Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Consult Clin Psychol. 2010 Oct;78(5):705–716. doi: 10.1037/a0020508

A randomized clinical trial of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy vs. Progressive Relaxation Training for obsessive compulsive disorder

Michael P Twohig 1, Steven C Hayes 2, Jennifer C Plumb 3, Larry D Pruitt 4, Angela B Collins 5, Holly Hazlett-Stevens 6, Michelle R Woidneck 7
PMCID: PMC2948415  NIHMSID: NIHMS220290  PMID: 20873905

Abstract

Objective

Effective treatments for adult OCD exist, but refusal and drop-out rates are high and treatments are not effective for all individuals who complete them. Thus, additional treatment options are needed. This study investigated the effectiveness of eight sessions of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) for adult obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) compared to Progressive Relaxation Training (PRT).

Method

79 adults (66% Female) diagnosed with OCD (M age=37, 89% Caucasian) participated in a randomized clinical trial of 8 sessions of ACT or PRT with no in-session exposure. The following assessments were completed at pretreatment, posttreatment, and three month follow-up by an assessor who was unaware of treatment conditions: Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS), Beck Depression Inventory-II, Quality of Life Scale, Acceptance and Action Questionnaire, Thought Action Fusion Scale, and Thought Control Questionnaire. The Treatment Evaluation Inventory-Short Form was completed at posttreatment.

Results

ACT produced greater changes at posttreatment and follow-up over PRT on OCD severity (Y-BOCS: ACT pre=24.22, post=12.76, follow-up=11.79; PRT pre=25.4, post=18.67, follow-up=16.23), and produced greater change on depression among those reporting at least mild depression before treatment. Clinically significant change in OCD severity occurred more in the ACT condition than PRT using multiple criteria (clinical response rates: ACT post=46-56%, follow-up 46-66%; PRT post=13%-18%, follow-up 16-18%). Quality of life improved in both conditions but was marginally in favor of ACT at post. Finally, treatment refusal (2.4% ACT, 7.8% PRT) and drop-out (9.8% ACT, 13.2% PRT) were low in both conditions.

Conclusions

ACT is worth exploring further as a treatment for OCD.

Keywords: acceptance and commitment therapy, progressive relaxation training, obsessive compulsive disorder, treatment, experiential avoidance, psychological flexibility

A randomized clinical trial of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy vs. Progressive Relaxation Training for obsessive compulsive disorder

There is a need to develop new approaches to the treatment of obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD). Exposure with ritual prevention (ERP) and ERP with cognitive procedures (ERP/CT) are recognized as well-supported treatment methods that have a clinically significant impact on the majority of patients (Abramowitz, Taylor, & MacKay, 2009; Fisher & Wells, 2005), but they raise various issues beyond clinical impact for those accessing care. ERP suffers from a drop-out rate of approximately 25% (Abramowitz et al., 2009) and overt refusal rates, while often not reported in the literature, represent an additional 5 to 22% of participants (e.g., Foa et al., 2005; McLean et al., 2001). ERP and ERP/CT are intensive treatments. The ERP studies covered in a recent review (Abramowitz et al., 2009) averaged 27.4 hours of direct therapist contact in addition to dozens of hours of client homework. ERP is also grossly underutilized (Olatunji, Deacon, & Abramowitz, 2009) perhaps in part because clinicians find it aversive (Richard & Gloster, 2007) and the public rates it negatively when the methods are described (Richard & Gloster, 2007). Finally, OCD tends to be a complex disorder with the majority of patients also having comorbid unipolar mood disorders, other anxiety disorders, or substance use disorders (Abramowitz et al., 2009) suggesting that narrowly focused treatments might require augmentation to deal with all presenting clinical syndromes.

Merely the act of exposing oneself to a feared situation and staying in that situation for a meaningful period of time is generally therapeutic (Abramowitz, 1996), but there is no consensus about the psychological processes that account for this benefit (Steketee & Barlow, 2002). Contemporary animal research suggests that extinction works by fostering new behaviors and new learning rather than unlearning (e.g., Bouton, 2002), which is consistent with the idea that exposure may work in part by fostering greater flexibility in responding in the presence of previously repertoire narrowing stimuli such as anxiety, obsessive thoughts, or physical sensations. Arch and Craske (2008) for example, postulate that exposure involves “optimizing learning .... based on increasing tolerance for fear and anxiety” (p. 269). If that is a process of importance, however, there may be additional methods of fostering response flexibility without exposure in a formal, procedural sense. This in turn might help avoid the dropout and refusal problems that are commonly experienced with traditional exposure methods (cf., Levitt, Brown, Orsillo, Barlow, 2004), and may increase the overall efficiency of treatment.

From its earliest explication as a treatment for anxiety disorders, Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) (Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999) has been said to be a method that is exposure-based in the sense that it “gives people an opportunity to practice experiencing anxiety without also struggling with anxiety” (Hayes, 1987; p. 365). In ACT theory, this approach comes under the general term “psychological flexibility” (Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006): engaging in positive behaviors rather than attempting to avoid difficult experiences and emotions. A lack of psychological flexibility is associated with OCD symptom levels in adults and children (Abramowitz, Lackey, & Wheaton, 2009; Briggs & Price, 2009). ACT seeks to establish psychological flexibility by targeting six major processes: acceptance (willingness to contact inner experiences), defusion (experiencing cognition as an ongoing process rather than allowing cognition to overly regulate behavior), self as context (experiencing oneself as the context in which inner experiences occur, rather than being defined by the content of experiences), being present (being able to flexibly attend to inner and outer events as they occur, non-judgmentally), values (choosing desired consequences of on-going patterns of behavior so as to establish reinforcers in the present), and committed action (flexibly moving in a valued direction). Component studies of these processes in isolation (outside of the entire treatment package) support their utility in a variety of contexts (e.g., Levitt et al., 2004; Masuda, Hayes, Sackett, & Twohig, 2004). A number of studies show that ACT impacts psychological flexibility and produces positive outcomes when psychological flexibility increases (e.g., Hayes et al., 2006).

It seems possible that ACT could be helpful in accomplishing these goals with OCD. There is evidence for the effectiveness of ACT in the treatment of OCD-spectrum disorders such as skin picking and trichotillomania (Twohig & Woods, 2004; Twohig, Hayes, & Masuda, 2006a; Woods, Wetterneck, & Flessner, 2006). In a multiple baseline with four adults diagnosed with OCD (Twohig, Hayes, & Masuda, 2006b), eight weekly one-hour sessions of ACT without in-session exposure had a significant impact on OCD severity, depression, and anxiety. OCD severity scores moved from the clinical to the nonclinical range and results showed near zero levels of compulsions by the end of treatment, with results maintained at follow-up. ACT processes were functionally related to outcomes. To date, however, there is no published randomized controlled trial on ACT for OCD.

This study tests if ACT for OCD can alter psychological flexibility and effectively reduce OCD. While formal in-session exposure procedures fit within an ACT approach to OCD, they were not included in this investigation in order to determine if ACT procedures were effective without these methods of known impact. Outside of treatment sessions, clients were not asked to confront feared stimuli and stay in their presence until a reduction in fear occurred, but instead were asked to practice ACT skills when they encountered feared stimuli as part of everyday functioning. Because the focus of the present study was on acceptability, efficiency, and applicability, the protocol tested was deliberately short, utilizing eight one-hour sessions, and included all subtypes of OCD. Very few additional exclusion criteria were applied: only individuals with active psychosis or a cognitive deficit that would make it difficult to participate (e.g., Autism) were excluded. Finally, given the limited data on the effectiveness of ACT for OCD, as a first step in establishing efficacy, it seemed prudent to compare ACT to a credible comparison condition that controls for many important factors such as contact with therapist, expectation of change, effects of measurement, passage of time, as well as other nonspecific factors. Progressive relaxation training was chosen given its successful use as a credible control condition in previous OCD outcome work (e.g., Fals-Stewart, Marcks, & Schafer, 1993), and given its moderate effectiveness as a treatment for other anxiety disorders (Manzoni, Pagnini, Castelnuovo, & Molinari, 2008).

Method

Participants

Participants were recruited in two Western US states through postings, announcements, advertisements, and referrals from health professionals from 2006 through 2009. In order to be included in the study, participants needed to meet criteria for OCD on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 2002) and be 18 years of age or older. Participants could not have initiated new psychotropic medications or changed dosages within 30 days of beginning the study; be receiving psychotherapy elsewhere or have ceased psychotherapy less than 30 days prior to enrolling in the study; or be diagnosed with a current psychotic disorder or have an organic mental disorder that would severely interfere with their ability to participate.

A participant flow chart is shown in Figure 1. Two hundred twenty two individuals contacted the researchers with questions, 116 of whom did not participate in the study. Of the 116 who did not participate, 56 (49%) did not qualify for the study during the initial phone screening (e.g., called to learn about OCD, called for someone else, were too young to participate, or called regarding symptoms for a related disorder such as trichotillomania) and 60 (51%) were invited to participate in the intake to determine their formal eligibility for the study but did not do so (e.g., they did not attend and were unable to be contacted, said that they wished to seek psychotropic medications or desired to change them, were not interested in psychotherapy, or lived more than 2 hours away from the study location and did not wish to commute). Of the 106 who participated in the intake sessions, 80 met criteria to participate in the investigation, and 79 agreed to participate after qualifying. The one eligible participant who refused participation (before random assignment) cited scheduling problems as the reason. Treatment assignment was made by a random sequence of concealed slips, opened following the agreement to participate by a person other than the assessors (41 ACT, 38 PRT). No participants overtly refused participation after random assignment. An n of 38 per condition was required for power of .8 to detect the average between group effect size (d = .66) in the existing ACT literature across all diagnoses (Hayes et al., 2006). All randomized participants were included in the main analyses. The rationale for the assigned treatment modality was covered extensively in the first treatment session. Participants were not informed of the details of the alternate treatment condition until the end of follow-up, at which time they were given the option to engage in eight sessions of the treatment they did not receive.

Demographic and diagnostic characteristics of the sample were assessed via a self-report questionnaire and the SCID. There were slightly more women in the study (61%); average age of participants was 37 years (SD=15.5, range 18-67). The sample was 88.6% Caucasian (1% African American, 2.5 % Asian American, 5% Latino/a, and 2.5% Native American). The average educational level was 14.9 years (SD=2.0) of schooling with first grade being the first year and senior year of high school being the 12th. Thirty-three percent of the sample reported being married and 50% reported being unmarried, with 11% being divorced, 1.3 % being separated, and 2.5% being widowed. Participants reported experiencing OCD symptoms for an average of 20.5 years (SD=17.1, range 1-62 years), and that their OCD symptomatology had been a significant problem for 14.5 years (SD=14.0, range 1-57 years). Sixty-three percent of the participants had received previous formal treatment (psychotropic medications or psychotherapy) for OCD. At intake, 40% were taking psychotropic medications (34% in ACT; 44% in PRT). The frequencies of psychotropic medications per condition were as follows: antidepressants 14ACT/16PRT, benzodiazepines 4ACT/2PRT, atypical antipsychotics 2ACT/3PRT, anticonvulsants 1ACT/1PRT, and psychostimulants 1ACT/2PRT. The frequencies of the total number of psychotropic medications per individual per condition were as follows: one medication 8ACT/11PRT, two medications 4ACT/5PRT, three medications 2ACT/0PRT, and four medications 0ACT/1PRT. There were no statistically significant differences between treatment conditions on any of these demographic factors.

Based on the SCID, all participants met criteria for OCD as their primary diagnosis with 51% receiving one or greater additional diagnoses. The frequency of co-occurring diagnosis per condition were as follows: mood disorders 12ACT/12PRT, anxiety disorder besides OCD 9ACT/13PRT, substance dependence 2ACT/2PRT, one participant met criteria for bulimia nervosa and one for hypochondriasis in the PRT condition, and one met criteria for adjustment disorder in ACT. The frequencies of total number of co-occurring diagnoses per individual per condition were as follows: one 14ACT/7PRT, two 4ACT/8PRT, and three 1ACT/2PRT.

OCD subtypes for the sample were assessed via the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS) (Goodman et al., 1989). Only 25% of the participants presented with one subtype of OCD; the remaining 75% presented with two or greater subtypes. It is not possible to rate severity of subtypes from these data. Counting no more than the three highest rated OCD subtypes for each person from the Y-BOCS, 19ACT/18PRT presented with contamination, 27ACT/15PRT presented with symmetry/exactness, 5ACT/4PRT presented with safety/harm, 18ACT/16PRT presented with doubt/checking, 12ACT/8PRT presented with religious/sexual/scrupulosity, and 9ACT/16PRT presented with hoarding. Based on a Fisher's exact test none of these differences in co-occurring disorders or sub-types of OCD were significantly different (p < .05) between conditions except that those in the ACT condition had more concern with symmetry/exactness (p = .03).

Therapists, Assessors, and Setting

The primary supervisors were Steven C. Hayes (SCH), Holly Hazlett-Stevens (HHS), and Michael P. Twohig (MPT). Each has written books relevant to areas of this study: ACT (Hayes et al., 1999), relaxation training (Bernstein, Borkovec, & Hazlett-Stevens, 2000), and acceptance – based approaches to OCD spectrum disorders (Woods & Twohig, 2008). All therapists (N: ACT=2; PRT = 4) and assessors (N=4) were advanced graduate students in clinical psychology. All assessors completed all assessments (pre, post, and follow-up) on the same participants. ACT therapists were trained and supervised by SCH; PRT therapists were trained and supervised by HHS (32 cases) or MPT (6 cases) who was trained by HHS in PRT. Assessors were supervised by SCH or MPT and were blind to treatment assignment throughout. All assessments and therapy sessions occurred in standard therapy rooms that were equipped with video recording equipment.

Diagnosis

The SCID was used to diagnose Axis I disorders. The SCID is considered the gold standard in psychiatric diagnosis and has shown largely consistent diagnostic reliability across studies (Segal, Hersen, & Van Hasselt, 1994). In general, Kappa > .70 has been found for most clinical disorders and validity is generally noted based on the SCID's similarity to the DSM (Summerfeldt & Antony, 2004).

Outcome Measures

Yale Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS)

The primary outcome was OCD severity as measured by the Y-BOCS (Goodman et al., 1989). The Y-BOCS is an assessor-rated measure of symptom severity for OCD. Total scores on the Y-BOCS range from 0 to 40. The Y-BOCS has good interrater reliability for the total score (rs between .80 and .97) and two-week test-retest reliability between .81 and .97. In the present trial Cronbach's α at pre was .79.

Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II)

Because depression is commonly associated with OCD, the BDI-II (Beck, 1996) was used as a secondary outcome measure. The BDI-II has shown good internal consistency, and high test-retest reliability. In the present trial α =.90.

Quality of Life Scale (QOLS)

The QOLS (Burckhardt, Woods, Schultz, & Ziebarth, 1989) measures how satisfied people are with the quality of their lives. It has been found to be internally consistent (αs between .89 and .92), with good temporal stability (rs between .78 to .84 over three weeks) (Burckhardt et al., 1989). In the present trial α =.89.

Measures of Treatment Process

Acceptance and Action Questionnaire

The 16-item Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (Hayes, Strosahl et al., 2004) was used to measure psychological flexibility. Questions are rated on a seven-point Likert-type scale. Lower scores reflect greater experiential willingness and ability to act in the presence of difficult thoughts and feelings. Sample items include: “It's OK to feel depressed or anxious,” and “It is unnecessary for me to learn to control my feelings in order to handle my life well.” The Acceptance and Action Questionnaire has good convergent and discriminant validity and test-retest reliability after a four month period was r =.64 (Hayes et al., 2004; Hayes et al., 2006). In the present trial α =.74.

Thought Action Fusion Scale (Shafran, Thordarson, & Rachman, 1996)

The Thought Action Fusion Scale scale contains 19 items designed to measure how much participants equate thought and action. Items are rated from 0 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). It has two distinct subscales: (1) having a thought makes an event more likely to happen (likelihood Thought Action Fusion) and (2) having the thought is the same as engaging in the action (moral Thought Action Fusion). Mean scores of an obsessional sample (diagnosed with OCD or high scores on OCD severity measure) on the likelihood subscale are 9.18, and 3.12 for the community sample, and scores on the moral subscale were 20.03 for the obsessional sample and 12.74 in the community sample. Three month test-retest reliability for the total score were r =.52, and it shows construct validity with other measures (Rassin, Merckelbach, Muris, & Schmidt, 2001). In the present trial α =.92; TAF moral subscale α = .91; TAF likelihood subscale α = .95.

Thought Control Questionnaire (Wells & Davies, 1994)

The Thought Control Questionnaire is a 30-item self-report measure that assesses the use of thought control strategies. An example item includes, “When I experience an unpleasant/unwanted thought I think pleasant thoughts instead.” Test-retest reliability is good (r =.83). This is a widely used measure of this construct for obsessive compulsive and anxiety related disorders and has been shown to change as a result of cognitive behavioral interventions (e.g., Reynolds & Wells, 1999). In the present trial α =.82.

Measures of Treatment Credibility and Acceptability

Treatment Credibility Measure

Treatment credibility was measured immediately after the initial therapy session using five questions from the credibility/expectancy questionnaire (Devilly & Borkovec, 2000) worded for the treatment of OCD. An example item is “How logical does this type of treatment seem to you for helping people reduce their obsessive compulsive disorder?” Questions were rated on a 0 to 8 point Likert scale. Participants were asked to complete the measure and seal it in an envelope that would not be opened until the participant had completed the study. This was done to preserve the confidentiality of this information during treatment.

Treatment Evaluation Inventory-Short Form

Acceptability of treatment was assessed at post only using the acceptability factor of the Treatment Evaluation Inventory-Short Form (Kelley, Heffer, Gresham, & Elliot, 1989), and leaving out two questions that pertain only to family interventions. A sample item is “I liked the procedures used in this treatment.” In the present trial α =.94.

Assessment Occasions

Participants were assessed one week prior to treatment (pre). In addition to consent for participation and demographic information, participants also completed the SCID and all measures. These were repeated one week (posttreatment) and three months (follow-up) after the final treatment session except as noted. Weekly outcome and process measures were taken weekly during treatment; these will be addressed in a subsequent publication.

Treatment Procedures

Participants attended 11 total sessions: an intake session, eight one-hour weekly treatment sessions, a post assessment session, and a 3-month follow-up assessment session. Following the initial intake, participants were randomly assigned to either the ACT for OCD or the PRT condition. A brief description of the interventions follows.

ACT for OCD

The protocol used in this investigation is the same as that used by Twohig et al. (2006b).1 While book length presentations of ACT for anxiety are available (e.g., Eifert & Forsyth, 2005; Woods & Twohig, 2008) ACT is still unfamiliar enough that an extended characterization of the protocol seems warranted. Sessions followed the same essential pattern: homework and events over the past week were discussed, the material from the previous session was reviewed and new material was presented, and new homework was assigned and behavioral commitment exercises were agreed upon (behavioral commitments began at session 3). Behavioral commitment exercises involved commitments to engage in valued-directed behavior that was both important and difficult. Examples of behavioral commitments included participating in church services, playing sports, eating lunch with others, or spending time with family. These tasks were difficult because of their OCD. For example, the person playing sports might get dirty and have fears of contamination; the person going to church might disappear into scrupulous ruminations; the person eating lunch with others might struggle with unwanted sexual images of those near him; the person spending time with family might contact an urge to clean the house instead. The goal of these commitments was not to reduce obsessions or anxiety levels, rather they were presented as opportunities to engage in important life values while practicing ACT skills (e.g., acceptance, defusion, present moment awareness) if private experiences arose that might interfere with valued actions. For example, a client playing sports might mindfully notice thoughts of contamination as if watching an external object, spend some moments feeling where anxiety arises in his or her body, and then shift attention toward playing the sporting activity with gusto. These behavioral commitments were established as specific activities for specified periods of time, without regard to the nature or intensity of the participant's obsessions or anxiety.

Session 1 began by examining in detail the participant's obsessions and compulsions, introducing the treatment, and establishing a verbal therapeutic contract. Next, therapists helped participants identify and distinguish between obsessions and compulsions. The participant was shown how one could occur without the other, but that in the participant's life they usually occurred together. The participant was asked what he or she had done to decrease the obsession, with particular attention paid to which strategies had worked to reduce obsessions and which had not. The purpose of this “creative hopelessness” phase of treatment was to note that seemingly the only way to obtain relief from the obsession was to engage in a compulsion, but that this strategy only worked for brief periods of time, and this process generally pulled the participant into a greater struggle with the obsessions.

Session 2 focused on the “Person in the Hole” metaphor (Hayes et al., 1999, p. 101) which demonstrates the ultimate ineffectiveness of attempts to inner experiences such as obsessions. The metaphor describes the participant falling in a hole (which represents the obsession) with only a shovel to get out (methods for reducing the obsession). The metaphor illustrates how the participant's attempts to dig out of the hole (representing attempts to reduce or control the obsession) are unsuccessful and actually make the hole larger (the paradox of how struggling with one's obsessions can make them more central, aversive, and connected with diverse situations or actions). Being stuck in a hole served as a metaphor for how such strategies often result in high personal costs such as reduced engagement in social and occupational activities. The intended function of the metaphor, and its extension to the participant's life, was to reduce the focus on reducing obsessions and increase awareness of the difficulty in controlling it.

Sessions 3 and 4 generally focused on illustrating how attempts to control obsessions might be exacerbating OCD rather than ameliorating it, and that allowing the obsessions to occur might be more useful in the long-term. This involved exercises aimed at illustrating the limitations of control when aimed at inner experiences, such as trying not to think of something, such as “Chocolate Cake” (Hayes et al., 1999, p. 124) or to not get nervous when hooked to a “Polygraph” (Hayes et al., 1999, p. 123) machine. These exercises were designed to help the participant experience the difference between an obsession (an uncontrollable inner experience) and a compulsion (a controllable public behavior), hopefully shifting the focus from decreasing the obsession to decreasing compulsive behaviors. The “Two Scales Metaphor” (Hayes et al., 1999, p.133) was discussed to illustrate the possible benefits of acceptance of obsessions and other inner experiences such as anxiety over attempting to control them. Scale one represented severity of the obsession, and it was shown that this scale is very difficult to regulate. Scale two represented willingness to experience the obsession, and it was shown how this scale could be regulated. The participant was told that treatment focused on increasing scale two (willingness) while learning not to address scale one (obsessions). It was presented that being willing would not necessarily decrease the obsession or anxiety, but that being unwilling certainly increases them or makes them more prominent. Willingness therefore means that the participant can allow the obsession and feelings of anxiety to rise and fall as they will, while the participant engages in important life activities. Attempts to control obsessions, on the other hand, can paradoxically increase its frequency, intensity, and capacity to control behavior.

Sessions 5 and 6 focused on changing the psychological function of the obsession from something threatening to simply another cognitive event. Defusion exercises, contact with the present moment or mindfulness exercises, and self as context work were employed to this end. Examples of defusion exercises were speaking a thought in a funny voice, or treating the obsessions as passengers on a bus where the participant is the bus driver. This metaphor was used to illustrate that the passengers had been allowed to take control of the bus rather than the driver (the participant's response to obsessions cost control over life direction), and that control of the bus could go back to the driver if the driver were willing to allow the passengers to come along for the ride. Participants were helped to recognize that the passengers grew more unruly (the obsessions felt more intense) as he or she began driving the bus in chosen life directions (toward values), but he or she had ultimate control over the bus's movement (Hayes et al. 1999, p. 157).

Contact with the present moment involved helping the participant flexibly attend to the world more directly, rather than the world as constructed by the evaluative language processes. This process is very similar and contains many of the same principals as other acceptance- or mindfulness-based therapies. Contact with the present moment was fostered by experiential exercises such as the “Leaves on a Stream” (Hayes et al., 1999, p. 158), wherein thoughts and other experiences can be witnessed as events floating down a stream. The goal of such exercises is to practice noticing experiences in the present moment without holding onto (inflexibly attending to or struggling with) any one thought or feeling – just observing what occurs.

Self as context work helps the participant experience a sense of self as more than his or her inner experiences—as an observer of them. Participants are helped to see inner experiences as simply transient events to be observed rather than as identifying personal characteristics. The “Chessboard Metaphor” (Hayes et al., 1999, p. 190) was used, in which the participant is described as the chessboard and the pieces are the obsessions, to help the participant see that the obsession could exist without causing harm to the participant—just as the chessboard can exist without being harmed by the pieces.

Sessions 7 and 8 involved discussing each participant's values and increased behavioral commitments to live more consistently with them. Values were assessed in session with the Valued Living Questionnaire (VLQ) (Wilson, Sandoz, Kitchens, & Roberts, 2010). The questionnaire assessed the discrepancy between the importance of several personal values domains including family, occupation, and recreation and one's success at living consistent with these values. For many participants, engaging in compulsions had been a barrier to living in accordance with one or more valued domains. Based on the responses to these questions, larger behavioral commitments were planned that involved following one's values and practicing willingness to experience obsessions as he or she did so. Finally, the therapist helped the participant see how to prevent relapse by practicing acceptance, present moment, defusion and self as context exercises while engaging in increasingly consistent patterns of values-directed action.

PRT for OCD

An abbreviated version of PRT based on Bernstein et al. (2000) was used in this study. It involved learning to tense and relax groups of muscles beginning with a larger number of smaller groups (in this case 16) and then proceeding in steps to a smaller number of larger groups (7 groups and 4); finally, relaxation was taught by recall and counting alone. PRT sessions followed a similar pattern as the ACT condition: events since the last session and homework were reviewed, the material from the previous session was reviewed and new material was presented, and new homework was assigned. In session 1, anxiety was presented as a precipitating factor for obsessions, and PRT as a way to help manage anxiety. Sessions 2 through 4 involved engaging in “16 muscle group” relaxation, twice each session. Participants were asked to engage in the procedure at least twice a day at home. In sessions 5 and 6, the focus was on “7 muscle group” relaxation; session 7 focused on “4 muscle group” relaxation. Recall (bringing on the relaxed state by remembering the experience of being relaxed) was also trained and practiced. Session 8 reviewed the “4 muscle group” method and recall, and trained a cuing procedure based on counting.

Reliability of OCD Diagnosis and Y-BOCS Scores

All assessment sessions were recorded. Twenty percent of the initial diagnostic interviews were independently coded by a second rater; there was 100% agreement on diagnosis and nondiagnosis of OCD. A random selection of 10% of the Y-BOCS tapes from pre, post, and follow-up were rated by a second rater. Overall agreement was .90 (95% CI: .87, .92) using a twoway mixed intraclass correlation coefficient, suggesting very high agreement on Y-BOCS scores between the original assessor scores and the additional rater scores taken from the tapes.

Treatment Integrity

ACT Condition

A coding system that has been successfully employed in a previous study on ACT for OCD (Twohig et al., 2006b) was used in the current study to rate treatment integrity. Twenty percent of each ACT therapist's sessions (distributed evenly across all eight treatment sessions) were rated. In addition to ratings of overall adherence to the manual, overall therapist competence, and the degree to which therapists assessed the participant's functioning, the coding system rated the frequency and depth of coverage of major components of the ACT model on a five-point scale. Each rating scale and item includes anchors and examples. Non-adherent items were also coded including challenging cognitions, use of a cognitive therapy rationale, supporting the idea that thoughts and feelings lead to action, using avoidant change strategies, or using ERP within session or suggesting its use outside of session. ERP exercises were generally defined as purposefully exposing oneself to a feared stimulus, for an extended duration in time, to produce anxiety and obsessions, and staying in that situation until a reduction in anxiety or obsessions occurs. Three independent raters completed the coding with 20% overlap in order to calculate reliability using two-way mixed intraclass correlation coefficients. An advanced clinical graduate student trained in ACT served as a master rater. Inter-rater reliability across all domains assessed for integrity (described in detail below) was .94 (95% CI: .90, .96) between the master rater and rater 1 and .80 (95% CI: .63, .90) between the master rater and rater 2.

Scores of 1 on the rating system indicate the variable was never explicitly covered, 2=the variable occurred at least once and not in an in-depth manner, 3=the variable occurred several times and was covered at least once in a moderately in-depth manner, 4=the variable occurred with relatively high frequency and was addressed in a moderately in depth manner, 5=the variable occurred with high frequency and was covered in a very in-depth manner. No non-adherent items were noted in any of the coded sessions. Overall adherence to the manual and overall competence were highly rated (M = 4.56, SD = .54 and M = 4.67, SD = .48 respectively). These ratings are the primary adherence and competence scores because particular processes varied from session to session. In order to characterize the sessions more completely, however, it is helpful to examine specific item scores for ACT processes. Means for ACT components over the eight sessions were as follows: creative hopelessness/workability = 3.09 (SD = 1.47), defusion = 2.82 (SD = 1.26), willingness/acceptance = 2.91 (SD = 1.41), values and goals = 2.68 (SD = 1.15), committed action = 1.74 (SD = 1.03), and assessment of participant's symptoms and overall functioning = 3.12 (SD = .95), indicating that for most processes the “variable occurred several times and was addressed at least once by the therapist in a moderately in-depth manner” in each session on average (the manual definition of a score of 3). The focus of specific sessions can be examined by considering the highest mean item rating for each session, which was as follows: Session 1 - General assessment 4.38 (SD = .74); Session 2 – Creative hopelessness/workability 5.00 (SD = .00); Session 3 – Creative hopelessness/workability 4.38 (SD = .92); Session 4 – Willingness/acceptance 3.33 (SD = .52) and Defusion 3.33 (1.03); Session 5 - Defusion 4.13 (.64); Session 6 - Defusion 3.86 (1.07); Session 7 – Values and goals 3.83 (SD = .75); Session 8 – Values and goals 3.67 (SD = 1.03).

PRT Condition

A PRT integrity coding manual was developed using the same framework as the ACT integrity coding manual (the rating scale, percentage of tapes examined, and reliability checks were the same across the two conditions), and included components based upon the PRT therapy manual. Rated non-adherent items were ERP and ACT processes (creative hopelessness/workability, defusion, acceptance, values, and committed action). An advanced clinical graduate student trained in PRT served as a master rater. Inter-rater reliability across all domains assessed for integrity (described in detail below) between the master rater and rater 1 was .98 (95% CI: .93, .98), and rater 2 was .95 (95% CI: .82, .96).

On a five-point scale, PRT therapists’ overall adherence to the manual and overall competence were highly rated (M = 4.60, SD = .73 and M = 4.65, SD = .57 respectively). The frequency of non-adherent items was very low but occurred in the areas of acceptance (M = 1.07, SD = .26), creative hopelessness/workability (M = 1.02, SD = .15) and ERP (M = 1.11, SD = .39). Means for PRT components over the eight sessions were as follows: PRT rationale = 2.74 (SD = 1.26), relaxation procedure = 3.56 (SD = 1.31), homework practice = 2.77 (SD = .84), and assessment of participant's symptoms and overall functioning = 3.44 (SD = .79). Examining the data session by session showed that the relaxation procedure item was scored highest of all items in all sessions (range of the session means for that item: 3.2 – 4.5) except the first session in which the PRT rationale was highest: 4.67 (SD = .82).

Results

Table 1 shows the obtained means for all outcome measures for the two conditions for each measurement occasion. Pre-treatment differences on parametric measures were compared using t-tests; other measures (or those violating homoscedasticity) were tested using a Mann-Whitney U test. There were no pre-treatment differences (p < .05) on any measure.

Table 1.

ACT Condition PRT Condition
Baseline Post Follow-up Baseline Post Follow-up
N except as noted: n = 41 n = 36 n = 33 n = 38 n = 33 n = 31
Outcome Measures
Y-BOCS 24.22 (4.80) 12.76 (8.35) 11.79 (8.97) 25.40 (5.26) 18.67 (5.68) 16.23 (7.46)
BDI-II 18.09 (11.83) 8.50 (10.99) 8.43 (10.13) 17.89 (8.39) 13.69 (9.83)
n = 32
10.89 (8.06)
QOLS 72.49 (15.00) 82.18 (14.86) 82.03 (14.58)
n = 32
69.58 (14.35) 73.59 (12.42)
n = 32
73.19 (17.01)
Process Measures
AAQ-16 59.76 (11.36) 73.69 (13.22) 73.37 (14.44) 57.16 (11.67) 63.81 (7.69)
n = 32
67.13 (10.74)
n = 30
TCQ 65.54 (12.23) 56.11 (10.76) 56.74 (9.66) 65.04 (8.04) 61.80 (8.44)
n = 32
60.26 (6.73)
TAF
Morality
21.46 (11.17) 12.72 (9.89) 14.14 (10.58) 18.47 (10.38) 17.94 (9.49) 18.13 (9.53)
TAF
Likelihood
9.76 (8.21) 5.47 (6.47) 6.90 (7.56) 7.76 (7.66) 6.22 (6.63)
n = 32
6.00 (5.81)

ACT=Acceptance and Commitment Therapy; PRT=Progressive Relaxation Training; Y-BOCS=Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale; BDI-II=Beck Depression Inventory-II; QOLS=Quality of Life Scale; AAQ-16 Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-16 item version; TCQ=Thought Control Questionnaire; TAF=Thought Action Fusion

Dropout and Dose

In the present study no participants overtly refused treatment assignment; 1 participant of the 41 the ACT condition (2.4%) and 3 of the 38 in the PRT condition (7.8%) did not attend Session 1, indicating very low passive treatment refusal. The rates for minimal participation or passive dropout were similarly low; 4 of 41 (9.8%) ACT participants and 5 PRT participants (13.2%) attended only 1 or 2 sessions.

Outcome Analytic Strategy

Longitudinal data were analyzed using Hierarchical Linear Modeling (HLM) and Mixed Model Repeated Measures (MMRM) approaches and an intent-to-treat sample containing all randomized participants (Hedeker & Gibbons, 2006). HLM analyses were conducted on all measures. Measurement occasion was used as a continuous temporal variable, with pre-treatment set as occasion zero. HLM analyses were first conducted assuming a random intercept and slope and allowing them to be correlated, followed by analyses assuming they were uncorrelated and a random intercept analysis. Final analyses relied on the simplest variance-covariance structure not significantly different than the most complex as determined by comparison of models through the restricted log-likelihood. If the HLM analyses did not fully converge (indicated by the Hessian matrix not positive definite) or the HLM model fit less well than MMRM (comparing an HLM with unstructured covariance to a MMRM analysis with compound symmetry covariance assumptions using the significance of restricted log-likelihood values) then a MMRM analysis was used that treated time as a categorical factor. Analyses first were conducted with unstructured covariance assumptions, estimating a separate variance for each time point and separate covariance for each distinct pair of time points, followed by more restrictive assumptions (using compound symmetry, Toeplitz, and compound symmetry heterogeneous covariance structures). The model with the fewest covariance parameters was used provided there was no significant difference in fit as compared to an unstructured covariance model as determined by comparison of models through the restricted log-likelihood.

Denominator degrees of freedom for fixed effects test statistics were based on the Sattherthwaite approximation. Effect sizes for MMRM outcomes were calculated as specified by Wackerly, Mendenhall, and Scheaffer (2008) and for HLM by examining end point outcomes as suggested by Feingold (2009). Effect sizes were discussed using Cohen's (1988) cutoffs (Small ≥ .2; Medium ≥ .5; Large ≥ .8).

Primary Outcome: Y-BOCS

The overall pattern of results from the Y-BOCS is shown in Figure 2. Because the slopes per se are included in the text, the figure shows the adjusted means from an MMRM analysis with unstructured covariance. An HLM model assuming that slopes and intercepts were uncorrelated best fit the data. There was a significant effect for time F (1, 80.64) = 115.54, p < .001, no effect for condition, F (1, 92.98) = 1.16, p = .28, and a significant interaction between condition and time, F (1, 80.64) = 5.15, p = .026. Participants in both conditions showed significantly improving slopes (ACT: Estimate = -1.22, SE = .13, t (80.90) = -9.40, p < .001, 95% CI: -1.47, -.96; PRT: Estimate = -.79 SE = .13, t (80.40) = -5.88, p < .001, 95% CI: -1.06, -.52) but the ACT condition produced a large and significant difference in rate of improvement (Estimate = .42, SE = .19, t (80.64) = 2.27, p = .026, 95% CI: .05, .80, end point outcome effect size = .84).

Clinical significance

The Jacobson methodology (Jacobson, Follette, & Revenstorf, 1984; Jacobson & Truax, 1991) was used to analyze the clinical significance of the obtained Y-BOCS scores. A change of 6.39 on the Y-BOCS was considered reliable. Method A for determining the cutoff score for clinically significant change subsequent to a reliable change yielded a cutoff of 14 at post or follow-up, which is the same cutoff as a recent meta-analysis evaluating ERP and ERP/CT for OCD (Fisher & Wells, 2005). All refusals, dropouts, or missing data were scored as unimproved and included in the denominator, providing a full intent to treat analysis. The two conditions differed at post (ACT: 19 of 41; PRT: 5 of 38; χ2 (1) = 10.27, p =.002, two-tailed, effect size = .77) and follow-up (ACT: 19 of 41; PRT: 7 of 38; χ2 (1) = 6.96, p < .009, two-tailed, effect size = .62). In order to deal with missing data, studies frequently report these figures using the last value carried forward, or including only completers instead of assuming that all missing data are negative. A more sophisticated approach to missing data is to use imputed values from the person-specific growth curves from the HLM analysis. Using these values the two conditions again differed at post (ACT: 23 of 41; PRT: 7 of 38; χ2 (1) = 10.30, p < .002, two-tailed, effect size = .77) and follow-up (ACT: 27 of 41; PRT: 6 of 38; χ2 (1) = 18.24, p < .001, two-tailed, effect size = 1.10).

Secondary Outcomes

Depression

Depression scores were analyzed for those participants who were at least mildly depressed at pre-treatment based on a BDI-II cutoff score of 13. Seventy percent of the sample (N: ACT = 27; PRT = 28) met that criterion. A MMRM analysis with a heterogeneous compound symmetry structure best fit the data and revealed a non-significant effect for treatment condition, F (1, 55.32) = .35, p = .56, but significant effects for time, F (2, 69.63) = 44.14, p < .001, and the interaction between condition and time, F (2, 69.63) = 5.61, p = .006. The interaction occurred because ACT participants with at least a mild level of depression improved significantly more on the BDI-II than did PRT participants from pre to post (Estimated Mdiff = -8.10, SE = 2.51, t (59.65) = -3.23, p = .002, 95% CI: -13.12, -3.09, effect size = .96) and from pre to follow-up (Estimated Mdiff = -4.87, SE = 2.37, t (62.82) = -2.06, p = .044, 95% CI: -9.60, -.14, effect size = .63). Within condition pre to post and pre to follow-up changes were significant in both conditions: effect sizes were respectively .5 and .93 in the PRT condition and 1.40 and 1.52 in the ACT condition.

Quality of life

A MMRM analysis with unstructured covariance best fit the data and revealed a non-significant effect for condition, F (1, 66.53) = 2.41, p = .126, but a significant effect for time, F (2, 64.62) = 11.44, p < .001, and trend toward an interaction between condition and time, F (2, 64.62) = 2.99, p = .057. There was no difference in improvement between conditions at follow-up (Estimated Mdiff = .18, SE = 3.14, t (63.49) = .06, p = .96, 95% CI: -6.10, 6.46, effect size = .01) but there was a trend toward a difference at post (Estimated Mdiff = 5.46, SE = 2.78, t (70.22) = 1.96, p = .054, 95% CI: -.09, 11.00, effect size = .47).

Process Results

Acceptance and Action Questionnaire

A MMRM analysis with an unstructured covariance structure best fit the data and revealed a significant effect for treatment condition, F (1, 69.78) = 7.30, p = .009, time, F (2, 66.36) = 22.14, p < .001, and their interaction, F (2, 66.35) = 5.60, p = .006. The interaction occurred because ACT participants improved significantly more from pre to post than did PRT participants (Estimated Mdiff = -7.73, SE = 3.09, t (70.22) = -2.50, p = .015, 95% CI: -13.90, -1.957, effect size = .59), but the two conditions were equivalent from pre to follow-up (Estimated Mdiff = -3.11, SE = 3.40, t (67.89) = -.92, p = .36, 95% CI: -9.89, 3.66, effect size = .22). Within condition pre to post and pre to follow-up changes were significant: effect sizes were respectively .47 and .64 in the PRT condition and 1.06 and .86 in the ACT condition.

Thought Control Questionnaire

A MMRM analysis with an unstructured covariance structure best fit the data and revealed an approaching significant effect for treatment condition, F (1, 77.28) = 3.18, p = .079, and a significant effect for time, F (2, 70.43) = 17.88, p < .001, and the interaction of condition and time, F (2, 70.43) = 5.11, p = .008. The interaction occurred because ACT participants improved significantly more from pre to post than did PRT participants (Estimated Mdiff = -6.67, SE = 2.12, t (72.51) = -3.14, p = .002, 95% CI: -10.90, -2.43, effect size = .75). There was a trend toward a difference from pre to follow-up (Estimated Mdiff = -4.56, SE = 2.59, t (75.70) = 1.77, p = .082, 95% CI: -9.72, .59, effect size = .42). Within condition pre to post and pre to follow-up changes were .31 (p = .071) and .40 (p = .022) in the PRT condition and .80 (p < .001) and .81 (p < .001) in the ACT condition.

Thought Action Fusion Scale

For the moral subscale, an HLM analysis assuming a random slope and intercept that may be correlated revealed a non-significant effect for treatment condition, F (1, 76.34) = 1.25, p = .27, and a significant effect for time, F (1, 71.72) = 16.32, p < .001, and the interaction of condition and time, F (1, 71.72) = 11.39, p = .001, end point outcome effect size = .73. ACT participants showed an improving slope (ACT: Estimate = -.86, SE = .16, t (71.93) = -5.32, p < .001) but PRT participants did not (PRT: Estimate = -.07, SE = .17, t (71.52) = -.46, p = .65). For the likelihood subscale, an HLM analysis assuming only a random intercept revealed a non-significant effect for treatment condition, F (1, 117.43) = 1.41, p = .24, and a significant effect for time, F (1, 135.78) = 18.94, p < .001, and a trend toward an interaction of condition and time, F (1, 135.78) = 3.33, p = .07, end point outcome effect size = .31. ACT participants showed an improving slope (ACT: Estimate = -.41, SE = .09, t (135.54) = -4.46, p < .001) while PRT participants showed a trend for one (PRT: Estimate = -.17, SE = .10, t (136.00) = -1.75, p = .082).

Treatment credibility, dose, and acceptability

Because the distribution of scores was not normal, treatment credibility was examined nonparametrically using a Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, which was non-significant (K-S = .99, N = 71, p = .27), indicating that credibility had the same central tendency and distribution in the two conditions. Credibility was not related to follow-up decreases in the Y-BOCS in either the PRT (Spearman's rho (31) = -.04, p = .83) or the ACT condition (Spearman's rho (30) = .07, p = .70). Post results showed the same pattern.

Because the distribution of scores was not normal, differences between conditions in the number of treatment sessions received was also examined non-parametrically using a Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, which was non-significant (K-S = .976, N = 79, p = .61). The number of treatment sessions was not related to follow-up decreases in the Y-BOCS in either the PRT condition (Spearman's rho (31) = -.12, p = .54), or the ACT condition (Spearman's rho (33) = -.15, p = .41). Post result were similar for the PRT condition, but the number of treatment sessions was correlated with post decreases in the Y-BOCS in the ACT condition (Spearman's rho (33) = -.30, p = .02).

At posttreatment, PRT was viewed as an acceptable treatment with an average item score of 3.68 on a 5-point scale. ACT was significantly more acceptable, however (t (65) = 3.57, p = .001, effect size = .89) with an average item score of 4.38. Because treatment acceptability was necessarily collected at the end of treatment, after there were already outcome differences in the Y-BOCS, this analysis was repeated with pre to post Y-BOCS change scores entered as a covariate. ACT continued to have significantly higher levels of acceptability (F (1,64) = 5.25, p = .025, effect size = .57). Seventy-five percent of the ACT participants rated the treatment with an average score of 4 or better and 25% rated it with the highest acceptability score. Entering treatment credibility, acceptability, and number of treatment sessions as covariates did not change the primary outcomes reported here (results generally became fractionally stronger in favor of ACT).

Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to examine the applicability, efficiency and acceptability of a deliberately limited ACT intervention for OCD done without any in-session exposure procedures as compared to PRT. Very limited exclusion criteria were utilized in this investigation to assess the broad applicability of the approach, and all subtypes of OCD were treated using the same protocol. Both treatments were equally credible and treatment integrity data were good for both conditions but results showed greater improvement at posttreatment and at follow-up in OCD symptoms for ACT as compared to PRT. ACT resulted in a greater number of participants showing clinically significant improvement compared to PRT and had a significantly greater effect on depression among those reporting at least mild depression before treatment. Quality of life improved in both conditions but was marginally in favor of ACT at post. Finally, psychological constructs thought to be associated with ACT showed greater change in the ACT condition than in PRT, at least initially. All of this suggests that an ACT model appears to be broadly applicable to an OCD population.

Efficiency

. The ACT for OCD protocol that was tested in this investigation is perhaps most notable for its short duration (8 one-hour sessions), and thus, for the efficiency of the results produced on a per unit of effort basis. The average therapist contact time for ERP and ERP/CT outcome studies (weighted by the size of the study) was 27.4 hours (range: 9 – 48 hours) for the studies in the review conducted by Abramowitz et al. (2009). The present study is below the range of durations found in that review (or an earlier review by Fisher and Wells, 2005) and represents 29-31% of the average duration of comparable protocols. On average (weighted by the size of the studies), the studies in the Abramowitz et al. (2009) review produced a 45.4% reduction in the Y-BOCS at post (follow up was not reported), which is virtually the same as the 47.3% reduction (post) and 51.3% (follow-up) reduction seen in the present study. The baseline Y-BOCS scores were very similar as well, with a weighted pre-score average of 23.9 in the ERP or ERP/CT groups in the studies in the Abramowitz et al. (2009) review as compared to 24.2 in the present study.

We are not arguing that ACT should yet be directly compared to ERP and ERP/CT, nor that research should proceed directly to this question. An ACT approach to OCD is still young, this was a deliberately brief and limited application of an ACT model, and comparisons across study populations need to be done cautiously. Rather, the point is simply that the efficiency suggested by these early results is promising enough that efforts should be made to help ACT for OCD mature. Future studies should examine how processes of change comport with outcomes, how best to structure ACT interventions to move the key processes involved (should exposure be included? How much intervention is optimal?), and whether ACT can aid ERP and ERP/CT failures. As these questions are answered, the field will be better prepared for the very large and expensive studies that will seemingly be needed to directly compare ACT to existing empirically supported methods on issues of both processes of change and outcomes produced.

Acceptability

Treatment acceptability was high in both conditions but statistically higher in ACT than in PRT, even after controlling for outcome differences. The acceptability of ACT approached the ceiling of the instrument used (an average of 4.38 out of 5). There were no overt refusals to participate in the ACT condition, either at assignment or during treatment; one ACT participant failed to attend the first session; four more attended fewer than three treatment sessions. Thus 5 of 41 ACT participants (12.2%) were categorized as actively or passively declining or dropping out of treatment, which appears to be below the rates expected from recent reviews of ERP and ERP/CT (e.g., Abramowitz et al., 2009).

ACT, as conducted in this trial, did not include in-session exposure exercises. ACT participants were not asked during treatment to approach a feared stimulus, including primary obsessions, and stay in its presence for a period of time. Based on treatment integrity ratings, suggesting that engaging in such activities would be helpful outside of session also never occurred. Participants were taught to learn to see obsessions for what they are, to accept their presence, and to do what is important to them whether or not obsessions were occurring. This is notable, because exposure exercises, while very useful clinically, are met with resistance by a marked number of professionals and clients (Richard & Gloster, 2007). The absence of these exercises may in part account for the good results seen in treatment acceptability, and thus the present approach may offer an alternative approach to the treatment of OCD when exposure has failed or is declined.

ACT and exposure

The absence of in-session exposure in a procedural sense does not mean that exposure in a broader sense was unimportant to the results seen. ACT researchers have long argued that ACT is an exposure based approach (see Hayes, 1987) and doing exposure in a procedural sense within an ACT approach is generally suggested for anxiety disorders (Eifert & Forsyth, 2005), even though it was eschewed for strategic reasons in the present study. At the level of process, ACT theory views exposure as organized, values-based contact with previously repertoire narrowing stimuli for the purpose of producing greater psychological flexibility (Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, in press). In an ACT model, these repertoire narrowing stimuli are not just external events, but also fused thoughts, avoided emotions, experiences that induce inflexible attentional processes, and so on. If that conception is broadly correct, increasing psychological flexibility and values-based action in the presence of difficult emotions and thoughts could well be a key process of change in other successful exposure-based methods. General research will be needed to know whether this is the case.

At the level of procedure, participants agreed to engage in values-based actions that were important and difficult. These generally were important activities that they had stopped engaging in or were only partially engaging in because of attempts to regulate obsessions. Participants were asked to practice the skills learned in the therapy sessions during these activities and to treat them as opportunities to do things that were important to them while more fully acquiring ACT skills. This might be little more than a procedural variant of exposure–if so, it appears to be one with a very high level of acceptability. There are some preliminary data that ACT methods can make exposure more acceptable and less aversive in individuals diagnosed with panic disorder (Levitt et al., 2004), and the same might be true with OCD. Regardless, it is important in future research to understand the processes of change occurring during these exercises and to compare them to those seen in ERP. Thus, the interplay of ACT and exposure at both the process and procedural level should continue to be explored to help better understand both.

Limitations

There are several limitations to this study. The quality of life measure was entirely self-report and the main outcome measure, while externally rated, was entirely focused on symptom severity. Such a mode of measurement fits uncomfortably with a view that emphasizes function over form and living more effectively over symptom reduction per se. Follow-up was only three months due to the preliminary nature of this study, but six-month to two-year follow-up is preferable. Assessors were blinded, but penetration of the blind was not formally assessed. Additionally, because the same assessor was used for all participants at all time points to maintain consistency in assessments, the participants were not blind to the timing of assessments. Also, the ACT and PRT conditions could have been matched better if the participants in the PRT condition were asked to use relaxation in response to obsessions because ACT participants used ACT strategies when obsessions were present. Also, the PRT protocol was briefer than suggested by the authors (Bernstein et al., 2000). These weaknesses need to be addressed in future studies. It should also be noted that the sample drawn for this investigation was not ethnically diverse (88.6% Caucasian), thus, extension of the present results in this area should be done with caution. Finally, additional work needs to be conducted to determine the overall effects of the different components and processes within the entire treatment.

The treatment of OCD has progressed substantially since the first treatment trials, but we are still far from helping enough individuals in an efficient, acceptable, and broadly effective manner. The development of novel and effective psychosocial interventions such as ACT are needed. It appears that ACT can lead to meaningful clinical outcomes without necessarily using traditional exposure procedures. ACT looks promising as a treatment for OCD, but clearly much more work is needed to develop this approach.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Kimberly Pratt, Michael Levin, Dahlia Mukherjee, Tami Jeffcoat, Lindsay Martin, Kate Morrison, and Heidi LaBash for their assistance with this project. We would also like to thank Maureen Whittal for her comments on the manuscript. This project was conducted as M. P. Twohig's dissertation at the University of Nevada, Reno for which he received the Virginia Roswell Dissertation Award from the Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies. The present project was funded by a grant from the National Institutes of Mental Health (R21 MH074968), S. C. Hayes, Principal-investigator.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: The following manuscript is the final accepted manuscript. It has not been subjected to the final copyediting, fact-checking, and proofreading required for formal publication. It is not the definitive, publisher-authenticated version. The American Psychological Association and its Council of Editors disclaim any responsibility or liabilities for errors or omissions of this manuscript version, any version derived from this manuscript by NIH, or other third parties. The published version is available at www.apa.org/pubs/journals/ccp

1

The protocols are available at contextualpsychology.org or from the senior author.

Contributor Information

Michael P. Twohig, Department of Psychology at Utah State University 2810 Old Main Hill, Logan, UT, 84322

Steven C. Hayes, Department of Psychology at the University of Nevada, Reno.

Jennifer C. Plumb, Department of Psychology at the University of Nevada, Reno.

Larry D. Pruitt, Department of Psychology at the University of Nevada, Reno.

Angela B. Collins, Department of Psychology at the University of Nevada, Reno.

Holly Hazlett-Stevens, Department of Psychology at the University of Nevada, Reno..

Michelle R. Woidneck, Department of Psychology at Utah State University 2810 Old Main Hill, Logan, UT, 84322

References

  1. Abramowitz JS. Variants of exposure and response prevention in the treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder: A meta-analysis. Behavior Therapy. 1996;27:583–600. [Google Scholar]
  2. Abramowitz JS, Lackey GR, Wheaton MG. Obsessive–compulsive symptoms: The contribution of obsessional beliefs and experiential avoidance. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2009;23:160–166. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Abramowitz JS, Taylor S, McKay D. Obsessive-compulsive disorder. The Lancet. 2009;374:491–499. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60240-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Arch JJ, Craske MG. Acceptance and commitment therapy and cognitive behavioral therapy for anxiety disorders: Different treatments, similar mechanisms? Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2008;15:263–279. [Google Scholar]
  5. Beck AT. Beck Depression Inventory (2nd ed.) The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1996. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bernstein DA, Borkovec TD, Hazlett-Stevens H. New directions in progressive relaxation training: A guidebook for helping professionals. Praeger; New York: 2000. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bouton ME. Context, ambiguity, and unlearning: Sources of relapse after behavioral extinction. Biological Psychology. 2002;52:976–986. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01546-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Briggs ES, Price IR. The relationship between adverse childhood experience and obsessive-compulsive symptoms and beliefs: The role of anxiety, depression, and experiential avoidance. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2009;23:1037–1046. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Burckhardt CS, Woods SL, Schultz AA, Ziebarth DM. Quality of life of adults with chronic illness: A psychometric study. Research in Nursing & Health. 1989;12:347–354. doi: 10.1002/nur.4770120604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cohen J. Set correlation and contingency tables. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1988;12:425–434. [Google Scholar]
  11. Devilly GJ, Borkovec TD. Psychometric properties of the credibility/expectancy questionnaire. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2000;31:73–86. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7916(00)00012-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Eifert GH, Forsyth JP. Acceptance and commitment therapy for anxiety disorders: A practitioner's treatment guide to using mindfulness, acceptance, and values-based behavior change strategies. New Harbinger Publications; Oakland: 2005. [Google Scholar]
  13. Fals-Stewart W, Marks AP, Schafer J. A comparison of behavioral group therapy and individual behavior therapy in treating obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1993;181:189–193. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199303000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Feingold A. Effect sizes for growth-modeling analysis for controlled clinical trials in the same metric as for classical analysis. Psychological Methods. 2009;14:43–53. doi: 10.1037/a0014699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition. (SCID-I/P) Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute; New York: 2002. [Google Scholar]
  16. Foa EB, Liebowitz MR, Kozak MJ, Davies S, Campeas R, Franklin ME, et al. Randomized, placebo-controlled trial of exposure and ritual prevention, clomipramine, and their combination in the treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162:151–161. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.1.151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Fisher PL, Wells A. How effective are cognitive and behavioral treatments for obsessive-compulsive disorder? A clinical significance analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2005;43:1543–1558. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Goodman W, Price L, Rasmussen S, Mazure C, Fleischman R, Hill C, et al. The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale: Development, use, and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1989;46:1006–1011. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810110048007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hayes SC. A contextual approach to therapeutic change. In: Jacobson N, editor. Psychotherapists in clinical practice: Cognitive and behavioral perspectives. Guilford Press; New York, NY US: 1987. pp. 327–387. [Google Scholar]
  20. Hayes SC, Luoma JB, Bond FW, Masuda A, Lillis J. Acceptance and commitment therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2006;44:1–25. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hayes SC, Strosahl KD, Wilson KG. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. Guilford Press; New York: 1999. [Google Scholar]
  22. Hayes SC, Strosahl KD, Wilson KG. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: A unified model of behavior change. 2nd ed. Guilford Press; New York: in press. [Google Scholar]
  23. Hayes SC, Strosahl K, Wilson KG, Bissett RT, Pistorello J, Toarmino D, et al. Measuring Experiential Avoidance: A Preliminary Test of a Working Model. The Psychological Record. 2004;54:553–578. [Google Scholar]
  24. Hedeker D, Gibbons RD. Longitudinal data analysis. Wiley-Interscience; Hoboken, NJ US: 2006. [Google Scholar]
  25. Jacobson NS, Follette WC, Revenstorf D. Psychotherapy outcome research: Methods for reporting variability and evaluating clinical significance. Behavior Therapy. 1984;15:336–352. [Google Scholar]
  26. Jacobson NS, Truax P. Clinical significance: A statistical approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1991;59:12–19. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.59.1.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kelley ML, Heffer RW, Gresham FM, Elliott SN. Development of a modified Treatment Evaluation Inventory. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 1989;11:235–247. [Google Scholar]
  28. Levitt JT, Brown TA, Orsillo SM, Barlow DH. The effects of acceptance versus suppression of emotion on subjective and psychophysiological response to carbon dioxide challenge in patients with panic disorder. Behavior Therapy. 2004;35:747–766. [Google Scholar]
  29. Manzoni GM, Pagnini F, Castelnuovo G, Molinari E. Relaxation training for anxiety: A ten-year systematic review with meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry. 2008;8:41. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-8-41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Masuda A, Hayes SC, Sackett C, Twohig MP. Cognitive defusion and self-relevant negative thoughts: Examining the impact of a ninety year old technique. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2004;42:477–485. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2003.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. McLean PD, Whittal ML, Thordarson DS, Taylor S, Söchting I, Koch WJ, et al. Cognitive versus behavior therapy in the group treatment of Obsessive-Compulsive disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2001;69:205–214. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Olatunji BO, Deacon BJ, Abramowitz JS. The cruelest cure? Ethical issues in the implementation of exposure-based treatments. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice. 2009;16:172–180. [Google Scholar]
  33. Rassin E, Merckelbach H, Muris P, Schmidt H. The thought-action fusion scale: Further evidence for its reliability and validity. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2001;39:537–544. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(00)00031-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Reynolds M, Wells A. The Thought Control Questionnaire--Psychometric properties in a clinical sample, and relationships with PTSD and depression. Psychological Medicine. 1999;29:1089–1099. doi: 10.1017/s003329179900104x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Richard DCS, Gloster AT. Exposure therapy has a public relations problem: A dearth of litigation amid a wealth of concern. In: Richard DCS, Lauterbach D, editors. Comprehensive handbook of the exposure therapies. Academic Press; New York: 2007. pp. 409–425. [Google Scholar]
  36. Segal DL, Hersen M, Van Hasselt VB. Reliability of the structured clinical interview for DSM-III-R: An evaluative review. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 1994;35:316–327. doi: 10.1016/0010-440x(94)90025-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Shafran R, Thordarson DS, Rachman SJ. Thought-action fusions in obsessive compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 1996;10:209–221. [Google Scholar]
  38. Steketee G, Barlow DH. Obsessive compulsive disorder. In: Barlow DH, editor. Anxiety and its disorder: The nature and treatment of anxiety and panic. Guilford Press; New York: 2002. pp. 516–550. [Google Scholar]
  39. Summerfeldt LJ, Antony MM. Structured and semistructured diagnostic interviews. In: Antony MM, Barlow DH, editors. Handbook of Assessment and Treatment Planning for Psychological Disorders. Guilford Press; New York: 2004. pp. 3–37. [Google Scholar]
  40. Twohig MP, Hayes SC, Masuda A. A preliminary investigation of acceptance and commitment therapy as a treatment for chronic skin picking. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2006a;44:1513–1522. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Twohig MP, Hayes SC, Masuda A. Increasing willingness to experience obsessions: Acceptance and commitment therapy as a treatment for obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behavior Therapy. 2006b;37:3–13. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2005.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Twohig MP, Woods DW. A preliminary investigation of acceptance and commitment therapy and habit reversal as a treatment of trichotillomania. Behavior Therapy. 2004;35:803–820. [Google Scholar]
  43. Wackerly D, Mendenhall W, Scheaffer RL. Mathematical Statistics with Applications. 7th ed. Duxbury Press; Pacific Grove, CA: 2008. [Google Scholar]
  44. Wells A, Davies M. The thought control questionnaire - a measure of individual differences in the control of unwanted thoughts. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1994;32:871–878. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)90168-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Wilson KG, Sandoz EK, Kitchens J, Roberts ME. The Valued Living Questionnaire: Defining and measuring valued action within a behavioral framework. The Psychological Record. 2010;60:249–272. [Google Scholar]
  46. Woods DW, Twohig MP. Trichotillomania: An ACT-enhanced behavior therapy approach therapist guide. Oxford Press; New York: 2008. [Google Scholar]
  47. Woods DW, Wetterneck CT, Flessner CA. A controlled evaluation of acceptance and commitment therapy plus habit reversal for trichotillomania. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2006;44:639–656. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES