Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2010 Nov;22(4):917–932. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000544

Substance Use Changes and Social Role Transitions: Proximal Developmental Effects on Ongoing Trajectories from Late Adolescence through Early Adulthood*

Jeremy Staff 1, John E Schulenberg 2, Julie Maslowsky 3, Jerald G Bachman 4, Patrick M O’Malley 5, Jennifer L Maggs 6, Lloyd D Johnston 7
PMCID: PMC2951309  NIHMSID: NIHMS217101  PMID: 20883590

Abstract

Substance use changes rapidly during late adolescence and early adulthood. Not coincidentally, this time in the life course is also dense with social role changes, as role changes provide dynamic context for individual developmental change. Using nationally representative, multiwave longitudinal data from age 18 to 28, we examine proximal links between changes in social roles and changes in substance use during the transition to adulthood. We find that changes in family roles, such as marriage, divorce, and parenthood, have clear and consistent associations with changes in substance use. With some notable exceptions, changes in school and work roles have weaker effects on changes in substance use compared to family roles. Changes in socializing (i.e., nights out for fun and recreation) and in religiosity were found to mediate the relationship of social role transitions to substance use. Two time- invariant covariates, socioeconomic background and heavy adolescent substance use, predicted social role status, but did not moderate associations, as within-person links between social roles and substance use were largely equivalent across groups. This paper adds to the cascading effects literature by considering how, within individuals, more proximal variations in school, work, and family roles relate to variations in substance use; and which roles appear to be most influential in precipitating changes in substance use during the transition to adulthood.

Keywords: social roles, substance use, intra-individual change, transition to adulthood

At the population level, late adolescence and early adulthood are marked by rapid changes in substance use (Bachman et al., 2002; Johnston, O’Malley, Bachman, & Schulenberg, 2009b). This time in the life course is also dense with regard to social role transitions (Shanahan, 2000), which provide the dynamic context for individual developmental change. Indeed, from a person–context interaction perspective, many of the individual-level changes can be viewed as a direct function of the social context transitions (Schulenberg, Maggs, & Hurrelmann, 1997); likewise, individual characteristics contribute to selection into different post–high school roles and contexts. Thus, it is not a coincidence that there are rapid changes in substance use at the same time in the life course as multiple social role changes. This emphasis on social role and context changes as they relate to changes in substance use and problem behaviors has a long history in the literature (e.g., Bachman, Wadsworth, O’Malley, Johnston, & Schulenberg, 1997; Bachman et al., 2002; Horney, Osgood, & Marshall, 1995; Leonard & Homish, 2005; Yamaguchi & Kandel, 1985), but several key questions remain concerning issues of role selection versus socialization, of causal connections, of cascading effects from earlier risk and protective factors, and of mediation and moderation effects.

Using national longitudinal data from the Monitoring the Future study (Johnston, O’Malley, Bachman, & Schulenberg 2009a; Johnston et al., 2009b), we examine proximal links between changing social roles and substance use during the transition to adulthood, investigating the extent to which licit and illicit drug use rises and falls in tandem with changes in social roles in the domains of work, family, and school (socialization hypothesis; see Yamaguchi & Kandel, 1985). Because both substance use and the timing of social role transitions vary consistently by gender, and like most prior research on drug use during the transition to adulthood, we estimate separate models for women and men. Unlike most prior research, however, we eliminate potential bias due to unmeasured, stable individual differences by estimating the relationships of within-individual change in substance use to changes in work, school, and family roles (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). Thus, our analysis strategy controls for all stable individual differences that could generate spurious relationships between social roles and substance use (selection hypothesis), as well as childhood and adolescent risk factors (e.g., low socioeconomic origins, lack of school readiness, early problem behaviors, academic failure, delinquent peer associations) that may have cumulative and indirect effects on long-term adult role attainment and substance use (i.e., cascading hypothesis; Masten et al., 2005). In addition, we contribute to the relevant literature by not only considering how, within individuals, variation in school, work, and family roles and contexts relates to change in substance use, but also considering which of the roles and contexts are most consistently and uniquely predictive of changes in substance use. In weaving together concepts from life course development and developmental psychopathology, we hope to provide a fuller understanding of the course and causes of substance use and abuse across the transition to adulthood.

Social Role Transitions and Experiences and Substance Use

Rates of substance use and heavy drinking increase dramatically during the teenage years and young adulthood, and then decline into adulthood (e.g., Bachman et al., 1997, 2002; Johnston, et al., 2009b; see Chassin, Hussong, & Beltran, 2009 for a review). Earlier initiation and heavier use of alcohol and drugs during adolescence and young adulthood predict a higher likelihood of lifetime substance use and alcohol problems (e.g., Zucker, 2006), and pose a potential threat to successful negotiation of developmentally normative tasks during this transition (Krohn, Lizotte, & Perez, 1997; Maggs & Schulenberg, 2005; Newcomb & Bentler, 1988). However, most adults—even those who began substance use early—do not continue these harmful drinking and drug use patterns (Jessor, Donovan, & Costa, 1991). This leads to the question, what are the specific social role changes that contribute to rises and falls in substance use during the transition to adulthood?

Post–secondary school attendance predicts higher rates of alcohol and substance use during the transition from adolescence to young adulthood (Bachman et al., 1997, 2002; O’Malley & Johnston, 2002; Paschall, Bersamin, & Flewelling, 2005), a relationship that reflects college students’ delayed family role transitions (e.g., marriage, parenthood), unique living arrangements (i.e., residence in a dormitory, fraternity, or sorority), and positive attitudes toward drug and alcohol use (Bachman et al., 2002; McMorris & Uggen, 2000; Schulenberg & Maggs, 2002). Importantly, young people who postpone heavy alcohol use (and other drug use) until college also tend to relinquish it later as they get married and have children (Burton, Johnson, Ritter, & Clayton, 1996; Chilcoat & Breslau, 1996; Gotham, Sher, & Wood, 2003; Mudar, Kearns, & Leonard, 2002; Sher, Bartholow, & Nanda, 2001). Bachman and colleagues (2002) found that family roles—such as engagement, marriage, pregnancy, and parenthood—predicted reduced use of alcohol, cigarettes, and illicit drugs in young adulthood, as “responsibilities to spouses and children appear to crowd out these kinds of recreation most likely to encourage substance use” (2002: p. 219). The authors showed that time spent hanging out with friends, religiosity, and positive attitudes toward alcohol use explained some, but not all, of the relationship between changing family, work, and school roles and alcohol use between ages 18 and 24. Nonetheless, these prior studies suggest that substance use declines with theses role transitions in part because the demands and constraints of adult work and family roles are generally incompatible with continued substance use.

Yet, when assessing links between role transitions and substance use, prior research has given insufficient attention to the quality of social roles. Age-graded social control theory (Sampson & Laub, 1993; Laub & Sampson, 2003) emphasizes the importance of work, family, and school roles that facilitate socialization and restrain substance use and deviant behaviors. In particular, high-quality employment and work stability indicate adult social bonds that may reduce substance use, law violation, and other problem behaviors in adulthood (Laub & Sampson, 2003; Uggen, 1999). By contrast, employment entailing low occupational status, part-time work hours, and temporary work arrangements may be less likely to lower substance use or reduce heavier use compared to work in more rewarding and permanent positions. Longitudinal samples extending into adulthood are especially needed to assess work effects on substance use given the lengthening of the school-to-work transition, as young people today are increasingly likely to continue their formal education into adulthood and delay the onset of career acquisition (Kerckhoff, 2003; Mortimer, Vuolo, Staff, Wakefield, & Xie, 2008). A key aim of our paper is thus not only to examine hypotheses about the effects of role transitions on substance use, but also to consider the quality of some of these age-graded social roles. For instance, do part- and full-time work have the same association with drug and alcohol use? Does the type of job matter?

Assessing the effects of family roles on substance use over the long-term is increasingly important as the timing of marriage and fertility behavior both lengthens and diversifies. For instance, among recent cohorts of young people, marriage is delayed, cohabitation and nonmarital childbearing are more common, marital instability has risen, and increasing numbers of young people are not residing with their children (Amato, Booth, Johnson, & Rogers, 2007; Bumpass & Lu, 2000; Ventura & Bachrach, 2000). Moreover, though research has shown that married young adults are less likely to use licit and illicit substances than young adults who remain single, the transition from marriage to divorce coincides with increased substance use (Bachman et al., 1997, 2002). The recent increases in cohabitation and age of first marriage, the high divorce rate, and declining remarriage rate (Amato et al., 2007) necessitate longitudinal samples of young people followed into adulthood to better understand how changes in fertility behavior and union formation and dissolution (i.e., divorce, separation, and widowhood) affect long-term patterns of substance use.

Role Selection, Cascading Effects, and Developmental Transitions

Transitions to adult work and family roles may reduce substance use and abuse because the constraints of adult roles are generally incompatible with continued substance use. For instance, role socialization (Yamaguchi & Kandel, 1985) suggests that alcohol and other drug use may decline with certain adult role transitions (i.e., full-time student to full-time worker, nonparent to parent, residential independence to cohabitation/marriage) by limiting unstructured leisure activities with friends, by providing norms and expectations against drinking and drug use, and by increasing conformity to conventional adult social norms (e.g., age-graded social control theory; Laub & Sampson, 2003). On the other hand, role selection (Yamaguchi & Kandel, 1985) highlights selectivity issues regarding the relationship between social roles and substance use. For instance, precocious development theory (Newcomb & Bentler, 1988) contends that impulsive young people are more likely to use substances and select into adultlike situations that are more compatible with continued substance use, such as leaving school early, acquiring full-time work, and moving away from the parental home. Willis (1977) identified a similar process among working class boys in England, as heavy drinking was viewed by some of the youth as a claim to a more mature status that was more compatible with full-time work than continued schooling. Likewise, problem behavior theory (Jessor & Jessor, 1977) views heavy drinking in adolescence as part of a syndrome of problem behaviors among transition-prone youth who are striving for a more “adultlike” status. Importantly, each of these theoretical perspectives highlights unobserved or difficult to measure childhood and adolescent risk factors (e.g., low self-control, transition proneness, working-class masculinity) that are generally stable over time and predict both the onset and stability of adult role acquisition and substance use. Despite this rich theoretical tradition of role selection, little research has adequately controlled for such unmeasured, time-stable factors that no doubt affect the timing, sequencing, and stability of social roles and substance use behaviors.

The literature on cascading effects, where earlier difficulties in one domain (e.g., peer relations) contribute to subsequent difficulties in another domain (e.g., substance use), similarly highlights early childhood risk factors that are central to understanding how psychopathology accumulates across the life course (e.g., Dodge et al., 2008; Masten et al., 2005). For instance, Dodge and colleagues (2008) demonstrate how an adverse economic and social context in childhood has cascading effects on adolescent violence through a sequence of diminished academic achievement, disengagement from school, reduced parental attachment and monitoring, and engagement with delinquent peers. Though economic disadvantage in childhood is not a consistent predictor of substance use and problem behaviors in adolescence (Wiles et al., 2007; Wright et al. 1999), youth from families of lower socioeconomic status are likely to enter the labor force at a younger age, have less connection to the educational system, and have greater job instability and nonstandard work arrangements than their more advantaged peers (Kerckhoff, 2002). Thus, adolescents from more advantaged social class origins may complete college and obtain good jobs regardless of periodic or even serious alcohol and substance use infractions (Hagan, 1991), whereas adolescents whose parents have working-class jobs may be more vulnerable to the risks of heavy adolescent drinking and substance use because they have less access to psychosocial and financial resources (Hagan, 1993; Newcomb & Bentler, 1988). Other research has shown that difficult temperament, low cognitive ability, behavior problems, and lower school achievement in first grade predict both adult alcohol abuse and dependence (Zucker, 2006), as well as marriage instability, educational failure, and labor market problems (Caspi et al., 1998; Flora & Chassin, 2005; Kandel et al., 1986). These studies highlight how pivotal childhood and adolescent resources and experiences can contribute, in a cascading fashion, to the timing and ordering of adult role transitions and long-term trajectories of substance use.

Consistent with a cascading effects model, earlier characteristics and events clearly contribute in important ways to later substance use (e.g., Dodge et al., 2009), but more developmentally proximal effects also prove to be important, especially during the transition to adulthood (Schulenberg & Zarrett, 2006; Laub & Sampson, 2003). This global transition is rich with new opportunities and social contexts that can provide entries into and exits out of substance use, and the successes and difficulties one experiences with the many developmental tasks during this period can contribute to continuities and discontinuities in ongoing trajectories of substance use, health, and well-being (e.g., Masten et al. 2005; Schulenberg, Bryant, & O’Malley, 2004; Schulenberg, Sameroff, & Cicchetti, 2004). To some extent, cascade models reflect continuity (e.g., bad circumstances contribute to subsequent bad circumstances, etc.); oftentimes, however, social role transitions can interrupt the ongoing person–context interactions that underlie cascading effects and thus serve as turning points in the behavioral trajectory of interest (Schulenberg & Maslowsky, 2009). This suggests that social role transitions provide a needed vantage point for testing for continuities and discontinuities in ongoing and even cascading behavioral trajectories (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2002; Rutter, 1996).

The Current Study

We use the national Monitoring the Future (MTF) multiwave longitudinal data to examine how changes in social roles correspond with changes in substance use during the transition to adulthood. We advance the relevant literature in at least three ways. First, and most importantly, we are able to isolate more definitively the effects of role socialization on alcohol and substance use by controlling for time-stable unobserved factors, such as self-control, temperament, and transition proneness, that may predict both adult role transitions and substance use. At the same time, we also contribute to the cascading effects literature by considering how, within individuals, variations in social roles and contexts relate to variations in substance use. Because each individual serves as his or her own control in the analyses, we are able to make stronger statements about causal connections than is typically possible from correlational longitudinal research. Often, within cascading effects analyses that focus on cross-lag effects, only stability within targeted characteristics is controlled, without addressing the possibility that the multitude of unmeasured and unobserved characteristics may set the stage for the smaller set of cascading effects under consideration. By controlling for all stable individual differences, we are able to rule out a multitude of selection effects, including early childhood and adolescent risk factors that may have long-term effects in adulthood. We control for selection into and out of social roles and can therefore make statements about the extent to which the given connection with changes in substance use is due to social role transition influences.

Second, we consider an extensive and diverse set of family, school, and work roles, allowing us to determine the relative power of the various roles in contributing to changes in substance use. In particular, taking advantage of the large sample covering ages 19–28, we account for diverse types of union formation (e.g., marriage as well as various combinations of engagement and cohabitation), fertility behaviors (e.g., pregnancy, resident vs. nonresident parenthood), and singlehood (e.g., never married vs. currently divorced, separated, or widowed; residing alone vs. living with roommates). We also account for diversity in school roles and educational attainments (e.g., part-time vs. full-time status; residence in a dormitory, fraternity, or sorority; baccalaureate degree attainment vs. college dropout), as well as work roles (full-time vs. part-time status; homemaker) and job types (e.g., military, professional, nonprofessional). In isolation, most of these role transitions and experiences likely relate to changes in substance use, but they are typically intertwined, indicating the need to consider them together to determine which are more strongly related to changes in substance use. For example, research shows that, relative to marriage, cohabitation has little effect on substance use (Duncan, Wilkerson, & England, 2006), but cohabitation in the context of an engagement provides norms and expectations that are similar to marriage in reducing substance use (Bachman et al. 1997). Work roles may have little effect on substance use when we consider whether the young person is also attending school full-time or residing with his or her parents. The effects of some of these role combinations on substance use may differ by gender. For instance, full-time work may reduce substance use among men because it corresponds with family formation, whereas for women, full-time work may not have this effect because it is associated with reduced fertility behaviors (Budig, 2003). We examine all of these social roles alone and simultaneously in order to consider their unique and combined effects.

Third, we include potential moderators and mediators of the relationship of social role transitions to substance use, both of which have implications for understanding cascading effects. Consideration of important moderators can illustrate variations in social role transition rates and effects on substance use, suggesting variations in underlying cascading effects. In addition to including gender as a predefined moderator (discussed above), we examine senior-year cohort, background socioeconomic status (SES), and adolescent drug use. Data consist of 22 senior-year cohorts (1977–1998) from the MTF panel data, followed to age 27–28, allowing us to determine whether social role effects on substance use have varied across two decades characterized by important changes in timings of social roles and by extensive variation in substance use rates (Bachman et al., 2002; Johnston et al., 2009a, 2009b). Both background SES and adolescent drug use are important markers of constraints and opportunities for young adult roles and experiences (e.g., Maggs & Schulenberg, 2005; Newcomb & Bentler, 1988; Zucker, Donovan, Masten, Mattson, & Moss, 2008), and we are interested in how they relate both to the social role transition experiences as well as to how the social role transitions contribute to changes in drug use. As for mediators, we examine two that are potentially important mechanisms for social role transition effects—socializing (i.e., evenings out for fun and recreation) and religiosity (Bachman et al., 2002). These mediators allow for the consideration of intra-individual cascading effects – that is, how within-individual variations in social roles relate to variations in these two constructs, which in turn relate to changes in substance use. By examining the effects of these potential moderators and mediators in a large, national multiwave longitudinal sample, we highlight their contributions to the cascading process that leads from social role transitions to changes in substance use.

METHOD

Sample

The data come from the Monitoring the Future project, which is conducted by the Institute for Social Research at the University of Michigan. Each year, large, nationally representative samples of about 16,000 12th graders are drawn from about 135 public and private high schools (Johnston et al., 2009a, 2009b). We use longitudinal data from 22 consecutive 12th-grade cohorts who were initially sampled in 1976 through 1997. Approximately 2,400 high school seniors each year are selected for biennial follow-ups using mailed questionnaires. The biennial follow-up surveys begin one year after high school for one random half of each cohort and two years after high school for the other half. For purposes of these analyses, the two halves were combined. Individuals who used illicit drugs in high school were oversampled for the follow-up surveys. Corrective weighting was used in the analyses to compensate for the effects of unequal probabilities of selection. Note that additional information about the MTF design and methods is available in Johnston et al., 2009a, and on the MTF Web site at www.monitoringthefuture.org.

Our longitudinal analysis is based on young adults who were in the sample during 12th grade and who completed at least one follow-up survey (at ages 19–20, 21–22, 23–24, 25–26, or 27–28). We restrict our longitudinal sample to those young people who provided information on the explanatory and outcome variables, though our strategy of analysis does not require observations across all waves of the study (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). For our analyses, we follow 21,868 women and 18,555 men over approximately 125,000 respondent/wave measurements.

Measures

Substance Use

To assess the robustness of the effects of school, work, and family roles on substance use, we consider a range of self-reported substance use indices. The measures of alcohol, marijuana, and cocaine use range on nine-point scales from “no use in past year” to “40 or more occasions in the prior month.” Because less than one percent of respondents had used cocaine more than 10 times in the past month, we constrained this variable to range on a six-point scale from “no use in past year” to “10 or more times in the past month.” The measure of heavy drinking is based on the number of times respondents reported having five or more drinks in a row during the past two weeks. This measure ranges on a six-point scale from “none” to “ten or more times.” The cigarette use measure indicates how frequently the respondent smoked cigarettes in the past 30 days (ranging on a six-point scale from “not at all” to “about one and one-half packs or more per day”). These items have been shown to have high validity and reliability (Johnston et al., 2009a). The measures of substance use had skewed distributions. Because the data are skewed, we transformed all of our outcome variables by first adding a constant of 1 to each variable, and then using the natural logarithm (Kirk, 1994).1

Table 1 provides descriptive statistics by gender for the substance use measures, as well as all our time-varying measures that we describe next. These descriptive statistics are based upon the pooled data set encompassing the five waves of data from ages 19–20 to 27–28.

Table 1.

Descriptive Statistics

Men Women
Mean SD Min value Max value Mean SD Min value Max value
Substance Use
 Alcohol (natural logarithm) 1.57 0.66 0 2.3 1.35 0.66 0 2.3
 Heavy alcohol (natural logarithm) 0.57 0.62 0 1.79 0.29 0.49 0 1.79
 Cigarette (natural logarithm) 0.42 0.65 0 1.79 0.39 0.62 0 1.79
 Marijuana (natural logarithm) 0.59 0.83 0 2.3 0.39 0.69 0 2.3
 Cocaine (natural logarithm) 0.18 0.45 0 1.79 0.11 0.36 0 1.79
Family Roles
 Married 26% 0 1 34% 0 1
 Separated, divorced, widowed 3% 0 1 4% 0 1
 Cohabiting (and engaged) 3% 0 1 4% 0 1
 Not cohabiting (and engaged) 4% 0 1 5% 0 1
 Cohabiting (not engaged) 7% 0 1 8% 0 1
 Pregnant (spouse/partner) 5% 0 1 6% 0 1
 Parent (not residing with child) 4% 0 1 3% 0 1
 Parent (residing with child) 14% 0 1 24% 0 1
 Residing with roommate(s) 36% 0 1 32% 0 1
 Reside with parent(s) 30% 0 1 27% 0 1
 Residing alone 11% 0 1 8% 0 1
Student Roles
 Non-student 63% 0 1 64% 0 1
 Part-time student 10% 0 1 10% 0 1
 Full-time in dormitory 7% 0 1 7% 0 1
 Full-time in fraternity/sorority 1% 0 1 <1% 0 1
 Full-time in other residence 20% 0 1 19% 0 1
 BA/BS degree earned 25% 0 1 25% 0 1
Work Roles
 Full-time 61% 0 1 50% 0 1
 Part-time 16% 0 1 22% 0 1
 Nonprofessional job 63% 0 1 62% 0 1
 Professional job 15% 0 1 16% 0 1
 Military 5% 0 1 1% 0 1
 Homemaker 7% 0 1
Mediators
 Socializing 3.1 1.28 1 6 2.72 1.23 1 6
 Religiosity 2.44 0.92 1 4 2.7 0.9 1 4

 Number of Occasions 54,789 71,600
 Number of Respondents 18,555 21,868
Family Roles and Experiences

At each wave, respondents were asked “what is your present marital status?” We created six dummy variables indicating whether the respondent was single, married, engaged and cohabiting, engaged and not cohabiting, cohabiting but not engaged, or separated/divorced/widowed at the time of survey administration. Respondents were also asked at each wave: “which of the following people live in the same household with you (mark all that apply)?” We used these variables to distinguish when respondents were living alone versus when they were residing with roommates other than their spouse, children, parents, or intimate partners. We also included a measure of whether the respondent resided with his/her parent(s). Regarding fertility, we included measures indicating whether the respondent had any children (including stepchildren and adopted children), and if so, whether they were currently residing with their child. We also created a measure indicating whether the respondent (or the respondent’s spouse or girlfriend) was currently pregnant. To best capture family complexity, only a few of the family roles listed above were coded as mutually exclusive and exhaustive [i.e., single, engaged (irrespective of cohabitation), married, and separated/divorced/widowed], which is important given that simultaneous occupation of many family roles (e.g., marriage and parenthood, singlehood and residing with parents; union dissolution and non-residential parenthood), as well as the blurred boundaries between some family transitions (e.g., cohabitation and singlehood; see Manning & Smock, 2005; Brown & Manning, 2009)

Table 1 shows the proportion of the five follow-up waves (from ages 19–20 to 27–28) in which each social role was reported by men and women. Note that these proportions are based upon number of observations, not individuals, and that one individual can contribute from 1 to 5 observations. Overall, men were less likely to report being married (26% vs. 34%, respectively), residing with their child (14% vs. 24%), and cohabiting (10% vs. 12%) than women during the observation period. Instead, men were more likely than women to report being single (64% vs. 53%), residing with their parents (30% vs. 27%), residing with roommates (36% vs. 32%), or residing alone (11% vs. 8%).

School Roles and Experiences

Respondents at each follow-up survey were asked “during March of this year, were you taking courses at any school or college?” We used this measure to distinguish those who were attending school part-time or full-time from others. As shown in Table 1, respondents were full-time students in approximately one third of the assessed waves between ages 19 and 28, whereas they were part-time students at only 10 percent of the data occasions. Among respondents who were full-time students, we further distinguished whether or not they were residing in a dormitory, in a fraternity or sorority, or in some other type of residence. During the first follow-up survey, almost all of the full-time students resided in a dormitory, whereas in subsequent surveys full-time students branched out into various residential arrangements, including fraternities and sororities. In addition, we created a time-varying measure indicating whether they had received a baccalaureate degree or higher. Respondents held a baccalaureate degree or higher in approximately one quarter of the follow-up observations.

Work Roles and Experiences

During each wave respondents were asked “which best describes your employment during the first full week in March?” We distinguished respondents who were not working from those working part-time or full-time. We also distinguished whether the respondent worked in a professional job (e.g., lawyer, physician, dentist, scientist, college professor, engineer, computer programmer, registered nurse, librarian, teacher, social worker, etc.) versus a nonprofessional job. Women reported working as a homemaker in approximately 7% of the waves. We did not distinguish homemaker status among men because only a handful of men identified this role. Finally, we created a measure of whether the respondent was in the military. In approximately 5% of the observations, men reported being in the military, compared to 1% of women; these differences reflect the different proportions of men and women who enlist, rather than differences in average length of service.

Mediating Variables: Socializing and Religiosity

As potential mediating variables for the effects of social roles and experiences on substance use, we include time-varying measures of socializing and religiosity. Socializing is a measure of the frequency of spending evenings out for fun and recreation (ranges on six-point scale from “less than one evening per week” to “six or seven evenings per week”). Religiosity is a composite measure of how often the respondent attended religious services (ranging on a four-point scale from “never” to “about once a week or more”) and the importance of religion in their life (ranging on a four-point scale from “not important” to “very important”). These two items were averaged (Cronbach’s α = .80).

Moderators: Historical Context, Adolescent Substance Use, and Socioeconomic Background

To determine whether social role effects on substance use have varied across two decades, we include a measure of cohort year, defined on year of high school graduation (a linear measure ranging from 1976 to 1997). To assess whether the effects of social roles vary by adolescent substance use, we included a dichotomous measure of whether the respondent had used marijuana at some point during the prior 12 months. We also included a measure of parental education to assess whether the effects of social roles vary by socioeconomic background. Parental educational attainment is based on the average educational level of the mother and father (ranging from 1=completed grade school or less to 6=graduate or professional school after college). The average educational level across all respondents’ parents is a high school degree.

Analytic Plan

We use a two-level hierarchical model (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002) to estimate relationships of within-individual change in alcohol and substance use to within-individual change in work, school, and family roles and other explanatory variables. This strategy of analysis has two primary advantages. First, this model more definitively addresses the selection versus socialization hypotheses (Yamaguchi & Kandel, 1985) by using an analysis of within-individual changes to control for all time-stable individual differences (see Halaby, 2003). Second, the hierarchical model is suitable for data sets in which some people provide data on some but not all occasions (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). Thus, respondents who do not participate in all waves of the study still contribute to the analyses.

In our model, we treat multiple observations over time (i.e., person-years, level-1) as nested within individuals (level-2). In the first level of the hierarchical model, we include time-varying measures of substance use as the criterion variable, as well as variables referencing time and time-varying covariates (e.g., family, work, and school roles) as predictors. In the second level, the level-1 parameters become outcome variables. Thus, the level-2 parameters address between-person variation in associations between social roles and substance use.

To account for unmeasured factors that may affect both changes in substance use and changing family, school, and work roles, we include the individual means from each time-varying covariate as predictors in the level-2 intercept equation (Halaby, 2003; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). The results for each social role thus represent the effect on substance use of being in that role versus not being in that role. Furthermore, the inclusion of the individual means of the explanatory variables to the intercept equation controls for selection effects by reducing the bias associated with unobserved, stable character traits that may influence both the time-varying covariates and the change over time in substance use (Halaby, 2003). Unlike most prior research, our analysis strategy thus accounts for both observed and unobserved stable differences between young adults, as well as potential mediators, when assessing the within-individual change in social roles and substance use during the transition to adulthood. This will allow us to draw stronger conclusions about effects of social contexts and social role transitions on substance use.

RESULTS

We present our findings in two parts. First, we examine how changes in work, family, and school roles and experiences correspond with changes in alcohol, cigarette, marijuana, and cocaine use, as well as whether changes in the potential mediating variables account for any relationship between social roles/experiences and substance use. Second, we examine the extent to which socioeconomic background and adolescent substance use moderate these relationships.

Social Roles and Experiences: Predictors of Change in Substance Use

Tables 2a and 2b present unstandardized regression coefficients and t-values (in italics) from within-individual regressions of alcohol, marijuana, cigarette, and cocaine use, as well as heavy drinking, on social roles for men and women, respectively. For each of the five outcomes, we show five models: Model A includes only family roles; Model B includes only school roles; and Model C includes only work roles. In Model D, we include all of the roles, and Model E includes the mediators (e.g., socializing and religiosity). Our purpose in presenting and summarizing all five models is to show how findings vary when the scope is limited to single domains of social roles compared to when they are all viewed together (Models D and E). Though not shown in Tables 2a and 2b, all of the models include the individual means of the explanatory variables to the intercept equation to produce within-individual relationships (level-2 estimates available upon request). We also controlled for age in all of the models in order to address changing social roles, leisure patterns, religious involvement, and substance use as young people transition from adolescence to young adulthood (the age coefficients are also not shown in Tables 2a and 2b but available upon request). All of our outcome variables show nonlinear changes with age, as substance use initially rises and then falls during the transition to adulthood.

Table 2a.

Unstandardized Regression Coefficients and T -Ratios (in italics) from Within-Individual Regressions of Substance Use on Social Roles (Men)

Family Roles Alcohol Heavy Drinking Cigarette Marijuana Cocaine
A B C D E A B C D E A B C D E A B C D E A B C D E
Married −.08 −.09 −.06 −.14 −.14 −.10 −.05 −.05 −.04 −.11 −.11 −.09 −.05 −.05 −.04
−8.7 −9.1 −6.3 −14.6 −14.3 −11.0 −6.9 −7.1 −5.9 −12.1 −12.0 −10.1 −9.7 −9.8 −8.6
Separated, divorced, widowed .06 .07 .07 .08 .08 .08 .04 .04 .04 .03 .03 .03 .02 .02 .02
2.9 3.3 3.4 3.7 3.7 3.8 2.5 2.4 2.5 1.5 1.3 1.4 1.8 1.7 1.7
Pregnant (spouse/partner) −.04 −.04 −.04 −.05 −.06 −.05 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00
−3.9 −3.9 −3.6 −5.2 −5.3 −5.0 −.3 −.4 −.3 .2 .1 .4 .6 .5 .7
Residing with roommates .01 .02 .01 .03 .03 .02 .01 .01 .01 .04 .04 .04 .02 .02 .02
2.3 3.0 2.5 5.3 4.3 3.7 2.1 1.8 1.5 6.1 6.1 5.8 5.4 5.5 5.3
Parent (not residing with child) −.07 −.06 −.05 −.03 −.04 −.02 .02 .02 .02 .00 .00 .01 .00 .00 .00
−4.7 −4.3 −3.6 −2.2 −2.5 −1.6 1.7 1.5 1.8 .3 .3 .7 −.2 −.2 .1
Parent (residing with child) −.12 −.10 −.08 −.06 −.06 −.03 .01 .01 .01 −.02 −.03 −.01 −.02 −.02 −.01
−10.7 −9.7 −7.8 −5.6 −6.0 −3.1 1.1 .6 1.5 −2.3 −2.9 −1.4 −3.3 −3.4 −2.3
Cohabiting (not engaged) −.03 −.03 −.02 −.08 −.08 −.07 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00
−2.7 −2.6 −2.2 −7.3 −7.0 −6.2 .3 .2 .3 .2 −.4 −.1 −.1 −.4 −.1
Cohabiting (and engaged) −.03 −.03 −.01 −.12 −.12 −.09 −.02 −.03 −.02 −.06 −.07 −.06 −.02 −.03 −.02
−2.0 −2.1 −.5 −7.5 −7.2 −5.5 −1.9 −2.0 −1.5 −4.0 −4.4 −3.6 −2.4 −2.7 −2.1
Not cohabitating (and engaged) −.05 −.05 −.03 −.12 −.12 −.10 −.02 −.02 −.01 −.08 −.08 −.07 −.03 −.03 −.03
−4.3 −4.6 −3.0 −10.6 −10.6 −8.9 −2.1 −2.1 −1.3 −8.4 −8.0 −6.7 −6.3 −6.0 −5.2
Reside with parent(s) −.04 −.04 −.04 −.06 −.05 −.05 −.01 −.01 −.01 −.01 −.03 −.03 .01 .01 .01
−5.1 −5.2 −5.7 −7.9 −6.0 −6.7 −1.5 −1.2 −1.4 −1.8 −3.6 −3.8 3.0 1.6 1.5
School Roles
Part-time student .00 .00 .00 −.01 −.01 −.01 −.01 .00 .00 −.01 −.02 −.01 −.01 −.01 −.01
−.4 .0 .2 −1.3 −1.2 −.9 −.9 −.7 −.6 −1.3 −1.9 −1.8 −2.0 −2.5 −2.3
Full-time in other residence −.02 −.01 .00 .01 .00 .01 −.01 −.01 −.01 .01 −.01 −.01 −.01 −.02 −.02
−3.1 −1.2 −.5 1.2 .4 1.6 −2.4 −1.9 −1.6 1.7 −1.5 −1.0 −3.4 −5.1 −4.7
Full-time in fraternity/sorority .09 .09 .09 .19 .16 .16 −.01 −.01 −.02 .08 .03 .03 .01 .00 −.01
4.7 4.8 4.6 6.5 5.6 5.5 −.7 −.8 −.9 2.6 1.1 1.0 .6 −.3 −.4
Full-time in dormitory −.03 −.02 −.01 .04 .02 .03 −.04 −.04 −.03 −.01 −.05 −.05 −.03 −.04 −.04
−2.1 −1.5 −1.0 3.5 1.6 2.3 −3.9 −3.7 −3.5 −1.1 −3.8 −3.4 −4.5 −5.4 −5.1
BA/BS degree earned .04 .03 .02 −.02 −.02 −.03 −.04 −.04 −.04 −.02 −.01 −.02 −.01 −.01 −.01
4.4 3.4 2.4 −1.7 −1.7 −3.1 −5.9 −5.4 −5.9 −2.1 −1.0 −1.7 −2.3 −1.6 −2.2
Work Roles
Full-time .00 .00 −.01 .00 .01 .01 .00 .00 .00 .00 .01 .00 .01 .00 .00
−.4 −.4 −.8 .2 1.0 .6 −.5 −.1 −.2 .2 .6 .3 .9 .8 .6
Part-time −.04 −.03 −.04 .01 .00 .00 −.01 −.01 −.01 .01 .01 .01 .00 .01 .01
−3.4 −3.0 −3.3 .9 .0 −.4 −1.0 −1.1 −1.2 1.2 1.3 1.1 .3 1.3 1.2
Nonprofessional job .02 .02 .03 −.04 −.02 −.01 .01 .01 .01 −.01 −.01 −.01 .00 −.01 −.01
2.1 2.2 2.4 −3.0 −1.2 −.8 1.1 1.1 1.2 −1.2 −1.0 −.9 −.3 −1.2 −1.0
Professional job .05 .04 .04 −.05 −.01 −.01 .00 .00 .00 −.04 −.03 −.03 −.01 −.02 −.02
3.7 3.0 3.1 −3.5 −1.0 −1.0 −.4 .4 .4 −2.8 −2.3 −2.3 −1.6 −2.5 −2.5
Military .10 .09 .08 .05 .06 .05 .04 .04 .04 −.19 −.20 −.21 −.05 −.05 −.06
5.2 4.4 4.1 2.2 2.8 2.4 2.4 2.5 2.4 −9.8 −9.9 −10.2 −4.3 −4.8 −5.0
Mediators
Socializing .04 .07 .01 .03 .01
18.3 27.5 7.2 12.5 9.4
Religiosity −.08 −.06 −.03 −.06 −.02
−14.4 −11.7 −7.7 −10.2 −5.6

Number of Occasions = 54,789

Number of Respondents = 18,555

Table 2b.

Unstandardized Regression Coefficients and T -Ratios (in italics) from Within-Individual Regressions of Substance Use on Social Roles (Women)

Family Roles Alcohol Heavy Drinking Cigarette Marijuana Cocaine
A B C D E A B C D E A B C D E A B C D E A B C D E
Married −.12 −.12 −.10 −.13 −.12 −.10 −.07 −.07 −.06 −.09 −.08 −.07 −.02 −.02 −.02
−15.0 −14.9 −12.1 −19.0 −17.7 −14.7 −11.7 −11.4 −10.0 −11.5 −11.3 −9.5 −5.7 −6.2 −5.3
Separated, divorced, widowed .07 .07 .07 .04 .03 .03 .06 .06 .05 .03 .03 .03 .01 .00 .00
5.0 5.1 4.7 2.8 2.7 2.3 4.7 4.6 4.4 2.3 2.2 1.9 .7 .6 .5
Pregnant −.20 −.20 −.20 −.13 −.13 −.12 −.12 −.12 −.12 −.03 −.03 −.03 .00 .00 .00
−23.3 −23.2 −22.7 −20.9 −20.9 −20.1 −17.6 −17.5 −17.2 −4.2 −4.4 −3.8 −1.1 −1.3 −.9
Residing with roommates .03 .03 .03 .04 .04 .03 .02 .02 .02 .04 .04 .04 .01 .01 .01
5.3 6.0 5.4 9.5 7.7 6.8 4.8 4.5 4.2 8.3 7.7 7.3 4.4 4.8 4.6
Parent (not residing with child) −.20 −.19 −.17 −.03 −.04 −.02 −.02 −.02 −.02 −.02 −.03 −.01 −.02 −.02 −.02
−12.7 −12.1 −11.2 −2.9 −3.2 −1.4 −2.3 −2.1 −1.6 −1.8 −2.0 −1.2 −3.6 −3.6 −3.1
Parent (residing with child) −.28 −.25 −.22 −.04 −.05 −.02 −.03 −.03 −.01 −.05 −.06 −.04 −.03 −.02 −.02
−33.4 −28.8 −25.0 −7.3 −8.1 −2.8 −5.1 −4.1 −2.2 −7.3 −7.8 −4.9 −7.1 −6.3 −4.7
Cohabiting (not engaged) −.03 −.03 −.03 −.08 −.08 −.07 −.01 −.01 −.01 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00
−4.0 −4.3 −3.4 −9.9 −9.3 −8.0 −1.7 −1.8 −1.5 .1 .0 .5 .0 −.3 −.1
Cohabiting (and engaged) −.04 −.04 −.02 −.13 −.12 −.10 −.04 −.04 −.03 −.06 −.06 −.05 −.02 −.02 −.02
−3.5 −3.9 −2.0 −12.6 −11.8 −9.8 −4.1 −4.1 −3.5 −5.1 −5.2 −4.2 −2.8 −3.2 −2.7
Not cohabitating (and engaged) −.05 −.05 −.04 −.10 −.09 −.08 −.05 −.05 −.04 −.07 −.07 −.06 −.02 −.02 −.02
−6.1 −6.2 −4.6 −13.0 −12.5 −11.2 −8.1 −7.9 −7.1 −9.6 −9.4 −8.3 −6.0 −6.1 −5.5
Reside with parent(s) −.03 −.04 −.04 −.05 −.04 −.04 −.02 −.02 −.02 −.04 −.04 −.04 .00 −.01 −.01
−5.4 −6.4 −6.7 −9.5 −7.1 −7.6 −3.6 −3.5 −3.5 −6.3 −6.6 −6.7 −1.1 −2.0 −2.0
School Roles
Part-time student .02 .00 .00 .01 −.01 .00 .01 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 −.01 −.01
3.4 .3 .8 1.0 −1.1 −.4 1.5 .2 .3 .8 −.6 −.3 −1.4 −2.1 −2.0
Full-time in other residence .01 −.01 .00 .03 .00 .01 .01 .00 .00 .01 −.02 −.02 −.01 −.01 −.01
2.0 −.8 .1 5.2 .0 1.4 1.7 −.8 −.5 1.2 −3.0 −2.4 −1.8 −3.0 −2.7
Full-time in fraternity/sorority .11 .08 .07 .21 .15 .14 .07 .05 .05 .12 .06 .06 .01 .00 .00
5.0 3.7 3.4 6.9 4.9 4.8 3.1 2.2 2.1 4.0 2.1 1.9 .8 −.2 −.3
Full-time in dormitory −.01 −.02 −.02 .07 .03 .03 .00 −.01 −.01 −.01 −.05 −.05 −.02 −.02 −.02
−.5 −1.7 −1.4 7.5 2.4 2.7 .6 −1.2 −1.1 −.6 −4.6 −4.5 −3.5 −4.8 −4.7
BA/BS degree earned .05 .01 .00 −.03 −.03 −.04 −.01 −.01 −.01 −.03 −.03 −.03 .00 .00 .00
6.2 1.6 .5 −4.9 −4.6 −5.7 −2.2 −1.8 −2.3 −3.6 −3.2 −3.9 .8 .1 −.4
Work Roles
Full-time −.01 .00 .00 −.02 −.01 −.01 −.01 −.01 −.01 −.02 −.02 −.02 .00 .00 .00
−1.0 .4 −.4 −3.1 −.7 −1.7 −2.5 −.9 −1.3 −2.9 −2.1 −2.6 −.6 −.3 −.7
Part-time −.05 −.02 −.02 −.01 .00 .00 −.02 −.01 −.01 −.02 −.01 −.01 −.01 .00 .00
−5.6 −1.8 −2.3 −.7 −.1 −.6 −2.8 −1.8 −1.9 −2.4 −1.0 −1.2 −2.0 −.5 −.6
Nonprofessional job .07 .03 .03 .00 .00 .00 .03 .02 .03 .01 .00 .00 .01 .00 .00
6.9 2.7 3.3 −.5 −.5 .3 4.5 3.5 3.7 1.4 .1 .5 1.5 −.1 .1
Professional job .07 .04 .04 −.03 −.01 −.01 .01 .01 .01 −.02 −.02 −.02 .01 .00 .00
6.9 3.2 3.4 −3.4 −.7 −.5 1.1 1.5 1.5 −1.8 −1.9 −1.9 1.7 .0 .0
Military .14 .07 .08 .06 .04 .05 .04 .03 .04 −.14 −.16 −.16 −.02 −.04 −.04
3.5 1.8 2.0 1.8 1.4 1.6 1.3 1.1 1.2 −4.5 −5.4 −5.2 −2.1 −3.0 −2.9
Homemaker −.13 −.05 −.04 −.07 −.03 −.02 −.03 −.01 −.01 −.05 −.03 −.02 −.01 −.01 −.01
−11.3 −4.2 −3.7 −9.1 −3.4 −2.8 −4.5 −1.5 −1.2 −5.9 −2.8 −2.4 −2.7 −1.3 −1.1
Mediators
Socializing .06 .06 .02 .04 .01
27.6 31.9 11.2 16.7 10.0
Religiosity −.09 −.04 −.04 −.05 −.01
−18.4 −10.6 −10.6 −11.9 −5.4

Number of Occasions = 71,600

Number of Respondents = 21,868

Family Roles and Experiences

As shown in Model A, after controlling for stable individual differences, family roles have powerful effects on alcohol and substance use—that is, entry and exit into family roles and experiences contributed to significant changes in levels of substance use. Heavy drinking, as well as alcohol, cigarette, marijuana, and cocaine use were significantly lower when men and women were married compared to when they were not. For instance, rates of substance use declined between 5 and 14 percent for men (for cocaine use γ = −.05, t-value = −9.7; for heavy alcohol use γ = −.14, t-value = −14.6) and between 2 and 12 percent for women (for cocaine use γ = −.02, t-value = −5.7; for heavy alcohol use γ = −.13, t-value = −19) when they were married compared to when they were single (the reference category). Union dissolution also contributes to substance use; rates of alcohol, heavy drinking, cigarette use, and marijuana (for women only) were significantly higher when respondents were separated, divorced, or widowed than when they were single. Rates of cocaine use were not significantly different. Substance use also declined when respondents were cohabiting, though these effects are weaker than for marriage. Engagement, irrespective of whether the respondent was cohabiting, significantly reduced substance use for men and women, though again the effects are not as strong as for marriage.

Rates of substance use also varied with fertility behaviors. For instance, when a woman was pregnant, her rate of alcohol use declined by approximately 20 percent (γ = −.20, t-value = −23.3) compared to when she was not pregnant. Men drank alcohol on fewer occasions when their spouse or girlfriend was pregnant compared to when she was not (for alcohol use γ = −.04, t-value = −3.9; for heavy alcohol use γ = −.06, t-value = −5.3), though spouse pregnancy had little effect on male cigarette, marijuana, and cocaine use. The coefficients in Tables 2a and 2b also indicate that when women were parents they used substances less than when they were not parents, and these reductions in substance use were strongest when women were residing with their children. Rates of alcohol, marijuana, and cocaine use were significantly lower when men were residing with their children compared to when they were not (for alcohol use γ = −.12, t-value = −10.7; for heavy alcohol use γ = −.06, t-value = −5.6; for marijuana use γ = −.02, t-value = −2.3; for cocaine use γ = −.02, t-value = −3.3). Rates of alcohol use also declined for fathers not residing with their children (for alcohol use γ = −.07, t-value = −4.7; for heavy alcohol use γ = −.03, t-value = −2.2), though nonresidential fatherhood had little effect on cigarette, marijuana, and cocaine use.

Finally, the estimates shown in Model A suggest that substance use was significantly lower when women and men resided with their parent(s) compared to when they did not. On the other hand, the frequency of substance use was higher when respondents resided with roommates compared to when they were single and resided by themselves. Compared to the other family roles, these roommate “effects” were generally small, though both men and women did report residing with someone other than a spouse, child, parent, or intimate partner on a third of the measured occasions.

School Roles and Experiences

With some notable exceptions, school roles and experiences (Model B) had weaker effects on changes in alcohol and substance use compared to family roles. Rates of heavy drinking were 19 percent higher for men (γ = .19, t-value = 6.5) and 21 percent higher for women (γ = .21, t-value = 6.9) when respondents attended school full-time and resided in a fraternity or sorority compared to when they were not attending school. Full-time students also consumed alcohol and used marijuana more frequently when they resided in a fraternity and sorority. Residence in a dormitory was also associated with more frequent heavy drinking, though the effects were weaker than residence in a fraternity or sorority (for men γ = .04, t-value = 3.5; for women γ = .07, t-value = 7.5). Moreover, full-time students were less likely to use illicit drugs when they resided in a dormitory compared to young adults not in school. In general, cigarette, marijuana, and cocaine use declined after the receipt of a baccalaureate degree; alcohol use, but not heavy drinking, increased.

Work Roles and Experiences

Substance use did not significantly change when men acquired full-time work (Model C). When men worked in professional jobs, compared to when they did not, their rates of heavy drinking and marijuana use declined by approximately 5% and 4%, respectively (for heavy drinking γ = −.05, t-value = −3.5; for marijuana use γ = −.04, t-value = −2.7), though they tended to drink alcohol more frequently (γ = .05, t-value = 3.7). For women, substance use was lower when they worked in full-time or part-time jobs than when they were not working, though these differences were small in magnitude. Substance use was also significantly lower when women reported working as homemakers. Men drank and smoked cigarettes more frequently when they were in the military than when they were not; however, when men were in the military they were less likely to use marijuana and cocaine. Military service had similar, though weaker, effects on substance use for women.

Family, School, and Work Roles and Experiences

In Model D, we include all the social roles to assess the extent to which each uniquely predicts substance use in the context of the others. Strikingly, the effects of family roles on substance use remained virtually unchanged when we control for school and work roles underscoring the importance of changes of family roles for changes in substance use. The effects of school and work on substance use, however, were reduced substantially when we controlled for family roles, though there were some notable exceptions especially for men. Military enrollment remained a strong predictor of reduced illicit drug use, irrespective of family and school roles. Rates of alcohol use and binge drinking were also higher when respondents were residing in a fraternity or sorority.

Potential Mediators

Finally, in Model E, we included religiosity and socializing as potential mediators of the effects of social role transitions and experiences on changes in substance use. As shown in Tables 2a and 2b, for both men and women, changes in socializing and religiosity had strong associations with changes in substance use. Heavy drinking, alcohol, cigarette, marijuana, and cocaine use were all significantly higher when young adults spend more evenings out for fun and recreation, whereas all forms of substance use were significantly lower when young people placed more importance on religion and spent more time in religious activities. Also shown in Tables 2a and 2b, when comparing coefficients in Models E and D, the inclusion of these two mediators contributed to some decline in the coefficients for the social roles and experiences. For instance, as can be seen from the decline in the unstandardized regression coefficients from Model D to Model E, socializing and religiosity reduced the effects of residing with children on substance use by between 14% and 66% for women and between 21% and 51% for men. The coefficients for marriage and cohabitation in the context of an engagement for women were also reduced substantially (between 12% and 20% for marriage; between 16% and 49% for cohabitation and engagement). Among men, these same mediators reduced between 13% and 31% of the effects for marriage and between 20% and 75% for cohabitation and engagement. Comparing the results of Model D to Model E, we see that in general, these mediators accounted for more of the effects of family roles on substance use than the effects of work and school roles and experiences, which can be seen by comparing Model D to Models A–C (though this is in part because such effects were smaller to begin with). Nonetheless, family roles still had strong and statistically significant associations with substance use. Based on this consideration of potential mediators, the findings suggest that many of the social role transition effects on changes in substance use could be at least partially explained by changes in socializing and religiosity that come with many of the most salient social role transitions and experiences during the 20s.

Moderating Factors

Although we controlled for between-person selection effects in the primary analyses, it is still likely that the effects of social role transitions and experiences in young adulthood on substance use are moderated by earlier pivotal experiences; that is, in a cascading fashion, earlier experiences may contribute to both social role transitions and experiences as well as the effects of the transitions and experiences on substance use. Thus, our second goal in the analyses was to examine potential moderators including socioeconomic background and substance use in adolescence. As part of this goal, we also considered whether senior-year cohort moderated the effects of the social roles and experiences on changes in substance use. Consistent with our prior work (Bachman et al., 1997, 2002), the current multi-level analyses revealed that there were few significant and no substantive differences by cohort in the effects, indicating that findings discussed above were robust with respect to historic variations across the past two decades in social role transition patterns and substance use.2

Next, we considered how the amount of time in the various social roles and experiences between ages 19–28, as indicated by the number of occasions within each role, varied by both adolescent substance use and socioeconomic background. As an example, Figure 1 shows the percentage of waves at which women and men reported being married, in parenthood (irrespective of whether they were residing with their child(ren)), and in full-time work based upon whether the respondent’s parent(s) had completed some post-secondary education (high SES) and whether they used marijuana at least once during the prior year in 12th grade. We chose these three roles to illustrate because they are among the most strongly associated with reductions in substance use during the transition to adulthood. As shown in Figure 1, senior-year marijuana use did not predict time spent in marriage. However, early drug users did spend more time in roles that would reduce subsequent substance use, such as in parenthood and working in a full-time job. Figure 1 also shows that socioeconomic origins relate to the number of occasions spent in these roles. For instance, women and men from lower SES backgrounds were married at approximately 10% more of measurement occasions compared to women and men from higher SES backgrounds. That is, women and men from low SES backgrounds spent more overall time in the roles that are strongly associated with reduced substance use in young adulthood compared to their more advantaged counterparts.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Percentage Time Spent Married, in Parenthood, and in Full-Time Work for Women and Men by Adolescent Marijuana Use and Socioeconomic Origins

Next, we examined whether the effects of social roles and experiences on young adult substance use varied by the two moderators. To test the moderating influence of adolescent drug use, we estimated within-individual regression models of substance use separately for men and women who did and did not use marijuana in the 12th grade and who did and did not have at least one parent who had post-secondary education; we then used z-tests to compare the equality of the coefficients (Clogg, Petkova, & Haritou, 1995). The pattern of results is convincingly clear: neither moderator had much impact on the effect of social role transitions and experiences on changes in adult substance use. In Appendix 1, we present the findings for men in regard to 12th-grade marijuana use. Note that the coefficients that differed significantly by 12th-grade marijuana use are in bold font (p < .05). As is shown, very few of the moderating effects are significant; this is especially noteworthy given that the expectation would be that high school use would set the stage for stronger role socialization effects given high school users likely would have larger declines in drug use post-high school. Indeed, among the few significant findings, the effects of marriage, engagement, parenthood, and military involvement were stronger on changes in illicit drugs for adolescent marijuana users than for nonusers. For instance, among men who used marijuana in 12th grade, subsequent rates of marijuana use declined by 14% (γ = −.14, t-value = −7.56) when they were married compared to when they were single and residing alone. Among nonusers in 12th grade, there was only an 8% decline with marriage (γ = −.08, t-value = −8.3). Cohabitation in the context of an engagement similarly had stronger effects on rates of marijuana use in young adulthood for men who used marijuana in 12th grade compared to nonusers (for 12th grade users γ = −.12, t-value = −4.9; for nonusers γ = −.05, t-value = −4.95). But, except for these few significant findings, 12th grade marijuana use did little to moderate the relationship between social role transitions and changes in adolescent drug use. And for women for adolescent drug use moderation and for women and men for parent education moderation, even fewer significant effects were found (results not shown).

Appendix 1.

Unstandardized Regression Coefficients from Within-Individual Regressions of Substance Use on Social Roles by 12th-Grade Marijuana Use (Men only)

Marijuana Use Heavy Drinking Alcohol Use Cocaine Use Cigarette
12th-grade drug use 12th-grade drug use 12th-grade drug use 12th-grade drug use 12th-grade drug use
yes no yes no yes no yes no yes no
Family Roles
Married −.14 −.08 −.11 −.11 −.04 −.08 −.09 −.02 −.06 −.04
−7.56 −8.30 −6.48 −9.28 −2.71 −6.19 −6.80 −5.77 −3.81 −4.75
Separated, divorced, widowed .04 .01 .06 .09 .05 .09 .04 .00 .06 .02
1.35 .35 2.00 3.31 2.18 3.13 1.59 −.10 2.35 1.09
Pregnant (spouse/partner) .01 .00 −.06 −.04 −.02 −.05 .00 .00 −.01 .00
.40 .44 −3.26 −3.61 −1.36 −3.28 .17 1.15 −.42 .19
Residing with roommates .06 .02 .03 .02 .01 .02 .05 .01 .02 .00
4.82 3.56 2.89 2.41 1.52 2.19 5.14 2.07 1.69 .42
Parent (not residing with child) −.02 .03 −.05 −.01 −.06 −.04 −.03 .01 .04 .01
−.86 1.86 −2.21 −.30 −3.11 −2.20 −1.34 1.66 1.97 .57
Parent (residing with child) −.01 −.01 −.04 −.03 −.06 −.10 −.03 .00 .02 .01
−.70 −1.06 −2.20 −2.18 −3.77 −6.96 −2.13 −.12 1.29 .98
Cohabiting (not engaged) .00 .00 −.07 −.06 −.02 −.01 .00 .00 .01 .00
.12 .13 −4.78 −3.94 −1.95 −.78 .15 −.40 .52 .02
Cohabiting (and engaged) −.08 −.04 −.08 −.09 −.01 .00 −.04 .00 −.01 −.02
−2.77 −2.08 −3.21 −4.29 −.47 −.05 −1.88 −.45 −.30 −1.66
Not cohabitating (and engaged) −.12 −.05 −.10 −.10 −.03 −.04 −.05 −.02 .00 −.01
−4.99 −4.95 −4.69 −7.48 −1.67 −2.71 −2.98 −4.00 −.03 −1.55
Reside with parent(s) −.02 −.03 −.05 −.05 −.03 −.05 .02 .00 .00 −.01
−1.61 −3.50 −4.27 −5.35 −2.81 −4.92 2.22 −.77 .18 −2.05
School Roles
Part-time student −.01 −.01 .00 −.01 .00 .00 −.02 −.01 −.01 .00
−.88 −1.68 .20 −1.41 −.39 .26 −1.57 −1.63 −.43 −.44
Full-time in other residence .00 −.01 .02 .01 −.01 .00 −.04 −.01 −.02 −.01
−.22 −.92 1.01 1.37 −.47 −.23 −3.68 −2.49 −1.43 −.78
Full-time in fraternity/sorority −.01 .04 .14 .17 .02 .11 .02 −.01 −.05 .00
−.11 1.25 3.33 4.44 .85 4.61 .39 −1.46 −1.70 .15
Full-time in dormitory −.02 −.04 .08 .02 .02 −.02 −.08 −.02 −.04 −.03
−.72 −2.55 3.10 1.40 1.37 −1.13 −3.62 −3.15 −1.85 −2.72
BA/BS degree earned −.05 −.01 −.04 −.02 .00 .03 −.03 .00 −.05 −.04
−2.65 −1.17 −2.65 −2.26 −.21 2.27 −2.16 −.90 −3.41 −4.97
Work Roles
Full-time −.02 .01 .00 .01 −.01 −.01 −.01 .01 −.02 .01
−.93 .74 −.13 .62 −.40 −.85 −.68 1.90 −1.47 .79
Part-time .00 .01 .00 −.01 −.04 −.03 .00 .01 −.03 .00
.20 .98 −.03 −.71 −2.71 −2.44 .12 1.70 −1.80 −.17
Nonprofessional job .00 .00 .00 −.01 .03 .03 .01 −.01 .03 .00
−.13 −.27 −.14 −.49 1.96 2.04 .49 −2.38 1.61 .38
Professional job −.03 −.03 −.01 −.01 .02 .04 −.02 −.02 .02 .00
−1.07 −2.40 −.53 −.85 1.25 2.64 −1.10 −3.20 .92 −.29
Military −.36 −.11 .06 .04 .08 .08 −.09 −.04 .06 .03
−9.51 −5.31 1.76 1.75 3.05 3.06 −3.37 −4.10 2.04 1.52
Mediators
Socializing .05 .01 .09 .05 .05 .04 .03 .00 .02 .01
9.86 5.40 20.69 18.21 13.89 12.19 8.36 3.26 5.83 3.91
Religiosity −.09 −.04 −.08 −.05 −.09 −.08 −.03 −.01 −.03 −.03
−8.73 −5.95 −9.01 −7.81 −9.96 −10.93 −4.06 −3.96 −4.44 −6.27

Note. Bolded coefficients indicate statistically significant (p < .05) difference between groups (based on z -tests comparing equality of coefficients).

Thus, as we show, early substance use, and to a greater extent, socioeconomic origins predict the onset and stability of roles that, in turn, are associated with changes in substance use in adulthood. Nevertheless, we find little evidence that these two moderators serve to alter the extent of role socialization effects on changes in substance use.

DISCUSSION

It is a fundamental truism in developmental psychopathology that difficulties accumulate over time, building on one another across domains in a cascading manner that serves to direct the course of development toward continued and increasing difficulties (e.g., Dodge et al., 2008; Masten et al., 2005). Alternatively, competencies and successes can accrue with time, and become self-perpetuating and self-reinforcing. These diverging cumulative—but not inexorable—courses within individual life courses have been described as straight and devious pathways (Robins & Rutter, 1990) and virtuous and vicious cycles (Bynner, 1998). Importantly, such characterizations of the cumulative nature of development are not to suggest a closed system with few possibilities for changing course. Especially during major life transitions like the multidimensional transition to adulthood, opportunities for changing course for better or worse are abundant (Schulenberg & Zarrett, 2006). Our purpose was to examine how the numerous social role transitions and experiences during the period between the end of high school and early adulthood contribute to changes in substance use. In doing so, our hope was to bring more of a life course perspective to the understanding of cascading effects, focusing particularly on issues of selection and socialization and how the various social role transitions provide the big picture for viewing cascading effects across adolescence into adulthood.

We summarize the findings below, highlighting the unique empirical, methodological, and theoretical contributions of this study to the understanding of substance use and cascading effects across the transition to adulthood. We then consider the strengths and limitations of this study and offer a series of conclusions.

Summary of Results

A long-standing debate in drug research is whether the well-documented association between adult family and work roles and reduced substance use results from role selection or socialization (Chassin et al., 2009). Consistent with what others have shown, our results indicate that, among women and men, changes in family, school, and work roles are associated with changes in licit and illicit drug use during the transition to adulthood (e.g., Bachman et al., 1997, 2002; Leonard & Homish, 2005; Yamaguchi & Kandel, 1985). We extend these findings by showing that these findings remain true once any potential selection effects are controlled (i.e., by using within-individual comparisons to rule out many pre-existing factors that might explain the relationship between social roles and substance use). Our findings are thus most consistent with the role socialization hypothesis (Yamaguchi & Kandel, 1985), and suggest that role transitions and experiences, along with the concomitant norms and expectations, contribute to changes in substance use. Indeed, these findings suggest that social role transitions are part of the cascading mechanisms for reductions in substance use that typically come with the transition to adulthood

Consistent with previous research (Bachman et al., 1997, 2002; Duncan et al., 2006), our results also suggest that social roles involving family formation behaviors are highly influential in reducing substance use, and perhaps more influential than either school or work roles. In particular, substance use decreased substantially when respondents were married, cohabiting and engaged, residing with their children, and when they or their spouse or girlfriend was pregnant. One could argue that family formation roles generally occur later in the life course than work and school roles, and thus the family effects we show in this paper may just represent the well-documented drop in substance use and problem behaviors with age (Hirschi & Gottfredson, 1983). Yet, we control for the nonlinear effects of age in all of our analyses and we still find statistically significant effects of family behaviors on substance use. Why do family roles have consistent and stronger effects than work and school roles?

One primary reason is that family role transitions and experiences may engender changes in socializing and religiosity, both of which are associated with changes in substance use (Bachman et al., 2002; Osgood, Wilson, O’Malley, Bachman, & Johnston, 1996). It is clear that family roles involving time with loved ones contribute to less free time for socializing with others; likewise, such family roles tend to bring religious involvement more to the forefront. Indeed, it may be useful to think of this as an intra-individual cascade, where within individual changes in family roles contribute to changes in socializing and religiosity, which in turn contribute to changes in drug use. In contrast, this intra-individual cascade has little effect for work and education transitions.

Another reason for the consistency of unique of family role transition effects is that norms surrounding family behaviors almost always discourage substance use and may encourage healthier behaviors in multiple domains, as couples settle down to establish a shared household. In contrast, norms about substance use and other health behaviors within work and school domains are less clear, and in some cases may encourage or facilitate risk tasking. Family roles in young adulthood involve “settling down,” “growing up,” and limiting time in the “fast lane.” These norms against substance use are well established in marriage, whereas in cohabitation they are less clear (see Duncan et al., 2006). Perhaps this is why our analyses show that cohabitation has weaker effects on substance use than engagement, because engagement serves as a transition and anticipatory period to marriage. Furthermore, the norms and expectations surrounding school and work roles are more diverse, which might account for the weak and generally inconsistent pattern of work and school effects on substance use. For instance, the stressors and demands of full-time and professional work may encourage more frequent alcohol use, and at the same time discourage illicit drug use. Likewise, post-secondary school roles may involve a mix of expectations and freedoms that simultaneously discourage and encourage substance use (e.g., studying, experimentation, role exploration).

Family roles also might matter more than school and work roles because family members are likely to monitor and enforce norms against substance use. Not only do family members have more opportunities to detect and discourage substance use (e.g., curfews, loss of privileges, etc.), spouses and fiancés may also have a greater stake in the health and well-being of their significant other. Of course, caring for children directly and indirectly influences the parent(s)’ own behavior, which might account for why we show generally weaker effects of nonresident parenthood on substance use in our analyses compared to when respondents were living with offspring.

Finally, family roles might matter more than other school and work roles because there are more opportunities to establish meaningful adult family roles. For example, disadvantaged social origins and early substance use may make it difficult to establish high-quality work and school roles in young adulthood, as post-secondary school roles, college degrees, and “good” jobs that can serve as turning points in substance use trajectories may be more difficult to attain (Krohn et al., 1997; Newcomb & Bentler, 1988). Compared to work and school, high-quality family roles may be more attainable for these young people, and in turn may provide norms and expectations that can reduce substance use (Laub & Sampson, 2003). One work role – military service – did have a strong effect on illicit drug use, particularly for men. This is consistent with earlier findings by Bachman et al. (1999), who suggest that the effect is likely due to the military’s extensive use of testing for illicit drugs.

Strengths, Limitations, and Future Directions

An important strength of this study is the use of national panel data spanning a 10-year age interval between late adolescence and early adulthood. The use of large-scale representative multiwave panel data to examine the contributions of a comprehensive set of social role transitions to changes in substance use represents a powerful approach to understanding the etiology of substance use and abuse, reflecting an important integration of large-scale survey research and developmental science. Furthermore, the inclusion of multiple cohorts (22 consecutive senior-year cohorts) allows for the needed consideration of historical variation and robustness in the relationships between social role transitions and substance use changes. The analytic approach provides another strength of the study reported here—by controlling for potential between-person selection effects and including time-varying mediators, we are able to draw stronger conclusions about causal influence. We were also able to examine the simultaneous impact of social role transitions in several domains (school, work, and family), and to conclude that family roles exert the strongest influences on substance use. Moving beyond main effects, we examined the role of two mediating and two moderating variables that have been implicated in substance use in the literature. We demonstrated their contributions to use of specific substances in the context of particular social role transitions, opening the door for future studies to examine these cascades in further detail.

Of course, our study does have some limitations. Because the sample included only those who remained in school until at least the 12th grade, generalizability to those who dropped out of high school may be limited. Although the use of multiwave panel data represents an important strength, the two-year lag between the waves may result in some lack of precision in capturing the link between social role transitions and changes in drug use. Future longitudinal research in this area would do well to start earlier in adolescence to gain a better “before” picture and to capture the ongoing cascades before young people begin their transition to adulthood.

In addition, future research should further examine more nuanced aspects of the quality of family and work roles. For instance, marital satisfaction and feelings of closeness to one’s spouse predict lower rates of substance use (Heinz, Wu, Witkiewitz, Epstein, & Preston, 2009; Thompson & Petrovich, 2009). Modeling changes in substance use as a function of changes in marital quality using a within-person design would make a major contribution to the literature. Moreover, our measures of job quality are based upon broad measures of occupational type. Future research should consider other dimensions of work that may affect substance use, such as wages, job satisfaction, advancement opportunities, and job stress. If measures of family and work role quality were demonstrated to show inverse within-person associations with substance use, this would further suggest the public health value of programs and policies designed to improve family and work life.

Conclusions

We found strong and robust evidence indicating that family roles and experiences during the 20s contribute to changes in substance use. Indeed, more than the transitions and experiences in the work and education domains, family role transitions consistently contributed to both decreases and increases in drug use. What is especially meaningful here is that family role effects were pervasive: they were evident for both men and women across all five types of substance use considered here; and they did not vary much by background SES or adolescent history of marijuana use. Perhaps most importantly, we can be confident that these effects are not due to between-person selection factors, as our analytic strategy controlled for stable individual differences. In other words, no matter what pathway one takes across adolescence and into young adulthood, our findings indicate that at the population level, most family role transitions serve as turning points resulting in declines in substance use. One’s background no doubt contributes to the type of and time in social roles during the transition to adulthood—as we found here, for example, those from lower SES backgrounds tend to spend less time as a college graduate and more time married and in full-time work. Similarly, those who were marijuana users in high school tend to spend less time as college graduates and more time in full-time work. But again, regardless of how one gets to the role transitions, once they happen, the effect on substance use is consistent across the moderators we considered here. Thus, at the population level during the transition to adulthood, regardless of earlier cascading influences, there are new pervasive cascading effects from social role transitions, particularly in the family domain, to changes in substance use.

Footnotes

*

The first author gratefully acknowledges support from a Mentored Research Scientist Development Award in Population Research from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (K01 HD054467). This paper uses data from the Monitoring the Future study, which is supported by grants from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01 DA01411; R01 DA016575); the second and other authors gratefully acknowledge support from these grants. The findings and conclusions in this report are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of the sponsors.

1

In unlisted analyses we recoded our substance use measures into ordinal variables and estimated ordered regression models. The overall pattern of findings remained the same.

2

Because social roles and experiences are age-ordered, we also tested for the possibility that the individual timing of social roles and experiences may moderate their effects on substance use. In a series of unlisted analyses, we included wave by roles/experiences interactions. Overall, we found little evidence that timing of social roles moderated their effects on substance use.

Contributor Information

Jeremy Staff, The Pennsylvania State University.

John E. Schulenberg, University of Michigan

Julie Maslowsky, University of Michigan.

Jerald G. Bachman, University of Michigan

Patrick M. O’Malley, University of Michigan

Jennifer L. Maggs, The Pennsylvania State University

Lloyd D. Johnston, University of Michigan

References

  1. Amato PR, Booth A, Johnson D, Rogers S. Alone together: How marriage in America is changing. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bachman JG, Freedman Doan P, O’Malley PM, Johnston LD, Segal DR. Changing patterns of drug use among US military recruits before and after enlistment. American Journal of Public Health. 1999;89:672–677. doi: 10.2105/ajph.89.5.672. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bachman JG, O’Malley PM, Schulenberg JE, Johnston LD, Bryant AL, Merline AC. Why substance use declines in young adulthood: Changes in social activities, roles, and beliefs. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bachman JG, O’Malley PM, Schulenberg JE, Johnston LD, Freedman-Doan P, Messersmith EE. The education–drug use connection: How successes and failures in school relate to adolescent smoking, drinking, drug use, and delinquency. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates/Taylor & Francis; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bachman JG, Wadsworth KN, O’Malley PM, Johnston LD, Schulenberg JE. Smoking, drinking, and drug use in young adulthood: The impact of new freedoms and new responsibilities. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  6. Brown SL, Manning WD. Family boundary ambiguity and the measurement of family structure: The significance of cohabitation. Demography. 2009;46:85–101. doi: 10.1353/dem.0.0043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Budig MJ. Are women’s employment and fertility histories interdependent? An examination of causal order using event history analysis. Social Science Research. 2003;32:376–401. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bumpass L, Lu H. Trends in cohabitation and implications for children’s family contexts in the United States. Population Studies. 2000;54:29–41. doi: 10.1080/713779060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Burton RP, Johnson RJ, Ritter C, Clayton RC. The effects of role socialization on cocaine use: An event history analysis from adolescence into middle adulthood. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1996;37:75–90. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bynner J. Education and family components of identity in the transition from school to work. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 1998;22:29–53. [Google Scholar]
  11. Caspi A, Wright BR, Moffitt TE, Silva P. Childhood Predictors of Unemployment in Early Adulthood. American Sociological Review. 1998;63:424–451. [Google Scholar]
  12. Chassin L, Hussong A, Beltran I. Adolescent Substance Use. In: Lerner RM, Steinberg L, editors. Handbook of Adolescent Psychology. 3. Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley and Songs, Inc; 2009. pp. 723–763. [Google Scholar]
  13. Chilcoat HD, Breslau N. Alcohol disorders in young adulthood: Effects of transitions into adulthood. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1996;37:339–349. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA. A developmental psychopathology perspective on adolescence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70(1):6–20. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.70.1.6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Clogg CC, Petkova E, Haritou A. Statistical methods for comparing regression coefficients between models. American Journal of Sociology. 1995;100(5):1261–1293. [Google Scholar]
  16. Dodge KA, Malone PS, Lansford JE, Miller S, Pettit GS, Bates JE. A dynamic cascade model of the development of substance use onset. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2009;74:1–120. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2009.00528.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dodge KA, Greenberg MT, Malone PS. Testing an Idealized Dynamic Cascade Model of the Development of Serious Violence in Adolescence. Child Development. 2008;79:1907–1927. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01233.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Duncan JG, Wilkerson B, England P. Cleaning up their act: The effects of marriage and cohabitation of licit and illicit drug use. Demography. 2006;43:691–710. doi: 10.1353/dem.2006.0032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Flora DB, Chassin L. Changes in drug use during young adulthood: The effects of parent alcoholism and transition into marriage. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2005;19:352–362. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.19.4.352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Gotham JJ, Sher KJ, Wood PK. Alcohol Involvement and Developmental Task Completion during Young Adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2003;64:32–42. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2003.64.32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hagan J. Destiny and drift: Subcultural preferences, status attainment, and the risks and rewards of youth. American Sociological Review. 1991;56:567–582. [Google Scholar]
  22. Hagan J. The social embeddedness of crime and unemployment. Criminology. 1993;31:465–490. [Google Scholar]
  23. Halaby CN. Panel Models for the Analysis of Change and Growth in Life Course Studies. In: Mortimer JT, Shanahan M, editors. Handbook of the Life Course. Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers; 2003. pp. 503–528. [Google Scholar]
  24. Heinz AJ, Wu J, Witkiewitz K, Epstein DH, Preston KL. Marriage and relationship closeness as predictors of cocaine and heroin abuse. Addictive Behaviors. 2009;34:258–263. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.10.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hirschi T, Gottfredson M. Age and the explanation of crime. American Journal of Sociology. 1983;89:552–84. [Google Scholar]
  26. Horney J, Osgood DW, Marshall IH. Criminal careers in the short-term: Intra-individual variability in crime and its relation to local life circumstances. American Sociological Review. 1995;60:655–673. [Google Scholar]
  27. Jessor R, Donovan JE, Costa FM. Beyond adolescence: Problem behavior and young adult development. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  28. Jessor R, Jessor S. Problem behavior and psychosocial development: A longitudinal study of youth. New York: Academic Press; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  29. Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future national survey results on drug use, 1975–2008. Volume I: Secondary school students (NIH Publication No. 09-7402) Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse; 2009a. [Google Scholar]
  30. Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future national survey results on drug use, 1975–2008. Volume II: College students and adults ages 19–50 (NIH Publication No. 09-7403) Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse; 2009b. [Google Scholar]
  31. Kandel DB, Davies M, Karus D, Yamaguchi K. The consequences in young adulthood of adolescent drug involvement. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1986;43:746–754. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1986.01800080032005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kerckhoff AC. The transition from school to work. In: Mortimer JT, Larson RW, editors. The changing adolescent experience: Societal trends and the transition to adulthood. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2002. pp. 52–87. [Google Scholar]
  33. Kerckhoff AC. From student to worker. In: Mortimer JT, Shanahan MJ, editors. Handbook of the life course. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum; 2003. pp. 251–267. [Google Scholar]
  34. Kirk RE. Experimental Design: Procedures for the Behavioral Sciences. Belmont, California: Wadsworth Publishing Company; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  35. Krohn MD, Lizotte AJ, Perez CM. The interrelationship between substance abuse and precocious transitions into adult statuses. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1997;38:87–103. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Laub JH, Sampson RJ. Shared beginnings, divergent lives: Delinquent boys to age 70. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  37. Leonard KE, Homish GG. Changes in marijuana use over the transition into marriage. Journal of Drug Issues. 2005;45:409–429. doi: 10.1177/002204260503500209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Maggs JL, Schulenberg JE. Initiation and course of alcohol consumption among adolescents and young adults. In: Galanter M, editor. Recent developments in alcoholism, vol. 17: Alcohol problems in adolescents and young adults. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publ; 2005. pp. 29–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Manning WD, Smock PJ. Measuring and modeling cohabitation: New perspectives from qualitative data. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2005;67:989–1002. [Google Scholar]
  40. Masten AS, Burt KB, Roisman GI, Obradovic J, Long JD, Tellegen A. Resources and resilience in the transition to adulthood: Continuity and change. Development and Psychopathology. 2004;16:1071–1094. doi: 10.1017/s0954579404040143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Masten AS, Roisman GI, Long JD, Burt KB, Obradovic J, Riley JR. Developmental cascades: Linking academic achievement and externalizing and internalizing symptoms over 20 years. Developmental Psychology. 2005;41:733–746. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.5.733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. McMorris BJ, Uggen C. Alcohol and employment in the transition to adulthood. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2000;41:276–294. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Mortimer JT, Vuolo M, Staff J, Wakefield S, Xie W. Tracing the timing of ‘career’ acquisition in a contemporary youth cohort. Work and Occupations. 2008;35:44–84. doi: 10.1177/0730888407309761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Mudar P, Kearns JN, Leonard KE. The Transition to Marriage and Changes in Alcohol Involvement among Black Couples and White Couples. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002;63:568–576. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2002.63.568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Newcomb MD, Bentler PM. Consequences of adolescent drug use: Impact on the lives of young adults. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  46. O’Malley PM, Johnston LD. Epidemiology of alcohol and other drug use among American college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002;14:23–39. doi: 10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Osgood DW, Wilson JK, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Johnston LD. Routine activities and individual deviant behavior. American Sociological Review. 1996;61:635–655. [Google Scholar]
  48. Paschall MJ, Bersamin M, Flewelling RL. Racial/Ethnic Differences in the Association Between College Attendance and Heavy Alcohol Use: A National Study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2005;66:266–74. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Raudenbush SW, Bryk AS. Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods. 2. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  50. Robins LN, Rutter M. Straight and devious pathways from childhood to adulthood. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  51. Rutter M. Transitions and turning points in developmental psychopathology: As applied to the age span between childhood and mid-adulthood. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 1996;19(3):603–626. [Google Scholar]
  52. Sampson RJ, Laub JH. Crime in the making: Pathways and turning points through life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  53. Schulenberg JE, Bryant AL, O’Malley PM. Taking hold of some kind of life: How developmental tasks relate to trajectories of well-being during the transition to adulthood. Development and Psychopathology. 2004;16:1119–1140. doi: 10.1017/s0954579404040167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Schulenberg JE, Maggs JL. A developmental perspective on alcohol use and heavy drinking during adolescence and the transition to young adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, Supplement. 2002;14:54–70. doi: 10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Schulenberg JE, Maggs JL, Hurrelmann K. Health risks and developmental transitions during adolescence. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  56. Schulenberg JE, Maslowsky J. Taking substance use and development seriously: Developmentally distal and proximal influences on adolescent drug use. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2009;74:121–130. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2009.00544.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Schulenberg JE, Merline AC, Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Laetz VB. Trajectories of marijuana use during the transition to adulthood: The big picture based on national panel data. Journal of Drug Issues. 2005;35:255–279. doi: 10.1177/002204260503500203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Schulenberg JE, Sameroff AJ, Cicchetti D. Editorial: The transition to adulthood as a critical juncture in the course of psychopathology and mental health. Development and Psychopathology. 2004;16:799–806. doi: 10.1017/s0954579404040015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Schulenberg JE, Zarrett NR. Mental health during emerging adulthood: Continuity and discontinuity in courses, causes, and functions. In: Arnett JJ, Tanner JL, editors. Emerging adults in America: Coming of age in the 21st century. Washington DC: American Psychological Association; 2006. pp. 135–172. [Google Scholar]
  60. Shanahan MJ. Pathways to adulthood in changing societies: Variability and mechanisms in life course perspective. Annual Review of Sociology. 2000;26:667–692. [Google Scholar]
  61. Sher KJ, Bartholow BD, Nanda S. Short- and long-term effects of fraternity and sorority membership on heavy drinking: A social norms perspective. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2001;15:42–51. doi: 10.1037/0893-164x.15.1.42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Thompson M, Petrovich M. Gendered transitions: Within-person changes in employment, family, and illicit-drug use. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. 2009;46:377–409. [Google Scholar]
  63. Uggen C. Ex-offenders and the conformist alternative: A job quality model of work and crime. Social Problems. 1999;46:127–151. [Google Scholar]
  64. Ventura SJ, Bachrach CA. National Vital Statistics Reports. 16. Vol. 48. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics; 2000. Nonmarital childbearing in the United States, 1940–1999. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Wiles NJ, Lingford-Hughes A, Daniel J, Hickman M, Farrell M, Farrell M, Macleod J, Haynes JC, Skapinakis P, Araya R, Lewis G. Socio-economic status in childhood and later alcohol use: A systematic review. Addiction. 2007;102:1546–1563. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.01930.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Willis P. Learning to Labor: How Working Class Kids Get Working Class Jobs. New York: Columbia University Press; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  67. Wright BR, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Miech RA, Silva PA. Reconsidering the relationship between SES and delinquency: Causation but not correlation. Criminology. 1999;37:175–194. [Google Scholar]
  68. Yamaguchi K, Kandel DB. Dynamic relationships between premarital cohabitation and illicit drug use: An event history analysis of role selection and role socialization. American Sociological Review. 1985;50:530–546. [Google Scholar]
  69. Zucker RA. Alcohol use and the alcohol use disorders: A developmental biopsychosocial systems formulation covering the life course. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ, editors. Developmental psychopathology, Volume 3: Risk, disorder, and adaptation. 2. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2006. pp. 620–656. [Google Scholar]
  70. Zucker RA, Donovan JE, Masten AS, Mattson ME, Moss HB. Early developmental processes and the continuity of risk for underage drinking and problem drinking. Pediatrics. 2008;121(Supplement 4):S252–S272. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-2243B. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES