Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Sep 15.
Published in final edited form as: Health Educ Behav. 2006 Aug 4;34(2):278–296. doi: 10.1177/1090198105277854

Ethnic Differences in Decisional Balance and Stages of Mammography Adoption

Regina Otero-Sabogal 1, Susan Stewart 2, Sarah J Shema 3, Rena J Pasick 4
PMCID: PMC2939724  NIHMSID: NIHMS230739  PMID: 16891624

Abstract

Behavioral theories developed through research with mainstream, English-speaking populations have been applied to ethnically diverse and underserved communities in the effort to eliminate disparities in early breast cancer detection. This study tests the validity of the transtheoretical model (TTM) decisional balance measure and the application of the TTM stages of change in a multiethnic, multilingual sample. A random sample of 1,463 Filipino, Latino, African American, Chinese, and White women aged 40 to 74 completed a phone survey of mammography beliefs and practices. Consistent with the TTM and independent of ethnicity, decisional balance was associated with mammography stage in all five ethnic groups when controlling for socioeconomic and other factors. In addition, having private insurance and a regular physician and being a long-time resident in the United States were positively associated with mammography maintenance. The application of the TTM for mammography is supported in a multiethnic and multilingual sample.

Keywords: attitudes, ethnicity, stages of mammography adoption, access, decisional balance


There are significant disparities in breast cancer detection, diagnosis, and mortality between White women and those in other ethnic/racial groups (Baquet & Commiskey, 2000). Despite impressive gains in the use of breast cancer screening overall, recent immigrants, non-English speakers, and women from ethnic minority communities often do not receive regular breast cancer screening (Hiatt, 1997; Hiatt & Pasick, 1996; Sabogal, Merrill, & Packel, 2001). The overwhelming influence of access to care on use of cancer screening (Breen, Wagener, Brown, Davis, & Ballard-Barbash, 2001) is now widely recognized. Yet, enhancing access alone is not sufficient in increasing utilization of breast screening among culturally diverse women (Kelaher & Stellman, 2000; Otero-Sabogal, Arnsberger, Nussey, Brown,&Fox, 1999; Sabogal et al., 2001). Understanding attitudes toward screening and the relationship of attitudes and behavior is key to the development of effective interventions. In particular, it is essential to assess the role of attitudes in relation to key factors such as health insurance and having a regular source of medical care (Garbers, Jessop, Foti, Uribelarrea, & Chiasson, 2003; Otero-Sabogal, Stewart, Sabogal, Brown,&Perez-Stable, 2003). Amajor problem in understanding attitudes and behaviors in culturally and linguistically diverse populations is valid conceptualization and measurement (Pasick, Stewart, Bird, & D’Onofrio, 2001). First, attitudes, which may differ across cultures, must be identified, and second, valid measures for attitudes must be developed and tested. An additional complication is that theories underlying the measurement of mammography-related attitudes were developed mainly among middle-class White women and have not been adequately tested for multicultural validity and comparability.

The Transtheoretical Model and Mammography: Delineating Stages of Adoption and Associated Positive and Negative Attitudes

The transtheoretical model (TTM) for behavior change has been adapted and applied in research on a variety of health behaviors including smoking, alcohol use, diet, exercise, breast-feeding, safe sex, and weight loss. In 1992, Rakowski and colleagues (1992) adapted the model to successfully predict mammography utilization. Rakowski et al. defined five stages associated with adoption of mammography ranging from precontemplation (women who have never had a mammogram and are not considering having a mammogram), contemplation (women planning to have a mammogram but who have not yet done so), action (women who have had a mammogram and intend to have another one), maintenance (women who have mammograms on a regular basis and plan to continue doing so), and relapse (women who have had a mammogram but do not intend to have another one in the near future).

A central tenet of the TTM is that in earlier adoption stages, negative attitudes toward behavior change (cons) outnumber positive attitudes (pros). Individuals advance in stage of readiness as attitudes shift from more negative to more positive. The excess of pros over cons is termed decisional balance. Several studies have confirmed the positive relationship between decisional balance and stage of change (Chamot, Charvet, & Perneger, 2001; Clark et al., 1998; Ma et al., 2002; Rakowski, Andersen, et al., 1997; Rakowski, Clark, et al. 1997; Rakowski, Fulton,&Feldman, 1993; Skinner, Arfken,&Sykes, 1998). Although the TTM has been widely applied to inform interventions in studies of ethnic populations (Champion & Scott, 1997; Kelaher et al., 1999; Otero-Sabogal et al., 2003), the validity of the decisional balance construct in diverse cultures has not been tested. Thus, it is not known whether measures of positive and negative attitudes toward mammography can be adapted and translated for other cultures and languages and if the resulting decisional balance measure is positively associated with the stage of change in diverse subgroups of women. The purpose of this study is to test the validity of the TTM decisional balance measure in women from five ethnic groups who speak four languages. We are particularly interested in mammography maintenance, because little is known about sustained, periodic mammography screening in accordance with guidelines among ethnic-women (Rakowski, Ehrich, Dube,&Pearlman, 1996) and because regular mammography is a national priority in cancer detection and control programs (U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, 2000).

In addition, this study seeks to answer two questions: (a) Are there ethnic differences in decisional balance associated with the stages of mammography adoption? and (b) What is the relative contribution of socioeconomic status, access to care, and decisional balance to mammography maintenance for Latino, African American, Chinese, Filipino, and White women?

METHOD

This research is part of a larger program project grant funded by the National Cancer Institute referred to as Pathfinders (1997–2003). One Pathfinders component study was a randomized, controlled, longitudinal-cohort, intervention trial designed to improve initial breast and cervical cancer screening and maintenance of interval screening under conditions of shifting access to medical care in a sample of African American, Chinese, Filipino, Latino, and White women in low-income areas of Alameda County, California.

Sample

Alameda County is an urban, industrialized region on the east side of San Francisco Bay. The 2000 census shows that the county has an ethnically diverse population of approximately 1.45 million residents of which 14.9% are African American, 20.4% are Asian or Pacific Islander, and 19% are Latino. The largest Asian populations are Chinese (7.5%) and Filipino (4.8%; California Department of Finance, 2001).

Study participants were recruited by telephone to obtain a random sample of women aged 40 to 74 stratified by race/ethnicity: 500 African American, 300 non-Hispanic White, 300 Latino, 200 Chinese, and 200 Filipino women. To identify potential participants, we selected telephone prefixes associated with zip codes corresponding to low-income multiethnic areas of Oakland and Berkeley (the main study prefixes) and used the Mitofsky-Waksberg random-digit-dialing (RDD) technique (Marin, Vanoss Marin, Perez-Stable,& Vanoss, 1990; Potthoff, 1994). We eliminated telephone prefixes associated with predominantly high-income areas and oversampled prefixes associated with large proportions of Chinese and Latino respondents. An RDD sample for Filipino recruitment was generated using telephone prefixes in Union City, an area in which approximately 20% of the residents are Filipino (California Department of Finance, 2001). To complete the desired sample of Chinese women, we purchased two Chinese-listed surname samples, one for the main study prefixes and another for all of Alameda County. A Spanish-listed and a Filipino-listed surname sample for Union City and a Filipino-listed surname sample for all of Alameda County were used to improve recruitment of Filipino women. Use of listed samples has proven to be productive in reaching less numerous subpopulations (Catania et al., 1992). The impact of the use of cell phones in our RDD sample was minimal in spite of using a sample frame of residential households with landline telephones. Results from the National Health Interview Survey in 2003 indicate that only 3.6% of U.S. households did not have a landline telephone but had a wireless telephone (National Center for Health Statistics, 2003). In addition, 4.8% of adults had only a working cellular phone or had no telephone. Thus, very few adults had given up their home phone in 2003. These results indicate that the population without a landline was small (National Center for Health Statistics, 2003). Thus, it was likely smaller still at the time of recruitment for this study (1999–2000), and the potential for bias in our RDD survey due to lack of a landline telephone is negligible. However, as wireless substitution continues to grow over time, RDD telephone surveys will soon find it necessary to include wireless telephones in their sampling frames (Blumberg, Luke, & Cynamon, 2004).

On contacting a household, we verified that it was in one of the targeted areas and confirmed race/ethnic eligibility. We then asked for the number of women aged 50 to 74 years and aged 40 to 49 years living in the home. If more than one woman in the household qualified, we asked to speak to the woman aged 50 to 74 with the most recent birthday. If there were no women aged 50 to 74, we asked to speak to the woman aged 40 to 49 with the most recent birthday. Only one woman per household was asked to participate.

During the initial telephone contact, the interviewer explained the study design and the monetary incentives for participation. A $25 money order was sent by mail to each respondent after completion of the survey. In addition to age, race/ethnicity, and area of residence, eligibility criteria included (a) being an English, Cantonese, Tagalog, or Spanish speaker; (b) having no history of cancer; and (c) intending to reside in the study area for the next 3 years. Each woman consented to be randomly assigned to the intervention or control group, provided her contact information and that of one or two relatives or friends, and agreed to be contacted every 6 months during the study period.

Verbal consent was obtained for eligible women who agreed to participate, and an interview was scheduled for approximately 2 weeks later. Before the interview, each woman received a letter describing the study, the incentives, and her rights as a study participant. Also enclosed was a note informing her that she could call us or send us a refusal letter in an enclosed envelope if she did not wish to participate. The committees on human research of the collaborating institutions (the Northern California Cancer Center and the University of California–San Francisco) approved the research protocol.

The baseline survey was conducted by phone in English, Cantonese, Tagalog, or Spanish by professional bilingual, bicultural female interviewers using a computer-assisted telephone interview system.

Measures

A 40-minute, structured questionnaire was developed. Many of the measures were obtained from surveys used in our prior cancer screening studies with comparable populations (Hiatt et al., 2001; McBride et al., 1998; Perez-Stable, Otero-Sabogal, Sabogal,& Napoles-Springer, 1996; Perez-Stable, Sabogal, & Otero-Sabogal, 1995). Additional items were newly developed based on extensive formative research using focus groups and in-depth interviews, which were adapted from other studies and refined by our multiethnic team of researchers for comprehensibility and for linguistic, cultural, and content validity (Chaturvedi & McKeigue, 1994; Pasick, D’Onofrio, & Otero-Sabogal, 1996). Items were initially developed in English terms known by the multilingual research team to be readily translated into Spanish, Cantonese, and Tagalog. Questions were back-translated, decentered, and pretested in English and the other three languages (Pasick, Sabogal, et al., 1996).

Decisional Balance

Our decisional balance measure was originally adapted from Rakowski (Rakowski et al., 1992; Rakowski et al., 1993) and expanded with new items derived from two focus groups in each of the targeted race/ethnic groups. These were extensively pretested with all five ethnic groups for cultural and linguistic appropriateness (Pasick et al., 2001; Sabogal, Otero-Sabogal, Pasick, Jenkins,&Perez-Stable, 1996). Con items were designed to reflect women’s concerns about or barriers to mammography including the cost of mammography, their fears of the test, results and risk of radiation, feelings of embarrassment, anticipation of having a negative mammography experience, being too busy, and dislike of the procedure. Our list of pros or facilitators was designed to measure factors that motivate women to get screened and includes, for example, the idea that a mother having a mammogram is good for the entire family, with family being a strong value in the cultures we are studying (Sabogal, Marin, Otero-Sabogal, Marin,&Perez-Stable, 1987). (New pro and con items and items adapted from Rakowski’s measure are shown in Table 2). Items also addressed misconceptions and knowledge about cancer screening because our previous studies (Perez-Stable, Sabogal, Otero-Sabogal, Hiatt, & McPhee, 1992; Zambrana, Breen, Fox, & Gutierrez-Mohamed, 1999) and focus group findings indicate these are important barriers to mammography screening among our target population. We used the same pro and con measures with all ethnic groups.

Table 2.

Percentage Agreeing With Decisional Balance Scale Items (Somewhat or Strongly Agree) by Ethnicity

African
American
Chinese Filipina Latina White Total
(N = 1,463)
(n = 497) (n = 199) (n = 167) (n = 300) (n = 300)
Variable % % % % % n %
Mammography Cons Scale
    You are too busy to have a mammogram 9.1 28.1 24.0 30.7 9.7 262 17.9
    You will only get a mammogram if you have a breast problema 10.3 28.1 9.0 25.0 7.0 218 14.9
    You are concerned that mammogram x-rays may be risky or dangerous 27.0 30.2 22.2 46.7 33.0 470 32.1
    Mammograms cost too much for you 16.5 22.6 15.0 36.7 8.0 286 19.5
    You do not like to have mammograms 42.3 27.6 15.6 44.7 56.7 595 40.7
    You just never thought of getting a mammogram 18.7 24.6 18.0 35.7 5.0 294 20.1
    Someone you know had a bad experience getting a mammogram 28.6 22.1 27.5 40.7 29.0 441 30.1
    You do not want a mammogram because you are afraid to find out if you have
cancer
12.9 17.1 8.4 26.0 7.7 213 14.6
    You worry that you would not be able to pay for treatment if you got breast
cancer
42.3 58.3 33.5 73.0 25.7 678 46.3
Mammography Pros Scale
    You would have a mammogram if your doctor told you that it is important 97.8 98.0 96.4 98.0 96.0 1,424 97.3
    Having a mammogram every year will give you a feeling of control over your
healtha
87.9 95.0 89.8 91.7 78.3 1,286 87.9
    It will be good for your family if you have a mammogram 89.7 97.5 95.2 93.0 86.0 1,336 91.3
    Yearly mammograms give you peace of minda 91.3 98.5 93.4 95.0 83.7 1,342 91.7
    A woman should get a mammogram even if no one in her family has had
breast cancera
97.0 96.5 97.0 94.0 95.0 1,403 95.9
    Having a mammogram every 1 to 2 years decreases a woman’s chance of dying
from breast cancer
75.7 92.0 83.2 83.7 91.3 1,223 83.6

NOTE: All variables differ across race/ethnicity at p < .001 except “you would have a mammogram if your doctor told you that it is important” (p = .43) and “a woman should get a mammogram even if no one in her family has had breast cancer” (p = .24).

a

Statements similar to Rakowski’s (1992) Pros and Cons Scale. Statement wording was written in simpler language.

We simplified the pro and con response scales, because pretesting shows some subsets of our target population had difficulty with the original scaled responses (a 4-point, Likert-type scale of 1 = strongly disagree to 4 = strongly agree), particularly over the phone. The result was a two-step response. First, women were asked if they agree or disagree with an item (e.g., “You are too busy to have a mammogram”). Next, using their response, they were asked if they “(dis)agree strongly” or “(dis)agree somewhat.” “Strongly agree” was coded as 4, “somewhat agree” as 3, “somewhat disagree” as 2, and “strongly disagree” as 1. “Don’t know” and “refused” responses were coded as 2.5. Pretesting indicated that these response categories offered better construct validity and reduced the number of “do not know” responses in the test sample.

To build the Decisional Balance Scale, items measuring cons were summed into one scale and pros were summed into another scale. The scales were standardized to a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10. Decisional balance was computed by subtracting the Cons Scale from the Pros Scale (Rakowski et al., 1997). We did not compute the Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficients for the Pros and Cons Scales because the items in the Cons (or Pros) Scale refer to different aspects of mammography that need not be related to one another. Alpha should be computed when the items in a scale are supposed to measure the same construct. However, one may have negative (or positive) attitudes about conceptually unrelated aspects of mammography (e.g., radiation and cost), and it could be argued that a cons (or pros) scale consisting of entirely unrelated items would be appropriate.

Sociodemographic Variables

Sociodemographic variables included self-reported race/ethnicity (non-Hispanic White, African American, Latino, Chinese, or Filipino), the preferred language of the interview (coded as English vs. non-English), and years of residence in the United States (coded as ≤ 10 or > 10). Other demographic characteristics included age at time of interview, years of education, marital status (married; living with a partner; or single, divorced, widowed), and insurance status (private, public only, or none). To measure respondents’ income, we asked women to select among one of seven annual household income categories (less than $5,000, $5,000–$9,999, $10,000–$19,999, $20,000–$29,999, $30,000–$39,999, $40,000–$49,999, and $50,000 or more), which we collapsed into three categories: less than $20,000, $20,000 to $49,999, and $50,000 or more.

Access to Medical Care

Based on prior literature, we measured access to medical care as follows: (a) having health insurance (private, public, none; Berk & Schur, 1998; Potosky, Breen, Graubard, & Parsons, 1998) and (b) having a regular doctor indicating continuity of care (Bindman et al., 1995; Osmond, Vranizan, Schillinger, Stewart, & Bindman, 1996). We have reported elsewhere in the Pathfinders study the relationship between race/ethnicity, access, satisfaction, and regular mammography and Pap testing in five racial/ethnic groups (Somkin et al., 2003).

Outcome Variable: Stages of Adoption for Mammography

The stage of adoption for mammography was our primary dependent variable. To measure the stage of adoption, women were asked if they have ever had a mammogram. Those who indicated they have had a mammogram were asked for the month and year of their most recent mammogram and the year of the mammogram before their most recent one. In this study, we used a 15-month window after the last mammogram as a cutoff point to define recent screening (Legler et al., 2002). Women are often required to wait 12 months after a mammogram to make their next appointment, and a wait of several weeks for an available appointment is not unusual. Thus, we allowed 3 extra months to compensate for a delay. In addition, women were asked if they planned to have a mammogram in the next 12 months. Accordingly, our mammography adoption stage definitions were as follows:

  • precontemplation: never had a mammogram; did not plan to have one in the next 12 months

  • relapse: had one or more mammograms in the past; did not plan to have one in the next 12 months

  • contemplation: had no mammogram in the 15 months prior to the interview; planned to have one in the next 12 months

  • action: had a mammogram within the past 15 months but not during the 2 years before that; planned to have one in the next 12 months

  • maintenance: had a mammogram within the past 15 months and a mammogram within the 2 years before that; planned to have one in the next 12 months

Statistical Methods

Our selection of the independent variables was guided by prior research about the factors influencing ethnic women’s decision to undergo mammography (Otero-Sabogal et al., 1999; Otero-Sabogal et al., 2003; Suarez, Roche, Nichols, & Simpson, 1997). Differences in sociodemographic characteristics and screening rates among race/ethnic groups were assessed using the chi-square test for categorical variables (marital status, income, years in the United States, language of interview, insurance status, regular doctor, and mammography stage) and an ANOVA for continuous variables (age, years of education, and decisional balance; see Table 1).

Table 1.

Distribution of Demographic Covariates and Screening by Ethnicity

African
American
Chinese Filipina Latina White Total (N = 1,463)
(n = 497) (n = 199) (n = 167) (n = 300) (n = 300)
Variable % % % % % n %
Age in years, M (SD)   52.1 (9.0) 52.3 (9.2) 54.8 (9.4) 50.7 (8.5) 52.6 (8.4) 1,462 52.3 (8.9)
Education in years, M (SD)   13.6 (2.4) 10.6 (4.4) 14.2 (3.1) 8.7 (4.5) 16.5 (2.9) 1,449 12.9 (4.3)
Marital status
    Married   32.2 85.4 73.1 65.7 54.3 812 55.5
    Not married   67.8 14.6 26.9 34.3 45.7 651 44.5
Income
    < $20,000   43.6 46.0 16.0 48.6 17.4 496 36.0
    $20,000–$50,000   35.6 38.1 28.8 40.5 31.2 483 35.1
    > $50,000   20.9 15.9 55.1 10.9 51.3 397 28.9
Years in United States
    ≤ 10 years     0.2 32.2 20.4 23.3   0.7 171 11.7
    > 10 years   99.8 67.8 79.6 76.7 99.3 1,292 88.3
Language of interview
    Not English     0.0 85.9 33.5 73.3   0.0 447 30.6
    English 100.0 14.1 66.5 26.7 100.0 1,016 69.5
Insurance
    Private   61.4 52.3 83.2 48.3 87.3 955 65.3
    Public   24.9 23.6 10.8 16.0   5.7 254 17.4
    None   13.7 24.1 6.0 35.7   7.0 254 17.4
Regular doctor
    Do not have one   19.1 17.1 15.0 35.3 14.3 303 20.7
    Have one   80.9 82.9 85.0 64.7 85.7 1,160 79.3
Decisional balance, M (SD)     1.4 (15.0)   0.1 (13.1)   4.3 (14.2) −5.3 (14.5)   0.5 (15.3) 1,463   0.0 (14.9)
Mammography stagea
    Precontemplation     3.2 16.6   2.4   3.7   5.7 81   5.5
    Relapse   10.1 31.2 16.8   9.3 14.0 210 14.4
    Contemplation   34.0 22.6 31.7 34.0 24.7 443 30.3
    Action     5.8   4.0 10.2 15.3   5.3 116   7.9
    Maintenance   46.9 25.6 38.9 37.7 50.3 613 41.9

NOTE: Age: 1 missing; income: 87 missing; education: 14 missing. All variables differ across race/ethnicity at p < .001 except years in the United States (p = .021). Language of interview and years in the United States tested among Chinese, Filipina, and Latina women only.

a

Precontemplation: never had a mammogram, does not plan to get one in next 12 months; relapse: had one or more mammograms in past, does not plan to get one in next 12 months; contemplation: no mammogram within 15 months, plans to get one in next 12 months; action: most recent mammogram within 15 months, none within 2 years of most recent, plans to get one in next 12 months; maintenance: most recent mammogram within 15 months, another within 2 years of most recent, plans to get one in next 12 months.

We compared the proportion of respondents who agreed with each decisional balance item by race/ethnicity using chi-square tests (see Table 2). The relationship between the decisional balance and mammography stage was assessed by using polytomous logistic regression with generalized logits (Stokes & Davis, 1995) to obtain adjusted odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs). In addition to the main effect of decisional balance, an interaction between race/ethnicity and decisional balance was included to model the specific race/ethnic effects of decisional balance on the stage of adoption (precontemplation, relapse, contemplation, and action vs. maintenance) controlling for the other covariates listed above (see Table 3). ORs were computed per 15 units (1 SD) of decisional balance; a chi-square test was used to test the interaction between race/ethnicity and the decisional balance. Observations with missing data for a particular variable were excluded from the relevant analyses, because the proportion missing was sufficiently low and did not threaten to bias our survey results. As described in the Method section, “don’t know” and “refused” responses to the pro and con items were coded to the midpoint (2.5) for decisional balance calculations. In the analysis of agreement with pro and con items, “don’t know” and “refused” responses were grouped with “disagree” responses so that there are no missing values for Table 2.

Table 3.

Logistic Regression Models of Mammography Stage Versus Maintenance (n = 1,368)

Precontemplation
OR (95% CI)
Relapse
OR (95% CI)
Contemplation
OR (95% CI)
Action
OR (95% CI)
Race/ethnicity
    African American 0.53 (0.17, 1.66) 0.58 (0.32, 1.04) 1.02 (0.68, 1.55) 0.90 (0.42, 1.94)
    Chinese 6.88 (1.61, 29.4) 5.93 (2.63, 13.4) 1.78 (0.86, 3.68) 0.62 (0.17, 2.30)
    Filipina 1.17 (0.22, 6.17) 2.51 (1.19, 5.27) 2.35 (1.27, 4.36) 2.41 (0.92, 6.34)
    Latina 0.32 (0.07, 1.55) 0.61 (0.28, 1.33) 0.86 (0.48, 1.53) 1.13 (0.43, 2.96)
    White 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
Agea 0.90 (0.86, 0.94) 0.96 (0.94, 0.98) 0.94 (0.93, 0.96) 0.93 (0.90, 0.96)
Educationa 0.97 (0.88, 1.07) 0.94 (0.88, 1.00) 0.97 (0.93, 1.02) 0.96 (0.89, 1.04)
Marital status
    Married 0.69 (0.34, 1.39) 0.93 (0.60, 1.43) 0.98 (0.71, 1.36) 0.93 (0.55, 1.57)
    Unmarried 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
Income
    < $20,000 1.08 (0.37, 3.10) 1.17 (0.63, 2.19) 1.03 (0.64, 1.66) 0.73 (0.34, 1.57)
    $20,000–$50,000 1.35 (0.54, 3.34) 1.29 (0.78, 2.14) 1.30 (0.89, 1.89) 0.59 (0.31, 1.12)
    > $50,000 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
Years in the United States
    ≤ 10 4.15 (1.65, 10.4) 1.51 (0.73, 3.12) 1.82 (1.00, 3.33) 2.66 (1.23, 5.75)
    > 10 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
Language of interview
    Non-English 0.69 (0.19, 2.59) 0.53 (0.25, 1.09) 0.70 (0.40, 1.22) 1.51 (0.66, 3.48)
    English 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
Insurance
    Private 0.35 (0.15, 0.80) 0.86 (0.46, 1.60) 0.43 (0.27, 0.69) 0.64 (0.31, 1.36)
    Public 0.79 (0.32, 1.93) 0.93 (0.46, 1.87) 0.78 (0.47, 1.30) 1.37 (0.64, 2.96)
    None 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
Regular doctor
    No 3.93 (2.05, 7.54) 1.89 (1.16, 3.10) 1.72 (1.18, 2.51) 1.56 (0.89, 2.74)
    Yes 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
Decisional balanceb
    African American 0.27 (0.18, 0.42) 0.39 (0.28, 0.54) 0.64 (0.49, 0.83) 0.91 (0.55, 1.52)
    Chinese 0.14 (0.06, 0.32) 0.36 (0.19, 0.69) 0.44 (0.23, 0.85) 0.68 (0.20, 2.35)
    Filipina 0.18 (0.06, 0.59) 0.21 (0.10, 0.41) 0.38 (0.21, 0.71) 0.46 (0.20, 1.07)
    Latina 0.34 (0.18, 0.66) 0.72 (0.45, 1.14) 0.76 (0.55, 1.06) 0.74 (0.48, 1.14)
    White 0.18 (0.10, 0.32) 0.28 (0.18, 0.42) 0.57 (0.39, 0.83) 0.85 (0.42, 1.72)

NOTE: OR = odds ratio, adjusted for all variables tabulated; CI = confidence interval.

a

Per year.

b

Per 15 units (1 SD).

RESULTS

Response Rate

At the end of recruitment, an ethnically diverse sample of 1,463 women (497 African American, 199 Chinese, 167 Filipina, 300 Latina, and 300 White) 40 to 74 years of age was enrolled and interviewed. A total of 46,206 telephone numbers were called, of which 32,521 (70%) were household numbers. Among households, 15,264 (47%) were screened for eligibility, of which 2,963 (19%) contained an eligible participant. Among eligible participants, 1,840 (62%) consented to participate in the study. Of those who agreed to participate, 1,463 (80%) were recontacted and interviewed. Reasons for ineligibility included not being a member of a study ethnic/racial group (20%), not speaking any of the study languages (10%), no woman aged 40 to 74 in the household (66%), having a history of cancer (2%), and residing or planning to move out of the area (1%).

Sample Characteristics

Among the 1,463 women enrolled in the survey, a total of 1,016 women were interviewed in English, 220 in Spanish, 171 in Chinese, and 56 in Tagalog. As shown in Table 1, a substantial proportion of women had no health insurance (17%), no regular doctor (21%), a household income below $20,000 (36%), or 10 or fewer years of residence in the United States (12%). There were significant race/ethnic differences in the distribution of sociodemographic characteristics: Latinas were least likely to have health insurance or a regular doctor and had the lowest mean years of education; Chinese women were least likely to have lived in the United States more than 10 years or to be interviewed in English; and Filipinas and White women had higher income levels and were more likely to have private insurance than African Americans, Chinese, and Latinas.

Decisional Balance and Stages of Mammography Screening

Although most women (87%) reported having had at least one mammogram, fewer than half (42%) were in the maintenance stage. There were significant race/ethnic differences in the stage of adoption. White and African American women were most likely to be in maintenance, and Chinese women were most likely to be in precontemplation or relapse. There were also substantial race/ethnic differences in the decisional balance. Latinas had the lowest mean scores, and Filipinas had the highest mean scores.

Race/ethnic differences were found in agreement with con items (Table 2). In general, Latinas showed relatively high levels of agreement with most cons (e.g., “being to busy,” “concerned about x-rays,” “never thought about it,” “someone you know had a bad experience,” “you worry that you will not be able to pay for treatment”) compared with African Americans and Filipinas. White women reported a higher level of agreement only for one con item: “do not like to have mammograms.” In addition, White and African American women reported the lowest level of agreement with “being too busy to get a mammogram.” Chinese women tended to show higher levels of agreement with all pros than women in other racial/ethnic groups (Table 2). White women agreed less often than the other groups with the attitudinal pros but agreed more often than African Americans, Filipinas, and Latinas with the pros that were knowledge-oriented (“having a mammogram every 1 to 2 years decreases a woman’s chance of dying from breast cancer”). African Americans reported the lowest level of agreement with that knowledge statement. Agreement with “you would have a mammogram if your doctor told you that it is important” and “a woman should get a mammogram even if no one in her family has had breast cancer” was high in all ethnic groups.

A multivariate model (Table 3) showed that younger women were more likely than older women to be in precontemplation (OR = 0.90 per year of age), relapse (OR = 0.96), contemplation (OR = 0.94), or action (OR = 0.93) rather than maintenance. Compared to White women, Filipinas were more likely to be in relapse (OR = 2.51) or contemplation (OR = 2.35), and Chinese women were much more likely to be in precontemplation (OR = 6.88) or relapse (OR = 5.93). Women who had lived 10 or fewer years in the United States were more likely than longer term residents to be in precontemplation (OR = 4.15) or action (OR = 2.66). Participants without a regular doctor were more likely to be in precontemplation (OR = 3.93), relapse (OR = 1.89), or contemplation (OR = 1.72); those with private insurance were less likely than the uninsured to be in precontemplation (OR = 0.35) or contemplation (OR = 0.43). There was a significant association between the decisional balance and mammography stage in all race/ethnic groups, and a higher decisional balance was associated with a higher mammography stage. On average, for each additional standard deviation of decisional balance, women were less likely to be in precontemplation (OR = 0.21, 95% CI = 0.15, 0.30), relapse (OR = 0.36, 95% CI = 0.28, 0.45), contemplation (OR = 0.54, 95% CI = 0.44, 0.67), or (marginally) action (OR = 0.71, 95% CI = 0.49, 1.02) rather than maintenance. Overall, there was no significant racial/ethnic difference in the effect of a decisional balance on screening, χ2(df = 16) = 18.75, p = .28.

DISCUSSION

The main contribution of our study is the demonstration that the decisional balance was associated with mammography screening behavior among women of different cultures and languages in accordance with the TTM. Among Latino, Chinese, Filipino, African American, and White women, the odds of mammography maintenance increased with higher levels of decisional balance. This result provides support for the cross-cultural application of the decisional balance measure to mammography screening—an important finding because the development of the TTM mammography measures occurred predominately among White women (Chamot et al., 2001; Lauver, Henriques, Settersten, & Bumann, 2003; Rakowski et al., 1992).

This study also examined stages of mammography adoption, positive and negative attitudes toward screening (pros, cons, and decisional balance), and key sociodemographic characteristics. We found that when controlling for sociodemographic characteristics and access to medical care, women from all ethnic groups who were in maintenance viewed mammography more favorably than women who were in precontemplation. Access to medical care and socioeconomic factors were also correlated with women’s stage of readiness for mammography adoption as reported by other studies (Levy-Storms, Bastani, & Reuben, 2004; Otero-Sabogal et al., 2003). Women who were younger, did not have insurance or a regular doctor, had lived in the United States for less than 10 years, and perceived more barriers relative to benefits of mammography were more likely to be in precontemplation. As expected, recent immigrants were more likely to be new to mammography (action) or unfamiliar with mammography (precontemplation). Those women with private insurance were less likely to be in contemplation or precontemplation but were as likely to be in relapse as women with no insurance. Our logistic regression results indicate that the association between insurance status and relapse can be explained largely by other factors. In particular, compared to women with a regular doctor, those without one were more likely to be in relapse and less likely to have private insurance. This suggests that the intention to maintain screening is fostered not by having insurance per se but by having an ongoing relationship with a health care provider.

Other important variables, such as knowledge, attitudinal, behavioral, and contextual factors, may influence a woman’s decision to get annual mammograms. As reported by our Pathfinders colleagues (Somkin et al., 2003), women who reported that there were too many forms to fill out and those who felt embarrassed at the last mammogram were less likely (ORs = 0.51 and 0.59, respectively) to receive mammograms regularly. Thus, although cost and lack of insurance are repeatedly cited as primary deterrents to regular use of breast cancer screening, simply having insurance is not enough to eliminate barriers to the regular use of mammography screening. In addition to cost and lack of insurance, individual, attitudinal, clinician, and health care system factors may further explain regular participation in mammography screening among low-income women (Otero-Sabogal et al., 2004). Continuity of medical care was strongly associated with stages of mammography.

Chinese women were least likely to have lived in the United States 10 years or more, but even so, they were disproportionately likely to be in precontemplation and relapse. Latinas were least likely to have a regular doctor, and they also reported agreement with more barriers to mammography than women from the other ethnic groups. This finding indicates that the factors included in our model did not explain the overrepresentation of Chinese women in the precontemplation and relapse stages. In our sample, Chinese women were much less likely than women in other ethnic groups to report an intention to get a mammogram in the next year regardless of their screening history. Perhaps this is due in part to differences in the degree of commitment implied by a “yes” answer to, “Do you plan to have a mammogram in the next 12 months?” Low perceived need, lack of physician recommendation, and attitudes related to modesty and sexuality may also account for Chinese women’s reluctance to be screened (Tang, Solomon, & McCracken, 2000). We also found ethnic differences in women’s agreement with the decisional balance statements assessing pros and cons. White and African American women who participated in regular mammography screening were significantly more likely to perceive greater benefits and fewer barriers to screening than women who did not get screened regularly (Champion & Menon, 1997; Holm, Frank, & Curtin, 1999).

Overall, Latinas agreed with a greater number of cons and had a lower decisional balance in maintenance than women in other ethnic groups when controlling for insurance and other demographic factors. This result is consistent with previous findings indicating that Latino ethnicity was a strong predictor of mammography underuse (Calle, Flanders, Thun, & Martin, 1993). It is important to further explore whether Latinas’ lower decisional balance compared with the other ethnic groups can be explained by a tendency to agree with statements measuring perceived cons rather than truly negative attitudes toward mammography. As reported in other studies, a high proportion of African American women reported a dislike of mammography (Andrews, 2001; Champion & Menon, 1997;Keemers-Gels et al., 2000). Fear of radiation, limited knowledge, (Miller&Champion, 1997), and past experiences (Champion & Menon, 1997) may explain this result. Chinese women reported a high number of pros as well as cons. Low education, low perceived need, lack of physician recommendation, and attitudes related to modesty and sexuality may account for Chinese women’s reluctance to be screened (Tang et al., 2000). Filipino women agreed with a high number of pros and a low number of cons. Given that they also reported having insurance, being bilingual, and having a regular physician, the proportion of Filipino women in mammography maintenance was lower than expected. Modesty, traditional health beliefs, and gender roles may explain these results (McBride et al., 1998).

Members of the Pathfinders research team have investigated this issue (McPhee et al., 2002). Following our baseline telephone survey regarding receipt of mammogram, computerized and written medical records were examined in a subsample of 846 women to validate dates and locations of tests. Overall, the validation rate for mammogram self-report was 75.4%. Validation rates differed significantly by ethnicity for mammograms with African American, Chinese, and Filipino ethnicity associated with lower odds of validation compared to White ethnicity. These findings suggest that studies such as ours and other estimates of mammography screening may need to be adjusted downward. Additional validations should be conducted to confirm this result and ascertain the degree of correction needed.

There is still potential to improve our Decisional Balance Scale by identifying additional pros and cons to cover the full range of attitudes toward mammography across ethnically diverse groups. Understanding sociodemographic, cultural, attitudinal, behavioral, and contextual factors associated with breast cancer screening among women of different ethnic/racial backgrounds and developing adequate measures for those factors requires group-specific ethnographic studies using qualitative methods (focus groups, observations, and unstructured interviews).

This study had limitations. First, our results are based on a cross-sectional baseline survey of a longitudinal study and thus do not allow causal attributions to be made. Subsequent analyses from this study may illuminate the causal relationship between the decisional balance and screening stage. Also, although self-report is the usual method for eliciting screening behavior among women, because the cost of obtaining medical record data is substantial, self-reported rates for mammography among racial and ethnic groups may overestimate its use, and this issue has been investigated by members of the Pathfinders research team (McPhee et al., 2002). Finally, our sample was selected from one urban county with a large multiethnic population, and we only selected women who could be contacted a second time. Thus, results may not be applicable to women who live in communities with different access to mammography screening services or even the broader population of women in Alameda County.

IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE

Our findings have significant implications for the promotion of cancer detection efforts among multiethnic populations. They suggest that, in addition to access to health care, attitudes toward screening matter. Efforts to improve mammography screening among ethnically diverse communities can and should address perceived barriers and facilitators that are associated with the stage of mammography adoption (Legler et al., 2002).

Future studies need to explore additional pros and cons toward mammography across ethnic groups as well as the validity of additional constructs of the TTM and other behavioral theories in explaining screening behaviors among ethnic/racial groups. Furthermore, a better understanding of the cultural context for positive and negative perceptions in relation to stages of adoption will be needed to develop the most effective screening promotion strategies and messages. Even as we continue to explore and gain understanding of the role and meaning of culture in cancer screening behavior and communication, the TTM offers a sound framework for tailoring intervention messages across very diverse cultural and socioeconomic groups. Our results suggest that individual tailoring to a woman’s stage of adoption must be performed in the appropriate cultural context. Pros and cons to screening not only differ by individual but may have different meanings and values across cultures. In addition, attitudes to health and health care providers may vary across ethnic and socioeconomic groups. These cultural variations need to be taken into account when providers attempt to move a woman from one screening stage to another.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the National Cancer Institute (Grant #5PO1 CA 55112). We would like to thank William Rakowski, PhD; Fabio Sabogal, PhD; and Stephen McPhee, MD, for their thoughtful comments on earlier versions of the manuscript. Special thanks to Laurel Haeusslein for her administrative support.

Footnotes

Contributor Information

Regina Otero-Sabogal, Institute for Health and Aging, University of California–San Francisco.

Susan Stewart, Comprehensive Cancer Center, University of California–San Francisco.

Sarah J. Shema, Northern California Cancer Center, Fremont.

Rena J. Pasick, Comprehensive Cancer Center, University of California–San Francisco.

References

  1. Andrews FJ. Pain during mammography: Implications for breast screening programmes. Australasian Radiology. 2001;45:113–117. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1673.2001.00889.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baquet CR, Commiskey P. Socioeconomic factors and breast carcinoma in multicultural women. Cancer. 2000;88:1256–1264. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-0142(20000301)88:5+<1256::aid-cncr13>3.0.co;2-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Berk ML, Schur CL. Measuring access to care: Improving information for policymakers. Health Affairs (Millwood) 1998;17:180–186. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.17.1.180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bindman AB, Grumbach K, Osmond D, Komaromy M, Vranizan K, Lurie N, et al. Preventable hospitalizations and access to health care. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1995;274:305–311. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Blumberg J, Luke J, Cynamon ML. The impact of wireless substitution on random digit dialing surveys. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics; Paper presented at the 59th Annual Conference of the American Association for Public Opinion Research; Phoenix, AZ. 2004. May, [Google Scholar]
  6. Breen N, Wagener DK, Brown ML, Davis WW, Ballard-Barbash R. Progress in cancer screening over a decade: Results of cancer screening from the 1987, 1992, and 1998 national health interview surveys. Journal of the National Cancer Institute. 2001;93:1704–1713. doi: 10.1093/jnci/93.22.1704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. California Department of Finance. U.S. Census bureau, census 2000 of population and housing, summary file 1. Sacramento, CA: Author; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  8. Calle EE, Flanders WD, Thun MJ, Martin LM. Demographic predictors of mammography and pap smear screening in U.S. women. American Journal of Public Health. 1993;83:53–60. doi: 10.2105/ajph.83.1.53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Catania JA, Coates TJ, Stall R, Turner H, Peterson J, Hearst N, et al. Prevalence of AIDS-related risk factors and condom use in the United States. Science. 1992;258:1101–1106. doi: 10.1126/science.1439818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chamot E, Charvet AI, Perneger TV. Predicting stages of adoption of mammography screening in a general population. European Journal of Cancer. 2001;37:1869–1877. doi: 10.1016/s0959-8049(01)00234-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Champion VL, Menon U. Predicting mammography and breast self-examination in African-American women. Cancer Nursing. 1997;20:315–322. doi: 10.1097/00002820-199710000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Champion VL, Scott CR. Reliability and validity of breast cancer screening belief scales in African-American women. Nursing Research. 1997;46:331–337. doi: 10.1097/00006199-199711000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chaturvedi N, McKeigue PM. Methods for epidemiological surveys of ethnic minority groups. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 1994;48:107–111. doi: 10.1136/jech.48.2.107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Clark MA, Rakowski W, Ehrich B, Pearlman DN, Goldstein M, Dube CE, et al. Stages of adopting regular screening mammography: Do women differ in decisional balance within stages? Journal of Health Psychology. 1998;3:491–506. doi: 10.1177/135910539800300404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Garbers S, Jessop DJ, Foti H, Uribelarrea M, Chiasson MA. Barriers to breast cancer screening for low-income Mexican and Dominican women in New York City. Journal of Urban Health: Bulletin of the New York Academy of Medicine. 2003;80:81–91. doi: 10.1007/PL00022327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hiatt RA. Behavioral research contributions and needs in cancer prevention and control: Adherence to cancer screening advice. Preventive Medicine. 1997;26:S11–S18. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1997.0205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hiatt RA, Pasick RJ. Unsolved problems in early breast cancer detection: Focus on the underserved. Breast Cancer Research and Treatment. 1996;40:37–51. doi: 10.1007/BF01806001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hiatt RA, Pasick RJ, Stewart S, Bloom J, Davis P, Gardiner P, et al. Community-based cancer screening for underserved women: Design and baseline findings from the breast and cervical cancer intervention study. Preventive Medicine. 2001;33:190–203. doi: 10.1006/pmed.2001.0871. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Holm CJ, Frank DI, Curtin J. Health beliefs, health locus of control, and women’s mammography behavior. Cancer Nursing. 1999;22:149–156. doi: 10.1097/00002820-199904000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Keemers-Gels ME, Groenendijk RP, van den Heuvel JH, Boetes C, Peer PG, Wobbes TH. Pain experienced by women attending breast cancer screening. Breast Cancer Research and Treatment. 2000;60:235–240. doi: 10.1023/a:1006457520996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kelaher M, Gillespie AG, Allotey P, Manderson L, Potts H, Sheldrake M, et al. The transtheoretical model and cervical screening: Its application among culturally diverse communities in Queensland, Australia. Ethnicity and Health. 1999;4:259–276. doi: 10.1080/13557859998047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kelaher M, Stellman JM. The impact of Medicare funding on the use of mammography among older women: Implications for improving access to screening. Preventive Medicine. 2000;31:658–664. doi: 10.1006/pmed.2000.0759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Lauver DR, Henriques JB, Settersten L, Bumann MC. Psychosocial variables, external barriers, and stage of mammography adoption. Health Psychology. 2003;22:649–653. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.22.6.649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Legler J, Meissner HI, Coyne C, Breen N, Chollette V, Rimer BK. The effectiveness of interventions to promote mammography among women with historically lower rates of screening. Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention. 2002;11:59–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Levy-Storms L, Bastani R, Reuben DB. Predictors of varying levels of nonadherence to mammography screening in older women. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2004;52:768–773. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52216.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Ma J, Betts NM, Horacek T, Georgiou C, White A, Nitzke S. The importance of decisional balance and self-efficacy in relation to stages of change for fruit and vegetable intakes by young adults. American Journal of Health Promotion. 2002;16:157–166. doi: 10.4278/0890-1171-16.3.157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Marin G, Vanoss Marin B, Perez-Stable EJ, Vanoss B. Feasibility of a telephone survey to study a minority community: Hispanics in San Francisco. American Journal of Public Health. 1990;80:323–326. doi: 10.2105/ajph.80.3.323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. McBride MR, Pasick RJ, Stewart S, Tuason N, Sabogal F, Duenas G. Factors associated with cervical cancer screening among Filipino women in California. Asian American and Pacific Islander Journal of Health. 1998;6:358–367. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. McPhee SJ, Nguyen TT, Shema SJ, Nguyen B, Somkin C, Vo P, et al. Validation of recall of breast and cervical cancer screening by women in an ethnically diverse population. Preventive Medicine. 2002;35:463–473. doi: 10.1006/pmed.2002.1096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Miller AM, Champion VL. Attitudes about breast cancer and mammography: Racial, income, and educational differences. Women and Health. 1997;26:41–63. doi: 10.1300/J013v26n01_04. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. National Center for Health Statistics. National Health Interview Survey. Hyattsville, MD: U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, Centers for Disease Control & Prevention; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  32. Osmond DH, Vranizan K, Schillinger D, Stewart AL, Bindman AB. Measuring the need for medical care in an ethnically diverse population. Health Services Research. 1996;31:551–571. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Otero-Sabogal R, Arnsberger P, Nussey B, Brown B, Fox PJ. Factors associated with low-income Latinas adherence to breast cancer rescreening. Women and Cancer. 1999;1:35–41. [Google Scholar]
  34. Otero-Sabogal R, Owens D, Canchola J, Golding JM, Tabnak R, Fox P. Mammography rescreening among women of diverse ethnicities: Patient, provider, and health care system factors. Journal of Healthcare for the Poor and Underserved. 2004;15:390–412. doi: 10.1353/hpu.2004.0048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Otero-Sabogal R, Stewart S, Sabogal F, Brown B, Perez-Stable E. Access and attitudinal factors related to breast and cervical cancer rescreening: Why are Latinas still underscreened? Health Education & Behavior. 2003;30:337–359. doi: 10.1177/1090198103030003008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Pasick RJ, D’Onofrio CN, Otero-Sabogal R. Similarities and differences across cultures: Questions to inform a third generation for health promotion research. Health Education Quarterly. 1996;23:S142–S161. [Google Scholar]
  37. Pasick RJ, Sabogal F, Bird JA, D’Onofrio CN, Jenkins CN, Lee M. Problems and progress in translation of health survey questions: The Pathways experience. Health Education Quarterly. 1996;23:S28–S40. [Google Scholar]
  38. Pasick RJ, Stewart SL, Bird JA, D’Onofrio CN. Quality of data in multiethnic health surveys. Public Health Reports. 2001;116:223–243. doi: 10.1093/phr/116.S1.223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Perez-Stable EJ, Otero-Sabogal R, Sabogal F, Napoles-Springer A. Pathways to early cancer detection for Latinas: En accion contra el cancer. Health Education Quarterly. 1996;23:S41–S59. [Google Scholar]
  40. Perez-Stable EJ, Sabogal F, Otero-Sabogal R. Use of cancer-screening tests in the San Francisco Bay Area: Comparison of Latinos and Anglos. Journal of the National Cancer Institute Monographs. 1995;18:147–153. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Perez-Stable EJ, Sabogal F, Otero-Sabogal R, Hiatt RA, McPhee SJ. Misconceptions about cancer among Latinos and Anglos. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1992;268:3219–3223. doi: 10.1001/jama.1992.03490220063029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Potosky AL, Breen N, Graubard BI, Parsons PE. The association between health care coverage and the use of cancer screening tests. Results from the 1992 national health interview survey. Medical Care. 1998;36:257–270. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199803000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Potthoff RF. Telephone sampling in epidemiologic research: To reap the benefits, avoid the pitfalls. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1994;139:967–978. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a116946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Rakowski W, Andersen MR, Stoddard AM, Urban N, Rimer BK, Lane DS, et al. Confirmatory analysis of opinions regarding the pros and cons of mammography. Health Psychology. 1997;16:433–441. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.16.5.433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Rakowski W, Clark MA, Pearlman DN, Ehrich B, Rimer BK, Goldstein MG, et al. Integrating pros and cons for mammography and pap testing: Extending the construct of decisional balance to two behaviors. Preventive Medicine. 1997;26:664–673. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1997.0188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Rakowski W, Dube CE, Marcus BH, Prochaska JO, Velicer WF, Abrams DB. Assessing elements of women’s decisions about mammography. Health Psychology. 1992;11:111–118. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.11.2.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Rakowski W, Ehrich B, Dube CE, Pearlman DN. Screening mammography and constructs from the transtheoretical model: Associations using two definitions of the stages-of-adoption. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 1996;18:91–100. doi: 10.1007/BF02909581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Rakowski W, Fulton JP, Feldman JP. Women’s decision making about mammography: A replication of the relationship between stages of adoption and decisional balance. Health Psychology. 1993;12:209–214. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.12.3.209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Sabogal F, Marin G, Otero-Sabogal R, Marin B, Perez-Stable E. Hispanic familism and acculturation: What changes and what doesn’t? Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences. 1987;9:397–412. [Google Scholar]
  50. Sabogal F, Merrill SS, Packel L. Mammography rescreening among older California women. Health Care Financing Review. 2001;22:63. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Sabogal F, Otero-Sabogal R, Pasick RJ, Jenkins CNH, Perez-Stable EJ. Printed health education materials for diverse communities: Suggestions learned from the field. Health Education Quarterly. 1996;23:S123–S141. [Google Scholar]
  52. Skinner CS, Arfken CL, Sykes RK. Knowledge, perceptions, and mammography stage of adoption among older urban women. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 1998;14:54–63. doi: 10.1016/s0749-3797(97)00008-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Somkin C, McPhee S, Nguyen T, Stewart S, Shema SJ, Nguyen B, et al. The effect of access and satisfaction on regular mammogram and Papanicolaou test screening in a multiethnic population. Medical Care. 2003;42(9):914–926. doi: 10.1097/01.mlr.0000135832.28672.61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Stokes M, Davis C. Categorical data analysis using the SAS system. Cary, NC: SAS Institute; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  55. Suarez L, Roche RA, Nichols D, Simpson DM. Knowledge, behavior, and fears concerning breast and cervical cancer among older low-income Mexican-American women. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 1997;13:137–142. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Tang TS, Solomon LJ, McCracken LM. Cultural barriers to mammography, clinical breast exam, and breast self-exam among Chinese-American women 60 and older. Preventive Medicine. 2000;31:575–583. doi: 10.1006/pmed.2000.0753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, Centers for Disease Control & Prevention, & National Institutes of Health. Healthy people 2010: Objectives for improving health: Cancer. Healthy people 2010. 2nd ed. Vol. 1. Washington, DC: Author; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  58. Zambrana RE, Breen N, Fox SA, Gutierrez-Mohamed ML. Use of cancer screening practices by Hispanic women: Analyses by subgroup. Preventive Medicine. 1999;29:466–477. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1999.0566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES