Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: Int Arch Occup Environ Health. 2009 Dec 6;83(7):771–777. doi: 10.1007/s00420-009-0497-3

A Population-Based Study of Blood Lead Levels in Relation to Depression in the United States

Natalia I Golub 1, Paul C Winters 2, Edwin van Wijngaarden 1,3
PMCID: PMC2939966  NIHMSID: NIHMS224941  PMID: 19967393

Abstract

Purpose

Lead is a known neurotoxicant. Several studies have suggested that occupational exposure to lead may lead to depression, anxiety and other psychiatric illness, but few studies have examined environmental lead exposure and depression. We evaluated the relationship between blood lead levels (BLL) and depression in a sample representative of the United States population.

Methods

We analyzed data from 4,159 adults ages ≥20 who participated in the 2005-2006 cycle of the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES). Depression was assessed by the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9). Relative risks were calculated using Poisson regression and odds ratios were calculated with ordinal logistic regression using SUDAAN, controlling for pertinent covariates.

Results

The risk of depression was only slightly elevated with increasing blood lead levels when lead was modeled as a categorical variable, with adjusted relative risks of 1.16 (95% confidence interval (CI) = 0.99-1.36), 1.20 (CI=1.07-1.36) and 1.16 (CI=0.87-1.54) for 0.89-1.40 μg/dL,1.41-2.17 μg /dL, and >2.17 μg/dL, respectively, as compared to 0-0.88 μg/dL using Poisson regression. Similar results were obtained with ordinal logistic regression. Analyses using BLL as a continuous variable did not show a significant relationship with depression.

Conclusions

This cross-sectional study did not provide consistent evidence for an association between environmental lead exposure and depression within the investigated blood lead levels.

Keywords: blood lead levels, depression, NHANES, PHQ-9

Introduction

Depression is a prevalent psychiatric condition in the United States with an estimated 16.2% lifetime prevalence of major depressive disorder (Kessler et al. 2003). Approximately half of all Americans will meet DSM-IV criteria sometime during their lives (Kessler et al. 2005). Family history of mood disorders (Sullivan et al. 2000), age (Kessler et al. 2005), sex, ethnicity, and chronic illness (Clarke and Currie 2009) are known to increase the risk of depression. Furthermore, it has been postulated that exposure to environmental contaminants may play a role in the development of psychiatric disorders (Shih et al. 2007). Lead, a known neurotoxicant with well-documented cognitive and neurobehavioral effects, is one such contaminant that is suspected to be associated with psychiatric disorders (Cory-Slechta et al. 2008).

The mechanisms of how exposure to lead may lead to such disorders are just beginning to be elucidated. Animal studies have demonstrated that lead exposure affects the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and can lead to permanent HPA axis dysfunction (Cory-Slechta et al. 2004; Rossi-George et al. 2009; Virgolini et al. 2005). Cory-Slechta and colleagues proposed that alterations in the HPA axis due to lead exposure result in changes in glucocorticoid and catecholamine levels, with neuropsychiatric disorders such as depression as potential consequences (Cory-Slechta et al. 2008). Human studies have shown that HPA axis dysregulation may lead to elevated CRH and cortisol levels in depression (Holsboer 2000; Vreeburg et al. 2009). In addition, normalization of HPA axis function in previously depressed patients portends a good prognosis, while persistent elevated cortisol and CRH make depression remission less likely and increase the chance of relapse (Holsboer 2000). HPA axis dysregulation involves altered glucocorticoid receptor function that leads to impaired negative feedback and HPA axis overdrive, resulting in increased CRH and cortisol. The increased levels of cortisol lead to a decrease in the ratio of activated mineralocorticoid/glucocorticoid receptors (resulting in memory impairment), attenuated monoaminergic systems in the brain, and hippocampal volume reduction; changes similar to those seen in depression (de Kloet et al. 2005; Holsboer 2000). In addition to the role of elevated cortisol in depression, increased CRH levels and CRH receptors are involved in depression, and CRH receptor antagonists have anti-depressant effects. Furthermore, lead affects levels and metabolism of serotonin (Cory-Slechta et al. 2004; Kala and Jadhav 1995; Pillai et al. 2003; Virgolini et al. 2005), and abnormalities in the serotonergic system are present in depression (Moore and Jefferson 2004). Finally, lead affects the mesocorticolimbic system (Cory-Slechta et al. 2004), aberrances in which have also been linked to depression (Martin-Soelch 2009; Naranjo et al. 2001).

Epidemiological studies addressing the neuropsychiatric effects of lead in general, and depression specifically, have been sparse. Nevertheless, several studies have found that increased blood (Baker et al. 1984; Baker et al. 1983; Hanninen et al. 1998; Hogstedt et al. 1983; Lilis et al. 1977; Lindgren et al. 1999; Maizlish et al. 1995; Rhodes et al. 2003; Sciarillo et al. 1992; Stanley and Wakwe 2002) and bone (Rhodes et al. 2003; Schwartz et al. 2005) lead levels are significantly associated with depression. A cross-sectional study of 99 occupationally exposed individuals and 61 controls found a dose-dependent association between lead exposure (10-80 μg/dl) and increased reports of depression, confusion, anger, tension, and fatigue as measured by Profile of Mood States (POMS) questionnaire, especially in individuals with lead levels above 40 μg/dL (Baker et al. 1984). Maizlish and coworkers (1995) found that current and cumulative measures of blood lead levels in 43 occupationally exposed individuals (mean blood lead 42 μg/dL) and 45 controls (mean blood lead 15 μg/dL) were associated with significantly increased symptoms of anger, depression, and fatigue. Multiple linear regression adjusting for covariates such as age, education, alcohol intake, and medical conditions showed a significant association between blood lead and tension-anxiety, hostility, and depression, as measured by POMS, in a dose-related manner (current, peak, time weighted average for lead). In a longitudinal study of 576 occupationally exposed individuals, there was a significant association between tibia lead levels (mean tibia lead = 8.4 μg/g) and depressive symptoms as measured by the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CES-D) scale. The study did not find a significant association between blood lead levels (mean blood lead 31.4 μg/dL) and depressive symptoms (Schwartz et al. 2005)

The majority of these studies included small samples consisting of workers occupationally exposed to high lead levels. It is not known whether environmental exposure to lead, which leads to lower blood lead levels than in occupationally exposed individuals, may lead to psychiatric illness in adults. We examined the association between depression and current background levels of environmental lead exposure in a cross-sectional study of individuals participating in the 2005-2006 cycle of the continuous National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES).

Methods

Study Population

NHANES is a complex, multi-stage survey of non-institutionalized civilians of the US population that collects information about health, nutrition, demographic, and socioeconomic factors (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) 2009). The survey is conducted by trained physicians, medical and health technicians, and dietary and health interviewers. Data is collected in the form of standardized home interviews and an examination component consisting of a medical, dental, and laboratory test evaluation conducted in Mobile Examination Centers. Approximately 5,000 individuals from 15 counties across the country are surveyed each year. The sample selected for data collection is representative of the US population. Persons over 60, African-Americans, and Hispanic individuals are over-sampled. For this study, adults ages 20 and over (Harris et al. 2009) from NHANES 2005-2006 were selected.

A total of 10,348 individuals participated in the 2005-2006 NHANES cycle, including 4,979 individuals ages 20 and over. Of those, 4,321 individuals answered the questionnaire addressing depression and 4,159 of those had reported blood lead levels. Thus, 4,159 individuals remained for our analyses.

Blood Lead Measurements

Blood lead levels (BLL) were obtained from individuals 1 year and older (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) 2009). Non-fasting blood samples (minimum of .25 mL/vial) were collected by venipuncture in pre-screened polyethylene vials and pre-screened vacutainers. Blood samples were transported and stored at ≤ 4°C. Samples were sent to Division of Laboratory Sciences, National Center for Environmental Health, and Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Once received, they were frozen at ≤ −20°C until analysis. For analysis, blood samples were diluted with 18 mega-ohm water and with diluent, containing 1% v/v tetramethylammonium hydroxide (TMAH), 0.5% disodium ethylenediamine tetraacetate (EDTA), 10% ethyl alcohol, 0.05% Triton X-100, and bismuth was added for standardization. Whole blood lead concentrations were determined using inductively coupled plasma mass spectrometry (PerkinElmer ELAN 6100 ICP-DRC-MS Plus System). Two lower detection limits of 0.25 μg/dL and 0.30 μg/dL were reported. When the value was below the detection limit, the original NHANES investigators divided the detection limit by square root of two as an estimate of the BLL.

Depression Assessment

The Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9) was included as part of the computer assisted personal interview (CAPI) in the mobile exam center (MEC) (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) 2009). PHQ-9 is a version of the Prime-MD diagnostic instrument for common mental disorders. It consists of nine questions based on DSM-IV symptoms of depression. Each question is scored ranging from 0-3, with 0 as having a symptom “not at all”, and 3 being “nearly every day”. A score out of 27 is tabulated based on the answers to the nine questions, with 0-4 representing no depression, 5-9 mild depression, 10-14 moderate depression, 15-19 moderately severe depression, 20-27 severe depression. PHQ-9 is commonly used as a screening method for depression in primary care settings and it is validated as an effective tool in diagnosis of depression, with a PHQ-9 score ≥ 10 having an 88% sensitivity and specificity for depression (Kroenke et al. 2001).

Potential Confounders

Data on all covariates was collected by an interviewer during the home interview portion of NHANES 2005-2006 using the Sample Person and Family Demographics questionnaires (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) 2009). The following covariates were evaluated: age (continuous), sex (male as reference), education level (<high school, high school, >high school (reference)), ethnicity (Mexican American, Other Hispanic, Non-Hispanic White (reference), Non-Hispanic Black, Other Race - Including Multi-Racial), and poverty income ratio (PIR; ratio of family income to poverty threshold, with a PIR value less than 1 indicating an income below the poverty threshold and a PIR greater than 1 indicating an income above the poverty threshold). No subjects had missing data for age, gender, and ethnicity, but 3,991 individuals (96% of our eligible sample) remained in the analysis after considering PIR and education.

Statistical Analyses

We used SAS 9.1 and SAS-callable SUDAAN 10, applying NHANES medical examination weights for all analyses. Initially we computed descriptive statistics for our study population, including proportions and means. Estimates of average BLLs and depression prevalence (as defined by PHQ-9 score of >4) were obtained. Subsequently we estimated prevalence ratios using Poisson regression analyses with the LOGLINK function in SUDAAN. In these analyses, depression was a dichotomous variable (PHQ-9 score 0-4: not depressed; score 5+: depressed) and BLL was examined as a continuous variable as well as a categorical variable based on quartiles of the distribution of BLL in the population (Q1: 0-0.88 μg/dL, Q2: 0.89-1.40 μg /dL, Q3: 1.41-2.17 μg /dL, Q4: >2.17 μg/dL). Additionally, odds ratios were estimated using ordinal logistic regression with the MULTILOG function in SUDAAN, with depression defined as an ordinal variable (PHQ-9 score 0-4: not depressed; score 5-9: mildly depressed; and score 10-27: moderately/severely depressed) and BLL again defined as a continuous and categorical variable (see above). The proportionality assumption of the ordinal logistic regression model was assessed via graphical methods (Scott et al. 1997). Analyses were performed with and without covariates to assess the extent of confounding.

Results

Table 1 displays characteristics of the study population by covariates. The sample population was majority non-Hispanic White at 73.27%, 58.11% received an education beyond high school, 51.59% were female, average age was 46.50, and the average PIR was 3.13. Mean blood lead in this population was 1.75 μg/dL. Individuals with depression (defined as PHQ-9 score >4, anyone with mild to severe depression) had a mean blood lead of 1.73 μg/dL while individuals who were not depressed had a mean lead of 1.75 μg/dL. The prevalence of depression was 20.09%.

Table 1.

Characteristics of Study Population: Individuals 20 years and older in NHANES 2005-2006

Characteristic Overall Depressed Not depressed
Percentage
Total 100.00 20.09 79.91
Gender
 Male 48.41 16.30 83.70
 Female 51.59 23.65 76.35
Ethnicity
 Non-Hispanic White 73.27 19.02 80.98
 Non-Hispanic Black 10.85 22.71 77.29
 Mexican American 7.76 20.14 79.86
 Other Hispanic 3.24 28.60 71.40
 Other Race 4.88 24.56 75.44
Education Level
 Less than high school 16.98 27.03 72.97
 High school 24.90 22.89 77.11
 More than high school 58.11 16.87 83.13
Mean (SE)
Lead (μg/dL) 1.75 (0.05) 1.73 (0.04) 1.75 (0.05)
Poverty Income Ratio (PIR) 3.13 (0.07) 2.64 (0.07) 3.26 (0.07)
Age 46.50 (0.73) 45.79 (0.90) 46.48 (0.77)

for depressed and non-depressed: % of participants in subgroup

Table 2 shows the results of Poisson regression analyses for BLL as a categorical and continuous variable and PHQ-9 score as a dichotomous variable (PHQ-9 score 0-4: not depressed; 5+: depressed). The percent weighted positive for depression ranged from 19.43%-20.19% across the BLL categories. Results of unadjusted analyses showed no increased risk of depression with increasing BLL. There was no significant effect of lead on depression when lead was modeled as continuous variable after controlling for age, gender and ethnicity. However, BLL modeled as a categorical variable showed significantly increased risk of depression with increasing quartile of lead, from 1.00 to 1.29 (p=.0148). Similarly, after additional adjustment for PIR, and education, BLL as a continuous variable did not increase risk of depression. BLL as a categorical variable continued to show a significantly increased risk of depression (p=.0185), but there was no clear trend in risk of depression with increasing BLLs.

Table 2.

Blood lead levels in relation to depression prevalence, Poisson regression: NHANES 2005-2006

% depressed Crude RR Adjusted RR I Adjusted RR II
Continuous lead 20.02 0.99(.94-1.04) 1.04 (0.98-1.10) 1.01 (0.96-1.07)
Categorical lead
 0-0.88 μg/dL 20.19 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
 0.89-1.40 μg/dL 20.53 1.02 (0.85-1.22) 1.17 (0.98-1.40) 1.16 (.99-1.36)
 1.41-2.17 μg/dL 19.93 1.00(0.90-1.11) 1.24 (1.09-1.41) 1.20 (1.07-1.36)
 2.18-26.4 μg/dL 19.43 0.97 (0.76-1.24) 1.29 (0.94-.77) 1.16 (.87-1.54)

adjusted for age, gender, and ethnicity (n=4,159)

adjusted for age, gender, ethnicity, education, and PIR (n=3,991)

Table 3 displays the results of ordinal logistic regression analyses for blood lead as a categorical and continuous variable and PHQ-9 score as an ordinal variable (PHQ-9 score 0-4: not depressed; 5-9: mildly depressed; and 10-27: moderately/severely depressed). The percentage of individuals with no depression was approximately 80%, mild depression 14% and moderate/severe depression 6%. Results were very similar to those attained with Poisson regression, with lead as a continuous variable showing no effect on depression (odds ratios ranging from .99-1.05) with and without covariates, and BLL as a categorical variable showing significantly increased risk for depression when age, sex, and gender were considered (p=.0210), with odds ratios of 1.00-1.36. However, after additional adjustment for PIR and education, BLL as a categorical variable showed no clear trend of increased risk of depression with increasing BLLs.

Table 3.

Blood lead levels in relation to depression prevalence, Ordinal logistic regression: NHANES 2005-2006

% no; mild; and severe depression Crude OR Adjusted OR I Adjusted OR II
Continuous lead 79.98; 14.36; 5.66 0.99 (0.92-1.06) 1.05 (0.97-1.14) 1.01 (0.94-1.09)
Categorical lead
 0-0.88 μg/dL 79.81; 14.39; 5.80 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
 0.89-1.40 μg/dL 79.47; 14.66; 5.86 1.03 (0.82-1.28) 1.22 (0.97-1.53) 1.22 (0.98-1.51)
 1.41-2.17 μg/dL 80.07; 14.80; 5.13 0.99 (0.87-1.12) 1.29 (1.10-1.52) 1.25 (1.07-1.47)
 2.18-26.4 μg/dL 80.57; 13.58; 5.85 0.97 (0.72-1.30) 1.36 (0.92-2.02) 1.18 (0.83-1.68)

adjusted for age, gender, and ethnicity (n=4,159)

adjusted for age, gender, ethnicity, education, and PIR (n=3,991)

Discussion

Lead levels have decreased drastically in the US over the past few decades and continue to decline (Muntner et al. 2005; Pirkle et al. 1994). However, lead accumulates in bone over time, and is then released into blood (Rabinowitz 1991). Thus, individuals that were previously exposed to higher lead levels have higher bone lead levels and have lead chronically released into the blood stream. This effect worsens with age as osteoporotic changes occur and more lead is released into the blood stream. In addition, while lead levels in the general US population have declined drastically, there are continuing problems with high lead exposure and lead poisoning in children living in old housing, especially minority children in urban settings (Pirkle et al. 1998). Furthermore, studies in humans have demonstrated that lead levels below 10 μg/dL, CDC's action level for children (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) 1991), have detrimental effects on cognitive and behavioral functioning (Canfield et al. 2003; Lanphear et al. 2005; Surkan et al. 2007). Similarly, there appears to be no known safe level of lead in environmentally exposed adults with demonstrated effects on oxidative stress, inflammation, hypertension and cardiovascular disease even at current low BLLs (Iavicoli et al. 2006; Lee et al. 2006; Menke et al. 2006; Navas-Acien et al. 2007; Vaziri 2008; Vaziri and Gonick 2008).

A recent study addressed environmental lead exposure and neuropsychiatric symptoms by assessing for anxiety, phobic anxiety, and depression in men enrolled in the Normative Aging Study using the Brief Symptom inventory and a combined outcome measure (anxiety, phobic anxiety, depression) (Rhodes et al. 2003). Tibia (mean = 21.9 μg/g), patella (mean = 32.1 μg/g), and blood lead (mean = 6.3 μg/dL) were measured. Logistic regression models showed that all measures of lead were significantly associated with increased risk of the combined outcome measure, and patella lead was significantly associated with increased risk of phobic anxiety. These results appear to corroborate studies demonstrating an association between lead exposure and psychiatric disorders.

In contrast, while our study found a statistically significant association between BLL and depression when exposure was modeled as a categorical variable and only age, gender and sex were considered, the effect was small with a relative risk around 1.3. In addition, when education level and PIR were added to the model, there were no clear trends of increasing risk of depression with increasing BLLs. These findings underline the importance of considering the effects of socio-economic measures such as education and PIR in the investigation of lead effects on health. It is possible that PIR and education are confounders in the effect of lead on depression, whereby individuals who have higher lead levels also have lower education levels and lower PIR, leading to depression. Alternatively, the effects of lead on depression may be mediated by socioeconomic factors because early lead exposure has been shown to result in behavioral/developmental problems which may in turn lead to lower educational achievement and lower PIR, potential risk factors for depression (Bjelland et al. 2008; Mezuk et al. 2008; Samaan 2000; Yen and Kaplan 1999). Finally, there is a possibility of a combination of confounding and mediation effect of socioeconomic factors on the relationship between lead exposure and depression.

The limitations of this study must be considered when interpreting our findings. In this cross-sectional study BLL and depression were measured at the same point in time. Therefore, there is no consideration of past lead exposure and past BLLs on the risk of depression. Blood lead levels reflect primarily recent environmental exposures and the mobilization of lead from the skeleton back into the circulation (Hu et al. 2007). Bone lead levels have been shown to accurately reflect accumulated exposure (Gerhardsson et al. 1993; Hu et al. 1998; Hu et al. 2007), which may be more relevant to the etiology of depression. Longitudinal studies are needed to fully assess the relationship between lead exposure and depression, including the potentially permanent changes in HPA axis due to early life lead exposure and consequent development of depression later in life.

In addition, the average BLL of individuals in the study was 1.75 μg/dL, which may be too low to detect an existing effect of current BLLs on depression, considering that the occupational exposure limit for blood lead is 40 μg/dL (Occupational Safety and Health Administration (OSHA) 2008). We were not able to account for other potentially important covariates, for example, genetic factors affecting the risk of depression such as polymorphisms in the 5-HT transporter and the glucocorticoid receptor (de Kloet et al. 2005). Also, although the PHQ-9 is an established, validated tool to assess for presence and severity of depression, its sensitivity and specificity of 88% will result in some people receiving a different diagnosis as compared to examination by a mental health professional (Kroenke et al. 2001).

It is clear that HPA axis dysregulation is associated with depression (de Kloet et al. 2005; Holsboer 2000; Vreeburg et al. 2009), and animal studies have demonstrated that lead can cause HPA axis dysregulation (Cory-Slechta et al. 2008; Cory-Slechta et al. 2004; Rossi-George et al. 2009; Virgolini et al. 2005). This study could not assess the relationship between BLLs and HPA axis dysfunction in humans as cortisol levels were not available. Despite these limitations, the study is a nationally representative sample of the US population, and NHANES employs rigorous standardized methods for data collection, generating high quality data.

Conclusion

This study did not demonstrate a consistent association between environmental lead exposure and depression within the investigated blood lead levels. Longitudinal studies will be necessary to more fully examine the effect of environmental lead exposure on depression and other neuropsychiatric outcomes, including measures of HPA axis function to help elucidate potential biological mechanisms.

Acknowledgments

This publication was made possible by Grant Number UL1 RR024160 from the National Center for Research Resources (NCRR), a component of the National Institutes of Health (NIH), and the NIH Roadmap for Medical Research. Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official view of NCRR or NIH. Information on NCRR is available at http://www.ncrr.nih.gov/. Information on Re-engineering the Clinical Research Enterprise can be obtained from http://nihroadmap.nih.gov/clinicalresearch/overview-translational.asp.

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest: The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Baker EL, Feldman RG, White RA, et al. Occupational lead neurotoxicity: a behavioural and electrophysiological evaluation. Study design and year one results. Br J Ind Med. 1984;41:352–361. doi: 10.1136/oem.41.3.352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baker EL, Feldman RG, White RF, et al. The role of occupational lead exposure in the genesis of psychiatric and behavioral disturbances. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1983;303:38–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb00940.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bjelland I, Krokstad S, Mykletun A, et al. Does a higher educational level protect against anxiety and depression? The HUNT study. Soc Sci Med. 2008;66:1334–45. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.12.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Canfield RL, Henderson CR, Jr, Cory-Slechta DA, et al. Intellectual impairment in children with blood lead concentrations below 10 microg per deciliter. N Engl J Med. 2003;348:1517–26. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa022848. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Centers for Disease Control and Preventio (CDC) Preventing Lead Poisoning in Young Children. A Statement by the Centers for Disease Control. Vol. 2009. Atlanta GA: 1991. [Google Scholar]
  6. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Vol. 2009. National Center for Health Statistics; Hyattsville MD: 2009. [Google Scholar]
  7. Clarke DM, Currie KC. Depression, anxiety and their relationship with chronic diseases: a review of the epidemiology, risk and treatment evidence. Med J Aust. 2009;190:S54–60. doi: 10.5694/j.1326-5377.2009.tb02471.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cory-Slechta DA, Virgolini MB, Rossi-George A, et al. Lifetime consequences of combined maternal lead and stress. Basic Clin Pharmacol Toxicol. 2008;102:218–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1742-7843.2007.00189.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cory-Slechta DA, Virgolini MB, Thiruchelvam M, et al. Maternal stress modulates the effects of developmental lead exposure. Environ Health Perspect. 2004;112:717–30. doi: 10.1289/ehp.6481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. de Kloet ER, Joels M, Holsboer F. Stress and the brain: from adaptation to disease. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:463–75. doi: 10.1038/nrn1683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Gerhardsson L, Attewell R, Chettle DR, et al. In vivo measurements of lead in bone in long-term exposed lead smelter workers. Arch Environ Health. 1993;48:147–56. doi: 10.1080/00039896.1993.9940813. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Hanninen H, Aitio A, Kovala T, et al. Occupational exposure to lead and neuropsychological dysfunction. Occup Environ Med. 1998;55:202–9. doi: 10.1136/oem.55.3.202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Harris KM, Perreira KM, Lee D. Obesity in the transition to adulthood: predictions across race/ethnicity, immigrant generation, and sex. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163:1022–8. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hogstedt C, Hane M, Agrell A, et al. Neuropsychological test results and symptoms among workers with well-defined long-term exposure to lead. Br J Ind Med. 1983;40:99–105. doi: 10.1136/oem.40.1.99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Holsboer F. The corticosteroid receptor hypothesis of depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;23:477–501. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00159-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hu H, Rabinowitz M, Smith D. Bone lead as a biological marker in epidemiologic studies of chronic toxicity: conceptual paradigms. Environ Health Perspect. 1998;106:1–8. doi: 10.1289/ehp.981061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hu H, Shih R, Rothenberg S, et al. The epidemiology of lead toxicity in adults: measuring dose and consideration of other methodologic issues. Environ Health Perspect. 2007;115:455–62. doi: 10.1289/ehp.9783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Iavicoli I, Carelli G, Stanek EJ, 3rd, et al. Below background levels of blood lead impact cytokine levels in male and female mice. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 2006;210:94–9. doi: 10.1016/j.taap.2005.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kala SV, Jadhav AL. Region-specific alterations in dopamine and serotonin metabolism in brains of rats exposed to low levels of lead. Neurotoxicology. 1995;16:297–308. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, et al. The epidemiology of major depressive disorder: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R) JAMA. 2003;289:3095–3105. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.23.3095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, et al. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:593–602. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB. The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Intern Med. 2001;16:606–13. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Lanphear BP, Hornung R, Khoury J, et al. Low-level environmental lead exposure and children's intellectual function: an international pooled analysis. Environ Health Perspect. 2005;113:894–9. doi: 10.1289/ehp.7688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Lee DH, Lim JS, Song K, et al. Graded associations of blood lead and urinary cadmium concentrations with oxidative-stress-related markers in the U.S. population: results from the third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Environ Health Perspect. 2006;114:350–4. doi: 10.1289/ehp.8518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Lilis R, Blumberg WE, Fischbein A, et al. Lead effects among secondary lead smelter workers with blood lead levels below 80 microgram/100 ml. Arch Environ Health. 1977;32:256–266. doi: 10.1080/00039896.1977.10667292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Lindgren KN, Masten VL, Tiburzi MJ, et al. The factor structure of the Profile of Mood States (POMS) and its relationship to occupational lead exposure. J Occup Environ Med. 1999;41:3–10. doi: 10.1097/00043764-199901000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Maizlish NA, Parra G, Feo O. Neurobehavioural evaluation of Venezuelan workers exposed to inorganic lead. Occup Environ Med. 1995;52:408–14. doi: 10.1136/oem.52.6.408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Martin-Soelch C. Is depression associated with dysfunction of the central reward system? Biochem Soc Trans. 2009;37:313–7. doi: 10.1042/BST0370313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Menke A, Muntner P, Batuman V, et al. Blood lead below 0.48 micromol/L (10 microg/dL) and mortality among US adults. Circulation. 2006;114:1388–94. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.106.628321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Mezuk B, Eaton WW, Golden SH, et al. The influence of educational attainment on depression and risk of type 2 diabetes. Am J Public Health. 2008;98:1480–5. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2007.126441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Moore DP, Jefferson JW. Handbook of Medical Psychiatry. Mosby Inc.; Philadelphia PA: 2004. Major Depressive Disorder (DSM-IV-TR #296.2–296.3. [Google Scholar]
  32. Muntner P, Menke A, DeSalvo KB, et al. Continued decline in blood lead levels among adults in the United States: the National Health and Nutrition Examination Surveys. Arch Intern Med. 2005;165:2155–61. doi: 10.1001/archinte.165.18.2155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Naranjo CA, Tremblay LK, Busto UE. The role of the brain reward system in depression. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2001;25:781–823. doi: 10.1016/s0278-5846(01)00156-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Navas-Acien A, Guallar E, Silbergeld EK, et al. Lead exposure and cardiovascular disease--a systematic review. Environ Health Perspect. 2007;115:472–82. doi: 10.1289/ehp.9785. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Occupational Safety and Health Administration (OSHA) Lead - 1910.1025. Washington, DC: 2008. [Google Scholar]
  36. Pillai A, Priya L, Gupta S. Effects of combined exposure to lead and cadmium on the hypothalamic-pituitary axis function in proestrous rats. Food Chem Toxicol. 2003;41:379–84. doi: 10.1016/s0278-6915(02)00247-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Pirkle JL, Brody DJ, Gunter EW, et al. The decline in blood lead levels in the United States. The National Health and Nutrition Examination Surveys (NHANES) JAMA. 1994;272:284–91. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Pirkle JL, Kaufmann RB, Brody DJ, et al. Exposure of the U.S. population to lead, 1991-1994. Environ Health Perspect. 1998;106:745–50. doi: 10.1289/ehp.98106745. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Rabinowitz MB. Toxicokinetics of Bone Lead. Environ Health Perspect. 1991;91:33–37. doi: 10.1289/ehp.919133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Rhodes D, Spiro A, 3rd, Aro A, et al. Relationship of bone and blood lead levels to psychiatric symptoms: the normative aging study. J Occup Environ Med. 2003;45:1144–51. doi: 10.1097/01.jom.0000094995.23808.7b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Rossi-George A, Virgolini MB, Weston D, et al. Alterations in glucocorticoid negative feedback following maternal Pb, prenatal stress and the combination: a potential biological unifying mechanism for their corresponding disease profiles. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 2009;234:117–27. doi: 10.1016/j.taap.2008.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Samaan RA. The influences of race, ethnicity, and poverty on the mental health of children. J Health Care Poor Underserved. 2000;11:100–10. doi: 10.1353/hpu.2010.0557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Schwartz BS, Lee BK, Bandeen-Roche K, et al. Occupational lead exposure and longitudinal decline in neurobehavioral test scores. Epidemiology. 2005;16:106–13. doi: 10.1097/01.ede.0000147109.62324.51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Sciarillo WG, Alexander G, Farrell KP. Lead exposure and child behavior. Am J Public Health. 1992;82:1356–1360. doi: 10.2105/ajph.82.10.1356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Scott SC, Goldberg MS, Mayo NE. Statistical assessment of ordinal outcomes in comparative studies. J Clin Epidemiol. 1997;50:45–55. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(96)00312-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Shih RA, Hu H, Weisskopf MG, et al. Cumulative lead dose and cognitive function in adults: a review of studies that measured both blood lead and bone lead. Environ Health Perspect. 2007;115:483–492. doi: 10.1289/ehp.9786. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Stanley PC, Wakwe VC. Toxic trace metals in the mentally ill patients. Niger Postgrad Med J. 2002;9:199–204. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Sullivan PF, Neale MC, Kendler KS. Genetic epidemiology of major depression: review and meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:1552–62. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.10.1552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Surkan PJ, Zhang A, Trachtenberg F, et al. Neuropsychological function in children with blood lead levels &lt;10 microg/dL. Neurotoxicology. 2007;28:1170–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2007.07.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Vaziri ND. Mechanisms of lead-induced hypertension and cardiovascular disease. Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol. 2008;295:H454–65. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.00158.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Vaziri ND, Gonick HC. Cardiovascular effects of lead exposure. Indian J Med Res. 2008;128:426–35. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Virgolini MB, Chen K, Weston DD, et al. Interactions of chronic lead exposure and intermittent stress: consequences for brain catecholamine systems and associated behaviors and HPA axis function. Toxicol Sci. 2005;87:469–82. doi: 10.1093/toxsci/kfi269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Vreeburg SA, Hoogendijk WJ, van Pelt J, et al. Major depressive disorder and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity: results from a large cohort study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:617–26. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Yen IH, Kaplan GA. Poverty area residence and changes in depression and perceived health status: evidence from the Alameda County Study. Int J Epidemiol. 1999;28:90–4. doi: 10.1093/ije/28.1.90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES