Abstract
People often prefer familiar stimuli, presumably because familiarity signals safety. This preference can occur with merely repeated “old” stimuli, but it is most robust with “new” but highly familiar prototypes of a known category (beauty-in-averages effect). However, is familiarity always warm? Tuning accounts of mood hold that positive mood signals a safe environment whereas negative mood signals an unsafe environment. Thus, the value of familiarity should depend on mood. We show that compared to a sad mood, a happy mood eliminates the preference for familiar stimuli, as shown in measures of self-reported liking and physiological measures of affect (EMG indicator of spontaneous smiling). The basic effect of exposure on preference and its modulation by mood were most robust on prototypes (category averages). All this occurs even though prototypes might be more familiar in a happy mood. We conclude that mood changes the hedonic implications of familiarity cues.
Keywords: Emotions, Memory, Electrophysiology, Preferences, Judgment
Liking for previously encountered stimuli, or the “warm glow of familiarity”, is a classic phenomenon (Titchener, 1910). One source of “warm glow” is a simple repetition (Zajonc, 1968; 2001), with such “mere exposure” enhancing familiarity and liking (Whittlesea & Price, 2001).1 This enhancement sometimes generalizes to new, but categorically similar exemplars (Gordon & Holyoak, 1983). However, the most robust exposure effects on familiarity and liking occur on prototypes of the presented category (after all, prototypes resemble all the exposed exemplars). Thus, prototypes are rated as highly familiar, even when they are objectively “new” -- a memory “illusion” observed with stimuli ranging from random dots to words (e.g., Deese, 1959; Posner & Keele, 1968; Whittlesea, 2002). Prototypes are also highly liked, an effect called “beauty-in-averages” (Halberstadt, 2006). It occurs with a range of stimuli, including abstract patterns, faces, watches and cars, and dependent measures, including attractiveness ratings and psychophysiological responses (e.g., Langlois & Roggman, 1990; Halberstadt & Rhodes, 2000). For example, prototypes of a dot-pattern from an exposed versus novel category elicit more incipient “smiles” (Winkielman, Halberstadt, Fazendeiro, & Catty, 2006).
Familiarity and liking
But is familiarity always warm? Some accounts assume so. According to conditioning models, familiarity inevitably increases positivity because repeated exposure is a form of conditioning to the absence of negative consequences – associating the stimulus with relief from fear of novelty (Zajonc, 2001). Some hedonic fluency models assume that familiarity is intrinsically rewarding as it is associated with efficient and conflict-free processing (e.g., Winkielman, Schwarz, Fazendeiro, & Reber, 2003). Finally, evolutionary models suggest that koinophilia (preference for common features) occurs because typicality is a cue to mate value (Thornhill & Gangestad, 1993).
In contrast, other accounts see the familiarity-positivity link as context dependent. Familiarity is a heuristic cue to safety. Thus, as with any cue, its validity and hedonic meaning varies by context (Hertwig, Herzog, Schooler, & Reimer, 2008). Specifically, the familiarity-positivity link should depend on whether individuals are tuned towards safety concerns (e.g., Bornstein, 1989). Familiarity should be valued in an unsafe environment, but less so in a benign environment. Analogously, in a strange city a familiar face elicits a warm glow, whereas locally the same face prompts a yawn. Numerous studies (and parents) observed that in unsafe environments infants are neophobic, but less so in safe settings (Shore, 1994). Similarly, in multiple species, stress increases neophobia whereas comfort reduces it (Zuckerman, 2005).
Much psychological research points out that one signal of environmental safety or danger is an individual’s mood (e.g., Clore, Schwarz, & Conway, 1994; Schwarz, 2002). Bad mood signals a problem, tuning individuals towards safety concerns, whereas good mood signals that an environment is benign. Tuning accounts assume that mood adjusts cognitive and affective reactions so that they best serve the individual in the specific context. Thus, mood should modulate affective responses to familiarity, with greater preference for familiarity in negative than positive mood.
The current research
We explored how mood modulates affective and cognitive responses to familiarity, expecting greater value of familiarity in a sad mood. One interesting prediction concerns the mood modulation of prototypicality preference, as happiness should eliminate the otherwise robust beauty-in-averageness effect. Importantly, happiness should not reduce familiarity per se. In fact, earlier studies reported that happiness increases familiarity of “new” but categorically-primed prototypes, enhancing false-memory effects, presumably because happiness promotes relation-based rather than item-specific processing (Storbeck & Clore, 2005).
Psychophysiological measurement
Testing our predictions required going beyond self-reports. Self-reports can reflect genuine “hot” reactions to stimuli, but also “cold” judgments of stimulus properties (e.g., pattern “goodness” or inferences about previous occurrence). Self-reports are also ill-suited for capturing early, spontaneous reactions, and may reflect later, deliberative processes. Therefore, in our main study, we also used psychophysiology.
To capture subtle changes in valence, we used facial electromyography (EMG) (e.g., Cacioppo, Petty, Losch, & Kim, 1986). EMG detects affective responses to familiarity, with repeated stimuli and prototypes eliciting greater EMG activity over the cheek “smiling” region, but not over the brow “frowning” region (Harmon-Jones & Allen, 2001; Winkielman & Cacioppo, 2001; Winkielman et al., 2006).
We also measured skin conductance responses (SCR), which reflect sympathetic arousal. The psychological meaning of SCR varies with context. When distinct and task-relevant, familiar stimuli can trigger SCRs (Tranel & Damasio, 1985; Morris, Cleary & Still, 2008). However, sometimes novel, surprising or fearful stimuli can too (Dawson, Schell, & Filion, 2000). Importantly, SCR provides information about physiological arousal responses that is separate from valence.
Paradigm
Our paradigm came from earlier research on prototypicality preferences (Winkielman et al., 2006). It uses abstract, random dot-patterns (Posner & Keele, 1968), which minimizes problems inherent to meaningful stimuli (e.g., greater symmetry or prior experience with prototypes; Rhodes, Sumich, & Byatt, 1999). In the first “exposure phase” participants view fourteen converging distortions (i.e., “seen members”) of a category prototype. Another, fifteenth distortion (i.e., “unseen member”) and the prototype are not shown. In the subsequent “test phase”, participants view six patterns: category prototype, seen member, unseen member, and three matched controls from an unexposed category. Participants rate each pattern on a continuous memory (confidence new/old) or a liking scale. See Figure 1, and the online supplement.
All data below are presented as a difference between the familiar versus unfamiliar (control) version of the pattern. This allows us to assess three related exposure effects. First, we compare prototypes of exposed (by presentation of converging distortions) categories to control patterns from unexposed categories. This assesses the ‘beauty-in-averageness’ effect and the prototype-memory illusion. Second, we compare “seen members” of exposed categories to members of the control category. This assesses the standard “mere-exposure” effect. Third, we compare “unseen members” of the exposed category to members of the control category. This assesses the “structural mere-exposure” effect (enhancement for categorically-related items).
Pretests: Experiments 1 and 2
We conducted two behavioral pretests, described in the supplement. Figure 2 shows the results. Experiment 1 established that our procedure most robustly enhances self-reports of familiarity (1A) and liking (1B) of prototypes from exposed categories under non-manipulated mood conditions. Experiment 2 tested the mood effects on self-reports of familiarity. It showed that our procedure enhances familiarity of “new” prototypes from exposed categories under both mood conditions. Interestingly, prototypes were rated as particularly familiar in the positive compared to the negative mood condition, extending reports that happiness increases false memory for verbal prototypes (Storbeck & Clore, 2005). These pretest-results set the stage for the main experiment exploring mood modulation of affective implications of familiarity.
Experiment 3: Psychophysiological examination
The main experiment examined mood effects on reactions towards familiar stimuli using self-reports and EMG as measures of affect, and SCR as a measure of familiarity. We predicted that compared to a positive mood, a negative mood would result in a stronger preference for familiar stimuli, especially the prototype.
Method
Sixteen undergraduates participated for extra credit. We first determined their resting, pre-manipulation (10-second period) physiological baselines. Next, we manipulated mood by instructing participants to focus on and describe a happy (or sad) autobiographical memory. We checked mood state on a 7-point scale. To maintain mood, participants subsequently listened to music (cf. Experiment 2). Again, we measured resting activity (10-second period), which served as a physiological mood-manipulation check (see supplement for details about data-recording and data-reduction).
Results and Discussion
Mood-manipulation check
On ratings, a Mood-Gender ANOVA revealed only a main effect, with participants reporting feeling better in the happy (M = 5.50) than sad (M = 3.75) condition, F(1,15) = 7.08, p < .01. Mood also influenced physiology. Specifically, for EMG, an ANOVA with Mood, Muscle (zygomaticus/corrugator), Time (10 seconds), and Gender, revealed a Muscle-Mood-Time interaction, F(9,108) = 2.29, p < .03, ηp2 = .16, driven by a significant Mood-Time interaction on the corrugator, F(9,108) = 3.76, p < .01. In the later five seconds, sad participants showed greater corrugator than zygomaticus activity, t(7), = 3.63, p < .01. Analyses of SCR, a non-specific measure of arousal, revealed an expected overall increase in response level after mood manipulation (p < .01), but no valence effects. In short, the mood manipulation was successful, as indicated by both self-report and physiological measures. Because there were no gender effects, this variable was dropped from further analyses.
Self-reports of liking
Figure 2 shows how familiarity influenced liking in different moods. ANOVA with Mood and StimulusType (prototype/seen/unseen) revealed a main effect of Mood, F(1,14) = 8.15, p < .02, ηp2 = .37, with familiar stimuli liked more in a sad mood. Next we focused on specific items. Sad participants robustly liked the old prototype more than the new prototype, t(7) = 3.11, p < .02. Critically, happiness eliminated this “beauty-in-averageness” effect (t < 1). Sad participants showed only weak effects on “seen items” and “unseen items” (ts = 1.7, p < .14). This was reflected in a linear trend -- prototype, seen, unseen, F(1,7) = 4.42, p = .07. There were no effects on seen and unseen items for happy participants (ts < 1).
EMG
A Mood-StimulusType-Time (1–5 seconds after stimulus onset) ANOVA revealed a significant 3-way interaction for cheek activity (smiling), F(8,112) = 2.10, p < .05, ηp2 = .13. Figure 3 shows a difference in zygomaticus response to old vs. control prototypes. The response was significantly larger in the sad than the happy mood condition as early as two seconds after stimulus onset, peaking at the fourth second and then disappearing by fifth second (ps < or = .05). There were no effects on “seen” and “unseen” items. As in earlier studies, no familiarity effects were obtained for frowning EMG – we return to this issue in the discussion.
SCR
A Mood-StimulusType-Time (1–5 seconds) ANOVA, revealed a main effect of mood, F(1,14) = 22.01, p < .001. Figure 4 (top vs. bottom) shows greater responses in the happy than sad mood condition; although this main effect is partly due to a decrease in SCR to familiar stimuli in the sad mood. The mood main effect was qualified by a Mood-Time interaction, F(4,112) = 3.62, p < .05, reflecting that mood differences on SCR became more pronounced after two seconds. There was no Mood-StimulusType interaction. However, Time interacted with StimulusType, such that after two seconds, prototypes elicited stronger responses than other patterns, F(8,112) = 2.02, p=.05.2
In sum, Experiment 3 showed that in a sad, but not in a happy, mood people prefer familiar patterns. The effects were most robust on prototypes which elicited higher judgments and more “smiling.” Interestingly, these hedonic changes occurred despite the fact that, as possibly suggested by SCR, the exposed patterns were more familiar in a happy than a sad mood.
General Discussion
We explored how mood modulates the value of familiarity. Experiment 1 shows that under non-manipulated mood conditions, participants prefer familiar stimuli, especially category prototypes. Experiment 3 suggests that the positivity of familiarity depends on mood. Sad participants preferred and “smiled to” familiar prototypes. Happiness eliminated this preference on self-reports and EMG measures. Importantly, this was not due to happiness reducing familiarity itself. First, happy participants showed robust familiarity effects, even rating prototypes as “older” than sad participants (Experiment 2). Second, one reading of SCR findings is that familiarity was higher in a happy than a sad mood (Experiment 3). In short, in happiness familiarity is present, but it just does not “glow” warmly.
Before we interpret these results theoretically, some findings deserve discussion. First, in a neutral3 (Experiment 1) and sad mood (Experiment 3), exposure influenced self-reported liking, with strongest effects occurring with prototypes, and weaker effects occurring with seen and unseen items. As in earlier studies, exposure influenced EMG responses only to prototypes, but not to seen and unseen members (Winkielman et al., 2006). Thus, in our paradigm the standard mere-exposure effect and structural mere-exposure effect were more fragile than the prototypicality effect. This might simply reflect that prototypicality is the strongest manipulation of familiarity or the underlying fluency. It is also possible that self-reports for seen and unseen members rely more on strategic inferences about category membership (Whittlesea, 2002). Second, as in earlier studies, the EMG effects for familiarity were limited to zygomaticus activity. This presumably indicates positivity of familiarity, rather than negativity of novelty (Harmon-Jones & Allen, 2001; Winkielman & Cacioppo, 2001; Winkielman et al., 2006). Third, although we interpret SCR responses in this context as familiarity, other interpretations are possible and in other contexts SCRs have been interpreted to indicate novelty, surprise, fear, excitement, and other mental states (Zajonc 1968; Dawson et al, 2000). More generally, feelings of familiarity and novelty reflect context-sensitive interpretations of non-specific arousal, which can be triggered by significant stimuli, both old and new (Goldinger & Hansen, 2005; Morris et al., 2008 Tranel & Damasio, 1985).
Turning to theoretical interpretations, our findings challenge proposals of a fixed familiarity-positivity link. Instead, familiarity’s value depends on affective context, consistent with tuning accounts of mood (e.g., Schwarz, 2002). If a mood signals an unsafe environment, familiarity is positive. If a mood signals a safe environment, familiarity loses its glow. Negative states strongly related to safety concerns, such as fear, might produce even stronger effects. Our results contain some (non-significant) hints that happiness boosts the value of novelty, perhaps supporting exploration via the “warm glow of the unfamiliar.” An alternative explanation of our results is that happiness makes it harder for the “warm glow” of familiarity to shine through the sunny affective background. Our EMG results, reflecting spontaneous and early responses, speak somewhat against this “subjective discriminability” interpretation, but future studies should test for salience of familiarity-induced affective changes. It is also worth exploring if enhancement of relational processing in happiness makes the prototype more salient and thus reduces its implicit exposure effects (Zajonc, 1968, Storbeck & Clore, 2005).
Our results resonate with proposals that hedonic reactions to familiarity are motivation-dependent (see Harmon-Jones & Allen, 2001 for correlational evidence). For example, participants rate fluent (and presumably familiar) stimuli higher in a prevention vs. promotion focus (Freitas, Azizian, Travers, & Berry, 2005). Their study left unanswered whether motivational focus changes fluency/familiarity itself, or its hedonic implications, but our results suggest the latter. More generally, current results highlight that hedonic implication of heuristic cues, such as familiarity, is context-dependent (Hertwig et al., 2008).
Finally, an exciting feature of our results is that happiness can reduce positivity of prototypicality. Thus, the otherwise robust “beauty-in-averages” effect appears sensitive to affective and motivational factors. This finding deserves exploration with faces and other objects that robustly show the classic effect. But for now, it appears that in a happy mood, prototypes are, well, just average.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
Support by EAESP and Radboud University travel grants to MdV, NWO VENI-grant 451-04-063 to RH and NSF BCS-grant 0350687 to PW.
Footnotes
The connection is robust that in reverse stimulus positivity enhances its familiarity (e.g., Monin, 2003; Phaf & Rotteveel, 2005).
Consistent with the notion that happiness loosens the familiarity-positive link, SCR (5 sec average) and liking of prototypes was positively correlated (p < .05) in happiness, but not in sadness (p > .2).
Non-manipulated moods are usually slightly positive on average, but less positive and more variable than experimentally induced positive moods.
Contributor Information
Marieke de Vries, Radboud University Nijmegen & Leiden University Medical Center.
Rob W. Holland, Radboud University Nijmegen
Troy Chenier, University of California, San Diego.
Mark J. Starr, University of California, San Diego
Piotr Winkielman, University of California, San Diego.
References
- Bornstein RF. Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968–1987. Psychological Bulletin. 1989;106:265–289. [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo JT, Petty RE, Losch ME, Kim HS. Electromyographic activity over facial muscle regions can differentiate the valence and intensity of affective reactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;50:260–268. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.50.2.260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clore GL, Schwarz N, Conway M. Affective causes and consequences of social information processing. In: Wyer RS, Srull TK, editors. Handbook of social cognition. 2. Vol. 1. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1994. pp. 323–418. [Google Scholar]
- Dawson ME, Schell AM, Filion DL. The electrodermal system. In: Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG, Berntson GG, editors. Handbook of psychophysiology. Vol. 2000. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2000. pp. 200–223. [Google Scholar]
- Deese J. On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1959;58:17–22. doi: 10.1037/h0046671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freitas AL, Azizian A, Travers S, Berry SA. The evaluative connotation of processing fluency: Inherently positive or moderated by motivational context? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2005;41:636–644. [Google Scholar]
- Goldinger SD, Hansen WA. Remembering by the seat of your pants. Psychological Science. 2005;16:525–529. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01569.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon PC, Holyoak KJ. Implicit learning and generalization of the "mere exposure" effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1983;45:492–500. [Google Scholar]
- Halberstadt J. The generality and ultimate origins of the attractiveness of prototypes. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2006;10:166–183. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1002_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halberstadt J, Rhodes G. The attractiveness of nonface averages: Implications for an evolutionary explanation of the attractiveness of average faces. Psychological Science. 2000;11:285–289. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harmon-Jones E, Allen JB. The role of affect in the mere exposure effect: Evidence from psychophysiological and individual differences approaches. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2001;27:889–898. [Google Scholar]
- Hertwig R, Herzog SM, Schooler LJ, Reimer T. Fluency heuristic: A model of how the mind exploits a by-product of information retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2008;34:1191–1206. doi: 10.1037/a0013025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langlois JH, Roggman LA. Attractive faces are only average. Psychological Science. 1990;1:115–121. [Google Scholar]
- Morris AL, Cleary AM, Still MS. The role of autonomic arousal in feelings of familiarity. Consciousness and Cognition. 2008;17:1378–1385. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monin B. The warm glow heuristic: When liking leads to familiarity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;85:1035–1048. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phaf RH, Rotteveel M. Affective modulation of recognition bias. Emotion. 2005;5:309– 318. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.3.309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Posner MI, Keele SW. On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1968;77:353–363. doi: 10.1037/h0025953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rhodes G, Sumich A, Byatt G. Are average facial configurations attractive only because of their symmetry? Psychological Science. 1999;10:52–58. [Google Scholar]
- Schwarz N. Situated cognition and the wisdom of feelings: Cognitive tuning. In: Feldman Barrett L, Salovey P, editors. The wisdom in feeling. New York: Guilford Press; 2002. pp. 144–166. [Google Scholar]
- Shore AN. Affect Regulation and the Origin of the Self: The Neurobiology of Emotional Development. Erlbaum; Hillsdale: 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Storbeck J, Clore GL. With sadness comes accuracy; with happiness false memory: Mood and the false memory effect. Psychological Science. 2005;16:785–791. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01615.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Titchener EB. A textbook of psychology. New York: Macmillan; 1910. [Google Scholar]
- Thornhill R, Gangestad SW. Human facial beauty: Averageness, symmetry and parasite resistance. Human Nature. 1993;4:237–269. doi: 10.1007/BF02692201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tranel D, Damasio AR. Knowledge without awareness: An automatic index of facial recognition by prosopagnosics. Science. 1985;228:1453–1454. doi: 10.1126/science.4012303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whittlesea BWA. False memory and the discrepancy-attribution hypothesis: The prototype-familiarity illusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2002;131:96–115. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.131.1.96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whittlesea BWA, Price JR. Implicit/Explicit memory versus analytic/nonanalytic processing: Re-thinking the mere exposure effect. Memory & Cognition. 2001;29:234–246. doi: 10.3758/bf03194917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winkielman P, Cacioppo JT. Mind at ease puts a smile on the face: Psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation leads to positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;81:989–1000. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winkielman P, Halberstadt J, Fazendeiro T, Catty S. Prototypes are attractive because they are easy on the mind. Psychological Science. 2006;17:799–806. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01785.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winkielman P, Schwarz N, Fazendeiro T, Reber R. The hedonic marking of processing fluency: Implications for evaluative judgment. In: Musch J, Klauer KC, editors. The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2003. pp. 189–217. [Google Scholar]
- Zajonc RB. Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1968;9:1–27. [Google Scholar]
- Zajonc RB. Mere exposure: A gateway to the subliminal. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2001;10:224–228. [Google Scholar]
- Zuckerman M. Psychobiology of personality. 2. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.