ** 1.07
 Some college 1.32 0.79 0.58 *** 1.08
 College or more 0.84 0.46 *** 0.19 *** 1.20
Mother grew up with single parent 0.80 1.14 1.04 1.07
Mother had teen birth 1.47 *** 1.51 *** 1.43 *** 0.84 *
Mother union dissolution before focal’s birth 1.24 1.39 *** 1.37 * 1.15
Missing data on mother’s background 0.27 * 1.04 1.02 0.75
N 1112 1939 1927 1093

Note: Flags are included, but not shown, for missing data on family income and mothering.

*

p < 0.10;

**

p < 0.05;

***

p < 0.01

Family income and mothering are not consistently linked to family-related transitions (Model 2). In the case of early sex, relative risks associated with income and mothering are close to 1 and are individually and jointly statistically insignificant. Income is negatively related to early cohabitation and nonmarital childbearing, with a one standard deviation increase in income associated with about a 10% reduction in the relative risk of these transitions. Improvements in mother-child relationship quality are associated with a reduction in the relative risks of early cohabitation, nonmarital childbearing, and union disruption, on the order of about 10% for a one standard deviation change in relationship quality. Net of other variables, mother’s time and harsh mothering appear to be little associated with family-related transitions.

Given their weak associations with these outcomes, it is not surprising that income and mothering account for little of the associations between conflict, family structure, and family-related transitions (column labeled “% Δ”). Adding income and mothering to Model 1 typically reduces the conflict and family structure coefficients by less than 10%. Income and mothering explain slightly more of the single-mother association with early cohabitation (reducing the coefficient by 11%) and nonmarital fertility (reducing the coefficient by 15%).

DISCUSSION

We set out to address how children who experience high levels of parental conflict fare compared to those in other family arrangements. Our results clearly illustrate that the advantages of living with two continuously married parents are not shared equally by all children. In models unadjusted for income and parenting, children from high conflict married-parent families (compared to low conflict married-parent families) had higher odds (ranging from about 50–75%) of dropping out of high school, reporting poor grades, smoking, and binge drinking; they were at greater risk (on the order of about 50%) of early sex, nonmarital childbearing, and union dissolution. Consistent with past research, we also found higher odds of poor schooling outcomes and substance use and greater risks of our family-related transitions among children from stepfather and single-mother families, relative to those from low conflict married-parent families. Differences also tended to be higher relative to children from high conflict married-parent families, although these were statistically significant in just about half the outcomes we examined. Often, associations with parental conflict were statistically indistinguishable from those with stepfather and single mother-families (this is true for poor grades, binge drinking, marijuana use, early sex, and either nonmarital childbearing or union dissolution, depending on Model 1 or 2).

We also set out to explore key factors that might account for associations between family type and child well-being – family income and parenting. Family income was negatively related to 4, and positively related to 2, of our 10 outcomes. It played a modest role in explaining the association between single-mother family structure and poor grades, college attendance, early cohabitation, and nonmarital childbearing and served as a suppressor in the case of binge drinking and drug use. As expected, it played little role in accounting for associations between either high conflict or stepfather families and young adult well-being. With a second potential earner and caretaker in the household, these families are more sheltered from the financial insecurity of single-mother families.

We found weak evidence of the importance of parenting behaviors in explaining differences across family types in child well-being: parenting behaviors varied modestly across family type (Table 1), and were in turn only inconsistently associated with our young adult outcomes. Contrary to our expectations, mother’s time explained little of the associations between family type and academic achievement (e.g., via help with homework) or substance use (e.g., via monitoring). Indeed, in only one case – smoking – was time with mothers statistically significant and associated with young adult well-being in the hypothesized direction, that is, more time with mothers associated with less smoking. Mothers’ harsh parenting was not associated with any of our outcomes, net of controls, although we expected it to be an important link between parental conflict and both substance use and early family transitions. The quality of the mother-child relationship was more consistently related to young adult well-being, having negative, statistically significant associations with 6 of 10 of our outcomes, including outcomes across the three domains of academic achievement, substance use, and family-related transitions. We hypothesized that family structure and conflict would affect aspects of parenting, which in turn would affect child outcomes. But given the modest associations of our mothering variables with family type, and in turn with young adult outcomes, these variables overall explained no more than about 15% of the associations between family type and young adult well-being.

Understanding how families matter for children requires identifying the most salient dimensions of families and measuring them well. Measurement issues may produce underestimates of conflict relative to family structure – as well as underestimates of the pathways through which families influence children. As noted earlier, conflict is observed at a point in time, representing a snapshot in the family lives of adolescents, such that past conflict and its effects may be captured within low conflict continuously married-parent, stepfather, and single-mother categories, resulting in inflated estimates of all groups compared to what we call high conflict families. Beyond issues relating to the timing of measurement, conceptualizing conflict is a more challenging enterprise than constructing family structure from reasonably reliable and complete marriage histories. Like many studies of parental conflict, we measure conflict by the frequency of disagreements common in marriage. This seems a good strategy when thinking about marital problems that affect children, as disagreements are observable and may set the emotional climate for the household. But how parents manage disagreement may be a key factor in children’s perceptions of conflict and thus how they are affected by conflict (Cox, 1999). Incorporating, for example, how often disagreements become angry and violent, or how often parents reach resolutions or offer reassurances to children, may lead to better estimates of the associations between parental conflict and child well-being.

The salient dimensions of parenting are also difficult to capture. Subtle aspects of tone and attentiveness may affect children’s perception of time with parents, just as small and scattered expressions of warmth may affect children’s reactions to yelling. Further, children in the same family may be affected differently by parents’ behaviors – there may be variation in children’s sensitivity and needs, as well as variation in their responses that in turn influence parent-child interactions. Recall that mother-child relationship quality was more often related to young adult well-being than our other indicators of mothering. The questions (at both waves) about relationship quality refer specifically to the focal child, whereas this is not the case for time or harsh behaviors, which refer more generally to all children.13 Sharpening our measurement of parenting could lead to stronger estimates of its mediating role in associations between family type and young adult well-being, better tapping how family conflict and structure might affect parenting, and how parenting in turn affects child outcomes. Of course, the weakness of our mothering variables could also point to the need to investigate other mechanisms, such as stress or attitudes and beliefs. Family type could also be working more directly in some instances, for example in the modeling of conflict that may lead to own union dissolution.

Should parents stay together for the sake of the children? Children tend to fare better with both married parents, but mean differences in child well-being mask important variation. Despite caveats concerning potential underestimates of conflict, we find that children from high conflict married-parent families do more poorly in the domains of schooling and substance use, and are at greater risk of early family formation and dissolution, relative to children from low conflict married-parent families. In half of our outcomes, high conflict, stepfather, and single-mother families are statistically indistinguishable in their associations with young adult well-being. These findings hold once account is taken of key mechanisms posited to link family type and child outcomes. They are consistent with recent research on marriage and the well-being of adults, showing that although marriage confers benefits to adults on average, those in poor quality marriages are no better off than the single and, indeed, may fare worse on some measures (Hawkins & Booth, 2005; Williams & Umberson, 2004). We conclude with the perhaps obvious point that marriage is not a blanket prescription for the well-being of children, any more than it is for the well-being of adults. Recent policy initiatives to promote marriage need to take account of how variation within marriage relates to child well-being.

Acknowledgments

Support for this research was provided by Grant Numbers K01 HD42690 and K01 HD49571 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development and the Office of Behavioral and Social Sciences to the first and second authors, respectively. We are grateful to Larry Bumpass, Paula England, Judith Seltzer, the editor of Social Science Research, and two anonymous reviewers for insightful comments and suggestions on earlier drafts.

Footnotes

1

Results available from the authors upon request.

2

Conversely, it may be that our high conflict two-parent families are in the process of divorce, and that any negative associations with child outcomes are due in part to subsequent separation – not the conflict that preceded it. For the older focal children living with both parents in adolescence (i.e., at NSFH1, when they were 12–18), we can use the next wave of main respondent interviews to ascertain how many of these parents separated by NSFH2, when children were 18–25. Only 5% of these two-parent families separated by NSFH2, including 13% of those labeled high conflict. The vast majority of parents still together in their child’s adolescence remained together at least over the course of the next 7 years, taking their children into young adulthood. Because of a combination of missing data and nonresponse among parents at NSFH3, it is difficult to ascertain how many of the younger focal child parents subsequently separated.

3

Because of the timing of birth and interview dates, a few cases fall outside these age ranges.

4

While this opens the possibility of differential period effects, recent work finds no evidence of change over time in the association between family structure and at least some aspects of child well-being (Li & Wu, 2002; Musick & Mare, 2006; Sigle-Rushton et al., 2005).

5

Focal children were followed up for interview at NSFH3 regardless of whether they were interviewed at NSFH2. Of the 1,914 older focal children at NSFH1, 1,069 were interviewed at NSFH2. At NSFH3, 889 were interviewed, including 247 who did not respond at NSFH2.

6

In 84% of cases, conflict scores are based on reports from both mothers and fathers on at least one of the six conflict questions (76% of cases include reports from both parents on all six items). The average level of conflict for those with reports from both spouses is somewhat higher than the average for the 16% with data from one spouse only. We ran baseline models flagging cases with conflict data from one spouse only, and found (in results not shown) that the flag was generally not statistically significant, and adding the flag did not alter our key findings.

7

Marriage chances drop off sharply following a nonmarital birth (Brien, Lillard, & Waite, 1999). Allowing parents one year to marry following the focal’s birth captures those parents that marry without including (many) step relationships (see Bumpass, Raley, & Sweet, 1995).

8

Missing data on these items are as follows: 11% on family income, 8% on mother-child relationship quality, 8% on mother’s time, and 13% on harsh behaviors.

9

High school dropout is defined as not having received a diploma at graduation, and it includes children who passed a high school equivalency test such as the GED. In terms of labor market outcomes, exam-certified high school equivalents bear a stronger resemblance to high school dropouts than to graduates (Cameron & Heckman, 1993).

10

When mothers’ childbearing and union histories are not available, we set values to 0 and include a flag for missing mother report. When we have no report of mother’s race or education, we fill this information in with partners’ reports, if available, since these characteristics tend to vary little within couples (see Schwartz & Mare, [2005] on education and Qian [1997] on race).

11

We ran sensitivity analyses including controls for child behavior problems at NSFH1. The argument for including them is mixed: if parents respond to children’s behavior and not vice versa, omitting behavior problems would inflate estimates of mothering; however, if the arrows of causality work in the hypothesized direction – from parents to children – including them would capture aspects of mothering and understate the total effects of mothering. Including child behavior problems resulted in little change in key coefficients, and we leave them out of final models.

12

Our baseline sample of 1,963 includes 1,825 focal children with valid reports on age at first sex. Of these, 438 had sex before their parents reported on their adolescent family environment, and 275 turned 16 (without first having sex) before this date, leaving 1,112 cases. Of the remaining observations, 261 had sex prior to the age of 16.

13

Harsh parenting behaviors are asked with respect to all children in the household at NSFH1, but specifically with respect to the focal child at NSFH2.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Contributor Information

Kelly Musick, Cornell University.

Ann Meier, University of Minnesota.

References

  1. Agresti A. Categorical data analysis. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  2. Alan Guttmacher Institute. In their own right: Assessing the sexual and reproductive health needs of American men. New York: Alan Guttmacher Institute; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  3. Allison PD, Furstenberg FF. How marital dissolution affects children: Variations by age and sex. Developmental Psychology. 1989;25:540–549. [Google Scholar]
  4. Amato PR. The impact of family formation change on the cognitive, social, and emotional well-being of the next generation. The Future of Children. 2005;15:75–96. doi: 10.1353/foc.2005.0012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Amato PR, Booth A. A generation at risk. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  6. Amato PR, Booth A. Parental predivorce relations and offspring postdivorce well-being. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2001;63:197–212. [Google Scholar]
  7. Amato PR, DeBoer DD. The transmission of marital instability across generations: Relationship skills or commitment to marriage? Journal of Marriage and Family. 2001;63:1038–1051. [Google Scholar]
  8. Amato PR, Spencer Loomis L, Booth A. Parental divorce, marital conflict, and offspring well-being during early adulthood. Social Forces. 1995;73:895–915. [Google Scholar]
  9. Amato PR, Sobolewski JM. The effects of divorce and marital discord on adult children’s psychological well-being. American Sociological Review. 2001;66:900–921. [Google Scholar]
  10. Astone NM, Upchurch DM. Socioeconomic and marital outcomes of adolescent marriage, adolescent childbirth, and their co-occurrence. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1989;51:203–212. [Google Scholar]
  11. Astone NM, McLanahan SS. Family structure, parental practices and high school completion. American Sociological Review. 1991;56:309–320. [Google Scholar]
  12. Aughinbaugh A, Pierret CR, Rothstein DS. The impact of family structure transitions on youth achievement: Evidence from the children of the NLSY79. Demography. 2005;42:447–468. doi: 10.1353/dem.2005.0023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Axinn WG, Thornton A. The influence of parents’ marital dissolutions on children’s attitudes toward family formation. Demography. 1996;33:66–81. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Baumrind D. The influence of parenting style on adolescent competence and substance use. Journal of Early Adolescence. 1991;11:56–95. [Google Scholar]
  15. Biblarz TJ, Gottainer G. Family structure and children’s success: A comparison of widowed and divorced single-mother families. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 2000;62:533–548. [Google Scholar]
  16. Biblarz TJ, Raftery AE. The effects of family disruption on social mobility. American Sociological Review. 1993;58:97–109. [Google Scholar]
  17. Biblarz TJ, Raftery AE. Family structure, educational attainment, and socioeconomic success: Rethinking the “pathology of matriarchy”. American Journal of Sociology. 1999;105:321–365. [Google Scholar]
  18. Booth A, Amato PR. Parental predivorce relations and offspring postdivorce well-being. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2001;63:197–212. [Google Scholar]
  19. Booth A, Edwards JN. Transition of marital and family quality over the generations: The effect of parental divorce and unhappiness. Journal of Divorce. 1990;13:41–58. [Google Scholar]
  20. Brien MJ, Lillard LA, Waite LJ. Interrelated family-building behaviors: Cohabitation, marriage, and nonmarital conception. Demography. 1999;36:535–551. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Bumpass L, Raley RK, Sweet J. The changing character of stepfamilies: Implications of cohabitation and nonmarital childbearing. Demography. 1995;32:425–436. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cameron SV, Heckman JJ. The nonequivalence of high school equivalents. Journal of Labor Economics. 1993;11:1–47. [Google Scholar]
  23. Carlson M. Family structure, father involvement, and adolescent behavioral outcomes. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2006;68:137–154. [Google Scholar]
  24. Cherlin AJ. Remarriage as an Incomplete Institution. American Journal of Sociology. 1978;84:634–650. [Google Scholar]
  25. Cherlin AJ, Kiernan K, Chase-Lansdale PL. Parental divorce in childhood and demographic outcomes in young adulthood. Demography. 1995;32:299–318. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Cherlin AJ, Chase-Lansdale PL, McRae C. Effects of parental divorce on mental health throughout the life course. American Sociological Review. 1998;63:239–249. [Google Scholar]
  27. Cherlin AJ, Furstenberg FF, Chase-Lansdale PL, Kiernan KE, Robins PK, Morrison DR, Teitler JO. Longitudinal studies of effects of divorce on children in Great Britain and the United States. Science. 1991;252:1386–1389. doi: 10.1126/science.2047851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Cleves MA, Gould WW, Gutierrez RG. An introduction to survival analysis using STATA. College Station, TX: Stata Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  29. Conger RD, Conger KJ, Elder GH., Jr . Family economic hardship and adolescent adjustment: Mediating and moderating processes. In: Duncan GJ, Brooks-Gunn J, editors. Consequences of growing up poor. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 1997. pp. 288–310. [Google Scholar]
  30. Cox DR. Regression models and life-tables. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. 1972;B74:187–220. [Google Scholar]
  31. Cox M. Marital conflict, parent-child relationships, gender, and outcomes for young adults in families. In: Booth A, Crouter AC, Shanahan M, editors. Transitions to adulthood in a changing economy: No work, no family, no future? Westport, CT: Praeger; 1999. pp. 103–108. [Google Scholar]
  32. Davies PT, Cummings EM. Marital conflict and child adjustment: An emotional security hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;116:387–411. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.3.387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Davis EC, Friel LV. Adolescent sexuality: Disentangling the effects of family structure and family context. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2001;63:669–681. [Google Scholar]
  34. Day JC, Newburger EC. Current Population Reports. U.S. Census Bureau; 2002. The big payoff: Educational attainment and synthetic estimates of work-life earnings. Retrieved June 5, 2008, from http://usgovinfo.about.com/gi/dynamic/offsite.htm?site=http://www.census.gov/prod/2002pubs/p23%2D210.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  35. DeLeire T, Kalil A. Good things come in threes: Single-parent multigenerational family structure and adolescent adjustment. Demography. 2002;39:393–413. doi: 10.1353/dem.2002.0016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Duncan GJ, Rodgers W. Has children’s poverty become more persistent? American Sociological Review. 1991;56:538–550. [Google Scholar]
  37. Duncan GJ, Yeung WJ, Brooks-Gunn J, Smith JR. How much does childhood poverty affect the life chances of children? American Sociological Review. 1998;63:406–423. [Google Scholar]
  38. Eggebeen DJ, Lichter DT. Race, family structure, and changing poverty among American children. American Sociological Review. 1991;56:801–817. [Google Scholar]
  39. Emery, Robert E. Interparental conflict and the children of discord and divorce. Psychological Bulletin. 1982;92:310–330. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Erel O, Burman B. Interrelatedness of marital relations and parent-child relations: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;118:108–132. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.118.1.108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Fauber R, Forehand R, Thomas AM, Wierson M. A mediational model of the impact of marital conflict on adolescent adjustment in intact and divorced families: The role of disrupted parenting. Child Development. 1990;61:1112–1123. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02845.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Furstenberg FF, Teitler JO. Reconsidering the effects of marital disruption: What happens to children of divorce in early adulthood? Journal of Family Issues. 1994;15:173–190. [Google Scholar]
  43. Gotlib IH, Lewinsohn PM, Seeley JR. Consequences of depression during adolescence: Marital status and marital functioning in early adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1998;107:686–690. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.107.4.686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Grall TS. Current Population Reports. U.S. Census Bureau; 2007. Custodial mothers and fathers and their child support: 2005. Retrieved March 5, 2008, from http://www.census.gov/prod/2007pubs/p60-234.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  45. Gruber J. Risky behavior among youths: An economic analysis. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  46. Grych JH, Fincham FD. Marital conflict and children’s adjustment: A cognitive-contextual framework. Psychological Bulletin. 1990;108:267–290. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.108.2.267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hanson TL. Does parental conflict explain why divorce is negatively associated with child welfare? Social Forces. 1999;77:1283–1315. [Google Scholar]
  48. Hawkins DN, Booth A. Unhappily ever after: Effects of long-term, low-quality marriages on well-being. Social Forces. 2005;84:451–471. [Google Scholar]
  49. Hoffmann JP, Johnson RA. A national portrait of family structure and adolescent drug use. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1998;60:633–645. [Google Scholar]
  50. Hogan DP, Astone NM. The transition to adulthood. Annual Review of Sociology. 1986;12:109–130. [Google Scholar]
  51. House JS. Understanding social factors and inequalities in health: 20th century progress and 21st century prospects. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2002;43:125–142. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Jekielek SM. Parental conflict, marital disruption and children’s emotional well-being. Social Forces. 1998;76:905–935. [Google Scholar]
  53. Kandel DB. Stages and pathways of drug involvement: Examining the gateway hypothesis. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  54. Kiernan K. The impact of family disruption in childhood on transitions made in young adult life. Population Studies. 1992;46:213–234. [Google Scholar]
  55. Kiernan K, Cherlin A. Parental divorce and partnership dissolution in adulthood: Evidence from a British cohort study. Population Studies. 1999;53:39–48. [Google Scholar]
  56. Kiernan K, Hobcraft J. Parental divorce during childhood: Age at first intercourse, partnership and parenthood. Population Studies. 1997;51:41–55. [Google Scholar]
  57. Li JCA, Wu LL. Unpublished manuscript. Center for Demography and Ecology, University of Wisconsin-Madison; 2002. No trend in the intergenerational transmission of divorce. [Google Scholar]
  58. McLanahan SS, Booth K. Mother-only families: Problems, prospects, and politics. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1989;51:557–580. [Google Scholar]
  59. McLanahan SS, Bumpass L. Intergenerational consequences of family disruption. American Journal of Sociology. 1988;94:130–152. [Google Scholar]
  60. McLanahan SS, Percheski C. Family Structure and the Reproduction of Inequality. Annual Review of Sociology. 2008;34:257–276. [Google Scholar]
  61. McLanahan SS, Sandefur G. Growing up with a single parent: What hurts, what helps. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  62. Moffitt T. Adolescent-limited and life-course persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review. 1993;100:647–701. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Morrison DR, Cherlin AJ. The divorce process and young children’s well-being: A prospective analysis. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1995;57:800–812. [Google Scholar]
  64. Morrison DR, Coiro MJ. Parental conflict and marital disruption: Do children benefit when high-conflict marriages are dissolved? Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1999;61:626–637. [Google Scholar]
  65. Musick K, Bumpass L. How do prior experiences in the family affect transitions to adulthood? In: Booth A, Crouter AC, Shanahan M, editors. Transitions to adulthood in a changing economy: No work, no family, no future? Westport, CT: Praeger; 1999. pp. 69–102. [Google Scholar]
  66. Musick K, Mare RD. Recent trends in the inheritance of poverty and family structure. Social Science Research. 2006;35:471–499. [Google Scholar]
  67. Nock SL. Marriage as a public issue. The Future of Children. 2005;15:13–32. doi: 10.1353/foc.2005.0019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Ooms T. Washington, DC: Center for Law and Social Policy; 2007. Adapting healthy marriage programs for disadvantaged and culturally diverse populations: What are the issues? Retrieved March 5, 2008, from http://www.clasp.org/publications/couplesmarriage_brief_10.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  69. Oppenheimer VK, Kalmijn M, Lim N. Men’s career development and marriage timing during a period of rising inequality. Demography. 1997;34:311–330. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Qian Z. Breaking the racial barriers: Variations in interracial marriage between 1980 and 1990. Demography. 1997;34:263–276. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Raley RK, Bumpass L. The topography of the divorce plateau: Levels and trends in union stability in the United States after 1980. Demographic Research. 2003;8 [np] [Google Scholar]
  72. Rendall MS, Clarke L, Peters HE, Ranjit N, Verropoulou G. Incomplete reporting of men’s fertility in the United States and Britain: A research note. Demography. 1999;36:135–144. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Resnick MD, Bearman PS, Blum RW, Bauman KE, Harris KM, Jones J, Tabor J, Beuhring T, Sieving R, Shew M, Ireland M, Bearinger LH, Udry JR. Protecting adolescents from harm: Findings from the National Longitudinal Study on Adolescent Health. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1997;278:823–832. doi: 10.1001/jama.278.10.823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Rindfuss RR. The young adult years: Diversity, structural change, and fertility. Demography. 1991;28:493–512. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Schwartz CR, Mare RD. Trends in educational assortative marriage from 1940 to 2003. Demography. 2005;42:621–646. doi: 10.1353/dem.2005.0036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Shanahan MJ. Pathways to adulthood in changing societies: Variability and mechanisms in life course perspective. Annual Review of Sociology. 2000;26:667–692. [Google Scholar]
  77. Sigle-Rushton W, McLanahan S. Father absence and child well-being: A critical review. In: Moynihan DP, Smeeding TM, Rainwater L, editors. The future of the family. New York: Russell Sage; 2004. pp. 116–155. [Google Scholar]
  78. Sigle-Rushton W, Hobcraft J, Kiernan K. Parental divorce and subsequent disadvantage: A cross-cohort comparison. Demography. 2005;42:427–446. doi: 10.1353/dem.2005.0026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Sobolewski JM, Amato PR. Parents’ discord and divorce, parent-child relationships and subjective well-being in early adulthood: Is feeling close to two parents always better than feeling close to one? Social Forces. 2007;85:1105–1124. [Google Scholar]
  80. Strohschein L. Parental divorce and child mental health trajectories. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2005;67:1286–1300. [Google Scholar]
  81. Sun Y. Family environment and adolescents’ well-being before and after parents’ marital disruption: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2001;63:697–713. [Google Scholar]
  82. Teti DM, Lamb ME. Socioeconomic and marital outcomes of adolescent marriage, adolescent childbirth, and their co-occurrence. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1989;51:203–212. [Google Scholar]
  83. Thomas A, Sawhill IV. For love and money? The impact of family structure on family income. The Future of Children. 2005;15:57–74. doi: 10.1353/foc.2005.0020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Thomson E, Hanson TL, McLanahan SS. Family structure and child well-being: Economic resources vs. parental behaviors. Social Forces. 1994;73:221–242. doi: 10.1093/sf/sos119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Thomson E, McLanahan SS, Curtin RB. Family structure, gender, and parental socialization. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1992;54:368–378. [Google Scholar]
  86. Thornton A. Influence of the marital history of parents on the marital and cohabitational experiences of children. American Journal of Sociology. 1991;96:868–894. [Google Scholar]
  87. Thornton A, Camburn D. Religious participation and adolescent sexual behavior. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1989;51:641–653. [Google Scholar]
  88. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Healthy Marriage Initiative – General Information. Administration for Children and Families. 2008 Retrieved March 5, 2008, from http://www.acf.hhs.gov/healthymarriage/about/mission.html.
  89. White L. Stepfamilies over the life course: Social support. In: Booth A, Dunn J, editors. Stepfamilies: Who benefits? Who does not? Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 1994. pp. 109–138. [Google Scholar]
  90. Williams K, Umberson D. Marital status, marital transitions, and health: A gendered life course perspective. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2004;45:81–98. doi: 10.1177/002214650404500106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Wojtkiewicz RA. Simplicity and complexity in the effects of parental structure on high school graduation. Demography. 1993;30:701–717. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Wolfinger NH. Trends in the intergenerational transmission of divorce. Demography. 1999;36:415–420. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Wu LL. Effects of family instability, income, and income instability on the risk of a premarital birth. American Sociological Review. 1996;61:386–406. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES