Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Brain Behav Immun. 2010 Jun 25;24(8):1294–1300. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2010.06.006

Evaluating the buffering vs. direct effects hypotheses of emotional social support on inflammatory markers: The Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis

Briana Mezuk 1, Ana V Diez Roux 2, Teresa Seeman 3
PMCID: PMC2949452  NIHMSID: NIHMS217815  PMID: 20600815

Abstract

Social support is associated with cardiovascular disease mortality, however the physiologic mechanisms underlying this relationship remains unspecified. This study evaluated the association of social support with inflammatory markers associated with cardiovascular risk: C-reactive protein (CRP), interleukin-6 (IL-6), and fibrinogen. We evaluated two competing models of the support-inflammation relationship: first, that low social support is directly associated with inflammation, and second, that high support acts to buffer the effect of stress on inflammation. Using data from the baseline interview of the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (N = 6,814, 53% female, age 45–84 years) we assessed the independent and interacting associations of social support and stress with inflammation. Social support was measured by the Emotional Social Support Index. Stressors in multiple domains (work, family, finances, interpersonal) were assessed. Serum CRP, IL-6, and fibrinogen were analyzed from fasting samples using high-sensitivity assays. Multivariate linear regression, including models stratified by gender and age group (45 – 64 and 65 – 84 years), was used to assess the direct and buffering relationships between social support, stress, and inflammation. In bivariate analyses low social support was associated with higher levels of all three markers. In adjusted models, low support was associated with higher lnCRP (B: 0.15, 95% CI: 0.01, 0.30) among men but not women. High social support buffered the relationship between stress and CRP among middle-aged women only (P for interaction 0.042). Overall, social support was only modestly associated with inflammation in this relatively healthy sample, and these relationships varied by age and gender.

Keywords: social support, inflammation, aging, gender differences

INTRODUCTION

Investigations of the interrelationships between the social environment and health have established that low social support, poor social integration and social isolation are associated with increased mortality, particularly from cardiovascular disease (CVD) (Berkman et al., 2003; Brummett et al., 2001; Frasure-Smith et al., 2000). Despite this epidemiologic evidence, the physiologic mechanisms underlying these relationships remain unspecified (Berkman et al., 2003; Knox & Uvnas-Moberg, 1998). Several researchers have speculated that alterations in immune function may be a mechanism by which psychosocial exposures, including social support, affect health. In particular, low social support has been associated with elevated levels of circulating inflammatory markers, including cytokines (e.g., interleukin-6 (IL-6), TNF-α), acute phase proteins (e.g., C-reactive protein), and clotting factors (e.g., fibrinogen) (Seeman, Berkman, Blazer, & Rowe, 1994; B. N. Uchino, Holt-Lunstad, Uno, Betancourt, & Garvey, 1999) that have been implicated in risk of CVD. However, not all studies have reported a significant association between social support and systemic inflammation (McDade, Hawkley, & Cacioppo, 2006); and even in those instances where support is associated with inflammation, it is unclear whether this relationship is due to a direct (i.e., independent) influence on physiology, or if support simply buffers (i.e., moderates) the effect of negative experiences (e.g., daily hassles, caregiving, stressful life events) which have direct effects on physiology (Cohen & Wills, 1985).

An integrative framework of the ways social experiences influence health should address the potential that the relationship between social support and physiologic indicators such as inflammation may vary by age and sex (Seeman and Crimmins 2001). As social roles evolve over the life course (i.e., marriage, parenthood), the influence of social experiences on health may also change. As with other psychosocial characteristics, social support may have cumulative effects over time, as well as acute effects in the context of events that affect social life (i.e., divorce, retirement) (Uchino et al. 1999). Also, many aspects of the receipt and provision of social support vary by sex (i.e., women are more likely to experience widowhood and are more likely to be caretakers than men) (Moen, 2001), and thus the implications for social relations to health may likewise differ for men and women (Ajrouch, Blandon, & Antonucci, 2005) Consistent with this general framework, there is suggestive evidence that the physiologic correlates of social integration and support vary by age and gender (Ford, Loucks, & Berkman, 2006; Hughes, 2007). For example, Ford and colleagues (2006) reported that low social integration was associated with elevated C-reactive protein among older men, but not among women or younger men (Ford et al., 2006). Loucks and colleagues (2006) also reported a significant association between CRP levels and social integration among older men but not older women (Loucks, Berkman, Gruenewald, Seeman 2006). However, relatively few studies have systematically examined variation by age and gender in this relationship, and most have only examined a single indicator of immune function. Thus, a systematic investigation of whether the relationship between social support and inflammatory indicators of CVD risk varies by age and gender is warranted.

The goal of this paper is to explore the relationship between social support and three markers of inflammation that have been implicated in CVD, interleukin-6, fibrinogen, and C-reactive protein, using the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA). The MESA sample was free of clinical atherosclerotic disease at baseline, and thus it is well-suited for examining the relationship between social support and physiologic changes isolated from the confounding effects of pre-existing CVD that may mask true associations or create spurious ones. While alterations in these inflammatory markers may not have immediate clinical significance, they may be early indicators of cardiovascular disease risk.

We investigated two main questions: (1) Is low social support directly associated with these inflammatory markers? and (2) Does high social support buffer the relationship between stress and inflammation? We hypothesized that if buffering is predominant, the association between stress and inflammation will be stronger among those experiencing lower social support than among those experiencing higher social support. However, if the direct model is predominant, the association of chronic stress with inflammation will not vary by level of support (and the association of social support with inflammation will not vary by stress level). In addition, we investigated whether these associations varied by age and gender.

METHODS

Sample

MESA is an on-going population-based multi-site study of the predictors of subclinical cardiovascular disease. Participants aged 45–84 years and free of clinical CVD at baseline (e.g., never experienced a heart attack, stroke, transient ischemia attack, heart failure, angina, atrial fibrillation, or cardiovascular procedures) were recruited from six study sites (Baltimore City and Baltimore County, MD; Chicago, IL; Forsyth County, NC; Los Angeles County, CA; New York City, NY; and St. Paul, MN). The final sample was 53% female, 40% non-Hispanic white, 30% African American, 20% Hispanic, and 10% Asian. Details of the sampling design and study procedures have been discussed previously (Bild et al., 2002). This report is restricted to the baseline MESA sample with complete data on the measures of social support, stress, and markers of inflammation (N = 6,153, 90% of the baseline sample). Participants excluded from the analysis (N = 611) were older than those included (mean age 66.0 vs. 61.9 years, p < 0.02), but otherwise did not differ from the remaining sample in terms of race, gender, education, employment, income, marital status, body mass index (BMI), smoking status, or experience of stress (data not shown).

This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at each site and all participants provided informed consent.

Independent variables

The primary independent variable was perceived emotional social support (ESS), indicated by the Emotional Social Support Index (ESSI, range: 6 – 30, Cronbach’s alpha: 0.88) which consists of six 5-point likert-scored items concerning availability of emotional support (e.g., Is there someone available to you whom you can count on to listen to you when you need to talk? 1 = None of the time, 2 = A little of the time, 3 = Some of the time, 4 = Most of the time, 5 = All of the time), with higher values indicating more available emotional support (ENRICHD, 2000; Mitchell et al., 2003). The ESSI has modest correlations with other indices of social support (i.e., Perceived Social Support Scale) (Mitchell et al., 2003). Because exploratory analyses indicated that the relationship between social support and inflammation was non-linear, the summed scale score was categorized to indicate low (score <12), moderate (score 12 – 24) or high (score ≥ 25) levels of ESS based on previous literature on social support and health in later life. In addition, each item was dichotomized as indicating low (score ≤ 2 or below) or moderate/high ESS (score ≥ 3).

Stress was measured by a composite of five items concerning contemporaneous, on-going stressors in five domains (i.e., personal health, health of a friend/relative, work-life, financial matters, and relationships with friends/relatives), with higher values indicating more stressors (Bromberger & Matthews, 1996). Each item that was endorsed was rated on a three-point scale in terms of stressfulness (1 = Not very stressful, 2 = Moderately stressful, 3 = Very stressful) (range: 0 – 5 very stressful events). The scores on this scale were strongly left-skewed, with 66% of the sample reporting only experiencing zero or one very stressful events, and therefore these responses were then combined into a dichotomous variable indicating presence of at least one very stressful event. In addition, to categorize stressors as either recent-onset or long-standing, participants were also asked if each of the stressors endorsed had been going on for 6 months or longer. If a participant responded positively to this duration question for at least on stressor, they were classified as experiencing chronic stress.

The main factors investigated as moderators of the relationship between ESS and the markers of inflammation were gender and age group, dichotomized as middle- age (45 – 64 years, N = 3543) and older adults (65 – 84 years, N = 2648).

Additional factors included as covariates were age (centered on the sample mean, 61.9 years), sex, race/ethnicity (modeled as an indicator variable with non-Hispanic whites as the reference group), educational attainment (dichotomized as at least some college versus high school or less (reference)), employment status (dichotomized as currently working versus other (reference)), annual gross household income (categorized as <$20,000 (reference), $20,000 to <$40,000, $40,000 to < $75,000, and ≥$75,000), and marital status (categorized as current married (reference), divorced/separated, never married, and widowed). Three additional factors known to influence inflammatory markers, current smoking status (with former/never smoker as the reference), BMI (kg/m2), and number of alcohol drinks per week were included. Two health conditions, hypertension and diabetes, were also included. Hypertension was dichotomously indicated as systolic blood pressure ≥ 140 mm Hg or diastolic ≥ 90 mm Hg from the average of three resting blood pressure readings, or use of antihypertensive medications. Diabetes was determined by the American Diabetes Association 2003 fasting glucose criteria (≥ 7.0mmol/L) or use of insulin or other hypoglycemic medications (American Diabetes Association, 2006). Consistent with previous MESA reports using these inflammatory markers, recent infection status was dichotomously coded based on self-report of cold or flu, sinus infection, urinary tract infection, tooth infection, bronchitis, or pneumonia in the preceding two weeks and accounted for in the regression models (Ranjit et al., 2007). Use of non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs (NSAIDs) was also recorded.

All variables were assessed at the baseline MESA interview concurrently with the measurement of serum inflammatory markers.

Dependent variables

The primary outcomes were three markers of inflammation, interleukin-6 (IL-6, pg/mL), C-reactive protein (CRP, mg/L), and fibrinogen antigen (mg/dL) which have been shown to be associated with risk of cardiovascular morbidity and mortality. We conducted separate analyses with each marker as an evaluation of the robustness of the relationships to inflammation more generally. The standardized procedures for the collection, processing, shipping, and storage of blood samples have been previously described (Bild et al., 2002). Fasting venous blood samples were taken from participants at baseline and processed at a centralized laboratory. IL-6 was measured using ultrasensitive enzyme-linked immunosorbant assay (ELISA) (R&D Systems, Minneapolis, MN). High-sensitivity CRP was measured by nephelometry (BNII nephelometer, Dade-Behring Inc., San Mateo, CA). Fibrinogen antigen was also measured by nephelometery (BNII N antiserum to human fibrinogen, Dade-Behring, San Mateo, CA).

Statistical analysis

Initially we used multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) to compare mean levels of the three inflammatory markers by emotional social support and investigate interactions between ESS, gender, and age group. We used the non-parametric Wilcoxon rank-sum test for linear trend to evaluate whether covariates varied across levels of ESS (categorized as low, moderate and high, described above). We used a series of linear regression models to assess the independent association between levels of ESS and the three markers of inflammation as the outcomes. Values of the inflammatory markers were log-transformed in order to normalize their distributions to better meet the model assumptions. High ESS was the most prevalent category (55.2% of the sample), and was used as the reference group for the analyses evaluating the direct associations between low and moderate emotional support and inflammation (Hypothesis 1). In order to evaluate whether high levels of ESS buffered the association between stress and inflammation, we dichotomized support as high versus moderate/low, with the latter as the reference group (Hypothesis 2). We evaluated the statistical significance of the interaction between this dichotomous indicator of high ESS with the dichotomous indicator of stress (both recent onset and chronic) in order to determine whether the relationship between social support and inflammation varied by exposure to stress.

The initial multivariate regression model was adjusted for age, gender, race/ethnicity, educational attainment, income, employment status, and marital status. The second model was additionally adjusted for additional behavioral and biomedical risk factors for inflammation including smoking status, alcoholic drinks per week, body mass index, hypertension, diabetes, NSAID use, and recent infection status. We also conducted post-hoc sensitivity analyses by (1) adjusting for depressive symptoms as indicated by the Centers for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CESD) scale (Radloff 1977; Ranjit et al., 2007); (2) adjusting for self-reported arthritis; and (3) excluding participants who reported a recent infection from the analysis, in order to better account for residual effects these exposures may have on inflammation. We examined whether the direct and stress-buffering association between support and inflammation varied by age and gender in stratified analyses. Any substantial differences were tested by including appropriate interaction terms in the regression models. All analyses were conducted using STATA v.9 software (StataCorp, College Station, TX). All p-values refer to two-tailed tests.

RESULTS

The sample overall reported high levels of emotional social support, and level of ESS was similar for men and women (M = 24.7 (SD = 5.2) for men, M = 23.8 (SD 5.2) for women out of a possible score of 30). Approximately one-third of the sample reported experiencing a very stressful event, whether recent onset or chronic. Women reported substantially more recent onset (M: 1.35 vs. 1.05, p<0.001) and chronic (M: 1.21 vs. 0.95, p<0.001) stress than men. Lower levels of support were associated with younger age, being male, having lower household income, marital status, being a current smoker, and prevalence of both recent onset and chronic stress. The three inflammatory makers were moderately correlated with each other (r2 = 0.41 to 0.46, all p < 0.001). Mean levels of all inflammatory markers were higher among those reporting low ESS relative to high, although this pattern was only statistically significant for fibrinogen (Table 1). Mean levels of inflammatory markers were higher in women than in men and increased with age.

Table 1.

Participant characteristics by emotional social support, Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (2000 – 2002)

Level of Emotional Social Support (ESS)
Overall Low Moderate High p-value*
N 6153 163 2593 3397
Emotional social support scale (mean, SD) 24.2 (5.23) 9.1 (1.6) 20.3 (3.4) 28.0 (1.8) <0.001
High recent onset stress (%) 19.1 38.7 23.6 14.9 <0.001
High chronic stress (%) 17.8 37.4 22.2 13.5 <0.001
Age, years (mean, SD) 61.9 (10.14) 62.0 (9.9) 61.4 (10.3) 62.2 (10.1) 0.009
Female (%) 52.4 50.3 57.3 48.9 <0.001
Race (%)
  White 39.3 35.0 39.8 39.1
  Black 27.4 27.6 26.3 27.9
  Hispanic 21.6 25.8 20.0 22.8
  Chinese 11.8 11.7 13.9 10.3 0.156
Education (%)
  High school or less 34.9 41.1 33.4 35.9
  At least some college 65.1 58.9 66.7 64.2 0.307
Income (%)
  <$20,000 22.4 35.2 24.7 20.0
  $20,000 to <$40,000 26.7 37.1 28.8 24.5
  $40,000 to <$75,000 10.1 7.6 10.3 10.0
  $75,000+ 40.8 20.1 36.1 45.5 <0.001
Marital status (%)
  Married 61.4 30.7 50.8 71.1
  Divorced/Separated 16.9 29.5 22.6 12.0
  Widowed 12.6 18.4 14.6 10.7
  Never married 9.1 21.5 11.9 6.3 <0.001
Hypertension (%) 44.4 49.1 42.4 45.5 0.11
Diabetes (%) 11.9 11.0 12.2 11.7 0.706
Alcoholic drinks per week (mean, SD) 2.4 (5.2) 2.6 (5.7) 2.3 (5.4) 2.4 (5.1) 0.516
BMI, kg/m2 (mean, SD) 28.3 (5.4) 28.9 (5.9) 28.1 (5.4) 28.4 (05.4) 0.248
Current smoker (%) 14.6 20.9 17.1 12.4 <0.001
Inflammatory markers (mean, SD)
  C-reactive protein (mg/L) 2.4 (2.2) 2.7 (2.1) 2.4 (2.2) 2.4 (2.2) 0.125
  Interleukin-6 (pg/mL) 1.5 (1.2) 1.7 (1.1) 1.5 (1.2) 1.5 (1.2) 0.077
  Fibrinogen (mg/dL) 345.4 (72.8) 358.7 (65.5) 345.1 (71.9) 344.7 (73.7) 0.035
*

p-value for Wilcoxon rank-sum test for linear trend across categories of ESS.

MANOVA indicated no relationship between the continuous measure of emotional support and the three inflammatory markers. However, levels of the markers did vary according to the categories of low, moderate, and high levels of ESS (F = 2.42, p<0.024), consistent with the exploratory analysis indicating the relationship was non-linear. Interaction terms between ESS levels and gender and ESS and age were not statistically significant (data not shown).

Evaluating the Direct Hypothesis

Bivariate regression indicated that low levels of ESS were associated with elevated levels of all three inflammatory markers in the sample overall (Tables 24), consistent with the direct hypothesis. After adjustment for demographic and socioeconomic characteristics, low ESS remained significantly associated with elevated CRP and fibrinogen, but not IL-6 (Model 1, Tables 24). After additional adjustment for health behaviors and biomedical factors, only CRP remained significantly associated with low support (Model 2, Tables 24). Stratification by sex indicated that the association of ESS with CRP was stronger and more likely to be statistically significant in men than in women (p for interaction 0.039 in fully-adjusted model). Within each gender there was no evidence that the association between low ESS differed significantly by age for CRP, IL-6 or fibrinogen (p for interaction > 0.05 for all markers).

Table 2.

Evaluating the direct model: Mean difference in log-IL-6 by categories of emotional social support

ESS
Moderate versus High
B (95% CI)
ESS
Low versus High
B (95% CI)
R2
Entire sample
  Unadjusted 0.01 (−0.02, 0.05) 0.13 (0.03, 0.24)* 0.001
  Model 1 0.02 (−0.02, 0.05) 0.09 (−0.01, 0.20) 0.097
  Model 2 0.02 (−0.02, 0.05) 0.06 (−0.04, 0.15) 0.230

Men
  Unadjusted 0.02 (−0.03, 0.07) 0.15 (0.01, 0.30)* 0.002
  Model 1 0.03 (−0.02, 0.08) 0.10 (−0.05, 0.24) 0.103
  Model 2 0.02 (−0.03, 0.07) 0.05 (−0.09, 0.18) 0.187
Women
  Unadjusted −0.01 (−0.05, 0.05) 0.11 (−0.03, 0.26) 0.001
  Model 1 0.01 (−0.04, 0.06) 0.07 (−0.07, 0.22) 0.102
  Model 2 0.02 (−0.03, 0.06) 0.06 (−0.07, 0.18) 0.276

Men, Age 45 – 64
  Unadjusted 0.02 (−0.05, 0.08) 0.14 (−0.05, 0.34) 0.001
  Model 1 0.03 (−0.04, 0.10) 0.08 (−0.12, 0.28) 0.050
  Model 2 0.02 (−0.04, 0.09) 0.07 (−0.12, 0.26) 0.152
Men, Age 65 – 84
  Unadjusted 0.05 (−0.02, 0.13) 0.17 (−0.04, 0.37) 0.003
  Model 1 0.02 (−0.05, 0.10) 0.12 (−0.09, 0.33) 0.076
  Model 2 0.01 (−0.07, 0.08) 0.03 (−0.17, 0.23) 0.133

Women, Age 45 – 64
  Unadjusted −0.02 (−0.08, 0.05) 0.20 (0.01, 0.39)* 0.003
  Model 1 0.01 (−0.06, 0.06) 0.13 (−0.06, 0.32) 0.105
  Model 2 0.01 (−0.04, 0.07) 0.09 (−0.07, 0.26) 0.300
Women, Age 65 – 84
  Unadjusted 0.03 (−0.04, 0.09) −0.01 (−0.22, 0.21) 0.001
  Model 1 0.02 (−0.05, 0.09) −0.02 (−0.24, 0.20) 0.065
  Model 2 0.01 (−0.05, 0.08) −0.02 (−0.22, 0.18) 0.209
*

p<0.05

Values of IL-6 have been log-transformed.

Model 1 adjusted for age, gender, race/ethnicity, educational attainment, income, employment status, and marital status.

Model 2 adjusted for Model 1 plus smoking status, alcohol use, body mass index, hypertension, diabetes, medication use, and recent infection.

Table 5.

Evaluating the direct model: Mean difference in log-Fibrinogen by categories of emotional social support

ESS
Moderate versus High
B (95% CI)
ESS
Low versus High
B (95% CI)
R2
Entire sample
  Unadjusted 0.01 (−0.01, 0.01) 0.04 (0.01, 0.08)* 0.001
  Model 1 −0.01 (−0.01, 0.01) 0.03 (0.01, 0.07)* 0.104
  Model 2 −0.01 (−0.01, 0.01) 0.03 (−0.01, 0.06) 0.178

Men
  Unadjusted −0.01 (−0.02, 0.01) 0.05 (0.01, 0.09)* 0.002
  Model 1 0.01 (−0.01, 0.02) 0.01 (−0.01, 0.02) 0.067
  Model 2 0.01 (−0.01, 0.02) 0.03 (−0.01, 0.08) 0.125
Women
  Unadjusted −0.07 (−0.02, 0.01) 0.04 (−0.01, 0.08) 0.001
  Model 1 −0.01 (−0.02, 0.01) 0.02 (−0.02, 0.07) 0.096
  Model 2 −0.01 (−0.02, 0.01) 0.02 (−0.02, 0.06) 0.183

Men, Age 45 – 64
  Unadjusted 0.01 (−0.02, 0.02) 0.03 (−0.03, 0.09) 0.001
  Model 1 0.01 (−0.01, 0.03) 0.02 (−0.04, 0.08) 0.055
  Model 2 0.01 (−0.01, 0.03) 0.01 (−0.04, 0.07) 0.125
Men, Age 65 – 84
  Unadjusted 0.01 (−0.02, 0.03) 0.08 (0.01, 0.14)* 0.004
  Model 1 −0.01 (−0.02, 0.02) 0.07 (0.01, 0.14)* 0.029
  Model 2 −0.01 (−0.03, 0.02) 0.06 (−0.01, 0.13) 0.081

Women, Age 45 – 64
  Unadjusted −0.01 (−0.02, 0.02) 0.06 (0.01, 0.12)* 0.002
  Model 1 0.01 (−0.02, 0.02) 0.04 (−0.01, 0.10) 0.091
  Model 2 0.01 (−0.01, 0.02) 0.03 (−0.02, 0.09) 0.206
Women, Age 65 – 84
  Unadjusted −0.01 (−0.03, 0.01) 0.01 (-0.07, 0.07) 0.001
  Model 1 −0.01 (−0.03, 0.01) −0.01 (−0.08, 0.06) 0.082
  Model 2 −0.01 (−0.03, 0.01) −0.01 (−0.08, 0.06) 0.139
*

p<0.05. Values of fibrinogen have been log-transformed.

Model 1 adjusted for age, gender, race/ethnicity, educational attainment, income, employment status, and marital status.

Model 2 adjusted for Model 1 plus smoking status, alcohol use, body mass index, hypertension, diabetes, medication use, and recent infection.

We conducted sensitivity analyses to determine whether depressive symptoms, arthritis, or recent infection were strong confounders in the stress-social support relationship. When CESD score was included in the models presented in Tables 35 the results did not change appreciably, and the interpretation that social support is only weakly directly associated with inflammation was upheld (data available upon request). Neither additional adjustment for arthritis, nor excluding participants who had experienced a recent infection (N = 1,467), substantially influenced the results (data available upon request)

Table 3.

Evaluating the direct model: Mean difference in log-CRP by categories of emotional social support

ESS
Moderate versus High
B (95% CI)
ESS
Low versus High
B (95% CI)
R2
Entire sample
  Unadjusted 0.01 (−0.04, 0.06) 0.21 (0.04, 0.37)* 0.001
  Model 1 0.01 (−0.04, 0.07) 0.20 (0.04, 0.36)* 0.099
  Model 2 0.02 (−0.03, 0.07) 0.15 (0.01, 0.30)* 0.226

Men
  Unadjusted 0.02 (−0.05, 0.10) 0.30 (0.08, 0.51)* 0.003
  Model 1 0.05 (−0.03, 0.12) 0.30 (0.08, 0.52)* 0.073
  Model 2 0.03 (−0.04, 0.10) 0.24 (0.04, 0.44)* 0.193
Women
  Unadjusted −0.06 (−0.14, 0.02) 0.09 (−0.14, 0.32) 0.001
  Model 1 −0.02 (−0.09, 0.06) 0.10 (−0.13, 0.33) 0.078
  Model 2 −0.01 (−0.07, 0.07) 0.07 (−0.15, 0.28) 0.229

Men, Age 45 – 64
  Unadjusted −0.01 (−0.11, 0.09) 0.27 (−0.03, 0.57) 0.002
  Model 1 0.06 (−0.04, 0.16) 0.24 (−0.05, 0.54) 0.084
  Model 2 0.04 (−0.05, 0.13) 0.26 (−0.01, 0.53) 0.222
Men, Age 65 – 84
  Unadjusted 0.08 (−0.04, 0.19) 0.33 (0.01, 0.65)* 0.005
  Model 1 0.04 (−0.07, 0.15) 0.37 (0.05, 0.70)* 0.064
  Model 2 0.02 (−0.09, 0.13) 0.25 (−0.06, 0.56) 0.163

Women, Age 45 – 64
  Unadjusted −0.08 (−0.19, 0.02) 0.17 (−0.14, 0.49) 0.003
  Model 1 −0.01 (−0.11, 0.10) 0.16 (−0.15, 0.47) 0.102
  Model 2 0.03 (−0.07, 0.12) 0.11 (−0.17, 0.38) 0.277
Women, Age 65 – 84
  Unadjusted −0.03 (−0.14, 0.08) −0.03 (−0.37, 0.32) 0.001
  Model 1 −0.03 (−0.14, 0.09) −0.01 (−0.36, 0.33) 0.084
  Model 2 −0.04 (−0.15, 0.07) −0.04 (−0.37, 0.28) 0.202
*

p<0.05

Values of CRP have been log-transformed.

Model 1 adjusted for age, gender, race/ethnicity, educational attainment, income, employment status, and marital status.

Model 2 adjusted for Model 1 plus smoking status, alcohol use, body mass index, hypertension, diabetes, medication use, and recent infection.

Table 5.

Evaluating the buffering model: Mean difference in log-CRP associated with emotional social support, chronic stress, and their interaction

Unadjusted
B (95% CI)
Model 1
B (95% CI)
Model 2
B (95% CI)
Entire sample
  High ESS (ref. Low/Moderate) 0.02 (−0.04, 0.07) −0.01 (−0.06, 0.05) −0.01 (−0.06, 0.05)
  High recent stress (ref. Low/Moderate) 0.24 (0.15, 0.34)* 0.10 (0.01, 0.19)* 0.04 (−0.05, 0.12)
  High ESS*High stress −0.09 (−0.22, 0.05) −0.06 (−0.20, 0.07) −0.06 (−0.18, 0.06)
Men
  High ESS (ref. Low/Moderate) −0.03 (−0.10, 0.05) −0.06 (−0.14, 0.02) −0.04 (−0.11, 0.04)
  High recent stress (ref. Low/Moderate) 0.15 (−0.01, 0.30) 0.06 (−0.09, 0.21) 0.02 (−0.11, 0.16)
  High ESS*High stress −0.05 (−0.26, 0.17) −0.02 (−0.23, 0.19) −0.03 (−0.23, 0.17)
Women
  High ESS (ref. Low/Moderate) 0.10 (0.01, 0.19) 0.05 (−0.04, 0.13) 0.03 (−0.06, 0.11)
  High stress (ref. Low/Moderate) 0.22 (0.10, 0.33)* 0.12 (0.01, 0.24)* 0.06 (−0.05, 0.17)
  High ESS*High stress −0.14 (−0.31, 0.04) −0.12 (−0.29, 0.05) −0.11 (−0.27, 0.05)
Men Age 45 – 64
  High ESS (ref. Low/Moderate) 0.01 (−0.10, 0.12) −0.06 (−0.17, 0.05) −0.05 (−0.15, 0.06)
  High recent stress (ref. Low/Moderate) 0.13 (−0.05, 0.32) 0.01 (−0.17, 0.19) −0.01 (−0.17, 0.17)
  High ESS*High stress −0.09 (−0.35, 0.18) −0.02 (−0.28, 0.25) −0.05 (−0.29, 0.19)
Men, Age 65 – 84
  High ESS (ref. Low/Moderate) −0.08 (−0.19, 0.03) −0.05 (−0.16, 0.07) −0.03 (−0.14, 0.08)
  High recent stress (ref. Low/Moderate) 0.28 (0.01, 0.55)* 0.19 (−0.09, 0.47) 0.10 (−0.17, 0.36)
  High ESS*High stress −0.04 (−0.42, 0.33) −0.07 (−0.45, 0.30) −0.03 (−0.39, 0.32)
Women, Age 45 – 64
  High ESS (ref. Low/Moderate) 0.14 (0.03, 0.26)* 0.05 (−0.07, 0.17) 0.03 (−0.07, 0.14)
  High recent stress (ref. Low/Moderate) 0.34 (0.18, 0.49)* 0.25 (0.10, 0.40)* 0.19 (0.05, 0.32)*
  High ESS*High stress −0.18 (−0.41, 0.06) −0.15 (−0.38, 0.07) −0.21 (−0.42, −0.01)*
Women, Age 65 – 84
  High ESS (ref. Low/Moderate) 0.05 (−0.08, 0.17) 0.03 (−0.09, 0.16) 0.03 (−0.09, 0.15)
  High stress (ref. Low/Moderate) 0.03 (−0.15, 0.21) −0.05 (−0.24, 0.13) −0.11 (−0.28, 0.06)
  High ESS*High tress −0.08 (−0.35, 0.19) −0.06 (−0.33, 0.21) 0.02 (−0.24, 0.27)
*

p<0.05. Values of CRP have been log-transformed.

Model 1 adjusted for age, gender, race/ethnicity, educational attainment, income, employment status, and marital status.

Model 2 adjusted for Model 1 plus smoking status, alcohol use, body mass index, hypertension, diabetes, medication use, and recent infection.

Evaluating the Buffering Hypothesis

Stress was consistently associated with elevated levels of all inflammatory markers in main effect models (data not shown). However, there was no support for the stress-buffering hypothesis in the full sample or sex stratified analyses, either in bivariate or multivariable regression models for any of the inflammatory markers (Table 5 shows results for CRP only. The other inflammatory markers revealed similar patterns and are not shown). In age- and sex-stratified analyses, interactions terms were consistently negative, consistent with a buffering, but the magnitude of the heterogeneity was small and it was not statistically significant except in the case of 45–64 year old women. In this group, there was evidence that high ESS buffered the association between high stress and CRP in the fully-adjusted models (p for interaction 0.042). There was no evidence of buffering for the other two markers among middle-age women. Additional analyses focused on chronic stress (lasting 6 months or longer) produced similar results (data not shown).

Sensitivity analysis: Marriage as a proxy for emotional social support

Marriage is a key source of social support, particularly for older adults, and ESS and levels of IL-6, CRP and fibrinogen varied significantly by marital status (all p < 0.001). We therefore conducted a series of post-hoc analyses to evaluate whether marital status moderated the association between stress and inflammation. In multivariate regression analyses, the relationship between stress and inflammation did not significantly differ by marital status in the sample overall, indicated by the non-significant interaction terms between marital status and stress (data not shown). Similar results were obtained in the age- and sex-stratified analyses.

DISCUSSION

The main finding from this study is that perceived emotional social support has little influence, either through direct or stress-buffering pathways, on inflammatory markers in this diverse sample of adults free of prevalent CVD. These findings are broadly consistent with recent reports that indicate only modest associations between perceived social support and integration with inflammation after accounting for perceived stress (McDade et al., 2006). There was modest evidence that the relationship between ESS and inflammation differed for men and women and by age. Among men, there was evidence to support the direct hypothesis of social support but only for CRP. In adjusted models there was evidence that high ESS buffered the association between high stress and CRP, but only for middle-age women. However, because these relationships were only observed with one of the three inflammatory markers examined, they should be interpreted with caution.

In fully-adjusted analyses, there was no evidence to support either the direct or buffering hypotheses for IL-6 or fibrinogen. The inconsistency in the relationships among ESS and the three inflammatory markers is noteworthy because it suggests that the association between social support and inflammation is not equivalent across physiologic systems. These markers were only moderately correlated, and each has different responsiveness to social stressors (Ranjit et al., 2007) and may be influenced by health behaviors, particularly smoking and obesity, to differing degrees (Ozbay, Fitterling, Charney, & Southwick, 2008; Piché et al., 2005).

These findings are somewhat consistent with previous reports suggesting that the relationship between social support and health differs for women and men over the life span (Akiyama & Antonucci, 1996; Loucks, Berkman, Gruenewald, & Seeman, 2006), but suggest that in adults free from major health problems such as CVD these differences are less pronounced. Several studies have reported gender differences in the associations between social integration with inflammatory markers (Ford et al., 2006; Loucks et al., 2006; Loucks et al., 2006) or measures of cardiovascular activity (Hughes, 2007). In particular, similar to our findings, several previous studies have found a direct association between measures of support or integration and inflammatory markers among men but not women. The reasons for this gender difference are unclear, although there is suggestive evidence that men and women differentially utilize social support as a coping strategy in the face of ongoing stress and may appraise stress differently (Chaplin, Hong, Bergquist, & Sinha, 2008; Gerin, Milner, Chawla, & Pickering, 1995; Unger, McAvay, Bruce, Berkman, & Seeman, 1999). Future research should work towards identifying the specific contexts and points in the life course in which gender differences in social support and health are expected to be most relevant.

The primary strength of this study is the sample composition and measures of inflammation. The MESA sample was free of CVD at baseline, thus reducing the likelihood that poor health status confounded the relationship between inflammation and support (i.e., individuals who report low support may do so because they have limited functioning due to health problems that are themselves associated with inflammation). Data on inflammatory markers were available on over 90% of the baseline sample and samples were collected using a standardized protocol in clinical settings.

The findings should be interpreted in light of the study limitations. Foremost, this study only explored the relationship between systemic inflammation and emotional social support, and it is possible that other aspects of social life, such as caregiving and social integration, which are also associated with morbidity and mortality, are more strongly associated with these markers. For example, a growing body of evidence suggests that perceived loneliness is associated with inflammatory markers (Hawkley & Cacioppo, 2003; Steptoe, Owen, Kunz-Ebrecht, & Brydon, 2004). These contrasting findings indicate that high levels of emotional social support cannot be conceptualized or treated as equivalent to low levels of loneliness (and vice versa) in terms of the relationship to physiology, despite similarities in these constructs. Also, other measures of social support, such as instrumental (i.e., assistance with specific tasks) or provision of support may also be more relevant to inflammation. In addition, the relatively high levels of ESS reported in the sample, and the relatively low variance in the inflammatory markers may have contributed to the failure to detect significant relationships. Finally, the measure of chronic stress may not have captured all meaningful aspects of the stress process that are relevant to health (e.g., we could not examine stress appraisal or coping, nor other aspects of stress exposure including daily hassles). The measure of chronic stress may be confounded with low ESS since some of the events included in this measure referenced social relationships, a salient limitation particularly in light of the cross-sectional nature of the study.

While the finding that ESS was generally not associated with inflammation, in either the directly or stress-buffering models, is surprising in light of the consistent epidemiologic relationships between social support and heath, these results indicate that other mechanisms may underlie the relationship between social support and health (Hawkley & Cacioppo, 2003; B. Uchino, 2006). For example, ESS may operate on health through other pathways such as improved access to services (i.e., having supportive ties may facilitate treatment seeking), through the relationship between ESS and health-related behaviors (i.e., smoking, diet, exercise), via psychological states such as depression, or through stress-linked biological mechanisms not involving inflammation. In addition, other dimensions of social life such as integration and isolation and perceived loneliness may be stronger predictors of inflammation (Ford et al., 2006; Seeman et al., 1994) than the ESS measure we studied.

Overall our results suggest that ESS is modestly associated with levels of CRP in men and that it may buffer the effects of stress in women. Given the large number of comparisons we performed these results need to be confirmed in other large samples. Consistently with prior work, these findings illustrate the utility of examining how factors such as gender and age influence the relationship between social life and health.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This research was supported by the Robert Wood Johnson Health and Society Scholars program and the Michigan Center for Integrative Approaches to Health Disparities, P60 MD002249 funded by the National Center on Minority Health and Health Disparities, and by contracts N01-HC-95159 through N01-HC-95169 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. The authors thank the other investigators, the staff, and the participants of the MESA study for their valuable contributions.

List of Abbreviations

BMI

Body mass index

CRP

C-reactive protein

CVD

Cardiovascular disease

ELISA

enzyme-linked immunosorbant assay

IL-6

Interleukin-6

MESA

Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis

NSAID

Non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drug

PSS

Perceived social support

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Competing interests: All authors declare they have no conflicting interests.

REFERENCES

  1. Ajrouch K, Blandon A, Antonucci TC. Social networks among men and women: The effects of age and socioeconomic status. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2005;60B:S311–S317. doi: 10.1093/geronb/60.6.s311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Akiyama H, Antonucci TC. Same-sex and cross-sex relationships. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Svi. 1996;51:374–382. doi: 10.1093/geronb/51b.6.p374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Diabetes Association. Diagnosis and classification of diabetes mellitus. Diabetes Care. 2006;29:S43–S48. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Berkman LF, Blumenthal J, Burg M, Carney RM, Catellier D, Cowan MJ, et al. Effects of treating depression and low perceived social support on clinical events after myocardial infarction: The enhancing recovery in coronary heart disease patients (ENRICHD) randomized trial. JAMA. 2003;289:3106–3116. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.23.3106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bild DE, Bluemke DA, Burke GL, Detrano R, Diez Roux AV, Folsom AR, et al. Multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis: Objectives and design. Am J Epidemiol. 2002;156:871–881. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bromberger JT, Matthews KA. A longitudinal study of the effects of pessimism, trait anxiety, and life stress on depressive symptoms in middle-aged women. Psychol Aging. 1996;11:207–213. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.11.2.207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brummett B, Barefoot J, Siegler I, Clapp-Channing N, Lytle B, Bosworth H, et al. Characteristics of socially isolated patients with coronary artery disease who are at elevated risk for mortality. Psychosom Med. 2001;63:267–272. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200103000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chaplin TM, Hong K, Bergquist K, Sinha R. Gender differences in response to emotional stress: An assessment across subjective, behavioral, and physiological domains and relations to alcohol craving. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32:1242–1250. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00679.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cohen S, Wills T. Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychol Bull. 1985;98:310–357. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. ENRICHDInvestigators. Enhancing recovery in coronary heart disease patients (ENRICHD): Study design and methods. Am Heart J. 2000;139:1–9. doi: 10.1016/s0002-8703(00)90301-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Ford ES, Loucks EB, Berkman LF. Social integration and concentrations of C-reactive protein among US adults. Ann Epidemiol. 2006;16:78–84. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2005.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Frasure-Smith N, Lesperance F, Gravel G, Masson A, Juneau M, Talajic M, et al. Social support, depression, and mortality during the first year after myocardial infarction. Circulation. 2000;101:1919–1924. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.101.16.1919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gerin W, Milner D, Chawla S, Pickering T. Social support as a moderator of cardiovascular reactivity in women: A test of the direct effects and buffering hypotheses. Psychosom Med. 1995;57:16–22. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199501000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness and pathways to disease. Brain Behav Immun. 2003;17:98–105. doi: 10.1016/s0889-1591(02)00073-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hughes B. Social support in ordinary life and laboratory measures of cardiovascular reactivity: Gender differences in habituation-sensitization. Ann Behav Med. 2007;34:166–176. doi: 10.1007/BF02872671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Knox SS, Uvnas-Moberg K. Social isolation and cardiovascular disease: An atherosclerotic pathway? Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1998;23:877–890. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(98)00061-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Loucks EB, Berkman LF, Gruenewald TL, Seeman TE. Relation of social integration to inflammatory marker concentrations in men and women 70 to 79 years. Am J Cardiol. 2006;97:1010–1016. doi: 10.1016/j.amjcard.2005.10.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Loucks EB, Sullivan LM, D'Agostino RB, Larson MG, Berkman LF, Benjamin EJ. Social networks and inflammatory markers in the Framingham heart study. J Biosoc Sci. 2006;38:835–842. doi: 10.1017/S0021932005001203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. McDade TW, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. Psychosocial and behavioral predictors of inflammation in middle-aged and older adults: The Chicago health, aging, and social relations study. Psychosom Med. 2006;68:376–381. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000221371.43607.64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Mitchell PH, Powell L, Blumenthal J, Norten J, Ironson G, Pitula CR, et al. A short social support measure for patients recovering from myocardial infarction: The ENRICHD social support inventory. J Cardiopulm Rehabil. 2003;23:398–403. doi: 10.1097/00008483-200311000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Moen P. The gendered life course. In: Binstock RH, George LK, editors. Handbook of aging: Social sciences. San Diego CA: Academic Press; 2001. pp. 176–196. [Google Scholar]
  22. Ozbay F, Fitterling H, Charney D, Southwick S. Social support and resilience to stress across the life span: A neurobiologic framework. Cur Psychiatry Rep. 2008;10:304–310. doi: 10.1007/s11920-008-0049-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Piché ME, Lemieux S, Weisnagel SJ, Corneau L, Nadeau A, Bergeron J. Relation of high-sensitivity C-reactive protein, interleukin-6, tumor necrosis factor-alpha, and fibrinogen to abdominal adipose tissue, blood pressure, and cholesterol and triglyceride levels in healthy postmenopausal women. Am J Cardiol. 2005;96:92–97. doi: 10.1016/j.amjcard.2005.02.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Radloff L. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
  25. Ranjit N, Diez Roux AV, Shea S, Cushman M, Seeman T, Jackson S, et al. Psychosocial factors and inflammation in the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. Arch Intern Med. 2007;167:174–181. doi: 10.1001/archinte.167.2.174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Seeman T, Berkman LF, Blazer D, Rowe J. Social ties and support and neuroendocrine function: The MacArthur studies of successful aging. Ann Behav Med. 1994;16:95–106. [Google Scholar]
  27. Seeman TE, Crimmins E. Social environment effects on health and aging; Integrating epidemiologic and demographic approaches and perspectives. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2001;954:88–117. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb02749.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Steptoe A, Owen N, Kunz-Ebrecht SR, Brydon L. Loneliness and neuroendocrine, cardiovascular, and inflammatory stress responses in middle-aged men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29:593–611. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00086-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Uchino B. Social support and health: A review of physiological processes potentially underlying links to disease outcomes. J Behav Med. 2006;29:377–387. doi: 10.1007/s10865-006-9056-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Uchino BN, Holt-Lunstad J, Uno D, Betancourt R, Garvey TS. Social support and age-related differences in cardiovascular function: An examination of potential mediators. Ann Behav Med. 1999;21(2):135–142. doi: 10.1007/BF02908294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Unger JB, McAvay G, Bruce ML, Berkman LF, Seeman T. Variation in the impact of social network characteristics on physical functioning in elderly persons: MacArthur studies of successful aging. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1999;54B:S245–S251. doi: 10.1093/geronb/54b.5.s245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES